You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/15595219

Construct Validity of the Test of Infant Motor Performance

Article  in  Physical Therapy · July 1995


DOI: 10.1093/ptj/75.7.585 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

148 3,516

5 authors, including:

Suzann Campbell Beth Osten


University of Illinois at Chicago Fielding Graduate University
98 PUBLICATIONS   2,324 CITATIONS    11 PUBLICATIONS   843 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Gay L. Girolami
University of Illinois at Chicago
30 PUBLICATIONS   619 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The Norwegian Physical therapy study for Preterm Infants View project

Play as a Therapeutic Tool in Pediatric Physical Therapy View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Gay L. Girolami on 28 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Research Report

Construct Validity of the Test of Infant Motor


Performance

Background and Purpose. The purpose of this study was to assess the con- Suzann K Campbell
struct validity of the Test of Infant Motor Performance (72MP),specijically the Thubi HA Kolobe
test's sensitivity for assessing age-related changes in motor skill and correlation Elizabeth T Osten
with risk for developmental abnormality. Subjects. Subjects were 13 7 term and Maureen Lenke
pretemz itzfants stratified by postconceptional age, medical complications score Gay L Girolami
on the Problem-Oriented Perinatal Risk Assessment System, and ethnicity and
race (non-Latino Caucasian, African-American, and Latino). Methods. Sub-
jects were tested on the TiMP at ages ranging from 32 weekspostconceptional
age to 3.5' monthspast term-equivalent age. Scores (Rasch logit ability mea-
sures) were correlated with postconceptional age. A multiple regression analysis
was used to assess the contributions of age, risk, and etbnicity to the variance
in 7TMP scores. Results. The correlation between postconceptional age and
iTMPpe$brmance measures was .83. Risk and age together eaplained 72% of
the variance in TlMPperformance (R= .85, P< .00001). No dzfferences related
to ethnicity were found. Conclusion and LHscussbn The TlMP has validity
for assessing age-related development of functional motor skills in young in-
fants and is sensitive to risk for poor developmental outcome. [Campbell SK,
Kolobe W A , Osten El: et al. Construct validity of the Test of Infant Motor Per-
formance. Phys Ther. 1995;75:5855%.1

Key Words: Infant motor development; Motor skills; Pediatrics, deuelopment; Tests
and measurements,functional.

The evolution of high-technology


neonatal care units has resulted in the
SK Campbell, PhD, PT, FAPTA, is Professor and Director of Graduate Studies, Department of
Physical Therapy, University of Illinois at Chicago, M/C 898, 1919 W Taylor St, Chicago, IL 60612 presewation of life for a growing
(USA) (S.K.CA4MPBELL@UIC.EDU). Address all correspondence to Dr Campbell. number of infants at high risk for
mortality, resulting in a large number
THA Kolobe, PhD, PT, is Lecturer, Depanment of Physical Therapy, University of Illinois at
Chicaeo. of children who have high risk for
-- - -
r r -
ET Osten, OT, is in private practice in Skokie, Ill. She is also Clinical Instructor, Department of
Occupational Therapy, University of Illinois at Chicago. mately 25% to 30% of at-risk Infants
exhibit some form of neuromotor
M Lenke, OT, is Clinical Supervisor Pediatrics, Department of Occupational Therapy, Lutheran disturbance early in As a result,
General Hospital, Park Ridge, IL 60068.
physical therapists and occupational
GL Girolami, PT, is Director, Pathways Center for Children, Glenview, IL 60025 therapists have increasingly become
This study was approved by the University of Illinois at Chicago (H-89-422) and Lutheran Gen-
incorporated as regular members of
eral Hospital (#888) institutional human subjects review boards. the neonatal special care team, provid-
ing assessment and treatment for ba-
This project was supported in part by a grant from the Foundation for Physical Therapy Inc. Dur-
ing the course of this project, Dr Campbell and Dr Kolobe were partially supported by Grant MCJ bies at especially high risk for devel-
IL 179590, Maternal and Child Health Bureau, US Public Health Service. opmental dysfunction.*In addition,
therapists provide education to those
This work was previously presented at the 1993 Annual Meeting of the American Academy for
Cerebral Palsji and Developmental Medicine and received the Patricia Miller Award for research in caring for these children.
physical therapy; as a result, an expanded version of the paper was presented at the 1994 Annual
Meeting of the American Academy for Cerebral Palsy and Developmental Medicine. Despite several decades of involve-
This article was submitted August 5, 1994, and was accepted March 2, 1995 ment by therapists in special care

