You are on page 1of 22

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol.

88 (3rd series)
Neuropsychology and behavioral neurology
G. Goldenberg, B.L. Miller, Editors
# 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 28

Neuropsychology of frontotemporal dementia

C.M. KIPPS, J.A. KNIBB, K. PATTERSON, AND J.R. HODGES*

MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK

28.1. Introduction 28.2. Behavioral variant FTD


28.2.1. Clinical features
The current understanding of frontotemporal lobar
degeneration (FTLD) owes no small part to the original
The disorder presented in Case Study 28.1 presents as a
observations and descriptions of Arnold Pick over a cen-
behavioral syndrome with an insidious onset. There is
tury ago (Pick, 1892; 1901; 1904). Current classifications
early decline in social conduct, both personal and inter-
recognize the heterogeneity within the disorder, and
personal, with emotional blunting and loss of insight
three main variants are commonly described (Lund and
(Neary et al., 1998; Bozeat et al., 2000; McKhann
Manchester Groups, 1994; Neary et al., 1998; Hodges
et al., 1999; McKhann et al., 2001). Frontotemporal et al., 2001; Table 28.1). However, only a minority of
dementia (FTD), also known as frontal variant (fv-FTD) patients display all of these core features on initial pre-
or behavioral variant (bv-FTD), typically presents with sentation (Mendez and Perryman, 2002). As descriptive
disturbed behavior and is the most common of the three criteria, many features remain difficult to quantify
syndromes. Two language variants are also described: although efforts are being made to operationalize them
semantic dementia (SD), known also as temporal variant (Rankin et al. 2005b).
FTD (tv-FTD) and a progressive nonfluent aphasic Several further deficits may be observed, and comprise
syndrome (PNFA). the supportive clinical features for diagnosis: decline in
The clinical phenotype in FTLD has an inconsistent self-care, mental rigidity, distractibility, hyperorality, per-
association with the underlying pathology. Clinical pre- severative and stereotyped behavior and several language
sentations are seldom pure, and several symptoms are features such as altered speech output, echolalia, speech
shared across the different variants, with further overlap stereotypies or mutism. Imaging abnormalities are not
on both a anatomical and pathological level. In this regard, essential for the diagnosis, but when present predomi-
although FTD has been typically associated with frontal nantly involve the frontal and temporal lobes. When
lobe disturbance, it is frequently accompanied by tem- abnormal, neuropsychological testing should demon-
poral lobe disease. The situation is mirrored in SD, and strate impairment on frontal lobe tests in the absence of
to a lesser extent in PNFA. It is helpful to view these dis- severe amnesia, aphasia or perceptuospatial disorder.
orders as individually reflecting the local extent of a wide- More recent clinically targeted criteria have broadened
spread pathological process affecting frontal and temporal the diagnostic scope, and simply require an early and
lobe networks. In clinical practice, diagnosis is based on progressive alteration in personality, with abnormal
the predominant symptom, which is probably most useful behavioral modulation resulting in responses or activities
in categorizing the types of impairment that patients are which are inappropriate enough to disrupt social or
likely to manifest. Although bv-FTD is defined by its occupational functioning (McKhann et al. 2001).
behavioral presentation, patients with language variants Diagnosis is usually easy when many abnormal clini-
of FTD often also exhibit behavioral disturbance. Conver- cal features are present, but can be particularly tricky in
sely, a mild degree of language impairment frequently the early stages of disease. It is important to establish that
accompanies the behavioral syndrome. symptom onset is indeed gradual, and that this represents

*
Correspondence to: John Hodges, MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, 15 Chaucer Rd, Cambridge CB2 2EF, UK. E-mail:
john.hodges@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk, Tel þ44-(0)1223-355294 Ext 690, Fax: þ44-(0)1223-359062.
528 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.

