You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/222286857

The basic empathy scale: A French validation of a measure of empathy in


youth. Personality and Individual Diefferences, 46, 160-165

Article  in  Personality and Individual Differences · January 2009


DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.020

CITATIONS READS

57 9,936

4 authors, including:

Fanny D'Ambrosio Chrystel Besche


Cabinet Libéral Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne
3 PUBLICATIONS   198 CITATIONS    92 PUBLICATIONS   1,408 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Psychological assessment tools View project

Le fou dangereux et le risque en question (The crazy monster and the risk in question) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Chrystel Besche on 09 March 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 160–165

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The basic empathy scale: A French validation of a measure of empathy in youth


Fanny D’Ambrosio *, Marie Olivier, Davina Didon, Chrystel Besche
University of Reims Champagne-Ardenne, UFR Lettres et Sciences Humaines, CLEA 57, Rue Pierre Taittinger, F-51096 Reims Cedex, France

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The aim of our study was to validate a French version of the basic empathy scale (BES Jolliffe and
Received 2 April 2008 Farrington (2006)) which measures both cognitive and affective empathy with a sample of 446
Received in revised form 15 September adolescents. The confirmatory factorial analysis showed that the French scale has the same factorial
2008
structure as in the original version with two-factors (cognitive and affective empathy). The BES exhibited
Accepted 26 September 2008
Available online 11 November 2008
satisfactory internal, test–retest and discriminant validity. Moreover, the females’ superiority in empathy
was replicated. So, the BES is one of the first empathy scales to be made available in French and possesses
good psychometric qualities.
Keywords:
Empathy
Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Adolescence
Basic Empathy Scale
Sex difference

1. Introduction behaviour on others (Fernandez & Marshall, 2003; Jolliffe &


Farrington, 2004). So, empathy is a central concept in the context
1.1. Definition of social interactions.

‘‘Empathy” is a multidimensional concept and has several defini-


tions. There is a relative consensus for two of its main dimensions 1.2. Empathy measures
(Lawrence, Shaw, Baker, Baron-Cohen, & David, 2004): cognitive
empathy (the intellectual apprehension of another’s mental state, Consequently, several scales have been developed to measure
currently associated with the theory of mind, ToM) and affective empathy in Anglo–Saxon countries, based on one or several of its
empathy (possession of an appropriate emotional response when dimensions: the balanced emotional empathy scale (BEES
confronted with the mental state attributed to another person). Mehrabian (2000)) (emotional empathy); the interpersonal reac-
Empathy is essential to our comprehension of social behaviour tivity index (IRI Davis (1980)) (‘‘perspective-taking”, ‘‘fantasy”,
(Decety, 2002), to our regulation of our own social behaviour ‘‘empathic-concern” and ‘‘personal distress” which could be sum-
(Findlay, Girardi, & Coplan, 2006; Laible, Carlo, & Roesch, 2004) marized into both cognitive and affective empathy); the empathic
and to the acquisition of morality (Berthoz, Grèzes, Armony, quotient (EQ Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright (2004)) (both cogni-
Passingham, & Dolan, 2006; Hoffman, 2000). There is also a link tive and affective empathy, and social skills). The IRI (Davis,
between cognitive and total empathy and alexithymia (Davies, 1980) is considered as the best measure of empathy developed
Stankov, & Roberts, 1998; Jolliffe & Farrington, 2006) with the (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004; Muncer & Ling, 2006) but
Toronto–Alexithymia scale – 20, (TAS-20; Bagby, Parker, and the fantasy and personal distress subscales of the IRI were regu-
Taylor (1994)). Consequently, someone who finds it difficult to larly criticized (Beven, O’Brien-Malone, & Hall, 2004; Lawrence
express/identify his/her own feelings is likely to have similar dif- et al., 2004). As for France Völlm et al. (2006), created an empathy
ficulties when confronted with another person’s emotions. This task, which interestingly differ from others as they used ToM and
may, therefore, also have an influence on the ability to empathize emotional empathy stimuli in one visual task (thus avoiding the
(Guttman & Laporte, 2002). Moreover, recent studies have shown difficulties inferred by a paper assessment). However, all these
that a lack of empathy could be a factor leading to aggressive scales were not specifics to adolescents while Jolliffe and Farring-
behaviour (Jolliffe & Farrington, 2004; Mehrabian, 1997) and ton (2004, 2006) thought that in youth, a deficit of empathy is
reducing the subject’s ability to perceive the effect of their own really important to detect as there are strong relationships be-
tween low empathy and violent behaviour and it may play a cru-
cial role in mid-adolescence, a period that has importance for
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +33 3 26 91 37 13; fax: +33 3 26 91 37 19.
adulthood (Lynam, Caspi, Moffitt, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber,
E-mail address: fanny.dambrosio@etudiant.univ-reims.fr (F. D’Ambrosio). 2007).