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1995


nurseries, little research exists to docu- the movements that are basic to these cesses for posture in response to
ment the effectiveness of their servic- functional competencies in newborns. changing environmental and task
es.5 Studying the effects of these thera- demands; and (5) development of
pies for hlgh-risk &ants is hindered Because poor postural control can anticipatory postural control processes
by the lack of functional motor out- limit the rate at which functional skills, to prepare for destabilizing internal
come measures appropriate for this such as manipulation and mobility, are and external forces. In their model,
population. In addition, available tests acquired during deveI0pment,9-~~ motor behaviors are divided into four
do not provide an adequate range of therapists often use assessment of functional categories: (1) sustaining
items to allow for detailed quantitative postural control as a basis for deter- posture; (2) regaining posture; (3)
assessment of motor development, mining the need for intervention to transitions between postures; and (4)
and characterization of deviations from promote motor development. integration of posture into movements
the norm, in infants during the first Shumway-Cook and Wo~llacott~~ such as locomotion, manipulation, and
few months after birth. The purpose define postural control as control of exploration. The TLMP assesses behav-
of this research was to assess the sen- the body's position in space for stabil- iors in the first three categories that
sitivity to age-related change and to ity (balance) and orientation. Stability we believe to be the basic sktlls lead-
risk for poor developmental outcome is the ability to maintain the center of ing to performance of activities at the
of a new test, the Test of Infant Motor body mass within the base of sup- fourth level. Head control and other
Performance (TIMP). The TIMP was port.12 Orientation is the alignment of functions are assessed in active and
developed for use by physical thera- the body segments with respect to one alert states, for example, by testing the
pists and occupational therapists in another as appropriate for the task.12 ability of the child to maintain head
assessing the components of func- stability in a variety of spatial orienta-
tional motor performance in high-risk Thelen and colleagues13 place special tions, right the head when the body is
premature and very young term-born emphasis on the physical and social tilted, turn the head in various posi-
infants. contexts of actions in daily life as tions, and stabilize or orient the head
aspects of the organization of postural in response to interesting visual or
A Theoretical Approach to and movement responses. Interactions auditory events. A variety of position-
Developmental Motor among environmental and task charac- ing and stimulus materials (ie, toys,
Assessment teristics, physical and neurologic prop- examiner's face) across items allows
erties of the individual infant's body, infants to demonstrate their abilities to
Movement is a way that the infant and the inherent self-organizing prop- perform in a number of environmental
establishes control over the world, erties of the human sensorimotor and task contexts.
communicates needs, and explores the system are the principal components
environment; movement is a basis for of their dynamical systems model of Comparison of the Test of
early learning as well as being impor- infant motor development.l3The two Infant Motor Performance WM
tant in its own right for developing related theoretical approaches, those Available Infant Tests
and maintaining musculoskeletal integ- of Shumway-Cook and Woollacottl2
r i t ~Infants
.~ use movement to orga- and Thelen and colleagues,~3have Available tests primarily assess infant
nize their environment; to cornmuni- Influenced the structure and develop- neurobehavioral responses, social
cate and interact physically with ment of the TIMP. interactions, and reflexes and muscle
objects or people; to change postures t0ne."l*-~9Each of these tests has one
or to adjust positions; to respond to The systems model of motor develop- or more problems that limits its use in
environmental demands; and for self- ment described by Shumway-Cook clinical practice, such as lack of sum-
consolation, such as thumb-sucking. and Woollacott12 assumes that the mary or quantitative scores, excessive
Active movement has been shown to processes of postural control include length, or questionable or inade-
be necessary for optimal perceptual (1) development of sensory systems quately demonstrated reliability or
development? an area that is fre- and central neural processes that orga- validity.20 In the tests developed by
quently problematic in prematurely nize visual, vestibular, and somatosen- Dubowitz and Dubowitz16 and by
born children.8 The goal of physical sory inputs for orientation of the body; Komer and colleague^,^^^^* the exam-
therapy or occupational therapy' for (2) musculoskeletal change, such as iner stabilizes the child's head position
young Infants with neurologic dys- increasing strength and changing body when testing many items, thus limiting
function is to assist them in develop- morphology; (3) development of neu- the infant's ability to express indepen-
ing these functional uses of movement romuscular synergies to maintain sta- dent postural control capacities. Many
and to limit disability. No test has yet bility; (4) development of adaptive of these tests, however, do contain
been developed, however, that reflects mechanisms to modlPy control pro- small numbers of items that assess the
ability to control the head and trunk in
several spatial orientations and in
response to interesting stimuli in the
'Because intervention provided by physical therapists and occupational therapists in the very
early months of life is similar, no distinctions are made between them in this article. At later ages, environment.
much more differentiation between therapeutic approaches is common.

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1 9 5


seated position and when sus-
pended in the prone position, and the
ability to flex the arms from an ex-
OBSERVED SCALE tended position in a prone posture;
1. Head in midline 2s and items developed by Arniel-Tison
2/3. R/L head turn 180" and GrenieF for eliciting evidence of
4. Hands together in midline developing postural control in sitting
5/6. R/L mouths hand and side-lying positions. In each case,
7/8. R/L individual finger movements however, new scoring descriptors
9/10. R/L individual wrist movements were developed to reflect the goals of
11/12. R/L financing objects the TLMP.
13. Pelvic lift
14. Bilateral hip and knee flexion Movements reflecting the qualitative
15/16. R/L individual ankle movements changes in coordination (oscillating
17. Kicking movements and ballistic [ie, rapid,
18/19. R/L head turn 180" in prone position forceful] movements) recently identi-
20. Head lift in prone position fied by Hadders-Algra and Prechtl22
23. Antigravity bilateral forearm movements and Cioni and Precht123are also
24. Antigravity bilateral arm movements scored when they occur spontane-
25. Ballistic movements ously. The presence of ballistic move-
26. Oscillating movements ments (called "swipes" and "swats") at
27. Reaching 7 to 10 weeks postterm is thought to
reflect increasing amounts of recipro-
ELICITED SCALE cal innervation in muscular coordina-
1. Neck rotation with visual stimulation in semiupright position tion, which makes these rapid, forceful
2. Head control in seated position movements possible.24 Because these
3. Trunk extension in seated position qualitative developments do not ap-
4. Head lifting from full neck flexion pear in children with spastic cerebral
5. Head lifting from full neck extension palsy, their absence at an appropriate
6. Head control when lowered from sitting to supine position age may have diagnostic signifi-
7. Inhibition of trunk rotation with head turn ~ a n c e . All
~ 5 other items in the TIMP
8/9. Midline head alignment withouvwith visual stimulation are original in conception and scor-
10/11. R/L neck rotation with visual stimulation in supine position ing26727; a complete list is presented in
12/13. Neck stretch/arm reactions to face covering Figure 1.
14. Antigravity hip/knee flexion in supine position
15. R/L rolling to prone position with leg adduction The TIMP requires an average of 36
16. R/L rolling to prone position with arm adduction minutes to administer and to score,
17. Neck flexion on pull-to-sit and it consists of two scales, one for
18. Lateral head and trunk righting with elbow extension rating the presence of spontaneous
19. Lateral hip abduction reaction motor behaviors and the other for
20. Neck and trunk extension in prone suspension rating the infant's responses to being
21. Crawling in prone position positioned and handled in a variety of
22. Head lift in prone position spatial orientations and to interesting
23. R/L head turning in prone position with auditory stimulation types of visual or auditory inputs.
24. Recovery of arm position after displacement in prone position Items on both scales are scored only
25. Standing when infants are in quiet alert or ac-
26. R/L head righting with tilt in upright suspension tive alert behavioral states, as defined
by Brazelton.l5
Figure 1. List of items in the Test of Infant Motor Performance (Version 2.3).Miss- The Observed Scale on TIMP Version
ing items were deleted after psychometric analysis. (WL= rightAeJ.)
2.3 (items denoted by 0-1 consists of
27 dichotomously scored behaviors
The TlMP is designed for prematurely infant tests.15J6These include items
reflecting the infant's spontaneous
born infants from 32 weeks' gesta- developed by Brazelton15involving
attempts to change positions or to
tional age up to about 4 months after orientation in response to visual and
orient the body in various ways, to
term-equivalent age or for full-term auditory stimuli and reactions to a
selectively move individual body seg-
mfants up to 4 months of age.21Some cloth over the eyes; items from the test
ments, and to perform the qualitative
items in the test were selected from by Dubowitz and Dubowitz16 involv-
types of movements mentioned earli-
those in the previously mentioned ing control of the head in the upright
er.22-24Examples include the ability to

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1995


delivery of effective interventions to
improve motor development.