Case Study 28.1


A 66-year-old man, married for 35 years, who at the Formal neuropsychological assessment showed
time of presentation ran a guest house, had developed intact story recall, with information retained after a
progressive and dramatic alteration in his personality delay. There was very mild impairment of backwards
over 7 years. Previously gregarious, he became rather digit span consistent with some impairment of work-
withdrawn, and ceased conversation with his wife. He ing memory. Language and visuospatial functions
lacked empathy, and was quite disinhibited with were well preserved, but there were marked deficits
friends and family, often making sexually suggestive on tests of frontal function, particularly on the Wis-
comments, despite a complete lack of libido. He was consin Card Sorting and Trails tests. There was sym-
profoundly unmotivated, and extremely repetitive. metrical frontal and temporopolar atrophy on an MRI
He overate to a gross degree, and was quite suspicious scan, and there was marked frontotemporal hypoper-
of those around him. His memory had also declined fusion on an 18FDG-PET scan.
somewhat, and there was a suggestion of mild diffi- There was a marked deterioration over the subse-
culty with naming objects. There was no insight into quent 12 months, and he became increasingly impul-
any of these problems, yet he was able to give a fluent sive and unpredictable. His MMSE dropped to 6 over
and accurate account of his earlier life. this time.
He had a pout and a grasp reflex, was slightly At postmortem there was a moderate degree of
unsteady on his feet, and had depressed ankle jerks, cerebral atrophy, particularly of the anterior frontal
but no other focal neurological signs. On cognitive and temporal lobes, with generalized enlargement of
evaluation he was well oriented, and achieved 28 the ventricles. Microscopic examination showed
out of 30 on the Mini Mental State Examination frontotemporal lamina II vacuolation with the pre-
(MMSE). On the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examina- sence of ubiquinated inclusions in the inferior olivary
tion (ACE), he scored 90 out a possible 100 points, nucleus, consistent with a diagnosis of frontotem-
with marked impairment only on verbal fluency. poral dementia.