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.020
F. D’Ambrosio et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 160–165 161

1.3. The basic empathy scale 2. Method

So, Jolliffe and Farrington have developed the basic empathy 2.1. Participants
scale (BES, 2006) a brief, accessible and easy-to-score self-report
questionnaire with an English sample of 720 adolescents (aged Four hundred and forty six adolescents of the same age as those
approximately 15 years). For the authors, one of the main defects studied by Jolliffe and Farrington (2006) participated in the test
of the earlier measures of empathy (e.g. Hogan empathy scale (250 girls, 196 boys; mean age 14.8 years, SD = 1.14) and attended
Hogan (1969); questionnaire measure of emotional empathy, various state secondary schools in a number of French towns. The
QMEE Mehrabian and Epstein (1972)) is that they only focus on retest phase was attended by 153 of the 446 original adolescents
one aspect of empathy. Although, we need to take account of both (100 girls, 53 boys; mean age 14.8 years, SD = 1.28). All the partic-
cognitive and affective empathy: certain individuals (e.g. paedo- ipants were native French speakers and participated voluntarily
philes) may exhibit normal cognitive empathy but have a poor le- after both the students and their parents gave informed consent.
vel of affective empathy (Nichols & Molinder, 1996, cited in Covell No one declined to participate.
& Scalora, 2002; Rice, Chaplin, Harris, & Coutts, 1994). So, the BES
assesses affective and cognitive empathy among 20-items (verified 2.2. Materials and procedure
by a confirmatory analysis).
Jolliffe and Farrington (2006), like others (Baron-Cohen, Richler, The test session was administered collectively to groups of
Bisarya, Gurunathan, & Wheelwright, 2003; Baron-Cohen & about 20 participants each. All questionnaires were in French.
Wheelwright, 2004; Davis, 1980; Lawrence et al., 2004; Mehrabian,
2000), observed that girls have a better level of empathy, particu- 2.3. The BES
larly in affective empathy, without observing a link between the
scales of empathy and social desirability (a valid measure has to They saw the French version of the BES (Jolliffe & Farrington,
measure participant’s real empathy rather than how empathic a 2006). It was back-translated by a French senior lecturer in English
person wishes to be perceived). It remains unclear; however, literature, and modifications were made in consultation with the
whether females are really more empathic than males (i.e. the re- authors. The final version was approved by the two original trans-
sult of differences in socialization and social roles associated with lators and a native English speaker, fluent in French. This scale fea-
females) or if is a result of the method of assessing empathy (i.e. a tures 20-items (appendix available from the authors): 9 for
result of biased responding in self-reports). Even though some cognitive empathy (items 3, 6, 9, 10, 12, 14, 16, 19, 20), and 11
behavioural data (Han, Fan, & Mao, 2008; Schulte-Rüther, for affective empathy (items 1, 2, 4, 5, 7, 8, 11, 13, 15, 17, 18). Re-
Markowitsch, Shah, Fink, & Piefke, 2008) suggest that females sponses were made on a five-point Likert scale ranging from
and males rely on different strategies (brain networks and electro- ‘‘strongly disagree” to ‘‘strongly agree”. The scores could range
physiological amplitudes) when assessing their own emotions in from 1 (deficit in empathy) to 100 (high level of empathy).
response to other people and this could explain why females have
better scores on empathy. 2.4. The TAS-20
Unfortunately, in France there was no validated scale to measure
empathy and, in particular, in adolescents. So, the validation of a The participants also completed the TAS-20 (Bagby et al.
French empathy scale could be an important contribution to the (1994); French version by Loas et al. (2001)) in order to assess alex-
clinical evaluation of empathy in French adolescents: the BES could ithymia in this sample. The TAS is a 20-item self-report question-
be one of the first empathy scales to be made available in French. To naire in which adolescents are asked to respond to statements on
evaluate the validity of the BES, we translated the BES into French a five-point Likert scale (from ‘‘strongly disagree” to ‘‘strongly
and here we report on the BES’s psychometric properties, as well agree”).
as on the correspondence between four empathy questionnaires
and measures of the ability to communicate emotional state. 2.5. The BEES