The analyses presented in this article


address the first two of these issues, as
well as possible ddterences related to
ethnicity of the infant. Our hypotheses
were that ability on the TIMP would
be positively correlated with postcon-
ceptional age and that ability on the
TIMP would be negatively correlated
with degree of perinatal medical com-
plications related to increased mortal-
ity and developmental deviance. The
literature on motor performance in
children of ddterent racial and e t h c
backgrounds also suggests that Cauca-
sian children might be expected to
have lower scores than African-
American children.2B-3O

Method

Subjects

The subjects in this study were 137


Infants born prematurely or after a
full-term pregnancy who were tested
one to three times (with the exception
of 1 child who was tested four times),
Figure 2. Three-month-oldinfant demonstrating spontaneous pet$omance of yielding 174 measures of performance
head lifting in prone position (Obsewed Scale item 20).
on the TIMP. Subject selection was
made in accordance with a preestab-
center the head in line with the mid- vertical orientation when placed in a
lished plan for stratifying mfants by
line of the body in the supine posi- standing position.
postconceptional age, medical risk,
tion, bring the hand to the mouth,
and race or ethnicity. Based on this
produce ballistic movements of the Purpose and Hypotheses
plan, we attempted to recruit 3 mfants
arms or legs, and llft the head in the
of each racial or e t h c group at each
prone position (Fig. 2). The purpose of this article is to
of three levels of risk for developmen-
present preliminary evidence of the
tal disability for a total of 27 mfants in
The second part of the test is the Elic- construct validity of the TIMP. If the
each of seven predefined postconcep-
ited Scale (items denoted by E-), made test is truly a measure of the postural
tional age ranges (Tab. 1). Most of the
up of 26 items (6 of which are scored and selective control underlying func-
sample was cross-sectional (ie, tested
separately for each side of the body), tional motor performance, it should
once); however, 9 infants who were
each rated on five- or six-point scales. possess certain characteristics. Perfor-
tested three or four times constituted a
Performance on these items reflects mance measures (or total scores)
sample of convenience for longitudi-
the infant's ability to solve movement should vary as a function of (1) age-
nal assessment. Children were tested
"problems" posed to elicit evidence of related changes in motor performance;
in their homes or in one of two inpa-
developing postural control in a vari- (2) the presence of risk factors for
tient special care nurseries (the Uni-
ety of spatial orientations. Examples developmental disability, such as ex-
versity of Illinois Hospital, Chicago, Ill,
include rolling to a prone position treme prematurity or perinatal as-
or Lutheran General Hospital, Park
with head righting when the leg is phyxia; (3) medical diagnosis, such as
Ridge, Ill).
rotated across the body (Fig. 3); inhib- chronic lung disease or cerebral palsy;
iting rolling when the head is turned (4) the presence of developmental
Age was calculated from the expected
to one side; turning the head to follow deviance in any of the subsystems that
date of confinement for each infant's
a visually interesting object or to contribute to the organization of func-
mother. For all subjects, the average
search for a sound in a prone, supine, tional movement (eg, visual or audi-
gestational age at birth was 33 weeks
or supported sitting position; and tow impairment, altered reflex activity
(SD=5); 29 of the subjects were full
aligning the legs, trunk, and head in a or defects in motor control); or (5)
term at birth (38-42 weeks), and the

12 / 588 Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 / July 1995


born mfants at postconceptional ages
32 weeks to 37 weeks; a term age
group consisting of 10 full-term infants
and 19 prematurely born infants who
had reached term-equivalent age; and
three groups of infants who were 2 to
13 weeks past term-equivalent age,
defined as 40 weeks postconception
(Tab. 1). The three oldest age groups
consisted of 9, 11, and 11 full-term
infants. The average gestational age at
birth (last column of Tab. 1) of infants
tested within each of the seven
postconceptional age groups varied
from 30 to 35 weeks.

Risk assignment (high, medium, low)


was made based on scores on the
Newborn form of the Problem-
Oriented Perinatal Risk Assessment
System (POPRAS),s1,s2derived from
Figure 3. Tbree-month-old infant demonstrating ability to roll to prone position reviews of medical records. Scores of
with head righting ajfer right leg adduction across body in supine position (Elicited 2 to 60 were considered low risk,
Scale item 15L). Scale scores: @pelvis lrjfs passively off surface; l=peluis lifts from scores of 61 to 90 were considered
support s u ~ a c eand head turns, but trunk and a m do not respond; .?=pelvis and medium risk, and scores over 90were
trunk lift fmm suppott surface and head turns to side, but a m remains behind trunk;
3=pelvis, trunk, and a m lift from support surface, head turns to side, and infant rolls considered to be high risk.
part way but not onto side; 4=peluis, trunk, and a m lift from support surface, head
turns, and infant rolls onto side or ouer without head righting; 5=when traction is Ethnicity/race was taken from each
applied to leg at the end of the maneuver, infant rolls to prone position with head infant's medical record. Only children
righting. who were African-American, Latino, or
non-Latino Caucasian were included
rest were born prematurely. Subjects postconceptional age into seven in the sample. Thirty-five percent of
were selected for stratification by groups: three groups of prematurely the infants were non-Latino Caucasian,
36.5% were African-American, and
28.5% were Latino, distributed across
all categories of age and risk.
Table 1. Number of Tests by Postconceptional Age Group, Medical Risk, and
Average Gestational Age at Birtha Testing

Gestational Age Informed consent was obtained from


Risk (wk) the parents of each mfant, and permis-
-
Age Groupb Low Moderate High Total X SD Range sion to test was obtained from the
mfant's physician or nurse before
assessment began. Children who were
1 (12%)
observed repeatedly were never tested
2 (18%) more than once while they were in
3 (16%) the same age category. Chronologic
4 (17%) age at the time of testing of the chil-
5 (10%) dren below term-equivalent postcon-
6 (13%) ceptional age averaged 20 days
7 (14%) (SD = 18, range=>68) for the 32- to
Column total 33-week-old mfants, 26 days (SD=20,
Total percentage range= 1-81) for the 34- to 35-week-
olds, and 29 days (SD=27, range=
"Percentage of total sample shown in parentheses for age and risk groups 1-77) for the 36- to 37-week-olds.
' ~ g egroup 1.=32-33 weeks postconceptional age; age group 2=34-35 weeks postconceptional
age; age group 3=36-37 weeks postconceptional age; age group 4=38-41 weeks postconcep- Five therapists (three physical thera-
tional age; age group 5=2-5 weeks postterm; age group 6=6-9 weeks postterm; age group pists, two occupational therapists)
7=10-13 weeks postterm. tested all infants in the study. Their