a distinct change from premorbid functioning. Apathy,


manifesting as passivity, inertia, or social withdrawal is
Table 28.1 perhaps the most common problem, and may result in sig-
nificant impairment in activities of daily living (ADL)
Clinical features of bv-FTD (not all present in every case)
(Kertesz et al., 1997; Gregory, 1999; Mendez and Perry-
Clinical features man, 2002; Pijnenburg et al., 2004). Lack of insight is
usual in bv-FTD, and stands in marked contrast to patients
social–behavioral disinhibition with predominant language impairment, who commonly
changes apathy recognize their deficits. Most FTD patients do not believe
lack of empathy they are unwell, and in fact up to a third have no com-
mental rigidity or inflexibility plaint at all when assessed (Pijnenburg et al., 2004). This
stereotypic behaviors can make the consultational challenging, but often these
decline in personal care
patients have minor somatic complaints or forgetfulness
eating behavior inability to regulate intake
changes preference for sweet foods which are less confrontational about discuss, and may
insight lost or markedly impaired be a way of engaging their co-operation (Pijnenburg
relatively episodic memory (usually) et al., 2004). It is important to interview caregivers sepa-
unimpaired visuospatial function rately, as there are often sensitive issues to consider which
language (variable) are difficult to broach during a joint discussion. It is often
imaging predominantly frontal and/or right easier to mention specific behavioral issues to the patient
temporal atrophy after the full extent of the problem has been ascertained
from the caregiver in private.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 529
Disinhibition is a common early symptom, and susceptible to deception by others, and liable to run up
seems particularly prevalent in those with predominant significant debt without concern for the consequences.
frontal and right temporal lobe dysfunction (Edwards- Concern about poor memory may prompt patients or
Lee et al., 1997; Lindau et al., 2000). Patients with their carers to seek medical advice, but it is oversha-
FTD may act impulsively without thought for the conse- dowed by accounts of the behavioral disturbance. Often
quences, or make tactless, inappropriate social remarks. the memory problems are actually the result of apathy,
Crude or improper sexual commentary is quite com- inattention, or a degree of semantic impairment for
mon, but does not appear to be invariably associated words and concepts. Although true amnesic symptoms
with hypersexuality. The use of pornography or sexual have good discriminatory value for AD, particularly in
chat lines is often mentioned, sometimes with signifi- the early stages, there are several reports highlighting
cant financial consequence, but in fact reduced libido genuine episodic memory disturbance in FTD at presen-
is reported in the majority (Miller et al., 1995). tation (Binetti et al., 2000; Caine et al., 2001; Hodges
Abnormal eating behavior can be profound, causing and Miller, 2001; Pasquier et al., 2001; Rosen et al.,
marked weight gain. Patients with bv-FTD typically 2002b; Hodges et al., 2004; Graham et al., 2005). In
become gluttonous with altered eating habits. Table practice, it is usually impaired registration and/or ineffi-
manners deteriorate; food is sometimes taken from cient retrieval of information—both probably related to
others, piled inappropriately high on the plate, or stuffed reduced attention or effort—that are to blame for the
into the mouth all at once. Often there is undue haste to memory complaint (Pasquier et al., 2001).
start eating. Changes in food preference such as refusing There is a distinct lack of empathy for the emotional
to eat new foods, insisting on eating particular favorites concerns of others, which may manifest as selfishness or
(e.g., fish and chips, or bananas and milk) for every meal, an insensitivity to embarrassment (Perry et al., 2001).
or becoming a vegetarian or fruitarian are less common in Similarly, it appears that personal emotional expression
bv-FTD than in SD, although they may occur in either is affected, as is the recognition of emotion in others
condition. As progressive temporal lobe pathology dis- (Lavenu et al., 1999; Snowden et al., 2001). Generalized
rupts semantic knowledge about food, bizarre food emotional blunting becomes particularly common as the
choices may emerge: orange juice used as gravy, or the disease progresses (Mendez and Perryman, 2002). Fail-
eating of partially thawed or moldy food. ure to recognize and respond to anger expressed by
Stereotypic and ritualistic behaviors such as preoccu- others may partly explain some of the socially inap-
pation with counting or clockwatching, consistently propriate behavior of these patients. Depression is rare
choosing the same leisure activity, repeatedly eating the in FTD, but this is not the case in SD. Irritability, aggres-
same food and rigid adherence to routine are all more sive behavior, and ‘cold-heartedness’ are common
