You are on page 1of 11

Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

Validation of an Italian version of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards


Appearance Questionnaire-4-Revised (SATAQ-4R) on non-clinical
Italian women and men
Cristina Stefanile a , Amanda Nerini b,∗ , Camilla Matera b , Lauren M. Schaefer c ,
Joel Kevin Thompson d
a
Department of Health Sciences, Section of Psychology, University of Florence, Via di San Salvi, 12 - Pad. 26, 50135 Florence, Italy
b
Department of Education, Languages, Intercultures, Literatures, and Psychology, Section of Psychology, University of Florence, Via di San Salvi, 12 - Pad.
26, 50135 Florence, Italy
c
Sanford Research, 120 Eighth Street South, Fargo, ND 58103, USA
d
Department of Psychology, University of South Florida, 4202 East Fowler Ave, PCD4118G, Tampa, FL 33620, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Appearance-ideal internalization and appearance pressures are empirically-supported risk factors for
Received 9 November 2018 body image disturbance and disordered eating in Western countries. Such a relationship has emerged
Received in revised form 7 August 2019 also in the Italian context, where high rates of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating are present.
Accepted 7 August 2019
The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire (SATAQ) and its revisions are among the
Available online 22 August 2019
most commonly used instruments to assess the role of sociocultural influences on body image. Two stud-
ies were conducted to examine the psychometric properties of the female (Study 1, N = 732) and male
Keywords:
(Study 2, N = 364) Italian versions of the SATAQ-4-Revised (SATAQ-4R-Female, SATAQ-4R-Male). Confir-
Sociocultural influence
Body image
matory factor analysis (CFA) indicated that the original seven-factor structure was replicated for women
Pressures and men. The seven subscales (Internalization: Thin/Low Body Fat; Internalization: Muscular; Inter-
Internalization nalization: General Attractiveness; Pressures: Family; Pressures: Peers; Pressures: Significant Others;
SATAQ-4R and Pressures: Media) showed good internal consistency and 4-week test-retest reliability. Associations
between SATAQ-4R subscales and body dissatisfaction, eating disorder symptomatology, drive for mus-
cularity, self-esteem, family influences, and peer influences, suggested good convergent validity among
both women and men. The present study provides evidence for the validity and reliability of the male
and female Italian versions of the SATAQ-4R.
© 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction towards unnecessary cosmetic surgery interventions (Matera,


Nerini, & Stefanile, 2018; Stefanile, Nerini, & Matera, 2014).
Much research has shown that body dissatisfaction is associ- Given the detrimental effects that body dissatisfaction can have
ated with negative consequences not only among women, but also on individuals’ physical and mental health, many studies have
among men. Some of these deleterious outcomes are decreased tried to identify its most potent predictors. Research findings
self-esteem (Paxton, Neumark-Sztainer, Hannan, & Eisenberg, consistently support the strong relationship between sociocul-
2006; Tiggemann, 2005), as well as increased eating pathol- tural influences and body dissatisfaction (Stice & Shaw, 2002). In
ogy (Amaral & Ferreira, 2017; Girard, Rodgers, & Chabrol, 2018; particular, several studies have shown that thin-ideal internaliza-
Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999), depressive tion and perceived appearance pressures are uniquely associated
symptoms (Olivardia, Pope, Borowiecki, & Cohane, 2004; Paxton with and predict body dissatisfaction (Girard et al., 2018; Knauss,
et al., 2006), unhealthy behaviours aimed at increasing muscle size Paxton, & Alsaker, 2007; Lovering, Rodgers, George, & Franko, 2018;
(Parent & Moradi, 2011; Tylka, 2011), and favourable attitudes Nerini, 2009; Shroff & Thompson, 2006; Stefanile, Matera, Pisani,
& Zambrini, 2009). Appearance-ideal internalization refers to the
acceptance of culturally-endorsed appearance ideals (e.g., thinness
for females, muscularity for males) as one’s own personal appear-
∗ Corresponding author.
ance standard (Thompson & Stice, 2001), while perceived pressures
E-mail addresses: stefanile@psico.unifi.it (C. Stefanile), nerini@psico.unifi.it
(A. Nerini), camilla.matera@unifi.it (C. Matera), lauren.m.schaefer@gmail.com
can be defined as the feeling of being encouraged to modify one’s
(L.M. Schaefer), jkthompson@usf.edu (J.K. Thompson).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.08.005
1740-1445/© 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58 49

physical appearance in order to reach such ideals (Thompson, van Cronbach’s alpha values for each subscale at .82 or higher) and
den Berg, Roehrig, Guarda, & Heinberg, 2004). good test–retest reliability among college women from the United
The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire States (Schaefer et al., 2017). Regarding construct validity, positive
(SATAQ) is one of the most commonly used instruments to assess relations between the SATAQ-4R-Female subscales and both eating
the role of sociocultural influences on body image. Following disorder symptomatology and body dissatisfaction were observed
publication of the initial version of this instrument (Heinberg, in the same college female sample. Negative associations were
Thompson, & Stormer, 1995), the SATAQ has undergone multi- observed with self-esteem, consistent with theory. Notably, factor
ple revisions in order to adapt to shifts in cultural appearance analysis of the SATAQ-4R-Female among adolescent girls, resulted
ideals, as well as to examine specific appearance ideals and pres- in a six-factor structure, where items assessing appearance related
sures most relevant to males and females (Cusumano & Thompson, pressure from peers and significant others loaded onto a single fac-
1997; Schaefer et al., 2015; Thompson et al., 2004). In 2015, Schae- tor (Schaefer et al., 2017). The male version of the SATAQ-4R is
fer and colleagues developed the SATAQ-4, which contains five composed of 28 items. It has demonstrated good internal consis-
subscales assessing three distinct sources of perceived weight tency (with Cronbach’s alpha values at .75 or higher) among college
and appearance pressures from family, peers, and media, as well men from the United States (Schaefer et al., 2017). In the same sam-
as internalization of the thin and muscular/athletic ideals. This ple, positive correlations were observed between the SATAQ-4R
version proved to have good psychometric properties and con- subscales and eating disorder symptomatology and body dissat-
vergent validity, demonstrating medium to strong associations isfaction, while negative associations were observed between the
with measures of body dissatisfaction and eating pathology in SATAQ-4R subscales and self-esteem.
diverse samples, including women from Italy (Schaefer et al., 2015). Although, earlier versions of the SATAQ have been translated
Despite the scale’s strong psychometric properties and conceptual for use with Italian samples (Bagnara, Huon, & Donazzolo, 2004;
advantages over previous iterations of the measure, the authors Schaefer et al., 2015; Stefanile, Matera, Nerini, & Pisani, 2011;), to
suggested further revision of the scale in order to address several the best of our knowledge, only one published study to date has
remaining limitations (Schaefer, Harriger, Heinberg, Soderberg, & adapted the SATAQ-4R for use with other populations outside of
Thompson, 2017). the United States. Specifically, the factor structure of this scale was
Specifically, the authors developed the SATAQ-4-Revised to explored by Cihan, Bozo, Schaefer, and Thompson (2016) among
achieve five primary aims. The SATAQ-4 assesses desire for a Turkish college women. The exploratory factor analysis revealed
muscular and athletic physique. However, the authors suggest a six-factor solution with items assessing perceived appearance
that athleticism may connote a broad range of competence-based pressure from peers and significant others contributing to a sin-
attributes such as agility and coordination that are independent gle factor, rather than two separate factors as was observed among
of aesthetic standards. Therefore, the newly revised SATAQ-4R women from the United States. The authors speculated that this
focuses exclusively on desire for muscularity, eliminating previ- difference might reflect Turkish culture and language, where sig-
ous references to an “athletic” physique. Second, the SATAQ-4R nificant others and peers may not represent highly distinct groups.
eliminated behavioral internalization items, focusing exclusively Given the advantages of the SATAQ-4R over earlier versions of
on the cognitive aspects of internalization, consistent with def- the measure, validation of the scale in other sociocultural con-
initions of the internalization construct. Third, items that assess texts where body image and disordered eating is a significant
internalization of a more general dimension of appearance (not concern, represents an important line of research. A recent sur-
specifically linked to thinness/low body fat or muscularity) were vey conducted with nearly 8000 Italian participants indicated that
included in this new version of the scale. Fourth, the SATAQ-4R 26.5% of young adults were almost or completely unsatisfied with
incorporated an additional pressures subscale to assess perceived their current body (Pirani & Matera, 2018). Moreover, almost 3
appearance pressures from significant others including romantic million people are affected by eating disorders in Italy (Ministero
partners, teachers, and coaches. Finally, the authors noted that pre- della Salute, 2017), with an estimated lifetime prevalence of 1.7%
vious versions of the SATAQ had been initially developed among (Carta et al., 2014). Although 95% of patients are women, the phe-
primarily female respondents, which had important implications nomenon is also increasing among the male population (Ministero
for the item retention and deletion process and resulted in a della Salute, 2017). Importantly, sociocultural influences such as
reduced emphasis on male appearance concerns such as muscular- desire to be similar to TV characters, consumption of reality and
ity pressures (Law & Labre, 2002). Therefore, with the SATAQ-4R, entertainment TV, and the perceived discrepancy between one’s
the authors sought to produce two separate versions of the mea- own body and those of TV characters, have been identified as sig-
sure (i.e., the SATAQ-4R-Male and SATAQ-4R-Female), which were nificant factors contributing to Italian women’s eating-disordered
each developed and validated within large samples of college behaviors (Barcaccia et al., 2018). Italian men also pay great atten-
men and women, respectively. As such, this revision significantly tion to their own physical appearance (Ruspini, 2007). In Italy, men
advanced the measurement of appearance-ideal internalization desire a more muscular body than the average male body shape
and appearance-related pressures among males. (Gilli & Ruspini, 2014; Zelli, Lucidi, & Mallia, 2010) and are par-
Both the male and female versions of the SATAQ-4R are ticularly concerned with the care of their appearance (Capellani,
comprised of seven thematically-consistent subscales. The three Codazzi, Del Greco, & Rossi, 2014). Moreover, sociocultural influ-
internalization subscales are labelled Internalization: Thin/Low ences and internalization processes seem to be particularly relevant
Body Fat (reflecting a desire to attain a thin figure); Internaliza- in explaining Italian men’s body dissatisfaction (Matera, Nerini, &
tion: Muscular (reflecting a desire to attain a muscular figure); and Stefanile, 2019). Therefore, further examination of these influences
Internalization: General Attractiveness (reflecting an overall con- among Italians represents an important area of research. To facili-
cern with appearance). The four pressures subscales are labeled tate this work, the current project sought to translate and validate
Pressures: Family; Pressures: Peers; Pressures: Significant Others; Italian versions of the SATAQ-4R among Italian women (Study 1)
and Pressures: Media. Each pressures subscale assesses perceived and men (Study 2).
pressures from a specific sociocultural source to improve one’s
appearance and attain a thin body (in the SATAQ-4R-Female) or 2. Study 1
a muscular physique (in the SATAQ-4R-Male).
The female version of the SATAQ-4R is composed of 31 items, Given the high rates of body dissatisfaction among Italian
and its scores have demonstrated good internal consistency (with women (Pirani & Matera, 2018) and the role played by sociocul-
50 C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58