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, umber 7 /July 1995


intrarater and interrater reliability on scores by imposing this measurement Item calibrations are shown in Figure
14 videotaped tests of ~nfantsfrom all model definition on the observed 4, in which every dichotomous item in
three ethnic and racial groups and responses of tested individuals evalu- the test and each level within multi-
with a variety of ages and degrees of ates the extent to which the data fit level items is placed at its median
medical risk was analyzed with the the model. Those parts of the data that dilf~cultycalibration in logits. The
FACETS computer program.33~34The do not fit the model are identified easiest item (0-14) is observed sponta-
analysis allows the investigator to with misfit statistics. If the data pro- neous hip and knee flexion or being
assess whether raters use items sys- vide an overall fit to the measurement able to flex the hips and knees in the
tematically across subjects and model, a scale has been formed that is supine position so that the legs are off
whether different raters use item rat- assumed to be based on a unidimen- the supporting surface at least mo-
ings similarly. All testers met the pre- sional construct (all items measure the mentarily; the most dficult item ( 0 -
determined criterion35 of fewer than same thing) and to approximate an 26) is spontaneous demonstration of
5% misfittingt ratings. interval-level scale (ie, measurement an oscillating quality of arm or leg
units are the same size over the whole movement.22Other dficult skills are
Data Analysis range of the scale), a property that reaching (0-27), and Arniel-Tison and
generally does not hold true for the Grenier19items for evolung evidence
Test performance for all children on original raw scores on items. Data of the development of lateral control
the TIMP was subjected to psychomet- from a scale with these properties can of the head and body in space (E-18
ric analysis for fit to a theoretical Ra- be used to order individuals by ability and E-19). Items demonstrating s d a r
sch measurement modeP7-39 using the and can be analyzed with parametric levels of dficulty, such as items
BIGSTEPS computer program.@In this statistics.*l 0-2/3, E-10/11, and E-16 R/L, are
model, raw ordinal-level scores for primarily items that rate the same
success in passing items, or levels Items on the TIMP were calibrated activity but on different sides of the
within items, are transformed into a according to dilficulty level and as- body. A few items of similar degree of
linear scale of mea~urement.~' The sessed for fit to the measurement dficulty involve use of different body
number of successes (S) indicates the model. Both item dficulties and per- parts (eg, 0-2/3 involve spontaneously
infant's level of function, and the num- son ability measures are expressed in turning the head a full 180°, 0-13
ber of levels the infant failed to pass logits, or logarithmically transformed involves pelvic control, and 0-23
(F) is the dficulty of the item. Taking probabilities of success given a partic- involves antignvity forearm move-
the log(S) and the log(F) can yield B ular level of ability (log-odds probabil- ments with elbows on the support
(the ability of individuals) and D (the ity scaling), ranging from about - 5.0 surface). These abilities, therefore,
dficulty of items), which, if plotted to +4.0 for the TIMP and forming a seem to appear at about the same
for the group, would be linear. The hierarchical linear scale with equal time during the course of
measurement model focuses on the inter~als.3~ The internal consistency development.
difference between ability and d f i - coefficient was .98, indicating that the
culty; thus, B - D = log(S) - log@) items on the TIMP form a coherent Floor or ceiling effece have not been
= 10g(S/F).~lThis statement is refor- scale. Clarity of the measure, as re- noted (ie, item difficulties extend well
mulated into a more general statement flected by the item separation index, below the lowest perfomance mea-
about probabilities (ie, the chances of was 7.38 (root mean square er- sures obtained for the subjects tested),
success on items by infants), so that ror=0.19). The practical meaning of and the highest or lowest ratings pos-
the model for dichotomously scored this measure is that the TIMP items sible for a few item have not been
items is described as log[P,/P,] = B can be separated into more than seven attained by any subjects. We therefore
- D. The model has an additional ddferent average levels of dficulty suspect that the test could be sensitive
term for the ddficulty of the various across the 5-month age span of the to reflecting the ability of children
steps in the item when several levels children tested. who are younger and older than those
of ratings are possible within each in our sample. Before the TIMP could
item. Analysis of the raw ordinal-level be used on older or younger children,
however, further research will be
needed. Items 0-21 and 0-22, sponta-
neous rolling to side-lying R/L, seri-
+The computer program is designed to identify as a misfit any observed item rating that differs ously misfit the Rasch model and have
with high probability from the rating that was expected based on the psychometric model of
unidimensionality of item structure. Fit to the model requires that higher item ratings are achieved been eliminated from the test because
by persons with higher ability (greater total raw score) and that persons with a given functional they did not show consistent differ-
level have a greater probability of scoring higher on easier items than o n more difficult items; the ences with increasing age. After con-
mean square fit statistic is used to identify ratings that deviate from the e~pectations.3~Ratings can
misfit because items are not well defined, because raters apply the item definitions inconsistently, sultation among the raters, several
or because infants perform in unexpected ways (eg, failing easy items relative to their overall additional misfitting item definitions
ability). Misfitting item scorings were deleted from the reliability analysis when unusual child were subsequently revised slightly to
performance appeared to cause misfit to the model but examiners' ratings were in close agree-
ment. A 5% occurrence of misfitting ratlngs could be expected by chance alone, so this criterion eliminate ambiguities in descriptors we
was used to establish an acceptable level for rater reliability, both within and across raters. were able to identdy as potentially