1.4. Hypothesis We used the French version of the BEES (Mehrabian, 2000)1 be-
cause of the absence of any scale measuring empathy in France. This
First, the French scale should have the same factorial structure is a 30-items measure of emotional empathy only. The subjects are
as the original scale (with 2 correlated factors: cognitive and affec- asked to respond to statements on a scale ranging from 4 (very
tive empathy). Secondly, girls should score significantly higher strong disagreement) to +4 (very strong agreement) depending on
than boys on the BES and most particularly in affective empathy. the degree of empathy. The score is calculated on the basis of the re-
There should be also a significant positive correlation between sponses to all of the 15 positively worded items and by subtracting
the levels of cognitive and affective empathy measured by the from this quantity the sum of the responses to all of the 15 nega-
BES (but also measured by the others scales: we expected a signif- tively worded items (M = 45, SD = 24, with a norm for males:
icant and positive correlation between the BEES and the BES and M = 29, SD = 28 and for females: M = 60, SD = 21).
also between the non-verbal test (‘‘empathy” items and ‘‘ToM”
items) and the BES). Indeed both reflect an aspect of global empa- 2.6. The non-verbal test
thy and involve reactions to the emotions of others. Moreover
Jolliffe and Farrington (2006), found that there was a negative Secondly, we used Völlm et al.’s non-verbal test (2006, see
and significant correlation between BES and the global scale of Völlm et al. (2006) for example). This test is, in part, an adaptation
the TAS-20 (Bagby et al., 1994). So, we should observe the same re- of Sarfati, Hardy-Baylé, Besche, and Widlöcher (1997) which was
sult and more especially between cognitive empathy and the developed for use in a neuro-imaging study. It assesses several
TAS-20. Furthermore, like Jolliffe and Farrington (2006) results, dimensions: affective empathy (n = 10), ToM (n = 10) and causality
the BES should not be significantly related to social desirability.
Finally, the BES should meet the criteria of test-retest reliability
(a significant and a strong positive correlation between the scores 1
Free translation, not validated, available from Stephanie Braun (sbraun@
during the test and at the retest phase). ulb.ac.be), ULB – Erasme Hospital, Brussels, Belgium.
162 F. D’Ambrosio et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 160–165

(n = 20) and it does not consider verbal abilities (it is a visual test). diverged from this value (items 3 and 16 amounted to less than
We, therefore, used it in order to obtain a countermeasure for 0.30 on the cognitive empathy factor).
empathy, and particularly for cognitive empathy. It consists in The internal consistency of the BES as measured by the Cron-
choosing one of two pictures which completes an initial uncom- bach a coefficient was 0.80 (0.77 for affective empathy measured
pleted sequence of three pictures. The test comprises 40 sequences on 11 items and 0.66 for cognitive empathy measured on 9 items).
and the maximum score is 40 points (one point for each correct The temporal stability coefficient (with correction for attenuation)
response). was 0.83.