tural influences (Barcaccia et al., 2018), the present study aimed peers encourage me to get thinner”), Pressures: Significant Others
to examine the psychometric properties of an Italian version of (four items, e.g., “I feel pressure from significant others to improve
the SATAQ-4R-Female. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was per- my appearance”), and Pressures: Media (four items, e.g., “I feel pres-
formed to examine the factor structure of the Italian version among sure from the media to look in better shape”). Items were rated
women. As the SATAQ-4R-Female has demonstrated two differ- on a 5-point Likert scale with response options from 1 (definitely
ent factor structures both among differently aged women in the disagree) to 5 (definitely agree).
United States (Schaefer et al., 2017) and among Turkish (Cihan,
Bozo, Schaefer, & Thompson, 2016) college women, the fit of both 2.1.2.2. Body dissatisfaction. Body dissatisfaction was measured
the six- and the seven-factor models was tested. Internal con- through the Italian version (Matera, Nerini, & Stefanile, 2013) of the
sistency and test-retest reliability were examined for the model Body Shape Questionnaire-14 (Dowson & Henderson, 2001), which
that presented the best fit to the data. Convergent validity for is composed of 14 items (e.g., “Have you felt that it is not fair that
the best-fitting model was also assessed via Pearson correlation other people are thinner than you?”) with a 6-point response for-
coefficients between the SATAQ-4R and measures of body dissat- mat (1 = never; 6 = always). Higher scores indicate higher levels of
isfaction, eating disorder symptomatology, drive for muscularity, body dissatisfaction. In previous work, exploratory and confirma-
self-esteem, family, and peer influences. Based on findings from the tory factor analyses supported the structural validity of the Italian
validation of the English version of the SATAQ-4R-Female (Schaefer version of the BSQ-14, Cronbach’s alpha was high, and convergent
et al., 2017), we expected that the Pressures subscales, Internal- validity was found to be good (Matera et al., 2013; Stefanile, Matera,
ization: Thin/Low Body Fat subscale, and Internalization: General & Pisani, 2009). Scores on this scale demonstrated high reliability
Attractiveness subscale would demonstrate medium to large pos- in the current study (␣ = .95).
itive associations with eating disorder symptomatology, small to
medium positive associations with body dissatisfaction, and small
2.1.2.3. Disordered eating. The Italian translation (Dalle Grave &
to medium negative associations with self-esteem. The Internal-
Calugi, 2010) of the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire
ization: Muscular subscale was expected to exhibit a large positive
(EDE-Q; Fairburn and Beglin, 2008) was used to measure disordered
association with drive for muscularity, but small positive associa-
eating symptomatology. The EDE-Q global score is composed of 22
tions with eating disorder symptomatology, and body dissatisfac-
Likert-type items (e.g., “Has your weight influence how you think
tion. The subscale was expected to demonstrate a small negative
about [judge] yourself as a person?”) assessing the frequency of
association with self-esteem. The SATAQ-4R-Female Pressures:
eating-disordered thoughts and behaviors over the previous four
Family subscale, Pressures: Peers subscale, and Pressures: Signif-
weeks. Items are rated on a 7-point scale (0 = no days; 6 = every
icant Others subscale were expected to exhibit medium to large
day). Higher scores on the EDE-Q indicate greater levels of eating
positive associations with measures of family and peer influence.
pathology. Prior work using the Italian version of the EDE-Q sup-
ports the validity and reliability of its scores (Calugi et al., 2017).
2.1. Method
The EDE-Q global score demonstrated high internal consistency in
the present study (␣ = .94).
2.1.1. Participants
Participants for the CFA and reliability analyses were 732 Ital-
2.1.2.4. Drive for muscularity. The Italian version (Nerini, Matera,
ian women aged between 18 and 65 years (89.6% of women aged
Baroni, & Stefanile, 2016) of the Drive for Muscularity Scale (DMS;
from 18 to 30 years, consistent with the original United States
McCreary & Sasse, 2000) was used to measure participants’ desire
validation sample). All participants had Italian nationality and
for muscularity and engagement in behaviors aimed at achieving
were born in Italy (age M = 25.13, SD = 8.34; body mass index, BMI,
a muscular physique. This version of the scale is composed of 14
M = 21.25, SD = 3.08). Given pragmatic limitations, only a subset
items scored on a 6-point Likert-type response format (1 = never;
of participants from the overall sample were administered addi-
6 = always). Higher scores indicate higher levels of drive for mus-
tional self-report measures to assess the convergent validity and
cularity. In prior work, scores on the Italian version of the DMS
test-retest reliability of the SATAQ-4R-Female. Convergent valid-
have demonstrated good validity and reliability within male and
ity was assessed using a subset of 335 participants from the larger
female samples (Nerini, Matera, & Stefanile, 2016; Nerini, Matera,
sample (age M = 26.10, SD = 8.74; BMI M = 20.94, SD = 2.88), which
Baroni et al., 2016; Stefanile, Nerini, Matera, & Antonelli, 2012). In
conferred adequate power to detect small to moderate effects
the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was .80.
(Hulley, Cummings, Browner, Grady, & Newman, 2013). The four-
week test-retest reliability of the SATAQ-4R was examined among a
subset of 70 participants (age M = 26.76, SD = 11.47; BMI M = 21.21, 2.1.2.5. Self-esteem. The Italian version (Prezza, Trombaccia, &
SD = 3.38), which conferred adequate power to detect moderate- Armento, 1997) of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (SES;
large effects (Hulley et al., 2013). Socio-demographic information Rosenberg, 1965) was used. The SES is a 10-item scale measuring
for each sample is reported in Table 1. global feelings of self-worth or self-acceptance. Items were rated
on a four-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 4 = strongly agree) with
2.1.2. Measures higher scores reflecting greater self-esteem. Previous work with the
2.1.2.1. Sociocultural attitudes towards appearance. The Italian Italian version of the SES has demonstrated good construct valid-
version of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance ity, test-retest reliability, and internal consistency with men and
Questionnaire-4-Revised-Female (SATAQ-4R-Female) was used to women (Prezza et al., 1997). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha
assess internalization of appearance ideals and appearance-related was .83.
sociocultural pressures (see the Procedure section for the transla-
tion steps). The SATAQ-4R-Female is a 31-item scale composed of 2.1.2.6. Feedback on physical appearance. The Italian version
seven subscales: Thin/Low Body Fat (four items, e.g., “I want my (Nerini & Massai, 2010) of the Feedback On Physical Appearance
body to look very thin”), Internalization: Muscular (five items, e.g., Scale-Female (Tantleff-Dunn, Thompson, & Dunn, 1995) is an 8-
“It is important for me to look muscular”), Internalization: General item self-report instrument assessing comments or behaviors from
Attractiveness (six items, e.g., “I think a lot about my appearance”), family that could be interpreted as negative feedback on one’s
and Pressures: Family (four items, e.g., “I feel pressure from family weight or other aspects of physical appearance (e.g., “Some of your
members to look thinner”), Pressures: Peers (four items, e.g., “My relatives watched closely what you ate”). Responses range from 1
C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58 51

Table 1
Demographic Characteristics for all Samples.