14 / 590 Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1995


responsible for misfit or to better re-
flect the developmental sequence
observed in the Rasch analysis.
LOGITS: -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6
E-SCALE ITEM
E26 TILT L
E26 TILT R
0
0
1Descriptive statistics were calculated
EASY
1
for each age group and risk group,
1 2 3 . 4 HARD
4
4
E25 STAND 0 STEP 1 . 2 34 5 STEP 5 and correlation and multiple regres-
E24 ARM REL 0 1 . 2 3 4 5 . 5
E23 TURN L 1 234 5 6 . 6 sion analyses were used to assess the
E23 TURN R 1 2.34 5 6 . 6 sensitivity of the test to reflect age-
E22 PRONE 0 1 . 2 . 3 4 5 . 5 related changes in motor develop-
E21 CRAWL 0 1 2 . 3 4 4
EZO SUSPEND 0 1 . 2 3 4 . 4 ment, ethrucity and race, and the risk
E l 9 LAT TRUN 0 1 2 . 3 3 for developmental deviance.
E l 8 ARM PROP 0 1 2 . 3 4 4
E l 7 PTS 0 . 1 2 . 3 4 .5 5
E l 6 ROLL L/A 2 3 . 4 . . 5 5
E l 6 ROLL R/A 2 3 . 4. . 5 5
El5 ROLL L/L 2 . 3 4 . 5 . 5
El5 ROLL R/L 2 . 3 4 . 5 5 The calibration of chld performance
E l 4 HIP/KN F 0 1 2 3 . 4 measures on the TIMP is given in
E l 3 OEFEN AR 0 1 . . 2 3 5 . 5 logits in Table 2, which shows the
E l 2 OEFEN HD 0 1 2 3 4 . 4
E l l L ROTATE 0 1 2.3 4 4 means, standard deviations, and
E l 0 R ROTATE 0 1 23 4 4 ranges for each age group and risk
E9 HO MIO/V 0 1 . 2 3 4 5 5
E8 HO MIO/SU 0 .1 2 3 . 4 4 group in the stratified sample. The
E7 INHIB NCK 0 1 2 .345 5 mean score progressively increased
E7 INHIB NCK 0 . 1 2 .345 5
E6 HEAD LOVE 0 1 2 3 4 . 4
with age, reflecting the sensitivity of
E5 LIFT FLEX 1 . 2 3 4 4 the test to development of motor
E4 LIFT EXT 0 1 2 . 3 4 5 5 slulls. The Pearson Product-Moment
E3 TRUNK BEN 0 1 2 .3 4 . 4
E2 HEAD CTL/ 0 1 2 . 34 5. 5 Correlation Coefficient between
E l TRN HEAD/ 0 1 2 3 4 . 4 postconceptional age in days and the
t I I I I I
i performance measure in logits on the
-6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6
TIMP was .83. The Rasch analysis
DISTRIBUTION 1211121111 derived a person separation index of
OF INFANTS: 1 1 1 14133356389401120636543214112 1 1 1
6.02 (root mean square error=0.21),
LOGITS: -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 indicating that the children tested can
0-SCALE ITEM be separated into at least six different
026 " OSCILLAT O .1 HARD 1
027 REACH 0 1 . ITEM 1 levels of ability across the 5-month age
025 BALLISTI 0 1 . 1 range tested.
0 9 R WRIST 0 1 1
010 L WRIST 0 1 1
07 R FING MV 0 1 1 Initial demonstration of the sensitivity
020 PRONE LF 0 1 1 of the TLMP to age-related change in
015RANK 0 1 1
016 L ANK 0 1 1 individual children is shown in Figures
0 8 L FING M 0 1 1 5 and 6 for nine children who were
04 HANDS TOG 0 .1 1
018 R TRN PR 0 1 1
assessed on three to four different
019 L TRN PR 0 1 1 occasions at least 12 days apart (with
012 FINGOBJ 0 1 1 one exception when two tests were
011 FINGOBJ 0 1. 1
06 L HAND MO 0 1 . 1 separated by only 6 days). Figure 5
05 R HAND MO 0 1 1 shows the performance of five infants
023 FOREARM 0 1 1
02 TURN L 0 1 1
013 PELV LIF 0 1 1
03 TURN R 0 1 1
024 ARMS OFF 0 1 1 The frequency distribution of child abil-
01 MI0 HEAD 0 .1 EASY 1 ity measures is located on the same logit
017 KICK 0 1. ITEM 1 scale and is shown in the middle of the
014 HIP/KN F 0 1 1 figure. Average performance is located at
I I I I
I I
I I I 0 logits (obtained by 11 infants) on the
-6 -4 -2 0 2 4 scale and indicates that the average
child in our sample would have about .5
Figure 4. Calibration ~ u r s t o n threshold
e median dzjiculty lew0 of Test of Infant probability of passing Observed Scale
Motor Pe$brmance items (in logits) based on Rasch psychometric analysis (after dele- item 4 (hands together in midline) and
tion of misfitting items). Top: Elicited Scale items in test-item order illustrating dzficulty a high probability of passing Observed
of each step within the item rating scale, with easiest step to the left and hardest step to Scale items with dzjiculty calibrations to
the rrght. tlottom: Observed Scale items arranged in order of dzficulty from items easy the left of item 4, and a .5probability of
to pass by less skilled, younger infants to hard items passed only by oldest and most receiving scores of 3 on Elicited Scale
able infants. The measurement ruler is shown at the top and bottom of each section. items 3, 7, 11, and 23, and so on.

