You are on page 1of 21

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/304375932

Reimagining Collective Behavior

Chapter · January 2016


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-32250-6_25

CITATIONS READS

11 1,983

2 authors:

Justin Van Ness Erika Summers-Effler


University of Notre Dame University of Notre Dame
14 PUBLICATIONS   249 CITATIONS    28 PUBLICATIONS   654 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Erika Summers-Effler on 22 February 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Reimagining Collective Behavior
25
Justin Van Ness and Erika Summers-Effler

25.1 Introduction stantial theoretical and methodological innova-


tion. Increasingly, theories of collective action
From religion, recreation, and city-life, to emer- are treating culture, emotions, and social psycho-
gency response, social movements, and revolu- logical processes seriously (e.g., Abrutyn 2014;
tions, people come together in time and space to Abrutyn and Van Ness 2015; Abrutyn et al. 2017;
engage in the business of social life. They seek Collins 2009; Gould 2009; Jasper 1997;
each other to define situations, to create order Klandermans 1997; Polletta 2008; Polletta and
during crisis, and to drive social, cultural, and Jasper 2001; Summers-Effler 2010; Summers-
political change. In all of its forms, collective Effler and Kwak 2015). In fact, many argue that
behavior is alive and well. However, can the explanations of social behavior not integrating
same be said about collective behavior theory? these dynamics remain undertheorized and leave
The 1960s witnessed a dismissal of collective much to be desired (Jasper 2011; Scheff 1990).
behavior theory as it was supplanted in favor of With new tools to explain individual, interac-
rational choice explanations in the burgeoning tional, and situational dynamics, and the thriving
field of social movements. Early theories of moti- interdisciplinary field of cognitive social science,
vation, emotionality, and the effects of groups on it is time to make use of theoretical and method-
individuals were often without systematically ological advances to revisit and rebuild the field
collected empirical data and thus became labeled of collective behavior. In this chapter, we contrib-
as conjecture and promptly rejected. Activists ute to this revitalization movement by reviewing
turned academics issued in an era of portraying what the past got right and wrong, and using new
the rational protestor (see Morris and Herring findings and theory to pave a way forward.
1987). Decades of social movement research fol- Specifically, we argue the field of collective
lowed suit, leaving long-lasting consequences to behavior has been trapped in old ways of think-
theory development. Though case studies of col- ing in spite of theoretical and empirical advances
lective behavior have continued, general collec- and when we turn towards these advances we find
tive behavior theory has withered. that early collective behavior theory had more
Over the past 20 years, the study of culture, right than we tend to credit.
emotions, and cognition have undergone sub- This chapter will continue in four parts. In the
first, we review the major approaches to catego-
rizing the study of collective behavior. Following,
J. Van Ness (*) • E. Summers-Effler we trace the history of major theoretical contribu-
University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, USA tions and perspectives while also discussing the
e-mail: jvanness@nd.edu; erika.m.effler.1@nd.edu

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016 527


S. Abrutyn (ed.), Handbook of Contemporary Sociological Theory,
Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-32250-6_25
528 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

rationalist’s turn away from early theory. Next, tive tend to use “collective action” interchange-
we revisit the prematurely dismissed theories in ably with collective behavior to attempt to
light of recent advances in cognitive social sci- emphasize rationality and purpose behind
ence with an emphasis on emotion, cognition, actions. This change-oriented perspective
and action. Finally, we end the chapter with fruit- emerges when “usual conventions cease to guide
ful paths for the future of collective behavior. social action and people collectively transcend…
This includes not only a suggestion for where our established institutional patterns and structures”
theoretical attention can be focused but also a (Turner and Killian 1987). Snow and Oliver
methodological approach which we believe (1995) define this as “extrainstitutional” behavior
affords great potential for creativity and theoreti- aimed at problem solving. Broadly conceived,
cal innovation. these events tend to be temporary gatherings and
less organizationally based than social movement
campaigns and protest events. They tend to arise
25.2 Defining Collective Behavior during moments of newly available opportunities
or when established conventions cease to guide
The study of collective behavior has referred to action. Scholars contributing to this perspective
wide ranging phenomena. Some scholars have often heavily contribute to the field of social
used the term for the study of crowds, mobs, pan- movements as well.
ics. Crazes, fads, manias and other spontaneous American sociology often fuses collective
acts also fall within this scope. Others have used behavior and social movements together.
the term for the study of riots and behavior dur- McCarthy (1991) states “scholars…have insisted
ing crises. Collective behavior can also describe on wedding the study of crowds and social move-
rather mundane events that take place when two ments…A distinctively American marriage, it
or more people come together in time and space. was one not consummated in Europe” (xii).
These may include waiting in line, marching, While all social movements are a form of collec-
singing in church, rooting for sports teams, vic- tive behavior, not all collective behavior takes the
tory celebrations, or mosh-pits at a concert. To form of an organizationally based social move-
account for such a wide range, Clark McPhail ment. Despite this, the distinction has been less
defines collective behavior as “two or more per- defined because most developments in the study
sons engaged in one or more behaviors (e.g., of collective behavior have come from the study
locomotion, orientation, vocalization, verbaliza- of protest events or from disaster research.
tion, gesticulation, and/or manipulation) judged Consequently, the breadth of cases with system-
common or concerted on one or more dimensions atic research is rather limited. Additionally, what
(e.g., direction, velocity, tempo, or substantive contributions have come from these two special-
content)” (1991: 159). As if McPhail’s definition izations have also been hindered by their own
wasn’t all encompassing, Park and Burgress methodological biases. Protest event research,
(1921) even went so far as to claim the entire for instance, largely relies on newspaper records
field of sociology as “the science of collective which limit the ability to record and theorize the
behavior”. To a fault, when theories begin to individual and interactional processes in natural-
“explain” everything, they lose their power to istic contexts (e.g., Amenta et al. 2009; Andrews
anticipate specific dynamics. This is part of the and Caren 2010; Earl et al. 2004; Oliver and
reason the theoretical baby was thrown out with Meyer 1999).
the underspecified empirical bath water. Generally, early collective behavior devel-
Another approach to the study of collective oped with an explicit attention to psychological
behavior focuses on a collectivity creating social, processes, both through psychological and socio-
political, and cultural change (e.g., Blumer 1969 logical standpoints. The former tended to focus
[1939]; Marwell and Oliver 1993; Marx and on the influence of crowds and group behavior on
Wood 1975; Oliver 1989). Many in this perspec- the individual’s cognition, behavior, and
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 529

emotions. The sociological approach tends to section, we review the major theoretical strands
focus on processes facilitating the emergence of in the field of collective behavior.
collective behavior, the interactional processes,
and the consequences of action. These two
approaches should be viewed as complementary 25.3.1 Transformation
rather than contrasting. The distinctions are less
visible than they were during the developments Arguably, LeBon’s The Crowd has been the most
of early collective behavior theory and sociolo- influential work in the early study of collective
gists often integrate psychological research into behavior. LeBon believed that when people came
their work (Thoits 1995). together, no matter their individual characteris-
Increasingly, scholars are integrating theories tics, the nature of being together transforms indi-
across specializations and disciplines in order to viduals into a crowd and induces within them a
provide the most well-informed explanations of collective mind (1895). A “mental unity” is born.
behavior (e.g., Abrutyn and Mueller 2015; Collett This new state of mind leads individuals to feel,
and Lizardo 2014; Summers-Effler et al. 2015). think, and act differently than they would if they
The field of collective behavior is particularly were in isolation. Consequently, he argued, this
well suited for integration; Marx and McAdam transformation lead to the disappearance of criti-
agree, stating “the eclectic nature of the field of cal reasoning skills, placing the crowd in a posi-
collective behavior…is an ideal area within tion “perpetually hovering on the borderland of
which to examine basic, and unfortunately often unconsciousness” (LeBon 1895: 14). He also
unrelated, theoretical perspectives” (1994: 4). believed that the anonymity of the crowd would
Thus, whether one chooses to focus on the emo- lead one to believe that they were unaccountable
tions of crowds, the potential for change, emerg- for their behaviors; that is, there is a sense of
ing interaction orders, stability through crises, or “invincibility” when acting within a crowd.
the conditions and consequences of collective Together, this would lead to otherwise normal
action, the field of collective behavior is ripe with individuals acting in extraordinary ways. This
potential for theoretical innovation (see transformation became all the more dangerous as
Summers-Effler 2007). the crowd increases “suggestibility” in individu-
als, making them more vulnerable to the influ-
ence and potential manipulations of leaders.
25.3 Collective Behavior Theory LeBon’s ideas were most directly introduced
to American sociology through Robert E. Park
Eighteenth and nineteenth century crowd psy- (1904; Park and Burgess 1921), while also influ-
chology birthed early collective behavior theory. encing French sociologists (e.g., Tarde 1901) and
During this time period, scholars were trying to American psychologists (e.g., Freud 1921;
explain the radical social, economic, and political Martin 1920). Park is often credited with found-
upheaval of urban Europe. Violent strikes, riots, ing the field of collective behavior within sociol-
conflicts, and repression painted the social scene. ogy (Turner and Killian 1987: 2). He argued that
Society was rapidly changing. Attempting to crowds and collective behavior played pivotal
explain this assault upon the status quo, scholars roles in social change. They were “forces which
honed their attention onto crowds. Many believed dealt the final blow to old existing institutions…
that by understanding the mechanisms and pro- and introduced the new spirit of new ones” (Park
cesses of crowds, they could better educate and 1972: 48). Park added that the crowd transforma-
assist the government’s ability to control the tion takes place within the context of “social
masses. This historical period, perspective, and unrest”. During these tense moments of unrest, a
intent filtered the theoretical lens through which mutual contagion – what he called “circular reac-
early theory developed (see Borch 2012). In this tion” – creates a shared mood and a common
530 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