Study 1 Study 1 Study 1 Study 2 Study 2 Study 2


Women Women Women Men Men Men
N = 732 n = 335 n = 70 N = 364 n = 110 n = 28
CFA sample Convergent validity Test-retest CFA sample Convergent validity Test-retest
subsample subsample subsample subsample

Educational level
Lower secondary school 4.5% 3% 1.4% 4.7% 8.2% –
Secondary school 74.2% 75.8% 87.1% 67.3% 58.2% 75%
Undergraduate degree 10.8% 10.4% – 15.7% 16.4% 25%
Graduate degree 8.6% 8.1% 11.4% 11.0% 15.5% –
Other qualification 1.8% 2.7% – 1.4% 1.8% –

Marital status
Married 14.9% 13.1% 20.0% 14.4% 25.5% –
Not married 83.6% 85.1% 78.6% 82.0% 68.2% 100%
Separate/Divorced/Other 1.5% 1.8% 1.4% 3.6% 6.4% –

(never) to 5 (always). Higher scores indicate higher perceived pres- retest reliability involved completing a single questionnaire at two
sures from one’s family. Scores on the Italian version of the FOPAS-F different time-points four weeks apart, we decided to recruit stu-
have demonstrated good internal consistency among women and dents who attended small university courses, who were more likely
men (Matera, Nerini, Marconi, & Stefanile, 2015; Matera, Nerini, Di to be present at retest, for this task. Consequently, a smaller number
Gesto, Policardo, & Stefanile, 2019; Nerini & Massai, 2010). In the of participants was obtained for this kind of analysis. Participa-
present study, Cronbach’s alpha was.78. tion was voluntary and no incentives were offered to respondents.
Participants were informed that all data were held confidential
2.1.2.7. Peer appearance attribution. The Italian version (Stefanile, and their responses were recorded anonymously. They provided
Pisani, Matera, & Guiderdoni, 2010) of the Peer Attribution Scale informed consent prior to completing study questionnaires. Study
(PAS), developed by Lieberman (see Shroff, 2004), was used to procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee of the Univer-
measure to what extent people feel that peers give importance sity of Florence.
to appearance for popularity. This is a 4-item scale ranging from
1 (false) to 6 (true) (e.g., “My friends would like me more if I lost
weight”). Higher scores indicate higher peer appearance attribu- 2.1.4. Data analyses
tion. This scale has been found to be unidimensional, with evidence Item distributions were first examined to identify items with
of good reliability among both women and men (Matera et al., excessive skew (> 2) and/or kurtosis (> 7; West, Finch, &
2018; Nerini, Matera, & Stefanile, 2017; Stefanile et al., 2010). In Curran, 1995) values. CFA was implemented through LISREL 8.80
the present study, Cronbach’s ␣ was .87. (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2006). Starting from a matrix of polychoric
correlations (Holgado-Tello, Chacón-Moscoso, Barbero-García, &
2.1.2.8. Body mass index. Participants reported their heights and Vila-Abad, 2010; Jöreskog, 1994), the Robust Diagonally Weighted
weights, which were used to calculate BMI (kg/m2 ). Least Squares were estimated. Model fit was evaluated using the
Satorra-Bentler Scaled Chi-Square (S–B ␹ 2 ), and several absolute,
2.1.3. Procedure comparative and parsimonious fit indices: the Root Mean Square
The English version of the SATAQ-4R-Female was translated Error of Approximation (RMSEA), the Standardized Root Mean
following widely accepted guidelines (Brislin, 1986). A native Ital- Square Residual (SRMR), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the
ian speaker translated the SATAQ-4R-Female and Male items from Non-Normed Fit Index (NNFI). The Akaike Information Criterion
English into Italian. It was then independently back translated into (AIC) was used to compare nested models. According to the criteria
English to ensure accuracy by another native Italian speaker who suggested by Schermelleh-Engel, Moosbrugger, and Müller (2003)),
was not affiliated with the study. The back-translated English ver- CFI and NNFI values between .95 and .97 suggest an acceptable
sion and the original version were carefully compared in order to model fit, while values greater than .97 indicate a good model fit. An
arrive at a final Italian version (see Appendix A), which could be RMSEA value between .05 and .08 suggests an acceptable fit, while
considered as clear, accurate and able to capture the meaning of an RMSEA value lower than .05 suggests a good fit. With respect to
the original scale. SRMR, well-fitting models obtain values lower than .05; however,
Participants were asked to take part in a study on body image values below .10 are deemed acceptable. With respect to the AIC,
and completed a paper-pencil version of the scales in the pres- the model that produces the lowest value is superior. Cronbach’s
ence of a researcher, and in the same place where they were alpha was used to determine the internal consistency reliabil-
recruited. Participants for the CFA and internal consistency analyses ity of the SATAQ-4R-Female subscales. Given the large range of
(n = 732) completed only the SATAQ-4R-Female, and were recruited participants’ age, bivariate Pearson correlation coefficients among
from public places such as libraries, leisure centers, universities, this variable and SATAQ-4R subscales were examined. Convergent
and study rooms. Participants for the analysis of both conver- validity was assessed via bivariate Pearson correlation coefficients
gent validity and test-retest reliability were recruited exclusively between SATAQ-4R-Female subscales and measures of body dissat-
from university settings to facilitate greater recruitment and reten- isfaction, eating disorder symptomatology, drive for muscularity,
tion. Although all 732 women completed the SATAQ-4R-Female, a self-esteem, family, and peer influences. A correlation between .10
smaller subset sample (n = 335) was asked to complete a longer and .30 was considered small, between .30 and .50 was medium,
questionnaire containing six additional measures, which were uti- and above .50 was considered large (Cohen, 1988). Pearson’s r was
lized to assess the scale’s convergent validity. These participants used to establish evidence of test-retest reliability. Correlations of
were recruited from university classes and study rooms. Another .70 or higher indicate good test-retest reliability, while correla-
subset sample (n = 70), drawn from the total sample, was used for tions between .60 and .70 indicate acceptable reliability (Crocker &
the test-retest reliability analyses. Given that the analysis of test- Algina, 2008).
52 C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58

Table 2
Italian SATAQ-4R Factor Loadings and Internal Consistency (Study 1, N = 732; Study 2, N = 364).

Internalization Pressures

Thin/Low Body Muscular General Family Peers Significant Media


Fat attractiveness Others

Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men

Item 1 .88 .89


Item 2 .81 .68
Item 3 .87 .88
Item 4 .88 .86
Item 5 .91 .81
Item 6 .88 .93
Item 7 .70 .89
Item 8 .93 .77
Item 9 -.69 .75
Item 10 -.62 .78
Item 11 .76 .71
Item 12 .73 .93
Item 13 .58 .82
Item 14 -.66 .92
Item 15 .79 .93
Item 16 .93 .96
Item 17 .88 .83
Item 18 .92 .86
Item 19 .66 .88
Item 20 .90 .79
Item 21 .91 .93
Item 22 .88 .89
Item 23 .87 .95
Item 24 .86 .90
Item 25 .91 .98
Item 26 .90 .91
Item 27 .85 .95
Item 28 .96 .92
Item 29 .96
Item 30 .84
Item 31 .82

Cronbach’s alpha .83 .80 .88 .90 .82 .88 .85 .84 .88 .92 .90 .91 .95 .97

Note. Robust Maximum Likelihood method of estimation.