2.7. The lie scale 3.2. Girls should score significantly higher than boys on empathy,
particularly on affective empathy
Finally, we used the lie scale of the Eysenck personality ques-
tionnaire-revised (Eysenck, Eysenck, & Barrett, 1985, cited in The participants’ scores on the TAS-20, BEES, non-verbal test
Bouvard (1999)) which taps people’s tendency to respond to the and BES, are presented in Table 1.
items in a socially desirable way. This scale features 21 items Internal consistencies (Cronbach a) for the data of the present
and each desirable response counted for 1 point. A high score indi- study were 0.67 for the TAS-20, 0.79 and 0.71 for the BEES (respec-
cates that the participant has a tendency to give a socially ap- tively, for the 15 positive items and the 15 negative items), 0.51
proved response rather than one, which corresponds to his/her and 0.42 for the non-verbal test (respectively, for the 10 items
real personality. for both empathy and cognitive subscales) and 0.67 for the lie
Three weeks later, the adolescents participated in the retest scale. However, according to Streiner (2003), it is generally recom-
phase in which they had to respond to the BES again. mended that a value should be about 0.80: so, here, the correla-
tions between the BES and these scales should be considered
2.8. Statistical analysis carefully.
The mean BEES scores were lower than those specified in
First, to evaluate the BES’s factor structure, we conducted a Mehrabian’s norms (2000) but our participants were adolescents
confirmatory factor analysis using structural equation modelling and not adults as in Mehrabian’s study (2000). The expected gen-
(ÓSTATISTICA, Version 7) in the same way as Jolliffe and Farring- der effect was observed (t (443) = 7.90, p < 0.01, d = 0.75): girls
ton (2006). We tested a two correlated factors model, using the scored considerably higher than boys on emotional empathy as
covariance matrix. This model was estimated using generalized measured by this scale (Table 1).
least square procedure and maximum likelihood method. Five On the other hand, the mean scores in the non-verbal test
goodness-of-fit indices were used to evaluate the adequacy of (empathy and ToM) were slightly lower than those obtained by
the model fit: the chi-squared test (v2), the Bentler comparative Völlm et al. (2006). No gender effect was observed for the ‘‘empa-
fit index (CFI), the standardized root mean squared residual thy” or for the ‘‘ToM” subscales of the non-verbal test (Table 1).
(SRMSR), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; The mean BES scores were greater than those found by Jolliffe
Steiger (1990)) and the goodness-of-fit index (GFI; Bentler and and Farrington (2006) (Table 1). The girls scored much higher than
Bonett (1980)) in order to compare with Jolliffe and Farrington’s the boys on the empathy scale (F (1, 444) = 83.19, p < 0.01,
model (2006). Actually, fit indices exceeding 0.90 and RMSEA g2 = 0.16) and the magnitude of the gender difference was greater
and SRMSR below 0.10 are generally considered evidence of ade- for the affective than the cognitive scale (F (1, 444) = 35.72,
quate model fit (Fan, Thompson, & Wang, 1999). We used the stu- p < 0.01, g2 = 0.07). Moreover, we observed that girls and boys
dent T-test and the Fisher–Snedecor F-test in order to compare scored higher on affective empathy than on cognitive empathy (F
results between boys and girls. Moreover, to evaluate relation- (1, 444) = 172.47, p < 0.01, g2 = 0.28) (when we focused on material
ships between the self-report scores we used Bravais–Pearson and not on human the g2 increase).
correlation coefficients. We also used this technique to compare
the test-retest scores. 3.3. There should be a significant positive correlation between the
levels of cognitive and affective empathy

3. Results The Pearson correlations between the BEES and the cognitive,
affective and total scales of the BES and the other scales are pre-
3.1. The French version of the BES should have the same factorial sented in Table 2.
structure as the original scale The affective and the cognitive scales of the BES were corre-
lated: r = 0.41 (p < 0.05) with r = 0.42 (p < 0.05) for the boys and
The results of the BES’s confirmatory factor analysis are pre- r = 0.27 (p < 0.05) for the girls (Table 2). This finding suggests that
sented in Fig. 1. The solution converged after nine iterations. De- there is a significant overlap between these two components of
spite the (v2) being significant (v2(169) = 482.14, p < 0.01) and empathy, particularly for the boys. However, it also suggests that
the CFI being less than 0.90 (CFI = 0.78), the goodness-of-fit can there is a difference between them and this difference is greater
be considered as acceptable according to Byrne (2001) regarding in girls (p = 0.04).
the SRMSR = 0.063, the RMSEA = 0.068, 90% CI = [0.061 0.075] The relatively high correlation between BES and BEES (Table 2)
and the GFI = 0.90. So, the confirmatory factor analysis supports indicates that each of these scales measures a part of the same con-
the idea of a two-factor solution and we obtained exactly the same cept but they are not redundant (given that BEES is a measure of
results as Jolliffe and Farrington (2006). The two specified factors emotional empathy). Indeed, the BEES is more highly correlated
accounted for 25.80% of the total variance (8.30% for the cognitive with the affective scale than the cognitive scale of the BES
empathy factor and 17.50% for the affective empathy factor instead (p < 0.01) (and particularly for boys: 0.34 points of difference for
of 7.60% and 19.50%, respectively, in Jolliffe and Farrington (2006). boys and 0.16 points for girls) and it is not due to a restriction of
However, when Jolliffe and Farrington (2006) reduced their data, range effect. Actually, several measures were made selecting 9
they selected only the items which equalled or exceeded .40 on items from the affective scale (at random) and showed that these
one or the other factor. However, in our analysis, some of the items nine items still had a significant different correlation. However,
F. D’Ambrosio et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 160–165 163