impulse to act. Interaction, communication, and withstood empirical scrutiny (Allport 1924b;
circular reaction then create unanimity within the McPhail 1991; Norris 1988). Interestingly
crowd and afford the potential to achieve com- enough, emerging work is supporting of some
mon ends. LeBon, Park, and Blumer’s theories which had
Herbert Blumer, a student of Robert Park and been dismissed long ago. We will return to this
George Herbert Mead, continued with an empha- later in the chapter.
sis on interaction and the communication pro-
cesses through which people construct and share
worldviews. Blumer argued that “social unrest” 25.3.2 Predisposition
emerges from the disruption of routine activities and Deprivation
or the onslaught of new impulses or dispositions
which the social order cannot accommodate. Another thesis views crowds as composed of
People respond to this lack of accommodation individuals with common predispositions, inner
with a feeling of restlessness act aimlessly, errati- impulses, and unmet needs (Hoffer 1951;
cally, excited, and are vulnerable to rumors. Lasswell 1930; Martin 1920). The predisposition
Blumer argued that crowds come to act explanation argues there is nothing unique about
through five steps. First, an exciting event cap- crowds or crowd behavior. There is no “transfor-
tures the attention of the crowd. People then mation” or “mob mentality” when people come
begin milling about by walking around, exchang- together. Rather, behavior is simply the result of
ing rumors, and focusing on the event. This is actors converging with similar interests towards
when circular reactions “make the individuals releasing tension. Some in this perspective even
more sensitive and responsible to one another” believed crowd participants converge to act out
(Blumer 1969 [1939]: 174). The crowd becomes narcissism and latent homosexuality (cf. Lasswell
more cohesive as a group. Third, a common 1930).
object of attention gives the group a shared orien- Floyd Allport (1924a, b), one of the first major
tation and mutual excitement. This furthers con- critics of the transformation hypothesis, argued
formity. As the group shares a common that innate and learned tendencies predispose
heightened mood and orientation, the fourth step people to crowd participation. These tendencies
is the stimulation of shared impulses. Finally, the compelled people to converge in common loca-
crowd acts upon impulse in a way they would not tions in order to satisfy drives or overcome barri-
have acted if alone. ers to rewards; any action would be the result of
The transformation hypothesis developed shared predispositions being triggered by a situa-
through LeBon, Park, and Blumer portrays crowd tional stimulus. This activation could come from a
behavior as irrational and strongly influenced by leader’s suggestion or from the crowd modeling
group emotions. From a distance, the crowd is behavior. To the extent that individuals influence
treated as a collective whole with universal char- one another, they are intensifying and activating
acteristics. Some argue that this is an oversimpli- latent impulses rather than generating new ones.
fication for the potential diversity of emotions, Allport continued the assumption that crowd
motives, roles, and actions within groups. Couch behaviors are driven by strong emotional
(1968) argues that behaviors such as protesting responses with tendencies towards violence
and rioting become described as irrational (1924b). In contrast to crowds, “common group
because they challenge the normative expecta- behaviors” were non-emotional and means-ends
tions from the analyst’s cultural expectations – oriented. Similar to Blumer’s “circular reaction,”
not because they are actually irrational. The Allport’s “social facilitation” was a reciprocal
transformation thesis remained the dominant col- process wherein actors model, suggest, and acti-
lective behavior theory into the 1960s. The idea vate behaviors within one another. The circular
of a “group mentality” was especially influential process may strengthen when spatial arrange-
in popular culture, though some claim it has not ments and other mediating ecological factors
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 531

increase crowd density, thus bringing nearness to The more extreme the negative affect, the greater
potential activating stimuli. the intensity of relative deprivation. Gurr states
This thesis has been critiqued as a sleight of “one innate response to perceived deprivation is
hand, simply taking the transformation view discontent or anger, and the anger is a motivation
from the group level and positioning it at the state for which aggression is an inherently satis-
individual level (McPhail 1991). Questions of fying response” (1968: 1105). Others have argued
motivation also became problematic as actions that absolute deprivation is also a precipitating
were used post-hoc to explain driving factors. factor for collective behavior (e.g., Toch 1965).
For instance, those participating in dancing For instance, Rude (1964) argues rising food
manias were believed to be victims of devil pos- prices and worsening conditions threatening min-
session and people in lynch mobs were argued to imum human survival motivated France’s revolu-
be driven by religious fanaticism. These assump- tionary crowds.
tions lie beyond the limits of our knowledge;
thus, this speculation is often a better reflection
of the theorist than the phenomena which being 25.3.3 Emergent Norms
studied. However, there are benefits to breaking
down types of emergent crowd behavior; we can Ralph H. Turner and Lewis Killian, both students
think of the differentiation as analogous to differ- of Herbert Blumer, posited the emergent norm
ent types of individual behavior. Cognitively, we theory of collective behavior. This interactionist
can see this as sometimes acting out of the highly approach diverged from the early work of LeBon,
reflexive cerebral cortex, other times the emo- Park, and Blumer by rejecting the “illusion of
tional amygdala, and other times the fight, flight, unanimity” premise. In contrast, Turner and
or freeze of the brain stem. Killian believed that crowds were composed of
Miller and Dollard (1941) developed the individuals with varying motives, emotions, and
deprivation thesis through the use of learning behaviors. Wherein day-to-day life is governed
theory which views behaviors as a result of by routine social norms, Turner and Kilian
learned tendencies in response to rewards. An described collective behavior as “extraordinary
inability to attain rewards through behaviors social behavior” which operates outside of habit-
which have previously been successful creates a ual norms and is the product of a negotiated
sense of frustration. This eventually leads to emergent norm particular to that situation in time
action in order to remove what is perceived as the and space. In these moments, an opening emerges
blockade when previous behavior-reward path- where actors within the situation have greater
ways become problematic. Thus, they argued that ability to shape the attitudes of others.
aggressive action always indicates the existence Crowds and gatherings emerge in response to
of prior frustration and collective behavior a condition or event that generates extraordinary
emerges as a solution to overcome circumstances. These circumstances may emerge
reward-barriers. from the physical world, such as tsunamis, earth-
Ted Gurr (1970) built on the deprivation thesis quakes, and wildfires, or from chemical spills or
by arguing that relative deprivation emerges with nuclear accidents. Events may also emerge from
an actor’s perception of the discrepancy between social problems in the normative order, social
what one deserves and what one is capable of structure, or communication channels. For
attaining and keeping. A point of reference may instance, sudden repression and censorship may
come from history, an abstract ideal, a leader’s motivate collective behavior. Alternatively, in
vision, or a comparison group. The relative depri- conditions that were previously repressive, new
vation thesis was largely used to explain urban opportunities for free speech and assembly may
riots of the 1960s and 1970s. Arguments were also facilitate gatherings. Across situations, peo-
made about perceived injustice, anger, and frus- ple utilize existing channels of communication to
tration because of the rioters’ position in society. exchange rumor in response to the changing
532 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