2.2. Results Table 3


Means and Standard Deviations of the Italian SATAQ-4R Subscales (Study 1, N = 732;
Study 2, N = 364).
2.2.1. Factor structure and reliability
Descriptive statistics were computed for all of the SATAQ-4R- Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Women Men
Female items. For Item 23, skew was higher than 2, while kurtosis
for all items was lower than 7 (West et al., 1995). Given that the Thin/Low Body Fat 2.92 (0.01) 2.16 (1.02)
assumption of multivariate normality was not met for one of the Internalization Muscular 2.22 (0.88) 2.66 (1.03)
General attractiveness 3.82 (0.73) 3.37 (1.14)
items, CFA was conducted using the Robust Maximum Likelihood
Family 1.91 (0.94) 1.68 (0.79)
method of estimation along with the Satorra-Bentler Scaled Chi- Peers 1.64 (0.85) 1.91 (0.98)
Pressures
Square, which corrects for non-normality. Significant Others 1.84 (0.97) 1.87 (0.93)
The six-factor model had a poor fit to the data. The S–B ␹ 2 was Media 3.08 (1.33) 2.14 (1.24)
significant (3217.65, p < .001), the CFI (.93) and the NNFI (.92) were
not within the standard recommended range. The RMSEA (.10) and
were positive and significant. Age showed only small correlations
the SRMR (.13) suggested a poor fit. The AIC was 3371.65. The seven-
with the scores that women obtained on the seven subscales of the
factor model had a good fit to the data. The S–B ␹ 2 was significant
SATAQ-4R (rs ranged from -.05 to -.20).
(1590.47, p < .001), but the CFI (.97) and the NNFI (.97) were within
the standard recommended range, suggesting a good fit. The RMSEA
2.2.2. Convergent validity
(.06) and the SRMR (.05) indicated an acceptable fit. The AIC was
Table 5 summarizes the correlations between SATAQ-4R-
1191.28. The seven-factor model provided the best fit according to
Female subscales and convergent measures (i.e., EDE-Q, BSQ-14,
both absolute and comparative fit indices. Moreover, it had the low-
RSES, DMS, FOPAS-F, PAS). The Pressures subscales, as well as
est AIC value, which suggests it was more parsimonious than the
the Thin/Low Body Fat and General Attractiveness Internaliza-
six-factor model, and it was the model that most plausibly gener-
tion subscales, demonstrated small to large positive associations
ated the data. Factor loadings for the seven subscales are presented
with eating disorder symptomatology and body dissatisfaction. As
in Table 2. Each item loaded strongly onto its intended subscale. All
predicted, these SATAQ-4R-Female subscales exhibited small to
of the primary loadings were greater than .30, ranging from .58 to
medium negative associations with global self-esteem. The Inter-
.96. The internal consistency of the seven subscales was good, rang-
nalization: Muscular subscale exhibited large positive associations
ing from .82 to .95 (see Table 2). Means and intercorrelations among
with drive for muscularity, but was not significantly associated
the subscales are presented respectively in Tables 3 and 4. Gener-
with eating disorder symptomatology, body dissatisfaction, or self-
ally, the intercorrelations between the SATAQ-4R-Female subscales
esteem. As predicted, the SATAQ-4R-Female Family, Peers, and
C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58 53

Table 4
Intercorrelations Among the Italian SATAQ-4R Subscales (Study 1, N = 732; Study 2, N = 364).

Pressures Internalization

Thin/Low Body Fat Muscular General attractiveness PeersFamily Significant Others Media

Thin/Low Body Fat – .43*** .27*** .36***.45*** .43*** .35***


Internalization Muscular .17*** – .44*** .53***.39*** .46*** .36***
General attractiveness .55*** .20*** – .24***.20*** .25*** .26***
Family .12** .05 .06 .52***– .64*** .37***
Peers .21*** .13*** .10** – .55*** .71*** .46***
Pressures
Significant Others .19*** .13*** .12** .66***.62*** – .41***
Media .38*** .05 .33*** .36***.23*** .28*** –

Note. Women are below the diagonal, men are above the diagonal.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 5
Correlations Between the Italian SATAQ-4R Subscales and Convergent Measures (Study 1, n = 335; Study 2, n = 110).

Internalization Pressures

Thin/Low Body Fat Muscular General attractiveness Family Peers Significant Others Media

Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men

EDE-Q .55*** .65*** .04 .50*** .39*** .44*** .23*** .55*** .30*** .36*** .31*** .52*** .38*** .65***
BSQ-14 .60*** – .08 – .41*** – .23*** – .32*** – .30*** – .40*** –
MBAS – .59*** – .44*** – .35*** – .51*** – .40*** – .51*** – .51***
RSES -.32*** -.37*** .02 -.28** -.19** -.14 -.11* -.32** -.24*** -.30** -.16** -.39*** -.19** -.27**
PAS .34*** .52*** .08 .42*** .23*** .32** .38*** .52*** .45*** .42*** .41*** .55*** .28*** .55***
FOPAS-F .25*** .57*** .11* .33*** .14* .27** .61*** .62*** .44*** .37*** .47*** .51*** .28*** .38***
DMS .12* .41*** .50*** .76*** .07 .45*** .04 .30** .04 .62*** .08 .54*** .03 .46***

Note. EDE-Q = Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire; BSQ-14 = Body Shape Questionnaire-14; MBAS = Male Body Attitudes Scale; RSES = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale;
PAS = Peer Attribution Scale; FOPAS-F = Feedback on Physical Appearance Scale-Family; DMS = Drive for Muscularity Scale.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Significant Others Pressures subscales presented medium to large 3. Study 2


positive associations with measures of family and peer influence.
While body dissatisfaction appears to be more prevalent among
women, Italian men pay great attention to the body (Ruspini, 2007),
2.2.3. Test-retest reliability
desire a more muscular body than the average male body shape
Test-retest reliability for each subscale was quite good, with
(Gilli & Ruspini, 2014; Zelli et al., 2010), and are particularly con-
r values ranging from .68 to .88 (Internalization: Thin/Low Body
cerned with the care of their appearance (Capellani et al., 2014). To
Fat = .84; Internalization: Muscular = .73; Internalization: General
help facilitate further research related to the influence of sociocul-
Attractiveness = .88; Pressures: Family = .86; Pressures: Peers = .70;
tural factors on Italian men’s eating and body image, the aim of the
Pressures: Significant Others = .74; and Pressures: Media = .68).
present study was to translate and validate an Italian version of the
SATAQ-4R-Male. A CFA was performed to evaluate the factor struc-
2.3. Discussion ture of the Italian version among men, and internal consistency and
test-retest reliability of the scale were examined. Exploratory factor
The present study provides evidence for the validity and relia- analysis on the English male version revealed a seven-factor struc-
bility of the Italian version of the SATAQ-4R-Female among Italian ture. The structure of the English male version was examined with
women. The original seven-factor structure identified among college men, while no studies analysed it with male adolescents.
college women from the United States (Schaefer et al., 2017) However, as the SATAQ-4R-Female demonstrated two different
was confirmed. Each of the original subscales cleanly replicated factor structures among differently aged women, we decided to
and showed good internal consistency and acceptable test-retest examine the fit of both the six- and the seven-factor model also for
reliability. Results also support the convergent validity of the men. Convergent validity was examined in relation to body dissat-
SATAQ-4R-Female among Italian women. Each Pressures sub- isfaction, eating disorder symptomatology, drive for muscularity,
scale, the Thin/Low Body Fat, and the General Attractiveness self-esteem, family, and peer influences. Consistent with previous
Internalization subscales were significantly associated with body work examining the SATAQ-4R-Male among a sample of college
dissatisfaction, self-esteem, and disordered eating symptomatol- men in the United States (Schaefer et al., 2017), the Pressures sub-
ogy. These findings are consistent with theory suggesting that scales and the Internalization: Thin/Low Body Fat subscale were
thinness pressures and internalization of the thin ideal contribute expected to demonstrated small to medium positive associations
to negative eating and body image outcomes (Thompson et al., with eating disorder symptomatology and body dissatisfaction, and
1999). In contrast, the Internalization: Muscular subscale was a medium negative association with self-esteem. The Internaliza-
highly related to drive for muscularity but was not associated with tion: General Attractiveness subscale was expected to exhibit a
disordered eating or body image. This finding is consistent with pre- small positive association with eating disorder symptomatology.
vious work indicating that the muscular ideal is more relevant to The Internalization: Muscular subscale was expected to exhibit a
negative eating outcomes for boys and men than girls and women large positive association with drive for muscularity, but small pos-
(Cafri et al., 2005). Family, Peers, and Significant Others Pressures itive association with eating disorder symptomatology, and a small
subscales demonstrated moderate to large positive associations negative association with self-esteem. The SATAQ-4R Family, Peers
with measures of general family and peer influence, supporting the and Significant Others Pressures subscales were expected to exhibit
convergent validity of these scales.
54 C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58