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1995


Multiple regression analysis demon-
strated the joint and unique contribu-
Table 2. Test of Infant Motor PerJomzance Logit Ability Measures by Age Group
and by Medical Risk Within Age Group tions to explaining variance in TIMP
performance measures of postconcep-
tional age, medical risk, and 0/1 di-
Logit Scoreb chotomous variables for ethmcity/race.
-
Age Groupa Risk X SD Range N The combination of these independent
variables in predicting TIMP logit
Total sample
measures resulted in a multiple R of
.85 ( K .00001). Seventy-two percent
1
of the variance in TIMP scores was
Low
explained by the combination of these
Moderate three variables. Postconceptional age
High was a sigmficant variable (beta= .80;
K .00001). Medical risk, as reflected
Low in the infant's total Newborn POPRAS
Moderate score, was also significant at K.OOO1
High (beta= -3.92). The negative coefficient
indicates that ability decreased with
Low increasing degree of medical risk; the
Moderate
simple correlation between TIMP and
High
POPRAS scores was -.29. Differences
in performance based on race/ethnic-
ity did not reach the .05 level of prob-
Low
ability. Figure 7 shows the relationship
Moderate among postconceptional age, POPRAS
H~gh risk score, and TIMP performance
measures in a three-dimensional plot.
Low
Moderate Discussion
High
The results of these analyses demon-
Low strate that the TIMP meets two impor-
Moderate
tant criteria necessary for potential
usefulness as a tool for assessing in-
High
fants at risk for poor motor outcome.
Scores increase systematically with
Low
increasing postconceptional age, and
Moderate children with greater numbers of med-
High ical complications do less well than
same-age peers. In infants assessed
"Age group 1=32-33 weeks postconceptional age; age group 2=34-35 weeks postconceptional over time, the average change in TIMP
age; age group 3=36-37 weeks postconceptional age; age group 4=38-41 weeks postconcep- scores over 2 weeks was more than
tional age; age group 5=2-5 weeks postterm; age group 6=6-9 weeks posttem, age group
7= 10-13 weeks postterm.
twice the error of measurement, and
rates of change were approximately
?he mean logit score is greater than zero because scores were anchored after about 60% of the
data were collected. linear. These findings suggest that it
will be possible to develop norms for
at low or moderate risk for poor mo- decreases of 0.04, 0.04, and 0.12- performance of premature and young
tor outcome; Figure 6 shows the data over periods of 12, 14, and 24 days, full-term infants that will be useful in
for four infants at high risk for poor respectively. Two infants were at high identlrying those chlldren whose mo-
motor outcome. Based on the data risk and one Infant was at low risk for tor performance is poorer than
points from all infants assessed repeat- poor motor outcome. Each was a expected.
edly, the average change in scores young premature infant who subse-
over a 2-week period would be ex- quently showed gains between the Of other tests for newborns that have
pected to be about 0.48 logit, greater second and third tests of 0.93, 1.09, been published, only the test devel-
than twice the average error value and 1.35 logits, respectively. The num- oped by Komer and c o l l e a g u e ~ ~ 7 ~ ~ ~
(0.21). Three children demonstrated bers of days between these later tests was designed for the purpose of mea-
ranged from 16 to 47 days. suring age-related change in behavior.
essentially no change in s c o r e s
Their test has been shown to be sensi-

16 / 592 Physical Therapy / Volume 7 5 , Number 7 /July 1995


passively positioned in a midline
orientation.

The test developed by Dubowitz and


Dubowitz16similarly requires hation
of the mfant's head in midline when
most items are tested. Testing the
infant with a passively positioned
head may explain the insensitivity of
the test in Darrah and colleagues'
research on the effects of waterbed
use to promote more flexed postures
in premature infants,42which we
would have expected to result in
improved ability to independently
center the head. Because head control
is an important aspect of postural
development in the early months of
life and is frequently impaired in chil-
dren with cerebral palsy, the infant's
200 220 240 260 280 300 320 340 360 380 400
ability to independently control head
PostconceptionalAge (d)
position in a variety of spatial orienta-
tions and in response to a variety of
Figure 5. Longitudinalperformance (in logits) on the Test of Infant Motor Perfor-
sensory and social stimuli is a major
mance offive infants at low or moderate risk forpoor motor outcome.
construct assessed in the TIMP.
tive in preterm infants to weekly mension called "motor development,"
Further evidence of the construct
changes in alertness and orientation to however, the test of Korner and col-
validity of the TIMP is found in the
sensory stimulation, irritability, and leagues is not useful for therapists
hierarchy of average item difficulty
muscle extensibility (degree to w h c h interested in the development of pos-
identdied by the Rasch analysis. For
a muscle can be passively extended or tural control because many of the
example, oscillating (0-26) and ballis-
elongated). Despite including a di- items require that the child's head be
tic (0-25) movements are ranked as
very dficult (ie, passed only by in-
fants with overall high scores), com-
mensurate with Hadders-Algra and
Prechtl'sZ2 and Cioni and Prechtl'sa
identification of the initial appearance
of these aspects of movement at about
7 to 10 weeks of age postterm. Other
dficult items are those involving lat-
eral righting of the head and trunk
(E-18, E-19, E-26). Diagonal and rota-
tional components of movement re-
quire advanced levels of skill relative
to sagittal-plane movements of flexion
and extension, as clearly demonstrated
by the developmental sequence of
behaviors on the Alberta Infant Motor
S~ale.~3

To be useful for the purpose of identi-


fying children who might benefit from
physical therapy or occupational ther-
apy in the period of early infancy, the
200 220 240 260 280 300 320 340 360 380 400
TIMP should demonstrate (1) accept-
PostconceptionalAge (d)
able rater reliability among therapists
who were not involved in test devel-
Figure 6. Longitudinal performance (in logits) on the Test of Infant Motor Perfor- opment and (2) test score stability
mance of four infants at high risk for poor motor outcome. across short time periods. Other as-

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 / July 1995


comes of sitting alone and beginning
to grasp objects, which are expected
of children at 4 to 6 months of age, or
whether other paths to achievement of
these skills are possible. Longitudinal
research would be necessary to inves-
tigate this question and related ques-
tions that could be posed based on
Shumway-Cook and Woollacott's
model. l2