conditions; the growing literature on social net- and shared symbols may also facilitate or inhibit
works has broadened our understanding and collective behavior. While still a theoretically
increased our ability to predict and explain these and empirically underdeveloped insight, more
mechanisms and processes (e.g., Beyerlein and recently, ecology is central to Zhao’s (1998)
Hipp 2006; Beyerlein and Sikkink 2008; Gould research on the 1989 Beijing student movement.
1991; McAdam and Paulsen 1993; Snow et al. He argues the university campuses had a unique
1980). spatial distribution with high density of students
Unlike previous conceptions of rumor, Turner in small areas which nurtured close knit student
and Killian did not assume that rumor was the networks. The layout of the dorms facilitated
perpetuation of inaccurate information. Rather, quick transmission of dissident ideas, created
in alignment with the symbolic interactionist predictable patterns of interactions, and made
perspective, it was a form of communication to communication between campuses easy. The
construct a definition of the problematic situa- campus density also directly exposed students to
tion in order to guide future lines of action. This a collective action environment once crowds
information could be spread through face-to-face formed, thus facilitating recruitment. Since the
interactions or various forms of mass media. It campuses were surrounded by a brick wall, stu-
may also precede or emerge concomitantly with dents were afforded protection from social con-
convergence in a common location. Rumor tends trol agents, creating a low-risk mobilization
to develop in reaction to the exciting event. In environment.
different degrees, actors provide suggestions for Exposure to emotionally powerful cultural
what has happened, what is happening, and what symbols can also facilitate the rumor and milling
should happen next. Others may be concerned process as people seek to construct a definition of
with leadership and who is going to act first. the situation. For example, in 2009, when anti-
After an exciting event and the rumoring and abortion activists protested President Barack
milling process, a common mood and imagery Obama’s commencement speech by flying a
emerges as the new definition of the situation plane with a banner in tow of an aborted fetus
and lines of action develop (Turner and Killian over the University of Notre Dame, they were
1987: 4). inciting a rumor and milling process. It was an
Questions of motivation lead Turner and attempt to make salient the supposed conflict in
Killian to posit five roles in collective behavior values between President Obama’s pro-choice
situations. The first were “ego-involved” who stance and the Catholic Church’s anti-choice
tended to have direct relationships to the extraor- position. Other examples may be more reactive
dinary event. “Concerned” participants were situations where culturally powerful symbols are
those with personal relationships but a lesser introduced less consciously into situations where
degree of involvement. “Insecure” persons they are not expected. That is, crowds may
sought out crowds for the direct satisfaction of emerge in reaction to a cultural symbol which
participation and security that stems from the may have conflicting meanings or has been inte-
emergence of definitions that make sense of grated into a situation where it is deemed
extraordinary conditions. “Spectators” can also inappropriate.
be found, motivated by curiosity and intrigue.
Lastly, “exploiters” are present to capitalize on
the concentration of people in a common location 25.3.4 Life Course
for self-interested gains. Breaking down crowds
into types is generally a useful theoretical move. Clark McPhail contends that too often scholars
In addition to arguing for a variation in conflate the study of crowds with the study of
motives and emerging norms in crowd situations, collective behavior (McPhail 1991; McPhail and
Turner and Killian’s second edition of Collective Miller 1973). This leads to an overemphasis on
Behavior (1972) argues ecology, social control, theories being developed about what happens
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 533

when a large group of people are already congre- ment downplaying an organizational role because
gated. He argues that not only does an emphasis of the “odd cultural belief” that spontaneous
on the crowd narrow the range of sociological crowds are more legitimate than calculated and
phenomena within the study of collective behav- organized crowd events. Some of these organiza-
ior, but the notion of crowds implies homogene- tional decisions may be choosing the time and
ity of motivations and behaviors. To counter, place and even planning for social control mea-
McPhail (1991) draws from Goffman (1963), sures to prevent “true” spontaneous crowd
suggesting collective behavior be studied in a formation.
life-course perspective with an analytic focus on Spontaneity has also returned to the study of
gatherings. collective behavior and social movements. An
When two or more people come together, a insight present in Turner and Killian’s Collective
gathering is created which creates the opportu- Behavior (1987), Snow and Moss (2014) revisit
nity for collective behavior, though it does not the concept and posit conditions when spontane-
guarantee it (Goffman 1963; McPhail 1991). ity becomes likely to emerge and consequential
Gatherings tend to be temporary and undergo to organization and collective action outcomes.
three stages: the assembling process, the assem- They argue nonhierarchical movements encour-
bled gathering, and the dispersal process. The age openness, innovation, and experimental
assembling process refers to the forces which forms of collective action. These dynamics
bring groups of people together, such as exciting increase the likelihood for unplanned action and
events. The assembled gathering is the moment spontaneity. They also argue that behavioral and
when people are in a similar space and time with emotional priming creates sensitivity to stimuli
the potential for collective action.1 Dispersals are prior to experiences and increases the probability
often unproblematic though they can also take of directing future emotions and lines of actions.
the form of emergency dispersals, such as exiting Priming becomes particularly influential during
a burning building, or through coercion, as when moments of ambiguity and situational break-
the police intervene. By differentiating the vari- down. Finally, they too argue spontaneity is influ-
ous stages of collective behavior, theories can be enced by the ecological arrangements in
developed with greater specificity and not mis- situations. Not only can ecology facilitate crowd
step by creating too general of arguments. Similar formation and mobilization, but it also may
to Turner and Killian’s move to differentiate roles increase the likelihood of unplanned action and
within collective behavior situations, differenti- confrontation from social control agents and
ating stages in time is also a useful move. exacerbate effects of ambiguity and priming.
Oliver (1989) argues that prior to assembling McPhail and Miller (1973) differentiate the
processes there are often “occasions” where assembling process between periodic assembly
actors engage in calculation and planning. During and non-periodic assembly. Periodic assemblies
occasions, people communicate and signal to one tend to have recurring participants who establish
another their intentions to act or not under future schedules to converge at the same time-space
conditions. Feelings of injustice and indignation locations. For instance, churches, which have
motivate future action and future successes build services at the same time and place every week,
a sense of efficacy in collective action. Through or classes, which announce their schedules
occasions, tactics become loosely structured and semesters at a time. Non-periodic assemblies sel-
are open to modification when they are deployed dom have completely sustained membership
in collective action situations. Oliver also argues across events and often have differing motivating
that behind some crowd events is a social move- forces. Communication channels can also lead to
differing assembly processes and can be distin-
1 guished between short range communication
For a complete discussion on how to systematically
record data during collective gatherings see The Collective (e.g., face-to-face interaction) and long range
Action Observation Primer (McPhail et al. 1997). communication (e.g., social media). Nearness to
534 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

events, such as a fire or a protest, may explain of coercion to suppress resistance, he says it
variation in who enters into non-periodic assem- allows insight into various strategies used by the
blies. Thus, for non-routine gatherings, theories state. Of the 14 forms of repression measured, all
about ecology, population density, communica- forms of repression increased collective action
tion, and afforded interactions, can be theorized except for one: home searches. This implies that
in distinctly different ways than those that are generally repression reinforces resistance regard-
more deeply rooted in personal and institutional less of costs (also see: De Nardo 1985; McAdam
histories. Non-routine dispersals, such as those [1982] 1999). However, this also suggests that
coerced by social control agents, also focus on repression at the group or crowd level (e.g., cur-
similar intervening variables. fews) may create mixed results compared to
repression at the individual level (e.g., invading a
family’s home).
25.3.5 Repression In addition to motivating increased resistance,
Khawaja argues repression can also strengthen
Research on the effects of social control on col- collective identity, provide a sense of belonging
lective behavior has produced mixed and even to a group, and can operate as a symbolic
contradictory findings. Rational choice explana- reminder of a group’s shared circumstances vis-
tions argue that repression depresses resistance à-vis authorities. Perhaps one of Khawaja’s most
because it increases the costs of participation interesting, yet arguably under-theorized, find-
(e.g., Opp and Roehl 1990; Snyder and Tilly ings is the notion that during acts of repression,
1972; Tilly 1978), while others argue the oppo- authorities are likely to violate moral standards
site claiming repression increases mobilization which may further draw in bystanders who were
(Khwaja 1993; Rasler 1996). Gurr (1970) argues previously unengaged. This insight can be inte-
the greatest magnitude of violence and resistance grated with Thomas Scheff’s theory shame/rage
emerges at medium levels of repression. Some spirals (1990). Scheff argues for a dynamic
argue for a various non-linear relationship understanding of shame and anger which can rise
between repression and resistance (De Nardo both between interactants and within actors,
1985; Muller and Weede 1990). manifesting as explosive outbursts or enduring
Zimmermann (1980) claims there to be argu- tones, and may operate at varying levels along
ments for all conceivable relationships between the micro-macro continuum. Enduring tones of
repression and mobilization, except for the claim shame and rage can give way to outbursts through
that there is no relationship. Indeed, this seems to group conflict. A historical and cultural analysis
be the case. Koopmans (1997) states this dis- of the group’s histories, with a particular atten-
agreement exists because of poor methodology, tion to emotions of shame and rage, can help
data, and theory. Most models use static, cross- explain reactions to particular triggers of
sectional data, despite the fact that repression is conflict.
dynamic (see Maher 2010) and varies across situ- Koopmans (1997) finds a lack of consistency
ations and time. Snyder (1976) has similarly cri- by repressive forces may also generate moral
tiqued the field for a lack of differentiation outrage among public sympathizers; that is, toler-
between forms and timing of coercion. ating a protest tactic 1 day and then repressing it
Despite the mixed findings, the field is ripe the next is likely to anger and motivate participa-
with case studies that afford potential for innova- tion. From violation of consistency, repression
tion. One such article is Khwaja (1993) response comes to “embod[y] the very message that [pro-
to Snyder’s critique. He uses data from the testors] seek to convey…a repressive political
Palestinian West Bank from 1976 to 1985 to system that is in need of revolutionary change”
address both shifts in form and level of repres- (Koopmans 1995: 32). Differentiating between
sion and the effects on collective action. Because institutional repression (e.g., bans, trials, raids)
the Israeli military uses various countermeasures and situational repression (e.g., tear gas, arrests),
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 535