medium to large positive associations with measures of family and (1016.16, p < .001), but the CFI (.98) and the NNFI (.97) were within
peer influence. the standard recommended range, suggesting a good fit; the RMSEA
(.08) and the SRMR (.06) suggested an acceptable fit. The AIC was
3.1. Method 1170.16. If we compare the fit indices of the two models, we can
observe that the seven-factor model provided a slightly better fit
3.1.1. Participants according to both absolute and comparative fit indices. Moreover, it
Participants were 364 Italian men aged between 18 and 72 had the lowest AIC value, which suggests it was more parsimonious
(77.5% of men aged from 18 to 30 years, consistent with the orig- than the other, and it was the model that most plausibly generated
inal United States. validation sample). All participants had Italian the data. Factor loadings for the seven subscales are presented in
nationality and were born in Italy (age M = 27.26, SD = 8.94; BMI Table 2. All items loaded strongly onto their intended subscale. All
M = 24.45, SD = 4.06). All participants were utilized for the CFA. primary loadings were greater than .30, ranging from .71 to .98.
Convergent validity analyses were conducted using a subset of The internal consistency of the seven subscales was good, with
110 participants (drawn from the larger sample) who completed Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .80 to .97 (see Table 2). Means and
the convergent measures (age M = 34.43, SD = 11.70; BMI M = 27.06, intercorrelations among the subscales are presented respectively
SD = 5.22). This sample size conferred adequate power to detect in Tables 3 and 4. All associations were significant and positive.
small to moderate effects (Hulley et al., 2013). The test-retest reli- Age showed only small correlations with the scores men obtained
ability of the SATAQ-4R-Male was examined among a subset of on the seven subscales of the SATAQ-4R (r ranged from .01 to -.16).
28 participants (age M = 23.29, SD = 4.62; BMI M = 22.57, SD = 2.50),
drawn from the larger sample, who completed the questionnaire 3.2.2. Convergent validity
on two occasions four weeks apart. This sample size conferred The correlations between the SATAQ-4R-Male subscales and
adequate power to detect a large effect (Hulley et al., 2013). Socio- body dissatisfaction (BSQ-14), drive for muscularity (DMS), eat-
demographic information for each of the samples is displayed in ing disorder symptomatology (EDE-Q), self-esteem (RSES), family
Table 1. influence (FOPAS-F), and peer attributions (PAS) are presented
in Table 5. The Pressures subscales and the Internalization:
3.1.2. Measures Thin/Low Body Fat subscale demonstrated medium to large positive
Participants completed the Italian version of the SATAQ-4R- associations with eating disorder symptomatology and body dissat-
Male (see Appendix B), which is composed of 28 items rated on isfaction, and small to medium negative associations with global
a 5-point Likert scale with response options ranging from 1 (def- self-esteem. The Internalization: General Attractiveness subscale
initely disagree) to 5 (definitely agree). The translation steps were exhibited medium positive associations with eating disorder symp-
the same that we followed for the SATAQ-4R-Female; the transla- tomatology and body dissatisfaction. The Internalization: Muscular
tors for the male and female versions of the scale were the same subscale exhibited large positive associations with drive for mus-
individuals. Similar to Study 1, participants completed the SATAQ- cularity and with eating disorder symptomatology, and a small
4R-Male as well as the Italian versions of the EDE-Q (␣ = .94), DMS negative association with self-esteem. The SATAQ-4R-Male Family,
(␣ = .91), RSES (␣ = .82), FOPAS-F (␣ = .84), and PAS (␣ = .92). Stud- Peers and Significant Others Pressures subscales exhibited medium
ies examining the reliability and validity for the Italian versions of to large positive associations with measures of family and peer
these scales are discussed in Study 1. influence.
Additionally, participants completed the Male Body Attitudes
Scale (MBAS). Male body dissatisfaction was assessed through the 3.2.3. Test-retest reliability
Italian version (Stefanile, Nerini, & Matera, 2016) of the MBAS Test-retest reliability for the seven subscales was acceptable,
(Tylka, Bergeron, & Schwartz, 2005). The MBAS is composed of 24 with values ranging from .60 to .86 (Internalization: Thin/Low Body
items (e.g., “I think my arms should be more muscular”) rated along Fat = .76; Internalization: Muscular = .77; Internalization: General
a 6-point scale (1 = never, 6 = always). Higher scores reflect more Attractiveness = .60; Pressures: Family = .76; Pressures: Peers = .67;
negative body attitudes. Previous work using the Italian version of Pressures: Significant Others = .60; and Pressures: Media = .86).
the MBAS (Stefanile et al., 2016) confirmed the same 3-factor struc-
ture of the English version and good reliability for the scale’s scores. 3.3. Discussion
Cronbach’s alpha in the current study was good (␣ = .86).
The present study provides evidence for the validity and relia-
3.1.3. Procedure and data analyses bility of the Italian versions of the SATAQ-4R-Male among Italian
The procedure for data-collection and analysis in Study 2 was men. The original seven-factor structure identified among college
identical to the procedures described in Study 1. men from the United States (Schaefer et al., 2017) was confirmed.
Each of the original subscales cleanly replicated and demonstrated
3.2. Results good internal consistency and acceptable test-retest reliability.
Results also support the convergent validity of the SATAQ-4R-Male
3.2.1. Factor structure and reliability among Italian men. Each Pressures subscale and the Internaliza-
Three of the SATAQ-4R-Male items (Items 9, 12, and 13) exhib- tion: Thin/Low Body Fat subscale was significantly associated with
ited skew was higher than 2, while kurtosis values for all items were body dissatisfaction, self-esteem, disordered eating symptoma-
lower than 7 (West et al., 1995). Given that the assumption of mul- tology, and drive for muscularity. In contrast to results obtained
tivariate normality was not met for some items, CFA was conducted within the female sample, the Internalization: Muscular subscale
using the Robust Maximum Likelihood method of estimation along was highly related to each of the unhealthy outcomes assessed. This
with the Satorra-Bentler Scaled Chi-Square, which corrects for non- finding suggests that Italian men who aspire to muscular aesthetic
normality. The six-factor model had a good fit to the data. Although ideals are more likely to experience decreased self-esteem, higher
the S–B ␹ 2 was significant (1205.65, p < .001), the CFI (.97) and the levels body dissatisfaction and eating symptomatology, as well as
NNFI (.97) were within the standard recommended range, show- and greater engagement in behaviors aimed at increasing muscu-
ing a good fit. The RMSEA (.09) and the SRMR (.06) suggested an larity. While the Internalization: General Attractiveness subscale
acceptable fit. The AIC was 1347.65. The seven-factor model had a was associated with body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
good fit to the data. The S–B ␹ 2 was significant also for this model among males, it did not correlate with self-esteem in the male sam-
C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58 55

ple. These results suggest that Italian men’s self-esteem might be versions of the scales among younger Italians is recommended.
more linked to specific features of appearance, such as thinness and Similarly, the scale may evidence a different structure or psycho-
muscularity, rather than to a general idea of attractiveness, which metric properties among older Italians. While age demonstrated
can be a more abstract and general concept. only small, often nonsignificant associations with SATAQ-4R sub-
scales in the current sample, further work may seek to evaluate
4. General discussion the psychometric properties of the scale using larger samples
of older adults. Second, the current study is unable to assess
The current set of studies examined the psychometric properties the degree to which other important demographic variables may
of the Italian versions of the SATAQ-4R-Male and SATAQ-4R- influence the psychometric properties of the scale and its associ-
Female. Results indicate that these measures provide a valid ation with relevant constructs. For example, although the impact
and reliable assessment of appearance-ideal internalization and of appearance-ideal internalization and appearance pressures on
appearance pressures among Italian men and women. The original body image and disordered eating may vary by sexual orientation
seven-factor structure identified among college men and women (Cella, Iannaccone, & Cotrufo, 2013; Cohen & Tannenbaum, 2001;
from the United States (Schaefer et al., 2017) was confirmed in the Hazzard et al., 2019; Nerini, Matera, & Stefanile, 2018; Yean et al.,
current samples of Italian community men and women. Each of 2013), the current study is unable examine the impact of sexual
the original subscales cleanly replicated and showed good inter- orientation on the scale structure or associations with convergent
nal consistency and acceptable test-retest reliability. Notably, some measures. Third, the SATAQ-4R assesses the influence of significant
scales presented lower test-retest reliability among men, which others as a whole (i.e., romantic partners, teachers, coaches) and is
suggests that men’s sociocultural attitudes towards appearance unable to examine the specific impact that more narrowly defined
might be more variable than the ones of women, for whom beauty groups may have on body image and disordered eating (Francisco,
ideals are more likely to be consolidated and socially shared. Over- Narciso, & Alarcão, 2012; Markey & Markey, 2014; Nerini, Matera &
all, these results support the distinctiveness of thin, muscular, Stefanile, 2016; Policardo et al., 2018; Schaefer & Blodgett Salafia,
and general appearance-ideal internalization among Italian adults. 2014). Finally, the decision to recruit participants from university
Moreover, results support the presence of distinct sources of per- classes led to obtain very small samples for the analysis of the
ceived appearance pressures in Italian culture. Notably, Italian test-retest reliability, especially for men; therefore, future studies
men and women considered significant others to be distinct from should seek to evaluate the stability of scale scores within larger
peers and family members, in contrast to findings from Turkish samples.
women (Cihan et al., 2016). These findings are consistent with In sum, internalization of appearance ideals and appearance-
a growing body of research using Italian samples which sug- related pressures are strongly implicated in the development of
gests that significant others (such as one’s romantic partner) are negative body image and disordered eating. Further, as evidence
relevant additional sources of appearance-related pressures for suggests high rates of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating
both women and men (i.e., Policardo, Matera, Nerini, & Stefanile, among Italian men and women (Carta et al., 2014; Ministero della
2018). Results also support the convergent validity of the SATAQ- Salute, 2017; Pirani & Matera, 2018), continued examination of
4R-Male and SATAQ-4R-Female among Italian men and women. sociocultural influences among Italians is needed. The current set
Overall, these findings confirm the association between sociocul- of studies supports the psychometric properties of commonly used
tural influences and unhealthy outcomes, such as dissatisfaction measures of appearance-ideal internalization and appearance pres-
with one’s body (Blond, 2008; Galioto, Karazsia, & Crowther, 2012; sures, the SATAQ-4R-Male and SATAQ-4R-Female, in Italian men
Grammas & Schwartz, 2009; Keery, van den Berg, & Thompson, and women. Thus, this research contributes to a growing body of
2004; Knauss et al., 2007; Shroff & Thompson, 2006; Stice & Shaw, work examining risk and maintenance factors among Italians and
2002; Thompson & Stice, 2001; Thompson et al., 2004; Tylka, provides an important tool for pursing further sociocultural risk
2011), eating pathology (Thompson et al., 1999; Tylka, 2011), factor work in this population.
and decreased self-esteem (Paxton et al., 2006; Tiggemann, 2005)
among Italian men and women. Appendix A
Some limitations of this study should be acknowledged. First,
in the current study, the psychometric properties of the scale were SATAQ 4R- Female version
examined among adults, but not among adolescents. Given that Legga adesso attentamente ognuna delle seguenti affermazioni
the SATAQ-4R-Female demonstrated a slightly different factor- e indichi con una crocetta il Suo grado di accordo o disaccordo con
structure among adolescent girls compared to college women in ciascuna di esse. Nel rispondere pensi al Suo aspetto e a come si
the United States, additional psychometric testing of the Italian sente riguardo al modo in cui appare. Le affermazioni che seguono
riguardano elementi diversi del Suo aspetto, inclusi peso, forma,
muscoli, grasso corporeo e aspetto globale.
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