common in the FTD subgroups than in AD (Nyatsanza (Rankin et al., 2003).
et al., 2003). Simple motor stereotypes involving grunt- Dysexecutive symptoms, such as impaired organiza-
ing, humming, lip smacking, hand rubbing, or foot tap- tion, planning, and goal-setting are present in both FTD
ping are seen in up to 75% of patients with prominent and AD, and are related to disease severity. As such
apathy (Snowden et al., 2001). Repeated wandering or they do not discriminate well between the two diseases
pacing a fixed route is quite common in bv-FTD. More (Bozeat et al., 2000), and such symptoms may be as
complex repetitive routines such as the use of a catch- much a consequence of apathy as poor judgment.
phrase (verbal stereotypy), or clapping the same rhythm,
are consistently seen in both bv-FTD and SD, but to a 28.2.2. Behavioral rating scales
greater extent in the latter. Many patients with SD develop
a particular, and obsessive, interest in puzzles and jigsaws. Behavior in FTD can be quantified using a variety of rat-
Neglect of self-care and impairment of other activ- ing scales: the Frontal Behavioural Inventory (FBI) (Ker-
ities of daily living is common, and reflects a combina- tesz et al., 1997; 2000b; 2003a), Frontal Behavioural
tion of apathy, unconcern, and poor judgment. As a Score (FBS) (Lebert et al., 1998), Neuropsychiatric
consequence, these patients are far more functionally Inventory (NPI) (Cummings et al., 1994), Cambridge
disabled than AD patients matched for Mini Mental Behavioural Inventory (CBI) (Bozeat et al., 2000) and
State Examination (MMSE) scores (Rosen et al., an inventory used by the Manchester Group (Barber
2004a). In the absence of a carer, the inevitable result et al. 1995). A rating scale of stereotypical behaviors
is varying degrees of squalor which in turn may prompt commonly found in FTD has also been reported (Shigen-
conflict with neighbors or local authorities. Inability obu et al., 2002). Some are based on direct interview;
to handle financial affairs renders these patients others, such as the CBI, which is the instrument used in
530 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
the Cambridge memory clinics, can be filled out by the It is difficult to be confident that behavioral rating
carer prior to the clinical interview. Incorporating a num- scales do not simply reflect biases in the predominantly
ber of memory items, as well as a functional assessment, clinical diagnoses that are being predicted, particularly
in the inventory is useful as it often allows one to distin- when these scales are being used to discriminate FTD
guish the symptom complex from that of AD, and to from other neurodegenerative diseases. There is the
estimate the likely level of disability and carer burden. potential for a degree of circularity as the inclusion cri-
The FBI, applied to a mixed group of neurodegen- teria for FTD involve mainly behavioral characteristics
erative disorders, was useful in discriminating FTLD which are also incorporated into behavioral rating
from AD, PNFA and depression, but had less success scales. Prospective studies with consecutive recruit-
when FTLD was compared with vascular dementia, ment, with diagnoses defined additionally on imaging
as there was significant overlap. In that situation it or pathological grounds, would be very useful.
was indifference, perseveration, and utilization beha- In a number of patients, it remains remarkably diffi-
vior that helped distinguish the two disorders (Kertesz cult to be certain about the clinical diagnosis. Such
et al., 2000b). Using the FBS—in which endorsement patients display the behavioral phenotype of the disor-
of any item within one of four domains (self monitoring der, but seem to have a particularly good prognosis,
dyscontrol, self neglect, self-centered behavior, affec- and do not deteriorate at the same rate as other cases.
tive disorder) is scored as positive for that feature— In some of them it is true to say that frank dementia
a distinction could be made between FTD and either supervenes. A normal MRI scan at presentation, parti-
vascular dementia or AD (Lebert et al., 1998). These cularly if the disease onset has been relatively slow,
patients were defined clinically, although several had suggests that a degree of caution is warranted when
pathological confirmation. giving prognostic information, or indeed a confirmed
Behavioral features that are common in FTD and diagnosis (Davies et al., 2005b). It is not until these
SD may cluster: using the CBI, abnormal stereotypic patients are followed longitudinally without significant
and eating behaviors, together with impaired social neuroradiological or behavioral deterioration that
awareness, were useful in contrasting the profile of doubts about the diagnosis can be resolved. To our
the FTLD variants with AD, but not each other. Other knowledge, well documented patients of this nature
researchers have reported separate behavioral profiles have yet to come to postmortem.