he finds situational repression escalating tensions ing social cohesion. Consequently, these strains
while institutional repression comparatively less- in the sociopolitical order incite frustration which
ening levels of protests. In Smith’s (1996) motivates collective action. To a fault, this
research with the Central America Peace approach perpetuates the assumption that society
Movement, the government used institutional can actually achieve a harmonious, perfectly
repression by relabeling Nicargua as a high-risk integrated and regulated state. Like strain theory,
destination, consequently making travel difficult, it has not fared well to empirical tests. Tilly et al.
while also requiring burdensome tax audits, tap- (1975) found little support for the breakdown
ping protestors phones, and going to great lengths thesis. Rather, they found it was new forms of
to intimidate and discredit protestors, sympa- organization, with new bonds of solidarity, which
thetic journalists, and academics. He finds vary- incited collective action. Thus, they posited the
ing effects on mobilization from discouragement, “solidaristic” approach as opposed to supporting
ineffectiveness, and even re-motivating protes- breakdown theories. Broadly conceived, these
tors. Generally, he argues when repression gener- theories can be viewed as push and pull theories
ates fear activism tends to be lessened; feelings of collective action whereas breakdown pushes
of anger, however, tended to increase commit- and solidarity pulls. One of the main tenets in the
ment and investment. With a similar attention to “push” hypothesis from strain theory which
perception and emotions, Maher (2010) finds that social movement research has rejected is the
in highly repressive environments, collective notion that collective behavior is incited by
action may emerge when the absence of group socially isolated actors. In fact, one of the most
action is perceived as posing a greater threat than well supported findings in the study of social
the potential response to resistance. movements confirms that participants are often
embedded in social networks and organizations
which draw one into participation (Gould 1991,
25.3.6 Structure 1993; McAdam and Paulsen 1993; Snow et al.
1980).
Three main macro concepts have received the Useem’s (1985) research on the 1980 New
most theoretical attention and development: Mexico prison riots suggests that breakdown pro-
strain, breakdown, and, quotidian-disruption. cesses can contribute to at least some instances of
Neil Smelser, a student of Talcott Parsons, argues collective action. The brutal prison riots were a
that collective behavior emerges in response to product of the termination of inmate programs,
“strain” in the social structure (Smelser 1962). crowding, idleness, poor administration, and bad
This strain emerges when environmental condi- living conditions. Over the course of 5 years, liv-
tions create impairments in the structural rela- ing conditions in the prison gradually worsened.
tions among components of society. As structure This incited feelings of deprivation and frustra-
breaks down, actors begin to feel tension and a tion which eventually amounted into a bloody
feeling of uncertainty. In a post-hoc explanation, prison riot. Useem’s findings challenge both the
Smelser argues that the nature of collective action Tilly’s (1975) solidaristic model and social
is proof of the existence of structural strain. movement theory’s resource mobilization thesis.
While some advocated for the merit of strain Collective action did not arise because of an
theory (Marx and Wood 1975), the Tilly’s (1975) increase in solidarity amongst inmates nor the
supplanted the strain metaphor with a distinction infusion of new resources – quite the opposite, in
between “breakdown” and “solidaristic” theories fact. The processes of disorganization and the
of collective action. The central tenet of break- fragmenting of bonds among inmates contributed
down theories is that underlying all forms of col- to the weak and chaotic leadership structures.
lective action is rapid social change and Even though inmates were in prison, prisoners
disintegration. Crises weaken the regulative and set standards by which how much deprivation
integrative functions in society, thereby threaten- can be tolerated. It was the violation of these
536 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

standards that created the frustration which on rationality is also present in much social
fueled the response. movement research, particularly within the ratio-
Another perspective focuses on the routine in nalist and resource mobilization tradition
day-to-day life. Snow et al. (1998) utilize cogni- (Klandermans and Oegema 1987; McCarthy and
tive psychology’s prospect theory to argue the Zald 1977; Walsh and Warland 1983). Many the-
key relationship between breakdown and collec- orists who have made this move continue to per-
tive action emergence resides in the “quotidian” petuate the false dichotomy between emotionality
and its actual or threatened disruption. Prospect and rationality. As we explain in the next section
theory argues that actors will be more likely of this chapter, this contrast is no longer empiri-
endure risk in order to protect what they already cally supportable. Thus, theorizing which
have rather than take on the same level of risk in assumes that action driven through emotions is
order to gain something new. Thus, when one’s reflective of a “cognitive deficiency” is inher-
everyday life – their quotidian – is disrupted, ently problematic.
actors are more likely to respond in order to In addition to research challenging the “irra-
restore life conditions than they are to attempt to tionality” of actors, the myth of the anonymous
improve them. Quotidian’s can become disrupted and violent crowd has also been critiqued with
by: (1) an increase in claimants or demand for much greater success. Despite popular concep-
resources, yet no change in resource availability; tions, crowds are not typically violent (Collins
(2) a decrease in available resources but constant 2009; Eisinger 1973; McPhail 1994). Violence is
claimants and demand; (3) crises which disrupt often carried out by small groups within a crowd
or threaten a community’s daily routines; (4) or by state authorities (Couch 1968; Marx and
actual or threatened intrusions on privacy and McAdam 1994; Stott and Reicher 1998). Collins
safety. The quotidian-disruption approach (2009) provides a thorough account of processes
appears most useful as it gives attention to indi- and pathways creating violent situations. By all
vidual perception, resource disparities, popula- measures, violence is a rare phenomenon and it is
tion pressures, and other structural phenomena a more useful theoretical move to consider the
which may threaten the true or perceived condi- conditions when situations become violent,
tions in one’s daily life. It also demonstrates how rather than emphasizing violent individuals.
the politically rich get richer and other forms of Collins argues violent situations create an emo-
inequality become exacerbated in addition to tional field of tension and fear. Within these
why resistance does not emerge despite condi- fields, actual violence happens when one side of
tions which one would expect to motivate action the confrontation turns emotional tension into
(see Della Fave 1980). emotional energy, becomes more attuned to the
situation’s audience in order to assert dominance,
or when one side has a fracture in solidarity and
25.3.7 Testing the Myths shows weakness.
The evolving histories of collective behavior
Some empirical research claims to have dispelled theories suggests not that some are completely
many of the early collective behavior “myths” wrong and others are right, but that it would
(see McPhail 1991). Disaster research, for behoove us to understand how and when differ-
instance, has argued that even in moments of cri- ent causes and conditions create variation in out-
sis, people in crowds do not suffer from irratio- comes. Certainly, the history of collective
nality or cognitive deficiencies (Bryan 1982; behavior has provided important pieces of the
Cantor 1980; Johnston and Johnson 1989). For puzzle. The most pressing work is finding a com-
example, Tierney (2002) describes the emer- mon foundation and putting them all together. In
gency evacuations in the World Trade Center on the next section, we work towards this endeavor
September 11th, 2001 as prosocial, orderly, and by drawing from interdisciplinary cognitive
“with a virtual absence of panic”. This emphasis social science to begin to salvage theories which
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 537

were prematurely dismissed in order to recover and Moss 2014; Van Dyke and Soule 2002).
some of our puzzle pieces. Recent research returning to the first movement
reveals that the early theorists had more right
than what the rationalist turn gave credit for.
25.4 Collective Behavior Theory
Redux
25.4.1 Dual Process
Thus far, we have covered the first two move-
ments within the field of collective behavior. The Research in cognitive science, neuropsychology,
first movement focused on theories of how indi- and social psychology have uncovered that
viduals are changed by a result of their participa- humans possess two memory systems which has
tion in crowds, as well as the nature, causes, and been developed under the “dual process frame-
consequences of collective behavior. The second work” (Brewer 1988; Gawronksi and
movement was marked by the emergence of the Bodenhausen 2006; Haidt 2001; Smith and
social movement field (see Chap. 26) and coin- DeCoster 2000). Within the dual process frame-
ciding rationalist turn in collective behavior work, there are dual process models which
(McPhail 1991; McPhail and Miller 1973; describe the implications for the enculturation
McPhail et al. 1997). The rationalist turn quickly process, culture in thinking, storage, and culture
became the dominant approach to collective in action (see Lizardo et al. unpublished). Of the
behavior and often scholars continue to set up varying models, those focused on culture in stor-
their contributions in reaction to the theories of age and action differentiate between schematic,
LeBon, Blumer, and Turner and Killian. associative memory processes and symbolically-
If one were to ignore developments outside of mediated, rule-based processes (see Kahneman
sociology, or work from the more recent emo- 2011; Smith and DeCoster 2000). Research in
tions turn within sociology, one would likely be this area has uncovered how and under what con-
content with where the field of collective behav- ditions actors tend to use one type of memory
ior and social movements is currently positioned over another. Both memory systems influence
vis-à-vis the early theories. However, when one perception, judgments, affective states, and lines
looks outside of sociology and into the interdisci- of action in distinct ways (e.g., Hunzaker 2014;
plinary field of cognitive social science, as many Lizardo and Strand 2010; Vaisey 2009).
in the emotions turn tend to do (Collins 2001; Schematic memory records information
Gould 2009; Jasper 2011; Summers-Effler 2010), through a slow, incremental patterning of experi-
one would likely be surprised by recent finding’s ences which develop into general, stable expecta-
resemblance to early collective behavior tions. Once created, schemas help “fill in”
theories. missing information pre-consciously in day-to-
In this section, we draw from recent advance- day life by automatically relating the current situ-
ments in interdisciplinary field of cognitive social ation to expected information and affective
science to discuss the implications for collective reactions from similar situation’s in one’s history
behavior theory with a specific focus on how (Strauss and Quinn 1997). When situations are
emotions and cognitions influence and are influ- predictable and stable, actions tend to flow auto-
enced by collective behavior. Specifically, we matically (Strack and Deustch 2004). Fast-
utilize research on dual process models, mirror binding, symbolically-mediated “rule-based”
neurons, and embodied cognition to re-center the processes encode episodic experiences and con-
body and to revisit theories of emotionality, cog- texts. This system constructs new representations
nition, and action. In so doing, we contribute to which bind together disparate information from
the revitalization of collective behavior now an immediate context and can often be con-
emerging with, what we believe, is the third structed, directed, and controlled strategically by
movement within collective behavior (e.g., Snow others within the situation (Smith and DeCoster
538 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