01. Per me è importante apparire muscolosa 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


02. Per me è importante avere un bell’aspetto con gli abiti che indosso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
03. Voglio che il mio corpo sembri molto magro 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
04. Penso molto a sembrare muscolosa 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
05. Penso molto al mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
06. Penso molto a sembrare magra 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
07. Voglio essere bella 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
08. Voglio che il mio corpo sembri muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
09. Non penso molto al mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
10. Non voglio che il mio corpo sembri muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
11. Voglio che il mio corpo sembri molto snello 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
12. Per me è importante essere attraente 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
13. Penso molto ad avere poco grasso corporeo 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
14. Non penso molto a come appaio 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
15. Vorrei avere un corpo che sembri molto muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
La preghiamo di rispondere alle seguenti domande attinenti alla Sua famiglia (inclusi genitori, fratelli, sorelle, parenti).
56 C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58

molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

16. Sento le pressioni dei membri della mia famiglia a sembrare più magra 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
17. Sento le pressioni dei membri della mia famiglia a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
18. I membri della mia famiglia mi incoraggiano a diminuire la quantità di grasso del mio corpo 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
19. I membri della mia famiglia mi incoraggiano ad essere in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
Risponda alle seguenti domande attinenti ai Suoi coetanei (inclusi amici, colleghi di corso o di lavoro, altre persone della Sua età).
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

20. I miei coetanei mi incoraggiano ad essere più magra 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


21. Sento le pressioni dei miei coetanei a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
22. Sento le pressioni dei miei coetanei a sembrare in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
23. Ricevo pressioni da parte dei miei coetanei a diminuire la quantità di grasso del mio corpo 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
Risponda alle seguenti domande attinenti alle persone significative nella Sua vita (inclusi il partner, insegnanti, allenatore, altre persone
particolarmente importanti per Lei).
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

24. Persone significative per me mi incoraggiano ad essere più magra 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


25. Sento le pressioni di persone significative a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
26. Sento le pressioni di persone significative a sembrare in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
27. Ricevo pressioni da parte di persone significative a diminuire la quantità di grasso del mio corpo 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
Risponda alle seguenti domande attinenti ai media (inclusi TV, riviste, internet, film, tabelloni e avvisi pubblicitari).
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

28. Sento le pressioni dei media ad apparire in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


29. Sento le pressioni dei media a sembrare più magra 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
30. Sento le pressioni dei media a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
31. Sento le pressioni dei media a diminuire la quantità di grasso del mio corpo 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5

Appendix B

SATAQ 4-R Male version


Legga adesso attentamente ognuna delle seguenti affermazioni e indichi con una crocetta il Suo grado di accordo o disaccordo con
ciascuna di esse. Nel rispondere pensi al Suo aspetto e a come si sente riguardo al modo in cui appare. Le affermazioni che seguono
riguardano elementi diversi del Suo aspetto, inclusi peso, forma, muscoli, grasso corporeo e aspetto globale.
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

01. Per me è importante apparire muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


02. Voglio che il mio corpo sembri molto magro 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
03. Penso molto a sembrare muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
04. Penso molto a sembrare magro 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
05. Voglio che il mio corpo sembri muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
06. Non penso davvero molto al mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
07. Non penso molto a come appaio 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
08. Vorrei avere un corpo che sembri molto muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
La preghiamo di rispondere alle seguenti domande attinenti alla Sua famiglia (inclusi genitori, fratelli, sorelle, parenti).
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

09. Sento le pressioni dei membri della mia famiglia a sembrare più magro 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
10. Sento le pressioni dei membri della mia famiglia a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
11. I membri della mia famiglia mi incoraggiano ad essere in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
12. Sento le pressioni dei membri della mia famiglia ad essere più muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
13. I membri della mia famiglia mi incoraggiano ad aumentare la dimensione o la definizione dei miei muscoli 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
Risponda alle seguenti domande attinenti ai Suoi coetanei (inclusi amici, colleghi di corso o di lavoro, altre persone della Sua età).
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

14. Sento le pressioni dei miei coetanei a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


15. Sento le pressioni dei miei coetanei a sembrare in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
16. Sento le pressioni da parte dei miei coetanei ad essere più muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
17. I miei coetanei mi incoraggiano ad aumentare la dimensione o la definizione dei miei muscoli 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
Risponda alle seguenti domande attinenti alle persone significative nella Sua vita (inclusi il partner, insegnanti, allenatore, altre persone
particolarmente importanti per Lei).
molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

18. Sento le pressioni di persone significative a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


19. Sento le pressioni di persone significative a sembrare in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
20. Ricevo pressioni da parte di persone significative a diminuire la quantità di grasso del mio corpo 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
21. Ricevo pressioni da parte di persone significative ad essere più muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
22. Sento le pressioni di persone significative ad aumentare la dimensione o la definizione dei miei muscoli 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
Risponda alle seguenti domande attinenti ai media (inclusi TV, riviste, internet, film, tabelloni e avvisi pubblicitari).
C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58 57

molto in disaccordo molto d’accordo

23. Sento le pressioni dei media ad apparire in gran forma 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5


24. Sento le pressioni dei media a sembrare più magro 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
25. Sento le pressioni dei media a migliorare il mio aspetto 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
26. Sento le pressioni dei media a diminuire la quantità di grasso del mio corpo 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
27. Sento le pressioni dei media ad essere più muscoloso 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5
28. Sento le pressioni dei media ad aumentare la dimensione o la definizione dei miei muscoli 䊐1 䊐2 䊐3 䊐4 䊐5

Declaration of Competing Interest Journal of Sport Psychology, 43, 479–502. http://dx.doi.org/10.7354/IJSP2012.43.