Although item scaling is ordered hier-


archically, the Rasch model does not
assume strict adherence to an order
on the part of an infant when deriving
a logit ability mea~ure.39.~~ Perfor-
mance on items that is strictly hierar-
chical generates a misfit statistic be-
cause such regularity is considered
suspect. The statistical model is instead
a probability model that generates an
Figure 7. nree-dimensional representation of individual Test of Infant Motor expectation of obtaining certain per-
Performance (TIMP) iogit measures by postconceptional age and Pwblem-Oriented
Perinatal Risk Assessment System (POPRAS) risk score. Each peak represents an indi- formances, given the difficulty calibra-
vidual's TIMP score (in logits). Infants'per$ormance increased with age (right arrow) tion of each item and the child's over-
and decreased with degree of medical complications on the POPRAS (lej arrow). all ability as estimated from the total
raw score. Because of this characteris-
pects of construct validity also remain performance related to this variable tic, one advantage of the Rasch model
to be assessed, such as responsiveness also warrant further attention with for clinical use is that small numbers
to change produced by effective inter- larger samples before the test is of item ratings can be missing from a
ventions and sensitivity to important normed for diagnostic use. child's test without compromising the
deviations from the norm, including ability to derive an overall ability mea-
the presence of signs of motor perfor- Future research might also assess the sure based on the item performances
mance deficits or delayed develop- TIMP from the perspective of dynami- available. The presence of multiple
ment. For example, we are currently cal systems theory.44For example, if items (or steps w i h n items) with
planning a research project to assess this theory holds, children with visual similar difficulty calibrations also
the discriminative validity of the TIMP or auditory impairment should dem- means that not all items may
in identifying differences in develop- onstrate poorer performance on those need to be used in any given test
mental growth curves of defined head control items in which stimuli administration.
groups of infants. These groups will are used to which they are unable to
include infants with documented brain respond when compared with perfor- One aspect of our research plan in-
insults, chronic lung disease, and mance on head control items that do volves attempting to develop individu-
extreme prematurity at birth without not require use of their impaired sub- ally tailored testing. With this ap-
brain or severe lung impairments, as system. If task characteristics are deter- proach, the therapist would assess the
well as both premature and full-term minants of motor responses, altering child on a small number of items that
infants at low risk for poor motor the toys used to elicit responses might cover a wide age range (eg, items E-3,
outcome. Developmental outcome at also reveal variations in age-related E-13, E-14, and E-17 in Fig. 4) to de-
1 year will be assessed with the Al- performance. rive an initial estimate of the child's
berta Infant Motor S ~ a l e . This
~ 3 will be ability. Then only items with difficulty
done to assess the predictive validity Dynamical systems theory also sug- calibrations slightly above and below
of the TIMP using a test with a similar gests that children might use different the child's estimated ability would be
theoretical basis emphasizing postural developmental paths to achieving used for further testing, and items
control during functional activities. sirmlar motor milest0nes.~5The overall expected to be too hard or too easy
Because the regression coefficients for item sequencing and individual item for the child could be omitted. Al-
ethcity/race were close to signifi- scaling on the TLMP, therefore, should though Infants have tolerated the time
cance (P= .lo, non-Iatino Caucasian be evaluated to determine whether the currently needed for testing with no
children's ability was lower than sequence of item difficulty indeed adverse effects, tailored testing would
African-American children's ability; forms a hierarchy of postural control be useful to reduce the time required
African-American and Latino children skills that all children need to achieve of the therapist and the demand on
performed s~milarly),differences in to reach the functional motor out- the infant.

Physical The]rapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1995


Two other major issues of construct Acknowledgments 14 Als H, Lester BM, Tronick EC, Brazelton
TB. Towards a research instrument for the
validity currently being addressed in
assessment of preterm infants behavior
our research on the TIMP are (1) the Appreciation is expressed to Mary (APIB). In: Fitzgerald HE, Lester BM, Yogman
relationship between postnatal age Murney, PT, Joyce Laskey, RN, David MW, eds. Theory and Research in Behavioral
and TIMP performance and (2) the Sheftel, MD, Lucky Jain, MD, Dharma- Pediatrics, Volume I . New York, W: Plenum
Publishing Corp; 1982:1123-1132.
ecological validity of TIMP items. A puri Vidyasagar, MD, and the nursing
15 Brazelton TB. Neonatal Behavioral Assess-
group of Infants born at a variety of staff of the special care nurseries at ment Scale. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa: JB Lip-
gestational ages is being assessed at 36 Lutheran General Hospital and Univer- pincott Co; 1984.
weeks postconceptional age to evalu- sity of Illinois Hospital for assistance in 1 6 Dubowitz L, Dubowitz V. The Neurological
Assessment of the Preterm and Full-Term New-
ate whether earlier exposure to the subject recruitment. We also thank the born Infant. Philadelphia, Pa: JB Lippincott
extrauterine environment affects TIMP parents of our subjects for their will- Co; 1981.
performance and, if so, which items ingness to allow their babies to be 17 Komer AF, Kramer HC, Reade EP, et al. A
vary by postnatal age. Ecological valid- tested and videotaped. methodological approach to developing an
assessment procedure for testing the neurobe-
ity is being studied by comparison of havioral maturity of preterm infants. Child
the items involving use of handling Dev. 1987;58:1478-1487.
References 1 8 Komer AF, Brown BW, Dimiceli S, et al.
during test administration (E-scale)
Stable individual differences in developmen-
with actual demands for movement 1 Leviton A, Paneth N. White matter damage tally changing preterm infants: a replicated
provided by parents' handling of their in preterm newborns. Early Hum Dev. 1990; study. Child Dev. 1989;60:502-513.
own infants daily routines such as 24:1-22. 19 Prechtl HFR, Beintema D. The Neurologi-
bathing, dressing, and playful interac- 2 coolman RB, B e m e n RC, Sells CJ, et al. cal Examination of the Full-Tenn Newborn
Neuromotor development of graduates of the Infant. London, England: William Heinemann
tion. Eventually, we could also study neonatal intensive care unit: oatterns encoun- Medical Books Ltd; 1964.
whether parents change the way they tered in the first two years of life. J Dev Behav 20 Cloptin N, Martin AS. A criticism of interra-
interact with their infants after observ- Pediatr. 1985;6:327-333. ter reliability procedures for the Brazelton
3 Piper MC, Mazer B, Silver KM, et al. Resolu- Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. Ph-ysi-
ing administration of the TIMP. We tion of neurological symptoms in high-risk cal and Occupational Theraky in Pediatrics.
have found that parents enjoy watch- infants during the first two years of life. ~ e v 1985;4(4):55-65.
ing the test and often report seeing Med Child Neurol. 1988;30:26-35. 21 Campbell SK, Osten ET, Kolobe THA,
evidence of skills they did not realize 4 Campbell SK. Organizational and educa- Fisher AG. Development of the Test of Infant
tional considerations in creating an environ- Motor Performance. Physical Medicine and
their possessed. U1timatelysOur ment to orornote ootimal develooment of Rehabilitation Clinics of North America. 1993;
plans also include a national norma- high-risk neonates. Physical and Occupa- 4:541-550.
tive study to establish standards for --, Therapy in Pediatrics. 1986;6(3/4).191-
tional 22 Hadders-Algra M, Prechtl HFR. Develop
LU4.
diferent ages and risk (and mental course of general movements in early
5 Kahn-D'Angelo L, The special care nursery, infancy, I: descriptive analysis of change in
ethnicity and race, if necessary) and In: Campbell SK, Vander Linden DL, Palisano form. Early Hum Dev. 1992;28:201-213.
investigation
" of whether the TIMP has RJ, eds. >hysical Therapyfor Children. Phila- 23 Cioni G, Prechtl HFR. Preterm and early
value in developmental diagnosis by delphia, Pa: WB Saunders Co; 1994:787-821. posnerm motor behavior in low-risk prema-
6 Campbell SK. The child's development of ture infants. Early Hum Dev. 1990;23:159-191.
defining 'limits its . . and
. functional movement, In: Campbell SK,
24 Hadders-Algra M, Van Eykem LA, Klip-Van
sensitivity for identification of develop- Vander Linden DW, Palisano RJ, eds. Physical den Nieuwendijk AWJ, Prechtl HFR. Develop-
mental deviance. Therapy for Children. Philadelphia, Pa: WE3 mental course of general movements in early
Saunders Co; 1994:3-37. infancy, 11: EMG correlates. Early Hum m.
7 Bertenthal BI, Campos JJ. New directions in 1992;28:231-251.
Conciusion the studv of earlv exoerience. Child Dev. 25 Ferrari F, Cioni G, Prechtl HFR. Qualitative
1987;58:560-567: '
changes of general movements in preterm
Our research to date has ~roduceda 8 Saigal S, Szatmari P, Rosenbaum P, et al. infants with brain lesions. Early Hum Dev.
standardizedversion of a;nique, new cogn"ive abilities and school performance of 1990;23:193-231.
extremely low birthweight children and 26 Girolami GL. Evaluating the Effectiveness
test of infant motor performance that matched control children at age 8 years: a re- of a Neurodeveloprnental Treatment Program
is based on a clinical - .
pers~ective fo- gional study. JPediatr. 1991;f18:751-760. to Improve Motor Control of High-Risk Preterm
cused on the ~osturalcontrol and 9 timiel-Tison C, Grenier A. Neurological AS- Infants. Chapel Hill, NC: Department of Physi-
sessment During the First Year of Life. New cal Therapy, University of North Carolina at
characteriz- York, W : Oxford University Press Inc; 1986. Chapel Hill; 1987. Master's thesis.
ing the 10 Shumwav-Cook A. Woollacott M. The 27 Girolami GL, Campbell SK. The efficacy of
behavior. The test was found to form growth of stability: postural control from a a neuro-developmental treatment program for
an internally consistent scale that is evel lop mental pkrsbective. Journal of Motor improving motor control in preterm infants.
Behavior. 1985;17:131-147. Pediatric Physical Therapy. 1994;6:175-184.
highly correlated with age-related 11 Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M. Dynam- 28 Cintas HM. Cross-cultural variation in in-
changes in motor performance and ics of postural control in the child with Down fant motor development. Physical and Occu-
that is sensitive to dserences in devel- syndrome. Phys Ther. 1985;65:1315-1322 pational Therapy in Pediatrics. 1988;8(4):1-
o~mentalrisk as a result of ~erinatal 12 Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M. Theoreti- 20.
cal issues in assessing postural control. In: 29 Lee AM. Child-rearing practices and motor
The 'pet- Wilhelm IJ, ed. Physical Therapy Assessment performance of black and white children. Res
trum of research activities described in in Early Infancy. New York, W: Churchill Q Exerc Spott. 1980;51:494 -500.
this article will, however, be necessary ~ivingsione~ n c ;1993:161-171. 30 Plimpton CE, Regimbal C. Differences in
to fully establish the construct validity 13 Thelen E, Kelso JAS, Fogel A. Self- motor proficiency according to gender and
organizing systems and infant motor develop- race. Percept Mot Skills. 1992;74:399-402.
of the TIMPfor '& intended purposes. ment. Developmental Review. 1987;7:39-65.