2000: 112). Fast learning systems particularly Blumer of “susceptibility to rumor,” their con-
attend to details of events which are novel and cepts emphasize where an actor’s attention lies
interesting, with a specific focus on the unex- and the potential the situation affords.
pected and unpredicted. That is, one is more Susceptibility to rumors can be understood as
likely to encode and act through rule-based pro- susceptibility to prioritizing emerging under-
cesses in atypical moments as opposed to relying standings over historical ones because they are
on deeply engrained schemas and habits. utilizing symbolically mediated rule-based pro-
Understanding that conditions influence which cessing instead of schematic associations. One
type of cognitive processes drive action reveals does not have increased suggestibility because
why some theorists see situations of radical they are a “dope,” but rather because in unex-
moments full of creativity while others see situa- pected situations actors hone their attention
tions of “rationality,” stability, and habit. towards processes in the immediate present and
LeBon, Park, and Blumer all centered their are more open for the construction of novel lines
analyses within conditions of social unrest. of action.
Turner and Killian described collective behavior LeBon’s fixation with the unequal influence of
as being precipitated by an “exciting event” and leaders during “suggestible” situations can be
“extraordinary circumstances”. All agreed these understood as an actor’s ability to influence the
were atypical moments which afforded potential emerging definition of the situation and lines of
for creativity and significant change. Specifying action. Inequality of attention within situations is
the context for the behavior being theorized is not a contested notion; the unequal distribution
important because humans utilize differing mem- of attention and emotional resources is a struc-
ory systems depending upon the novelty, stabil- tural property of situations, not a property of
ity, or predictability of a situation (also see individuals (Collins 2004). Thus, when certain
Harvey 2010). Thus, to understand variation in situational conditions encourage rule-based pro-
lines of action, we must make the microsocio- cessing, there is an increased opportunity for the
logical move towards the situation (Collins 2004, transmission of emergent meanings and lines of
2009; Goffman 1974). By doing this, the analyst action which may be influenced in unequal ways
observes how the situation evokes varying emo- dependent upon the distribution of attention
tions, motives, and actions rather than presuming within the situation.
that individuals are constant across situations. It Turner and Killian’s theory of emergent norms
is problematic to assume that theories of action in has also found its support from the dual process
abnormal situations can be “disproven” by devel- framework. In situations where emergent mean-
oping theories in mundane situations because ings are in tension with one’s historical under-
variation in the situation evokes variation in cog- standing, conformity towards the emergent
nitive and emotive processes. develops through the perception of situational
Here we discover the importance of specify- group consensus (Smolensky 1988). When lines
ing situational conditions. Atypical situations are of action are articulated through explicit, con-
likely to shift one out of associative, schematic scious thought, actors are more likely to assume
processing and into rule-based symbolically- proposals are valid (Mackie and Skelly 1994).
mediated processing and action (Smith and Once perceived as valid, actors are more likely to
DeCoster 2000). Conditions of social unrest attribute emergent meanings as objectively true
evoke cognitive and emotive processes qualita- and become less likely to assume they are an arti-
tively different than those evoked during com- fact of possible interpretive errors or misrepre-
paratively stable conditions. Such situational sentations (Smith and DeCoster 2000: 112). The
conditions afford opportunities for the creative notion that actors may align themselves to emer-
development of lines of action not afforded in gent meanings, despite the possibility that they
comparatively stable, predictable situations. may be in conflict with one’s historic understand-
Thus, when LeBon spoke of “suggestibility” and ings, aligns with Turner and Killian’s suggestion
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 539

that norms may emerge within situations and bodily and emotional states in the perceiver, cre-
align actions even though co-present actors may ating a “resonance” which “is the functional out-
hold varying motives and dispositions. Such an come of attunement that allows us to feel what is
attention to situational conditions, and the corre- felt by another person” (Siegel 2007: 166).
sponding perceptual, emotive, and cognitive Co-presence initiates this attunement as mirror
effects, also reveals the power of a skilled frame neurons are particularly receptive to face-to-face
articulator and a successful frame alignment pro- interactions, responding to even the most micro
cess; alignment processes are more complex and of gestures such as facial expressions (Christakis
situationally contingent than simply broadening, and Fowler 2009; Iacoboni 2008), in addition to
bridging, or transforming symbolically-mediated other synchronization processes which respond
meanings – an insight underdeveloped within the to body posturing (Bernieri et al. 1988) and voice
extensive framing literature (see Snow et al. tones (Hatfield et al. 1995). Importantly, mirror
2014). neuron activation does not require explicit, delib-
erate recognition of the information being simu-
lated; rather, it is an “effortless, automatic, and
25.4.2 Contagion unconscious inner mirroring” (Iacoboni 2008:
120).
Many early collective behavior theorists Research within the dual process framework
described processes of conformity. LeBon also finds that moods can influence the types of
described a “mental unity,” both Park and Blumer memory systems driving action. Actors are more
spoke of a “circular reaction,” and Allport empha- likely to rely on schematic processes during posi-
sized “social facilitation”. While Allport might tive moods, while negative moods tend to
disagree with how LeBon, Park, and Blumer increase a reliance on emergent meanings (Smith
chose to emphasize the circular nature to conta- and Decoster 2000: 117). Aligning this insight
gion, even Allport argued that co-presence facili- with mirror neuron research, we know that emo-
tates the activation of inner impulses. These tions can spread contagiously, often in pre-
insights can broadly be referred to as contagion. conscious ways when co-present. The tone of the
Contagion is the process where members imitate emotions being spread influences whether greater
the emotions, action states, and behaviors of oth- weight is placed on the present situation or on
ers. Interestingly enough, and perhaps to the sur- historical meanings. When co-presence facili-
prise of many, neuropsychology has decades of tates the contagion of negative emotions, actors
research supporting contagion theories (Gallese are more likely to rely on symbolically-mediated
and Sinigaglia 2011; Hatfield et al. 1994, 2009; rule based processing, just as they are when
Knoblich and Flach 2003; Rizzolatti and embedded within novel situations. In addition to
Sinigaglia 2007). Indeed, actors do influence the the interactional contagion of negative moods, an
emotions and readiness for action in other indi- inability to rely on historical dispositions may
viduals in pre-conscious ways (for a list of conta- also give rise to a sense of frustration. This insight
gion mechanisms, see Hatfield et al. 2009). is shared with the pragmatists who suggest that
One major mechanism facilitating contagion engaging in an unpredictable and novel situation
is through the activation of mirror neurons. may force attention towards the immediate pres-
Research has found that human brains are bio- ent because of the inability to rely on historical
logically wired to be social. Mirror neurons link understandings (Dewey 1922; James 1890
perception and motor action directly, affording [2007]; Mead 1934). Contagion research sup-
the potential for perceived sights and sounds to ports the insights of the early theorists who
activate embodied simulation (Gallese and believed there to be emergent processes which
Sinigaglia 2011; Iacoboni 2008; Rizzolatti and arise when groups of people come together in
Sinigaglia 2007). This simulation alters the time and space.
540 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