Item 3 Item 1

.27
Item 2
Item 6 (.43)
.57
.44
(.62)
(.45)
.68 Item 4
Item 9
.36 (.71)

(.54) .48
Item 5
Cognitive (.41)
.41
Item 10 .67
(.62) Empathy
(.60)
.37 Item 7
.65
(.53) .64
Item 12 (.64)
(.62)
.35
.59 Item 8
(.68) Affective (.64)

Item 14 .74
Empathy
.29
(.58)
Item 11
(.62) .31

(.45)
Item 16
.63
.36 Item 13
(.56) .60 (.45)
(.52) .41
Item 19
(.60) Item 15
.56

(.70)
Item 17
Item 20

Item 18

Fig. 1. Two-factor confirmatory analysis model of the 20 BES items for the overall sample.

Table 1
Participants’ (n = 446) scores on the self-report measures.

Gender Group total


Boys (n = 196) Girls (n = 250) (n = 446)
Min Max Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max Mean SD
TAS-20 28 83 55.29 9.04 1 81 56.12 9.89 1 83 55.75 9.52
BEES 76 74 11.57 23.93 62 109 30.59 26.12 76 109 22.25 26.84
Non-verbal test ‘‘empathy” 3 10 7.97 1.74 3 10 8.22 1.51 3 10 8.11 1.62
Non-verbal test ‘‘ToM” 1 10 8.46 1.40 2 10 8.51 1.18 1 10 8.49 1.28
BES total 34 94 70.28 10.27 53 98 78.07 7.76 34 98 74.65 9.74
BES ‘‘emotional empathy” 13 52 36.22 6.89 24 55 41.90 5.71 13 55 39.41 6.85
BES ‘‘cognitive empathy” 15 45 34.07 5.22 22 45 36.17 3.94 15 45 35.25 4.66
164 F. D’Ambrosio et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 160–165

Table 2
Correlation of the BES with the other scales.

Boys (n = 195) Girls (n = 250) Total (n = 445)


BES BES BES BEES BES BES BES BEES BES BES BES BEES
cognitive affective total cognitive affective total cognitive affective total
BEES 0.22* 0.56* 0.49* / 0.27* 0.43* 0.45* / 0.30* 0.56* 0.54* /
Non-verbal test 0.23* 0.15 0.22* 0.06 0 0.07 0.05 0.09 0.14* 0.13* 0.16* 0.10*
‘‘empathy”
Non-verbal test ‘‘ToM” 0.14* 0 0.07 0 0 0.04 0.02 -0.08 0.08 0.02 0.05 0.03
Lie scale 0.09 0.02 0.03 0.07 0.07 0.04 0.06 0.12* 0.09 0.04 0.07 0.12*
TAS-20 0.21* 0.02 0.11 0.01 0.11 0.01 0.07 0.01 0.14* 0 0.06 0.01
*
p < 0.05.