for FTD and SD, and describe an apathetic (FTD-A)
and disinhibited (FTD-D) form of the classic beha- 28.3. Neuropsychological tests in FTD
vioral disorder. Emotional impairments and compul-
sive, repetitive behaviors were more common in FTD A wide range of neuropsychological tests have been
than SD, as was gluttony, and separated the groups applied to patients with FTD in order to characterize
using discriminant analysis. The FTD-D group shared their deficits, and to distinguish them from patients
characteristics from each group, and no feature had a who have other neurodegenerative disorders.
unique association with this group. Using the NPI,
disinhibition and apathy were more commonly seen 28.3.1. Cognitive screening tests
in FTD than AD, with the reverse pattern seen for
depression (Levy et al., 1996). In predominantly behavioral cases, both bedside cogni-
The overall benefit of such rating scales undoubt- tive assessments and standard formal neuropsychology
edly lies more in providing a structured behavioral may be normal (Gregory and Hodges, 1996; Gregory
symptom profile than in any summated behavioral et al., 1998). A high score on the mini-mental state
score. This profile may then be used to guide subse- examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975) does not
quent, more detailed enquiry. Interestingly, total beha- exclude the diagnosis, as this scale focuses on memory,
vioral scores appear to remain relatively stable over orientation, and a small number of language items,
time in FTD (Kertesz et al., 1997; Gregory, 1999; which can be preserved until late in the course of FTD.
Marczinski et al., 2004), and high scores are often pre- In a recent epidemiological study across several sites
sent early in the disease. Relatively stable scores may (Johnson et al., 2005), the mean initial MMSE score
well reflect adaptation by carers to the changed circum- was 22.4 out of a maximum possible 30. The variance
stances, as well as the effect of interventions such as of this estimate was large, emphasizing that some
psychotropic medication. High scores at presentation patients may score very well on this test at presentation.
may also reflect a bias whereby mild behaviors, The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) is a
initially tolerated as being eccentric, escalate and reach more comprehensive instrument, with a maximum score
crisis point around the time of diagnosis. of 100, that incorporates all of the MMSE components
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 531
but additionally provides more assessment of language on the Brixton spatial anticipation task (Lough et al.,
and semantic memory than the 30-item MMSE. 2005). Ability to inhibit prepotent responses to a sen-
The frontal assessment battery (FAB) combines tence completion task such as the Hayling test may also
tests of concept similarities, letter fluency, and motor be quite abnormal, even in mild FTD (Lough et al.,
sequencing with assessment of inhibitory control and 2005). In contrast, ability to correctly plan the minimum
frontal release signs such as the grasp reflex (Dubois number of moves needed on the Tower of London task
et al., 2000). Performance correlates with scores on was not significantly impaired in mild FTD relative to
the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS) and with per- controls, although patients were slower at it (Rahman
formance (number of categories achieved and number et al., 1999b).
of perseverative errors) on the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test (WCST), but does not distinguish well between 28.3.4. Verbal fluency
FTD and subcortical frontal syndromes such as PSP.
The FAB similarities test assesses concept knowledge A reduction in verbal output is a common finding in
and the ability to express it via questions such as how FTD. Many studies report verbal fluency measures
a table and a chair, or a banana and an orange, are alike. (Elfgren et al., 1993; Gregory et al., 1997; Mathuranath
This is easy to perform at the bedside and is the most et al., 2000; Rosen et al., 2002b; Perri et al., 2005), prob-
discriminating subtest of the battery. Scores in AD ably because these are so simple to administer. Letter
are higher than in FTD, and the performance is inde- fluency involves the timed generation of as many words
pendent of age (Slachevsky et al., 2004). Although as possible beginning with a particular letter (e.g., F, A,
the battery was also assessed on a small subgroup of S), while category fluency tests the ability to provide
patients with relatively preserved MMSE, this may words that are semantically related (e.g., animals).
not adequately match for degree of dementia severity, In FTD there is usually a marked deterioration in letter
and it remains unclear as to what happens in more fluency, accompanied to a lesser extent by category flu-
subtle situations when FTD patients score very well ency, whereas in SD, scores are usually much lower for
on the MMSE. categories than letters.