015
Galioto, R., Karazsia, B. T., & Crowther, J. H. (2012). Familial and peer modeling and
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest con- verbal commentary: Associations with muscularity-oriented body
cerning this article. dissatisfaction and body change behaviors. Body Image, 9, 293–297. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.12.004
Gilli, M., & Ruspini, E. (2014). What is old and what is new? Representation of
References masculinity in travel brochures. In T. Thurnell-Read & M. Casey (Eds.), Men,
masculinities, travel and tourism (pp. 204–218). Basingstoke: Palgrave
Amaral, A. C. S., & Ferreira, M. E. C. (2017). Body dissatisfaction and associated MacMillan.
factors among Brazilian adolescents: A longitudinal study. Body Image, 22, Girard, M., Rodgers, R. F., & Chabrol, H. (2018). Prospective predictors of body
32–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.04.006 dissatisfaction, drive for thinness, and muscularity concerns among young
Barcaccia, B., Balestrini, V., Saliani, A. M., Baiocco, R., Mancini, F., & Schneider, B. H. women in France: A sociocultural model. Body Image, 26, 103–110. http://dx.
(2018). Dysfunctional eating behaviors, anxiety, and depression in Italian boys doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.07.001
and girls: The role of mass media. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 40, 72–77. Grammas, D. L., & Schwartz, J. P. (2009). Internalization of messages from society
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2016-2200 and perfectionism as predictors of male body image. Body Image, 6, 31–36.
Bagnara, S., Huon, G., & Donazzolo, S. (2004). Factorial structure of the http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.10.002
sociocultural attitudes towards appearance questionnaire among italian and Hazzard, V. M., Schaefer, L. M., Shaumberg, K., Bardone-Cone, A. M., Frederick, D. A.,
australian girls. European Eating Disorders Review, 12, 321–326. http://dx.doi. Klump, K. L., . . . & Thompson, J. K. (2019). Testing the Tripartite Influence
org/10.1002/erv.584 Model among heterosexual, bisexual, and lesbian women. Body Image, 30,
Blond, A. (2008). Impacts of exposure to images of ideal bodies on male body 145–149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.07.001
dissatisfaction: A review. Body Image, 5, 244–250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Heinberg, L. J., Thompson, J. K., & Stormer, S. (1995). Development and validation of
bodyim.2008.02.003 the sociocultural attitudes towards appearance questionnaire. The
Brislin, R. W. (1986). The wording and translation of research instruments. In W. J. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 81–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
Lonner & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research (pp. 1098-108X(199501)17:1<81::AID-EAT2260170111>3.0.CO;2-Y
137–164). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Holgado-Tello, F. P., Chacón-Moscoso, S., Barbero-García, I., & Vila-Abad, E. (2010).
Cafri, G., Thompson, J. K., Ricciardelli, L., McCabe, M., Yesalis, C., & Smolak, L. Polychoric versus Pearson correlations in exploratory and confirmatory factor
(2005). Pursuit of the muscular ideal: Physical and psychological consequences analysis of ordinal variables. Quality & Quantity: International Journal of
and putative risk factors. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 215–239. http://dx.doi. Methodology, 44, 153–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11135-008-9190-y
org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.09.003 Hulley, S. B., Cummings, S. R., Browner, W. S., Grady, D., & Newman, T. B. (2013).
Calugi, S., Milanese, C., Sartirana, M., El Ghoch, M., Sartori, F., Geccherle, E., . . . & Designing clinical research: An epidemiologic approach (4th ed.). Philadelphia,
Dalle Grave, R. (2017). The eating disorder examination questionnaire: PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Reliability and validity of the italian version. Eating and Weight Jöreskog, K. G. (1994). On the estimation of polychoric correlations and their
Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 22, 509–514. http://dx.doi. asymptotic covariance matrix. Psychometrika, 59, 381–389. http://dx.doi.org/
org/10.1007/s40519-016-0276-6 10.1007/BF02296131
Capellani, A., Codazzi, M., Del Greco, M., & Rossi, M. (2014). Quando il corpo va in Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2006). LISREL 8.80 for Windows (Computer software).
scena: Donne e uomini tra uniformità, identità e ruoli. In L. Carminati (Ed.), Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.
Specchio delle mie brame. Corpi pubblicitari tra inganno e desiderio (pp. 33–46). Keery, H., van den Berg, P., & Thompson, J. K. (2004). An evaluation of the Tripartite
Parma: Spaggiari S.p.A. Influence Model of body dissatisfaction and eating disturbance with adolescent
Carta, M. G., Preti, A., Moro, M. F., Aguglia, E., Balestrieri, M., Caraci, F., . . . & Bhugra, girls. Body Image, 1, 237–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.03.001
D. (2014). Eating disorders as a public health issue: Prevalence and attributable Knauss, C., Paxton, S. J., & Alsaker, F. D. (2007). Relationships amongst body
impairment of quality of life in an Italian community sample. International dissatisfaction, internalisation of the media body ideal and perceived pressure
Review of Psychiatry, 26, 486–492. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2014. from media in adolescent girls and boys. Body Image, 4, 353–360. http://dx.doi.
927753 org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.06.007
Cella, S., Iannaccone, M., & Cotrufo, P. (2013). Influence of gender role orientation Law, C., & Labre, M. P. (2002). Cultural standards of attractiveness: A thirty-year
(masculinity versus femininity) on body satisfaction and eating attitudes in look at changes in male images in magazines. Journalism & Mass
homosexuals, heterosexuals and transsexuals. Eating and Weight Communication Quarterly, 79, 697–711. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 18, 115–124. http://dx.doi. 107769900207900310
org/10.1007/s40519-013-0017-z Lovering, M. E., Rodgers, R. F., George, J. E., & Franko, D. L. (2018). Exploring the
Cihan, B., Bozo, Ö., Schaefer, L. M., & Thompson, J. K. (2016). Psychometric Tripartite Influence Model of body dissatisfaction in postpartum women. Body
properties of the sociocultural attitudes towards appearance Image, 24, 44–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.12.001
Questionnaire-4-Revised (SATAQ-4R) in turkish women. Eating Behaviors, 21, McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. K. (2000). An exploration of the drive for muscularity in
168–171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.03.003 adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health, 48(6), 297–304.
Cohen, A. B., & Tannenbaum, I. J. (2001). Lesbian and bisexual women’s judgments http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07448480009596271
of the attractiveness of different body types. Journal of Sex Research, 38, Markey, C. N., & Markey, P. M. (2014). Gender, sexual orientation, and romantic
226–232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224490109552091 partner influence on body image. An examination of heterosexual and lesbian
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). women and their partners. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 31,
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 162–177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407513489472
Crocker, L., & Algina, J. (2008). Introduction to classical and modern test theory. Matera, C., Nerini, A., Di Gesto, C., Policardo, G. R., & Stefanile, C. (2019). Influenze
Mason, OH: Cengage Learning. socioculturali e spinta alla muscolosità in uomini e donne con orientamento
Cusumano, D. L., & Thompson, J. K. (1997). Body image and body shape ideals in omosessuale. In XIII Congresso Nazionale Associazione SIPSA – Prospettive di
magazines: Exposure, awareness, and internalization. Sex Roles, 37, 701–721. Psicologia della Salute.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02936336 Matera, C., Nerini, A., Marconi, M. N., & Stefanile, C. (2015). Commenti verbali di
Dalle Grave, R., & Calugi, S. (2010). Eating disorder examination-questionnaire fratelli e sorelle e insoddisfazione corporea: Proprietà psicometriche di una
(EDE-Q 6.0). In A. Carrozza & R. Dalle Grave (Eds.), La terapia cognitivo versione italiana della Verbal Commentary on Physical Appearance
comportamentale dei disturbi dell’alimentazione Italian translation of: Fairburn, C. Scale-Siblings (VCOPAS-S). Counseling, 8(3), 72–93.
G. (Ed.), Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders (pp. 401–407). New Matera, C., Nerini, A., & Stefanile, C. (2013). Assessing body dissatisfaction:
York, NY: Guilford Press. Firenze: Eclipsi. Validation of the Italian version of the Body shape Questionnaire-14 (BSQ-14).
Dowson, J., & Henderson, L. (2001). The validity of a short version of the Body Counseling, 6, 235–244.
shape Questionnaire. Psychiatry Research, 102, 263–271. http://dx.doi.org/10. Matera, C., Nerini, A., & Stefanile, C. (2018). Why are men interested in cosmetic
1016/S0165-1781(01)00254-2 surgery procedures? Examining the role of different forms of peer influence,
Fairburn, C. G., & Beglin, S. J. (2008). Eating disorder examination-questionnaire social comparison, internalization, and body dissatisfaction. Body Image, 26,
(EDE-Q 6.0). In C. G. Fairburn (Ed.), Cognitive behavior therapy and eating 74–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2018.06.003
disorders (pp. 309–313). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Matera, C., Nerini, A., & Stefanile, C. (2019). Sexual orientation, peer influence, body
Francisco, R., Narciso, I., & Alarcão, M. (2012). Specific predictors of disordered dissatisfaction, and eudaimonic well-being in Italian men Manuscript under
eating among elite and non-elite gymnasts and ballet dancers. International review.
58 C. Stefanile et al. / Body Image 31 (2019) 48–58