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1995


31 Davidson EC, Hobel CJ. POPRAS: A Guide tient Evaluation and Conference System. In: opments in Functional Assessment. Philadel-
to Using the Prenatal, Intraparturn, Posipar- Granger CV, Gresham GE, eds. New Develop- phia, Pa: WB Saunders Co; 1993:475491.
tum Record. Torrance, Calif: South Bay Re- ments in Functional Assessment. Philadelphia, 42 Darrah J , Piper M, Byrne P, Watt MJ. The
gional Perinatal Project Professional Staff Pa: W B Saunders Co; 1993:495515. use of waterbeds for very low-birthweight
Association; 1978. 37 Andrich D. Rasch Models for Measure- infants: effects o n neuromotor development.
32 Ross MG, Hobel CJ, Bragonier JR, et al. A ment. Newbury Park, Calif: Sage Publications DeY Med Child Neurol. 1994;36:989-999.
simplified risk-scoring system for prematurity. Inc; 1988. 43 Piper MC, Darrah J. Motor Assessment of
Am J Perinatol. 1986;3:339-344. 38 Hambleton RK, Swaminathan H , Rogers the Developing Infant. Philadelphia, Pa: W B
33 Linacre JM. FACETS: Computer Program HJ. Fundamentals of Item Response Theory. Saunders Co; 1994.
for Many-Faceted Rasch Measurement. Chi- Newbury Park, Calif: SAGE Publications Inc; 44 Heriza C. Motor development: traditional
cago, 111: MESA Press; 1988. 1991. and contemporary theories. In: Lister MJ, ed.
34 Linacre JM. Multi-Faceted Rasch Measure- 39 Wright BD, Masters GN. Rating Scale Contemporary Management of Motor Control
ment. Chicago, 111: MESA Press; 1989. Analysis: Rasch Measurement. Chicago, Ill: Problems: Proceedings of the II Step Confer-
35 Osten ET. Eramination of the Rater Reli- MESA Press; 1982. ence. Alexandria, Va: Foundation for Physical
ability of the Test of Infant Motor Perfor- 40 Wright BD, Linacre JM. A User's Guide to Therapy Inc; 1991:99-126.
mance. Chicago, 111: Department of Occupa- BIGSmS, Version 2.2. Chicago, 111: MESA 45 Thelen E, Corbetta D, Kamm K, et al. The
tional Therapy, University of Illinois at Press; 1992. transition to reaching: mapping intention and
Chicago; 1993. Master's thesis. 41 Wright BD, Linacre JM, Heinemann AW. intrinsic dynamics. Child Deu. 1993;64:1058-
36 Kilgore KM, Fisher WP Jr, Silverstein B, Measuring functional status in rehabilitation. 1098.
et al. Application of Rasch analysis to the Pa- In: Granger CV, Gresham GE, eds. New Devel-

Physical Therapy Treatment

Find out how physical therapy research can


guide society's response to this demographic
imperative. Order the proceedings from
PROJLCT FOCUS '94 and receive the formal Virginio residents odd 4 5% vlln tor

~~ - ~ - ~ -
~

Check enclosed papble to APTA


$ I-Mail:Send your c d cad~ and cr&a card# .
membership informationa Inmet Signature

Physical Therapy / Volume 75, Number 7 /July 1995

View publication stats

You might also like