25.4.3 Rationality and Emotionality potential for revisiting and rebuilding emotional
dynamics in collective behavior.
Finally, the fallacy that emotionality and rational-
ity are at odds has long been dispelled – though
this belief unfortunately continues to linger in 25.5 Future Directions
Sociology. Frequently, sociologists perpetuate
this misconception because of the false assump- In this section, we focus primarily on three areas
tion that individual reflexive thought is the seat of where we believe collective behavior theory can
all rationality. As discussed, reflexive thought is develop. First, we emphasize a methodological
only a portion of cognition and much which approach which re-centers the body. Second, we
drives action happens through pre-conscious believe questions of time and space should be
habitual associations linking situations to revisited with the methodological approach
expected cognitions and affective states. When advocated, particularly with an attention towards
emotions are accounted for, they’re often associ- emotional dynamics. Finally, we argue that ques-
ated with intuition, as if emotions do not influ- tions of motivation, which are rising to the sur-
ence both reflexive reasoning and intuition (Haidt face in recent theories of collective action, should
2001). Turning this misconception on its head, be developed with an awareness of cognitive
research in neuroscience suggests rational think- social science’s contributions.
ing requires emotional attunement (Damasio
1994). The human amygdala, for instance, has
been found to link emotional cues to other cogni- 25.5.1 Re-centering the Body
tive systems underlying cognition and action (see
Phelps 2006; Whalen 1998). In fact, when the The eclectic nature of collective behavior is
prefrontal lobe becomes separated from the sub- reflected in the diversity of methods scholars
cortical emotion stem, individuals have a diffi- have employed. Despite wide variation and the
cult time making decisions and often engage in creative combination of multiple approaches
actions which one may classify as irrational (e.g., Collins 2009), detailed ethnographic
(Damasio 2003). Again, this demonstrates that accounts of collective behavior are in short sup-
emotionality is a requisite for rational thinking. ply. With some exceptions, much of what is clas-
Early collective behavior theory was well sified as ethnographic research is simply a form
attuned to emotional dynamics in group situa- of interviewing in naturalistic contexts. This cre-
tions. Granted, many of these theorists relied on ates a tendency to privilege discourse over emo-
the perception of emotionality as an indication of tive processes and habitual behaviors which may
a group’s irrationality. Despite this, it is surpris- fall outside of discursive awareness (see
ing that it took decades for the study of emotions Summers-Effler et al. 2015). It also makes theo-
to return to theories of collective behavior (Jasper rizing processes of time and space more difficult
2011). The decades-long extraction of all emo- when the researcher’s data is limited by the cog-
tional dynamics from theories of collective nitive constraints of the interviewee (see
behavior and social movements actually perpetu- Baddeley 1986).
ated this antagonism between emotionality and A particularly fruitful method which may help
rationality rather than challenge and critique such address this bias in data collection comes from
an assumption. Fortunately, those who treat emo- Summers-Effler’s (2010) comparative ethnogra-
tions seriously have begun to rebuild theories of phy. Her multisensory approach utilizes the Self
collective action and a growing body of literature as a form of social propioception in order to
has emerged (e.g., Collins 2009; Goodwin et al. account for how one’s social position and the role
2001; Gould 2009; Summers-Effler 2002, 2010; of the body, timing, and emotions influence pro-
Turner and Stets 2005). Among other salvageable cesses of social organization (see Summers-
components of early theories, there is still great Effler 2010: 203–212). This approach affords
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 541

researchers the potential to purposefully align should situate themselves within collective
oneself in relational fields so as to take up vary- behavior situations and record how material and
ing positions in order to develop a multidimen- social conditions constrain or encourage particu-
sional theory. It also encourages reflexivity of lar meanings, emotions, and actions. In this
one’s research role in relation to the phenomena respect, J.J. Gibson’s (1979) theory of affor-
being explained which helps reveal why some dances becomes particularly useful and can be
scholars distant from the field tend towards view- integrated with the pragmatists emphasis on his-
ing action as rooted in more rational motivations tory in order to understand how and when history
while those more embedded tend to see the emo- enables or constrains moments of change (Dewey
tional and cultural motivations. Ethnographers 1922; James 1912; Mead 1934). Situations afford
who adopt an active research role have produced particular emotions, meanings, and interactions
compelling ethnographic research (e.g., Desmond which may become constrained or enabled by
2007; Pagis 2009; Tavory 2009; Wacquant 2004) perceptual and material conditions. An embed-
and Summers-Effler’s approach lends itself well ded analyst can purposefully problematize com-
for creative theorizing of collective behavior. ponents of situations in order to theorize variation
in affordances (e.g., McDonnell 2016). This may
include an attention towards potential cultural,
25.5.2 Space and Time political, and emotional meanings of places (e.g.,
Fuss 2004; Gieryn 2000, 2002; Mukerji 1994),
By re-centering the body, dynamics of space and ecological conditions facilitating or inhibiting
time can be more directly and creatively theo- group formation (e.g., Haffner 2013; Lefebvre
rized. Indeed, spatial and temporal dynamics are 1991; Scott 1998; Zhao 1998), or the way mate-
often implicit and, in some cases, explicit, in both rial conditions interact with an actor’s perceptual
early and contemporary collect behavior theo- capabilities (e.g., Griswold et al. 2013; Klett
ries. At its core, collective behavior developed as 2014; McDonnell 2010).
a way to make sense of radical change. Thus,
when an analyst seeks to make sense of how
change evolves through space and time, she finds 25.5.3 Motivation
herself in the realm of rhythmanalysis (Lefebvre
2014 [2004]). Such theorizing fits well with Finally, in light of recent advancements in cogni-
Summers-Effler’s approach to ethnography, as tive science and the study of emotions, questions
Lefebvre states “the theory of rhythms is founded of motivation and theories of action are rising to
on the experience and knowledge of the body… the surface in studies of social movements and
the rhythmnanalyst calls on all of his [sic] senses” collective behavior (Jasper 2010). More often
(Lefebvre 2014 [2004]: 31). With the Self as a than not, scholars leave implicit their assump-
resource, rhythm becomes a tool for analysis tions about human motivation or they simply fall
rather than simply an object of study. As a novel back on utilitarian rational-choice assumptions.
way of seeing, the analyst unveils that which It is important to realize that the rationalist
rhythms make apparent and that which they con- approach is an assumption of motivation – but it
ceal. For instance, one may make perceptible pat- is not the only perspective. Future research can
terns of stability and change by varying temporal intentionally cycle through alternative theories of
constraints or positions of perception. By doing motivation to uncover novel insights. For
so, analysts will reveal how collective action instance, one could substitute utilitarian
leaves imprints on the social, cultural, and politi- approaches for assumptions of self-consistency
cal fabric of an era. (Robinson and Smith-Lovin 1992), solidarity
To aid future research, the microsociological (Durkheim 1912), or self-expansion (Summers-
unit of analysis – situations – is due resurgence in Effler 2004). Creativity will emerge as theorists
the field of collective behavior. Researchers combat seemingly incompatible findings by
542 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

engaging in theoretical and methodological inno- Amenta, E., Caren, N., Olasky, S. J., & Stobaugh, J. E.
(2009). All the movements fit to print: Who, what,
vations (Summers-Effler 2007).
when, where, and why SMO families appeared in the
“New York Times” in the twentieth century. American
Sociological Review, 74(4), 636–656.
25.6 Conclusion Andrews, K. T., & Caren, N. (2010). Making the news:
Movement organizations, media attention, and the
public agenda. American Sociological Review, 75(6),
In this chapter, we revisited and reimagined early 841–866.
collective behavior theories through the lens of Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford
recent cognitive social science. We began by University Press.
Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988).
illustrating the wide variation in how scholars
Synchrony, psuedosynchrony, and dissynchrony:
conceptualize collective behavior and then we Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant
focused on the major theoretical contributions to interactions. Journal of Personality and Social
the field. Following, we revisited major theories Psychology, 54, 243–253.
Beyerlein, K., & Hipp, J. R. (2006). A two-stage model
with a renewed understanding of emotion and
for a two-stage process: How biographical availability
cognition. We then suggested areas where the matters for social movement mobilization.
future of collective behavior can continue to Mobilization, 11(3), 219–240.
develop. This included a methodological Beyerlein, K., & Sikkink, D. (2008). Sorrow and solidar-
ity: Why Americans volunteered for 9/11 relief efforts.
approach, a renewed focus on space and time,
Social Problems, 55(2), 190–215.
and an attention to motivation and action. Blumer, H. (1969 [1939]). Collective behavior. In Lee,
Moreover, we’ve argued that cognitive social sci- A. M. (Ed.), Principles of sociology (pp. 67–121).
ence provides the foundation from which the New York: Barnes and Noble.
Borch, C. (2012). The politics of crowds: An alternative
future of collective behavior theory can and
history of sociology. Cambridge: Cambridge
should be built. This foundation also affords the University Press.
potential for novel methodological develop- Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression
ments, ways of seeing, and an opportunity for formation. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances
in social cognition (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
new understandings of past contributions. All
Bryan, J. (1982). An examinaiton and analysis of the
said, the future of collective behavior theory dynamics of human behavior in the MGM Grand
looks promising. Hotel fire: Clark Couny, Nevada November 21, 1980.
Washington, DC: National Fire Protection
Association.
Cantor, D. (1980). An overview of human behavior in
References fires. In D. Cantor (Ed.), Fires and human behavior.
New York: Wiley.
Abrutyn, S. (2014). Revisiting institutionalism in sociol- Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2009). Connected: The
ogy: Putting the “Institution” back in institutional surprising power of our social networks and how they
analysis. New York: Routledge. shape our lives. New York: Back Bay Books.
Abrutyn, S., & Mueller, A. S. (2015). When too much Collett, J. L., & Lizardo, O. (2014). Localizing cultural
integration and regulation hurts: Reenvisioning phenomena by specifying social psychological mech-
Durkheim’s altruistic suicide. Society and Mental anisms: Introduction to the special issue. Social
Health. doi:10.1177/2156869315604346. Psychology Quarterly, 77(2), 95–99.
Abrutyn, S., & Van Ness, J. (2015). The role of agency in Collins, R. (2001). Social movements and the focus of
sociocultural evolution: Institutional entrepreneurship emotional attention. In J. Goodwin, J. M. Jasper, &
as a force of structural and cultural transformation. F. Polletta (Eds.), Passionate politics: Emotions and
Thesis Eleven, 127(1), 52–77. social movements (pp. 27–44). Chicago: University of
Abrutyn, S., Van Ness, J., & Taylor, M. A. (2017). Chicago Press.
Collective acton and cultural change: Revisiting Collins, R. (2004). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton:
Eisenstadt’s evolutionary theory. Journal of Classical Princeton University Press.
Sociology 17(1). Collins, R. (2009). Violence: A micro-sociological theory.
Allport, F. H. (1924a). The group fallacy in relation to Princeton: Princeton University Press.
social science. Journal of Abnormal and Social Couch, C. J. (1968). Collective behavior: An examination
Psychology, 19, 60–73. of some stereotypes. Social Problems, 15, 310–322.
Allport, G. (1924b). Social psychology. Boston: Houghton Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, rea-
Mifflin. son, and the human being. New York: Putnam.
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 543

Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, Gurr, T. R. (1968). A causal model of vicil strife: A com-
and the feeling brain. Orlando: Harcourt Inc. parative analysis using new indices. American
De Nardo, J. (1985). Power in numbers: The political Political Science Review, 67, 1104–1124.
strategy of protest and rebellion. Princeton: Princeton Gurr, T. R. (1970). Why men rebel. Princeton: Princeton
University Press. University Press.
Della Fave, R. L. (1980). The meek shall not inherit the Haffner, J. (2013). The view from above: The science of
earth: Self-evaluation and the legitimacy of stratifica- social space. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
tion. American Sociological Review, 45(6), 955–971. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A
Desmond, M. (2007). On the fireline: Living and dying social institutionist approach to moral judgment.
with wildland firefighters. Chicago: University of Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.
Chicago Press. Harvey, D. C. (2010). The space for culture and cognition.
Dewey, J. (1922). The nature of human conduct. Poetics, 38(2), 185–204.
Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. (1994). Emotional
Durkheim, E. (1912). The elementary forms of religious contagion: Studies in emotion and social interaction.
life. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Earl, J., Martin, A., McCarthy, J. D., & Soule, S. A. Hatfield, E., Hsee, C. K., Costello, J., Weisman, M. S., &
(2004). The use of newspaper data in the study of col- Denny, C. (1995). The impact of vocal feedback on
lective action. Annual Review of Sociology, 30, 65–80. emotional experience and expression. Journal of
Eisinger, P. (1973). The conditions of protest behavior in Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 293–312.
American cities. American Political Science Review, Hatfield, E., Rapson, R., & Le, Y. L. (2009). Emotional
67, 11–28. contagion and empathy. In J. Decety & W. Ickes
Freud, S. (1921). Group psychology and analysis of the (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy
Ego. London: International Psychoanalytical Press. (pp. 19–30). Boston: MIT Press.
Fuss, D. (2004). The sense of an interior: Four writers Hoffer, E. (1951). The true believer: Thoughts on the
and the rooms that shaped them. New York: Routledge. nature of mass movements. New York: Harper
Gallese, V., & Sinigaglia, C. (2011). What is so special Collins.
about embodied simulation? Trends in Cognitive Hunzaker, M. B. F. (2014). Making sense of misfortune:
Sciences, 15(11), 512–519. Cultural schemas, victim redefinition, and the perpetu-
Gawronksi, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative ation of stereotypes. Social Psychology Quarterly,
and propositional processes in evaluation: An integra- 77(2), 166–184.
tive review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Iacoboni, M. (2008). Mirroring people: The new science
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. of how we connect with others. New York: Farrar,
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual Straus and Giroux.
perception. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. James, W. (1890 [2007]). The principles of psychology.
Gieryn, T. F. (2000). A space for place in sociology. New York: H. Holt.
Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 463–496. James, W. (1912). Essays in radical empiricism.
Gieryn, T. F. (2002). What buildings do. Theory and New York: Dover Publications.
Society, 31, 35–74. Jasper, J. M. (1997). The art of moral protest. Chicago:
Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places. New York: University of Chicago Press.
The Free Press. Jasper, J. M. (2010). Social movement theory today:
Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the Toward a theory of action? Sociology Compass, 4(11),
organization of experience. Boston: Northeastern 965–976.
University Press. Jasper, J. M. (2011). Emotions and social movements:
Goodwin, J., Jasper, J. M., & Polletta, F. (2001). Why Twenty years of theory and research. Annual Review
emotions matter. In J. Goodwin, J. M. Jasper, & of Sociology, 37(1), 285–303.
F. Polletta (Eds.), Passionate politics (pp. 1–24). Johnston, D. M., & Johnson, N. R. (1989). Role extension
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. in disaster: Employee behavior at the Beverly Hills
Gould, R. V. (1991). Multiple networks and mobilization Supper Club Fire. Sociological Focus, 22(1), 39–51.
in the Paris commune, 1871. American Sociological Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York:
Review, 56(6), 716–729. Farrar, Strauss, Giroux.
Gould, R. V. (1993). Collective action and network struc- Khwaja, M. (1993). Repression and popular collective
ture. American Sociological Review, 58(2), 182–196. action: Evidence from the West Bank. Sociological
Gould, D. B. (2009). Moving politics: Emotion and ACT Forum, 8(1), 47–71.
UP’s fight against AIDS. Chicago: University of Klandermans, B. (1997). The social psychology of protest.
Chicago Press. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Griswold, W., Mangione, G., & McDonnell, T. E. (2013). Klandermans, B., & Oegema, D. (1987). Potentials, net-
Objects, words, and bodies in space: Bringing materi- works, motivations, and barriers: Steps towards par-
ality into cultural analysis. Qualitative Sociology, 36, ticipation in social movements. American Sociological
343–364. Review, 52(4), 519–531.
544 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

Klett, J. (2014). Sound on sound: Situating interaction in McCarthy, J. D. (1991). Foreword. In The myth of the
sonic object-settings. Sociological Theory, 32(2), madding crowd (pp. xi–xviii). New York: Aldine De
147–161. Gruyter.
Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2003). Action identity: McCarthy, J. D., & Zald, M. N. (1977). Resource mobili-
Evidence from self-recognition, prediction, and coor- zation and social movements: A partial theory.
dination. Consciousness and Cognition, 12(4), American Journal of Sociology, 82(6), 1212–1241.
620–632. McDonnell, T. E. (2010). Cultural objects as objects:
Koopmans, R. (1995). Democracy from below. New social Materiality, urban space, and the interpretation of
movements and the political system in west Germany. AIDS campaigns in Accra, Ghana. American Journal
Boulder: Westview. of Sociology, 115(6), 1800–1852.
Koopmans, R. (1997). Dynamics of repression and mobi- McDonnell, T. E. (2016). Best laid plans: Cultural entropy
lization: The German extreme right in the 1990s. and the unraveling of AIDS media campaigns.
Mobilization: An International Journal, 2(2), Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
149–164. McPhail, C. (1991). The myth of the madding crowd.
Lasswell, H. D. (1930). Psychopathology and politics. New York: Aldine De Gruyter.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. McPhail, C. (1994). The dark side of purpose: Individual
LeBon, G. (1895). The psychology of the crowd. and collecitve purpose in riots. The Sociological
New York: Viking. Quarterly, 35(1), 1–32.
Lefebvre, H. (1991). The production of space. Malden: McPhail, C., & Miller, D. (1973). The assembling pro-
Blackwell. cess: A theroetical and empirical examination.
Lefebvre, H. (2014 [2004]). Rhythmanalysis: Space, time American Sociological Review, 38(6), 721–735.
and everyday life. London: Bloomsbury. McPhail, C., Schweingruber, D., & Berns, N. (1997). The
Lizardo, O., & Strand, M. (2010). Skills, toolkits, contexts collective action observation primer. Urbana:
and institutions: Clarifying the relationship between University of Illinois.
different approaches to cognition in cultural sociology. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society from the
Poetics, 38(2), 205–228. standpoint of social behaviorist. Chicago: University
Lizardo, O., Mowry, R., Sepulvado, B., Stoltz, D. S., of Chicago Press.
Taylor, M. A., Van Ness, J., & Wood, M. What are dual Miller, N., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imi-
process models? Implications for cultural analysis in tation. New Haven: Yale University Press.
sociology. Unpublished manuscript. Morris, A., & Herring, C. (1987). Theory and research in
Mackie, D. M. & Skelly, J. J. (1994). The social cognition social movements: A critical review. Annual Review of
analysis of social influence: Contributions to the Political Science, 2, 137–198.
understanding of persuasion and conformity. In Mukerji, C. (1994). The political mobilization of nature in
Devine, P. L., Hamilton, D. L., & Ostrom, T. M. (Eds.), seventeenth century French formal gardens. Theory
Social cognition: Impact on social psychology and Society, 23, 651–677.
(pp. 259–291). Academic. Muller, E. N., & Weede, E. (1990). Cross-national varia-
Maher, T. V. (2010). Threat, resistance, and collective tion in political violence: A rational approach. Journal
action: The cases of Sobibor, Treblinka, and of Conflict Resolution, 34, 624–651.
Auschwitz. American Sociological Review, 75(2), Norris, J. (1988). Fire in a crowded theater. International
252–272. Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 6, 7–26.
Martin, E. D. (1920). The behavior of crowds: A psycho- Oliver, P. (1989). Bringing the crowd back in: The nonor-
logical study. New York: Harper and Brothers. ganizational elements of social movements. Research
Marwell, G., & Oliver, P. (1993). The critical mass in col- in Social Movements, Conflicts and Change, 11,
lective action: A micro-social theory. Cambridge: 1–130.
Cambridge University Press. Oliver, P. E., & Meyer, D. J. (1999). How events enter the
Marx, G. T., & McAdam, D. (1994). Collective behavior public sphere: Conflict, location, and sponsorship
and social movements: Process and structure. in local newspaper coverage of public events.
Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. American Journal of Sociology, 105(1), 38–87.
Marx, G. T., & Wood, J. L. (1975). Strands of theory and Opp, K.-D., & Roehl, W. (1990). Repression, micromobi-
research in collective behavior. Annual Review of lization, and political protest. Social Forces, 69,
Sociology, 1, 363–428. 521–547.
McAdam, D. ([1982] 1999). Political process and the Pagis, M. (2009). Embodied self-reflexivity. Social
development of black insurgency, 1930–1970. Psychology Quarterly, 72(3), 265–283.
Chicago: University of Chicago. Park, R. E. (1904). The crowd and the public. Chicago:
McAdam, D., & Paulsen, R. (1993). Specifying the rela- University of Chicago Press.
tionship between social ties and activism. American Park, R. E. (1972). The crowd and the public and other
Journal of Sociology, 99(3), 640–667. essays. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
25 Reimagining Collective Behavior 545