the BES shows stronger validity as it is not correlated with the lie own emotions in response to others but less for the identification
scale contrary to the BEES. of another’s emotion. There is a significant correlation between
There were no (or only very small) correlations between the BES affective and cognitive empathy. The correlation between the BEES
and both ‘‘ToM” and ‘‘empathy” of the non-verbal test (particularly and BES further illustrates the BES’s concurrent validity and sug-
for girls) (Table 2). However, we can suppose it is normal as the gests that these scales measure affective empathy in almost the
scores in the non-verbal test (for both ‘‘ToM” and ‘‘empathy”) same way (as BEES is a measure of emotional empathy). It indi-
showed a ceiling effect and that the internal consistency of the cates also that these measures are not redundant with the smaller
non-verbal test was not really good. correlation with cognitive empathy. Furthermore, the absence or
small size of the correlations between the non-verbal task and
3.4. There should be a significant negative correlation between the BES the BES in no way reflected the nature of the cognitive scale or
and the TAS-20 the affective scale of the BES. Indeed, the poor internal consistency
of the non-verbal test, the difference in the material used (non-ver-
The mean TAS-20 scores were slightly higher than those ob- bal/verbal) might have influenced the observed results. Jolliffe and
tained by Zimmermann, Quartier, Bernard, Salamin, and Maggiori Farrington (2006) found a negative relationship between cognitive,
(2007) with an adolescent sample. No gender effect was found total empathy and alexithymia in both boys and girls. We failed to
(Table 1). replicate this result, except for boys (there was a negative link be-
We found a significant negative correlation between the TAS-20 tween the difficulty in expressing and understanding one’s emo-
and the cognitive scale of the BES, but only for boys (r = 0.21, tions and cognitive empathy). This may reflect the gender
p < 0.05) (Table 2). difference in offending or anti-social behaviour observed (Broidy,
Cauffman, Espelage, Mazerolle, & Piquero, 2004; Guttman &
3.5. The BES should not be significantly related to social desirability Laporte, 2002).
Consequently, the French version of the BES exhibits acceptable
The correlation between the BES and the lie scale of the Eysenck internal consistency and test-retest reliability as well as concur-
personality questionnaire was not significant for either the boys or rent and convergent validity. However, the poor internal consisten-
the girls in our sample (Table 2). Moreover, our boys and girls cies observed in the various scales used to correlate with the BES
exhibited no significant difference on the measure of social desir- limit the implications of our study (concerning particularly the ab-
ability (t(443) = 1.49, n.s.) (Table 1). Consequently, the girls’ higher sence of correlation between ToM and cognitive empathy except-
score on the BES cannot be the result of a bias in favour of social ing for boys, the measure of social desirability and alexithymia).
desirability. So, we suggest further investigation of the psychometric qualities
of the BES using other scales (with stronger internal consistency).
3.6. The BES should presented a good test-retest reliability Future research could use behavioural scales in order to limit bias
of social desirability, neuro-imagery in order to observe and ex-
Finally, 153 adolescents completed the BES on two occasions, plain the different strategies used by boys and girls during empa-
and the test-retest reliability as measured by the coefficient of cor- thy tasks. We could also use the BES, which integrates both
relation between the BES scores at time 1 and time 2 (separated by cognitive and affective empathy, for clinical application in popula-
a 3-week interval) was r = 0.66 (p < 0.05) with r = 0.70 (p < 0.05) for tions presenting a lack of empathy as violent sexual and non-sex-
the affective empathy and r = 0.54 (p < 0.05) for the cognitive ual offenders for example (Covell & Scalora, 2002; Jolliffe &
empathy. It is not as strong as we expected, particularly for cogni- Farrington, 2004). Indeed, we could further investigate the distinc-
tive empathy, but it is acceptable. tion between the lack of empathy in ‘‘normal” and sexual offenders
in adolescence and adulthood.
4. Discussion
Acknowledgements
Our results were globally similar to those obtained by Jolliffe
and Farrington (2006). Actually, the BES exhibits a relatively high We would like to thank Aurore Carlier, Nathalie Goguet, Elodie
level of cross-cultural stability, even if in France, boys seem to be Ismert, Anne-Laure Lepissier, Sophie Griffini, and the students at
a little more empathic (on affective empathy, and, therefore, on the University of Reims Champagne-Ardenne for their participa-
the BES in general) than their Anglo–Saxon counterparts. More- tion in the collection of the data.
over, we found a classic effect (Jolliffe & Farrington, 2006): girls
scored higher than boys, particularly in affective empathy and it References
did not result from a social desirability bias. It tends to be linked
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto
to Han et al. (2008) and Schulte-Rüther et al. (2008) results: fe- Alexithymia scale – I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure.
males and males rely on different strategies when assessing their Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23–32.
F. D’Ambrosio et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 160–165 165

Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Empathy and offending: A systematic review
The systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 441–476.
or high functioning autism, and normal sex difference. Philosophical Transactions Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of the basic
of the Royal Society London, 358, 361–374. empathy scale. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 589–611.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation Laible, D. J., Carlo, G., & Roesch, S. C. (2004). Pathways to self-esteem in late
of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex adolescence. The role of parent and peer attachment, empathy, and social
differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163–175. behaviours. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 703–716.
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness-of-fit in the Lawrence, E. J., Shaw, P., Baker, D., Baron-Cohen, S., & David, A. S. (2004). Measuring
analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606. empathy: Reliability and validity of the empathy quotient. Psychological
Berthoz, S., Grèzes, J., Armony, J. L., Passingham, R. E., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Affective Medicine, 34, 911–919.
response to one’s own moral violations. NeuroImage, 31(2), 945–950. Loas, G., Corcos, M., Stephan, P., Pellet, J., Bizouard, P., Venisse, J. L., et al. (2001).
Beven, J. P., O’Brien-Malone, A., & Hall, G. (2004). Using the interpersonal reactivity Factorial structure of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia scale confirmatory
index to assess empathy in violent offenders. International Journal of Forensic factorial analyses in non-clinical and clinical samples. Journal of Psychosomatic
Psychology, 1(2), 33–41. Research, 50, 255–261.
Bouvard, M. (1999). Questionnaires et échelles d’évaluation de la personnalité. Paris: Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2007).
Masson. Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict
Broidy, L., Cauffman, E., Espelage, D. L., Mazerolle, P., & Piquero, A. (2004). Sex adult psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 155–165.
differences in empathy and its relation to juvenile offending. Violence end Mehrabian, A. (1997). Relations among personality scales of aggression, violence,
Victims, 18, 503–517. and empathy: Validational evidence bearing on the risk of eruptive violence
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modelling with AMOS. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence scale. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 433–445.
Erlbaum Associates Inc. Mehrabian, A. (2000). Manual for the balanced emotional empathy scale (BEES).
Covell, C. N., & Scalora, M. J. (2002). Empathic deficits in sexual offenders. An Unpublished manuscript available next to the author.
integration of affective, social, and cognitive constructs. Aggression and Violent Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of
Behavior, 7, 251–270. Personality, 40, 525–543.
Davies, M., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. D. (1998). Emotional intelligence. In search of Muncer, S. J., & Ling, J. (2006). Psychometric analysis of the empathy quotient (EQ)
an elusive construct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(6), 1111–1119.
989–1015. Nichols, H. R., & Molinder, I. (Eds.). (1996). Multiphasic sex inventory II handbook.
Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional difference in empathy. JSAS Catalogue of Tacoma, WA: Nichols & Molinder Assessments.
Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85. Rice, M. E., Chaplin, T. C., Harris, G. T., & Coutts, J. (1994). Empathy for the victim and
Decety, J. (2002). Naturaliser l’empathie. L’Encéphale, 28, 9–20. sexual arousal among rapists and nonrapists. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9,
Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the 435–447.
psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 21–29. Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Baylé, M. C., Besche, C., & Widlöcher, D. (1997). Attribution of
Fan, X., Thompson, B., & Wang, L. (1999). Effects of sample size, estimation methods, intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: A non-verbal exploration
and model specification on structural equation modeling fit indexes. Structural with comic strips. Schizophrenia Research, 25, 199–209.
equation modeling, 6(1), 56–83. Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2008).
Fernandez, Y. M., & Marshall, W. L. (2003). Victim empathy, social self-esteem, and Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy. NeuroImage, 42,
psychopathy in rapists. Sexual abuse: A journal of research and treatment, 15(1), 393–403.
11–26. Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval
Findlay, L. C., Girardi, A., & Coplan, R. J. (2006). Links between empathy, social estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 173–180.
behaviour, and social understanding in early childhood. Early Childhood Streiner, D. L. (2003). Starting at the beginning: An introduction to coefficient
Research Quarterly, 21(3), 347–359. alpha and internal consistency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 80(1), 99–
Guttman, H. A., & Laporte, L. (2002). Alexithymia, empathy and psychological 103.
symptoms in a family context. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43, 448–455. Völlm, B. A., Taylor, A., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., et al. (2006).
Han, S., Fan, Y., & Mao, L. (2008). Gender difference in empathy for pain: An Neural correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic
electrophysiological investigation. Brain Research, 1196, 85–93. resonance imaging study in a non-verbal task. NeuroImage, 29, 90–98.
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and Zimmermann, G., Quartier, V., Bernard, M., Salamin, V., & Maggiori, C. (2007).
justice. Cambridge University Press. Qualités psychométriques de la version française de la TAS-20 et prévalence de
Hogan, R. (1969). Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and l’alexithymie chez 264 adolescents tout-venant. L’Encéphale, 33, 941–946.
Clinical Psychology, 33, 307–316.

View publication stats

You might also like