Ministero della Salute. (2017). Disturbi dell’alimentazione Retrieved from. http:// Dissertation) Retrieved from. Tampa, USA: University of South Florida. https://
www.salute.gov.it/portale/donna/dettaglioContenutiDonna. scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/1249
jsp?lingua=italiano&id=4470&area=Salute%20donna&menu=patologie Shroff, H. M., & Thompson, J. K. (2006). Peer influences, body-image dissatisfaction,
Nerini, A. (2009). Influenza dei media, autostima e insoddisfazione corporea in un eating dysfunction, and self-esteem in adolescent girls. Journal of Health
gruppo di preadolescenti maschi e femmine. Psicologia della Salute, 1, 69–84. Psychology, 11, 533–551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105306065015
http://dx.doi.org/10.3280/PDS2009-001005 Stefanile, C., Matera, C., & Pisani, E. (2009). Body shape questionnaire (BSQ-14): An
Nerini, A., & Massai, F. (2010). Influenza dei mass media, della famiglia e dei pari e italian version. Psychology & Health, 24, 376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
insoddisfazione corporea in un gruppo di preadolescenti maschi e femmine. In 08870440903126421
S. Russo & E. Viola (Eds.), AIP – X Congresso Nazionale della Sezione di Psicologia Stefanile, C., Matera, C., Nerini, A., & Pisani, E. (2011). Validation of an Italian
Sociale (pp. 229–230). Torino: TLC Grafica. version of the Sociocultural Attitudes towards Appearance Questionnaire-3
Nerini, A., Matera, C., Baroni, D., & Stefanile, C. (2016). Drive for muscularity and (SATAQ-3) on adolescent girls. Body Image, 8, 432–436. http://dx.doi.org/10.
sexual orientation: Psychometric properties of the Italian version of the Drive 1016/j.bodyim.2011.06.001
for Muscularity Scale (DMS) in straight and gay men. Psychology of Men & Stefanile, C., Matera, C., Pisani, E., & Zambrini, I. (2009). Insoddisfazione corporea in
Masculinity, 17, 137–146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039675 adolescenza: Influenze di fattori bio-psico-sociali. Psicologia della Salute, 2009-2.
Nerini, A., Matera, C., & Stefanile, C. (2016). Siblings’ appearance-related pp. 51–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.3280/PDS2009-002004
commentary, body dissatisfaction, and risky eating behaviors in young women. Stefanile, C., Nerini, A., & Matera, C. (2014). The factor structure and psychometric
European Review of Applied Psychology/Revue Européenne de Psychologie properties of the Italian version of the Acceptance of Cosmetic Surgery Scale.
Appliquée, 66, 269–276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2016.06.005 Body Image, 11, 370–379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.06.005
Nerini, A., Matera, C., & Stefanile, C. (2017). Influenza dei pari e interesse verso la Stefanile, C., Nerini, A., & Matera, C. (2016). Commenti verbali da parte del partner,
chirurgia estetica in un gruppo di maschi italiani. In S. Casale & A. Nerini (Eds.), insoddisfazione corporea e soddisfazione di coppia in giovani adulti. In F.
La psicologia come scienza della Salute. Pre-atti del XII Congresso Nazionale Procentese & C. Esposito (Eds.), XIV Congresso Nazionale della Sezione di
Associazione SIPSA Società Italiana di psicologia della Salute (pp. 90–91). Firenze: Psicologia Sociale della Associazione Italiana di Psicologia. Proceedings (pp.
Firenze University Press. Firenze, 3-5 Novembre 2017. 99–100).
Nerini, A., Matera, C., & Stefanile, C. (2018). Body dissatisfaction, drive for Stefanile, C., Nerini, A., Matera, C., & Antonelli, P. (2012). Proprietà psicometriche di
muscularity and well-being in Italian men: The role of sexual orientation and una versione italiana della Drive for Muscularity Scale. In Congresso Nazionale
sociocultural influences Manuscript submitted for publication. delle Sezioni dell’AIP, Chieti, 20-23 settembre 2012, Espress Edizioni. pp. 466.
Olivardia, R., Pope, H. G., Jr, Borowiecki, J. J., III, & Cohane, G. H. (2004). Biceps and Stefanile, C., Pisani, E., Matera, C., & Guiderdoni, V. (2010). Insoddisfazione
body image: The relationship between muscularity and self-esteem, corporea, comportamento alimentare e fattori di influenza socioculturale in
depression, and eating disorder symptoms. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 5, adolescenza. In L. Ceccarini & C. Rollero (Eds.), Problemi umani e sociali della
112–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1524-9220.5.2.112 convivenza. SIPCO - 8◦ Convegno Nazionale Società Italiana di Psicologia di
Parent, M. C., & Moradi, B. (2011). His biceps become him: A test of objectification Comunità. Torino: TLC (p. 148).
theory’s application to drive for muscularity and propensity for steroid use in Stice, E., & Shaw, H. (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and
college men. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58, 246–256. http://dx.doi.org/ maintenance of eating pathology: A synthesis of research findings. Journal of
10.1037/a0021398 Psychosomatic Research, 53, 985–993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-
Paxton, S. J., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Hannan, P. J., & Eisenberg, M. E. (2006). Body 3999(02)00488-9
dissatisfaction prospectively predicts depressive mood and low self-esteem in Tantleff-Dunn, S., Thompson, J. K., & Dunn, M. F. (1995). The Feedback On Physical
adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology : Appearance Scale (FOPAS): Questionnaire development and psychometric
the Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, evaluation. Eating Disorders, 3, 332–341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
American Psychological Association, Division 53, 35, 539–549. http://dx.doi.org/ 10640269508250063
10.1207/s15374424jccp3504 5 Thompson, J. K., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internalization: Mounting evidence
Pirani, E., & Matera, C. (2018). Ruolo della famiglia di origine nella relazione tra for a new risk factor for body image disturbance and eating pathology. Current
corporeità e sessualità. Paper presented at the Workshop Selfy, Dipartimento di Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 181–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
Statistica, Informatica, Applicazioni, Università degli Studi di Firenze 17-18 1467-8721.00144
settembre 2018. Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting beauty:
Policardo, G. R., Matera, C., Nerini, A., & Stefanile, C. (2018). Tu come mi vedi? Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington, DC:
Commenti del partner circa l’aspetto fisico, immagine corporea e soddisfazione di American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10312-000
coppia: Uno studio empirico. Psicologia della Salute, 2018-2. pp. 137–157. http:// Thompson, J. K., van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., & Heinberg, L. S. (2004).
dx.doi.org/10.3280/PDS2018-002011 The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-3):
Prezza, M., Trombaccia, F., & Armento, L. (1997). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Development and validation. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35,
Italian translation and validation. Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata, 223, 35–44. 293–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.10257
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Tiggemann, M. (2005). Body dissatisfaction and adolescent self-esteem:
University Press. Prospective findings. Body Image, 2, 129–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Ruspini, E. (2007). Nuove mascolinità in Italia: trasformazioni, pluralizzazioni ed bodyim.2005.03.006
educazione alla diversità. Corso on line - Introduzione agli studi di genere Tylka, T. L. (2011). Refinement of the tripartite influence model for men: Dual body
Retrieved from. https://www.cirsde.unito.it/sites/c555/files/allegatiparagrafo/ image pathways to body change behaviors. Body Image, 8, 199–207. http://dx.
04-05-2016/2. nuove mascolinita in italia 0.pdf doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.008
Schaefer, M. K., & Blodgett Salafia, E. H. (2014). The connection of teasing by Tylka, T. L., Bergeron, D., & Schwartz, J. (2005). Development and psychometric
parents, siblings, and peers with girls’ body dissatisfaction and boys’ drive for evaluation of the Male Body Attitudes Scale (MBAS). Body Image, 2, 161–175.
muscularity: The role of social comparison as a mediator. Eating Behaviors, 15, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.03.001
599–608. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.08.018 West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with
Schaefer, L. M., Burke, N. L., Thompson, J. K., Dedrick, R. F., Heinberg, L. J., Calogero, nonnormal variables. Problems and remedies. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural
R. M., . . . & Swami, V. (2015). Development and validation of the sociocultural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 56–75). Newbury Park,
attitudes towards appearance Questionnaire-4 (SATAQ-4). Psychological CA: Sage.
Assessment, 27, 54–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037917 Yean, C., Benau, E., Dakanalis, A., Hormes, J. M., Perone, J., & Timko, A. (2013). The
Schaefer, L. M., Harriger, J. A., Heinberg, L. J., Soderberg, T., & Thompson, J. K. relationship of sex and sexual orientation to self-esteem, body shape
(2017). Development and validation of the sociocultural attitudes towards satisfaction, and eating disorder symptomatology. Frontiers in Psychology, 4,
appearance Questionnaire-4-Revised (SATAQ-4R). The International Journal of 887. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00887
Eating Disorders, 50, 104–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22590 Zelli, A., Lucidi, F., & Mallia, L. (2010). The relationships among adolescents’ drive
Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of for muscularity, drive for thinness, doping attitudes and doping intentions.
structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 4, 39–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jcsp.4.1.
goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 8, 23–74. 39
Shroff, H. M. (2004). An examination of peer-related risk and protective factors for
body image disturbance and disordered eating among adolescent girls (Doctoral

You might also like