Park, R. E., & Burgess, E. W. (1921). Introduction to the Snow, D. A., Cress, D. M., Downey, L., & Jones, A. W.
science of sociology. Chicago: University of Chicago (1998). Disrupting the “quotidian”: Reconceptualizing
Press. the relationship between breakdown and the emer-
Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: Insights gence of collective action. Mobilization, 3, 1–22.
from studies of the human amygdala. Annual Review Snow, D. A., Benford, R. D., McCammon, H. J., Hewitt,
of Psychology, 57, 27–53. L., & Fitzgerald, S. (2014). The emergence, develop-
Polletta, F. (2008). Culture and social movements. The ment, and future of the framing perspective: 25+ years
Annals of the American Academy of Political and since “frame alignment”. Mobilization: An
Social Science, 619(78). International Journal, 19(1), 23–45.
Polletta, F., & Jasper, J. M. (2001). Collective identity and Snyder, D. (1976). Theoretical and methodological prob-
social movements. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), lems in the analysis of governmental coercion and col-
283–305. lective violence. Journal of Political and Military
Rasler, K. (1996). Concessions, repression and political Sociology, 4, 277–293.
protest in the Iranian revolution. American Snyder, D., & Tilly, C. (1972). Hardship and collective
Sociological Review, 61, 132–152. violence in France, 1830 to 1960. American
Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2007). Mirror neurons Sociological Review, 37, 520–532.
and motor intentionality. Functional Neurology, 22(4), Stott, C., & Reicher, S. (1998). Crowd action as inter-
205–210. group process: Introducing the police perspective.
Robinson, D. T., & Smith-Lovin, L. (1992). Selective European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 508–529.
interaction as a strategy for identity maintenance: An Strack, F., & Duetsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive
affect control model. Social Psychology Quarterly, determinants of social behavior. Personality and
55(1), 12–28. Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.
Rude, G. (1964). The crowd in history, 1730–1848. Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1997). A cognitive theory of cul-
New York: Wiley. tural meaning. New York: Cambridge University
Scheff, T. J. (1990). Microsociology: Discourse, emotion, Press.
and social structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Summers-Effler, E. (2002). The micro potential for social
Press. change: Emotion, consciousness, and social move-
Scott, J. C. (1998). Seeing like a state: How certain ment formation. Sociological Theory, 20(1), 41–60.
schemes to improve the human condition have failed. Summers-Effler, E. (2004). A theory of the self, emotion,
New Haven: Yale University Press. and culture. Advances in Group Processes, 21,
Siegel, D. (2007). The mindful brain: Reflection and 273–308.
attunement in the cultivation of well-being. New York: Summers-Effler, E. (2007). Vortexes of involvement:
W.W. Norton. Social systems as turbulent flow. Philosophy of the
Smelser, N. J. (1962). Theory of collective behavior. Social Sciences, 37(4), 433–448.
New York: Free Press. Summers-Effler, E. (2010). Laughing saints and righ-
Smith, C. (1996). Resisting Reagan: The U.S. Central teous heroes: Emotional rhythms in social movement
American peace movement. Chicago: The University groups. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
of Chicago Press. Summers-Effler, E., & Kwak, D. (2015). Weber’s missing
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models mystics: Inner-worldly mystical practices and the
in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual inte- micro potential for social change. Theory and Society,
gration and links to underlying memory systems. 44, 251–282.
Personality and Social Psychological Review, 4(2), Summers-Effler, E., Van Ness, J., & Hausmann, C. (2015).
108–131. Peeking in the black box: Studying, theorizing, and
Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connec- representing the micro-foundations of day-to-day
tionism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–74. interactions. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography,
Snow, D. A., & Moss, D. M. (2014). Protest on the fly: 44(4), 450–479.
Toward a theory of spontaneity in the dynamics of Tarde, G. (1901). Opinion and the crowd. In T. Clark
protest and social movements. American Sociological (Ed.), Gabriel Tarde: On communication and social
Review, 79(6), 1122–1143. influence. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Snow, D. A., & Oliver, P. (1995). Social movements and Tavory, I. (2009). Of yarmulkes and categories: Delegating
collective behavior: Social psychological dimensions boundaries and the phenomenology of interactional
and considerations. In K. S. Cook, G. A. Fine, & J. S. expectation. Theory and Society, 39(1), 49–68.
House (Eds.), Sociological perspectives on social psy- Thoits, P. A. (1995). Social psychology: The interplay
chology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. between sociology and psychology. Social Forces, 73,
Snow, D. A., Zurcher, L. A., Jr., & Ekland-Olson, S. 1231–1243.
(1980). Social networks and social movements: A Tierney, K. (2002). Strength of a City: A disaster research
microstructural approach to differential recruitment. perspective on the world trade center attack. Social
American Sociological Review, 45(5), 787–801. Science Research Council.
546 J. Van Ness and E. Summers-Effler

Tilly, C. (1978). From mobilization to revolution. Reading: levels of patriot and militia organizing in the United
Addison-Wesley. States. Social Problems, 49(2), 497–520.
Tilly, C., Tilly, L., & Tilly, R. (1975). The Rebellious Wacquant, L. (2004). Body and soul: Ethnographic note-
Century, 1830–1930. Cambridge: Harvard University books of an apprentice-boxer. New York: Oxford
Press. University Press.
Toch, H. (1965). The psychology of social movements. Walsh, E. J., & Warland, R. H. (1983). Social movement
Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill Co. involvement in the wake of a nuclear accident:
Turner, R. H., & Killian, L. M. (1972). Collective behav- Activists and free riders in the TMI area. American
ior (2nd ed.). New York: Prentice-Hall. Sociological Review, 48(6), 764–780.
Turner, R. H., & Killian, L. M. (1987). Collective behav- Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity:
ior (3rd ed.). New York: Prentice Hall. Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala.
Turner, J. H., & Stets, J. E. (2005). The sociology of emo- Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7,
tions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 177–188.
Useem, B. (1985). Disorganization and the New Mexico Zhao, D. (1998). Ecologies of social movements: Student
prison riot of 1980. American Sociological Review, mobilization during the 1989 prodemocracy move-
50(5), 677–688. ment in Beijing. American Journal of Sociology,
Vaisey, S. (2009). Motivation and justification: A dual‐ 103(6), 1493–1529.
process model of culture in action. American Journal Zimmerman, E. (1980). Macro-comparative research on
of Sociology, 114(6), 1675–16715. political protest. In Gurr, T. R. (Ed), Handbook of
Van Dyke, N., & Soule, S. A. (2002). Structural social political conflict: Theory and research (pp. 167–237).
change and the mobilizing effect of threat: Explaining New York: The Free Press.

View publication stats

You might also like