28.3.2. Executive function measures 28.3.5. Digit span

Poor performance on tests of executive function has Digit span, a measure of working memory which is
been demonstrated in many studies comparing FTD dependent on executive and phonological processes, is
with controls, but the ability of such tests to discriminate inconsistently impaired in FTD, thus limiting its useful-
between FTD and AD remains relatively poor because ness as a measure of impaired attention (Gregory et al.,
the performance of both groups may be impaired (Miller 1997; Boone et al., 1999; Rahman et al., 1999b; Pasqu-
et al., 1991; Frisoni et al., 1995; Pachana et al., 1996; ier et al., 2001; Gregory et al., 2002; Rosen et al., 2002b;
Hodges et al., 1999; Pasquier et al., 2001). Nevertheless, Graham et al., 2005). This is likely to reflect poor
they remain useful for documenting one aspect of the disease severity matching across studies. In our own
overall profile of the disorder, both behavioral and experience, it is a relatively insensitive measure,
cognitive. particularly in early disease.

28.3.3. Trails, card sorting, Brixton, Hayling, and 28.3.6. Visuospatial function
Tower of London tests
Visuospatial function remains intact in FTD until the
Patients with FTD do badly on the Trails test, especially late stages (Brun et al., 1994; Barber et al., 1995; Varma
the alternating letter–number task in Part B of this test. et al., 1999) although poor organizational strategies may
Their performance is below that of controls (Rosen impair scores on some tasks such as copying of the Rey
et al., 2002b), but is similar to that of many patients with Figure. Inconsistencies can often be resolved by normal
AD (Kramer et al., 2003). They take longer to reach the performance on visuospatial tasks such as those con-
first category on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test tained in the Visual Object and Space Perception Bat-
(WCST), achieve fewer categories overall, and make a tery (VOSP) (Warrington and James, 1991). In tests of
greater number of perseverative errors relative to con- spatial span and spatial working memory, patients with
trols (Miller et al., 1991; Neary et al., 1998; Snowden mild FTD perform similarly to controls (Rahman
et al., 2003; Diehl et al., 2005; Thompson et al., 2005). et al., 1999a). Their performance on spatial and pattern
A similar failure to shift attentional set has been shown recognition is normal, but they are slower than controls.
532 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
28.3.7. Language of AD in delayed free recall conditions. Verbal priming
improves performance on a word completion task in
In addition to a commonly observed degree of abnorm- FTD, prompting suggestions that memory dysfunction
ality on the verbal fluency measures mentioned above, results from inefficient retrieval strategies, rather than
severe reduction in fluency without phonological, syn- true amnesia (Pasquier et al., 2001). On the other hand,
tactic, or semantic deficits (‘dynamic aphasia’) has a study comparing forced-choice recognition and free
occasionally been documented in FTD (Snowden recall for verbal and nonverbal material showed no dif-
et al., 1996). Otherwise, language skills appear to be ference in performance between AD and FTD variants,
relatively unaffected in FTD (Thompson et al., 2005), leading the authors to propose that it was encoding
with good comprehension and largely intact perfor- rather than retrieval that accounted for differences
mance on tests of picture naming, word–picture match- between groups (Glosser et al., 2002). They commented
ing, generation of word definitions and other on the rather inefficient, serial order, stimulus-bound
semantically based tasks such as the Pyramids and means of this encoding in FTD. While item detection
Palm Trees Test (Hodges et al. 1999; Perry and (discriminating previously seen from novel items) is at
Hodges, 2000; Howard and Patterson, 1992). There control levels, temporal source memory (remembering
are, however, several reports of disproportionate that an item came from list A vs. list B) is at chance
impairment in comprehension of verbs (actions) rela- (Simons et al., 2002). Spatial source memory (memory
tive to nouns (objects) in this patient group (Cappa for an item’s spatial location), by contrast, seems to be
et al., 1998; Rhee et al., 2001; Snowden et al., 2003). normal in FTD (A Graham, unpublished data). In AD,
On a word–picture matching task, verb processing abil- patients are typically impaired at both item detection
ity correlated with executive function performance, and source memory, temporal or spatial. There remain,
namely the Stroop, letter fluency, and Trails B tasks, however, a number of FTD patients with true amnesic
suggesting that this skill is sensitive to executive symptoms, who remain indistinguishable from AD
resource demands. Similar findings have been seen in during life notwithstanding the presence of behavioral
FTD associated with motor neuron disease (FTD- disturbance (Caine et al., 2001).
MND). Syntactic deficits can be shown using the Test Assessing accuracy at task performance is the most
of Reception of Grammar (TROG) (Bishop, 1983; commonly used means of neuropsychological evalua-
Bak et al., 2001). tion, but such an approach lacks specificity; patients
with either AD or FTD may perform poorly, but the rea-
28.3.8. Episodic memory sons for this may be quite different. Analysis of the dif-
ferent kinds of errors typical of the two disorders has
Memory complaints in FTD are relatively common, but produced interesting results (Thompson et al., 2005).
usually not specific. About 8% of pathologically con- Concrete thought and interpretation, perseveration, con-
firmed cases have reported memory symptoms in the fabulation, and poor organization were responsible for
initial stages of disease (Hodges et al., 2004), and the profile of performance of FTD subjects across a
showed documented impairments on verbal memory range of neuropsychological tasks, and seem to echo
tasks relative to controls (Rosen et al., 2002b). The pre- their behavioral dysfunction.
dictive value of such amnesic symptoms may not be
very high, given both the nonspecific way in which 28.3.9. Newer tests
carers and patients describe memory complaints, and
the heterogeneous manner in which they manifest. The relative lack of success in delineating a typical pat-
Although most patients with primary memory com- tern of neuropsychological dysfunction in FTD has
plaints are likely to have AD, the diagnostic criteria prompted investigators to explore a range of novel
for this disease have poor specificity with respect to tasks in an attempt to quantify the social cognitive def-
FTD (Varma et al., 1999), and impairments in memory icit in these patients. Executive function measures are
are poorly discriminatory. Thus, absence of severe believed to be sensitive to abnormalities in dorsolateral
amnesia (giving a high negative predictive value) mark- prefrontal cortex (Duncan and Owen, 2000) but the
edly increases the odds ratio for FTLD to be confirmed initial locus of pathology in FTD often seems to
pathologically, but the converse is not true (Rosen involve the orbitofrontal and superior medial frontal
et al., 2002b). cortices (Broe et al., 2003; Kril and Halliday, 2004).
In contrast to AD, patients with FTD perform better Lesions in these regions have been associated with
on both recognition and recall tasks (Glosser et al., abnormalities of social conduct and emotion (Hornak
2002), and do not have the accelerated forgetting typical et al., 2003).
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 533
28.3.10. Decision-making of omission, and to provide inappropriately concrete
responses in a cartoon interpretation task. They also
In view of the prominent impairments shown by used relatively fewer verbs depicting mental states
patients with FTD in the areas of appetite and reward (e.g., thinking, believing) when asked to describe a
processing, there is surprisingly little data on learning mental state cartoon. This was not just an effect of poor
for reward in this condition. Rahman et al. (1999a) verbal fluency, as mental state verbs were affected
tested decision-making using a reversal paradigm more than physical state verbs. When tested on a story
called the ID-ED shift task. Patients were specifically comprehension task which involved representation of
impaired at the reversal aspects of the task. They could first and second order Theory of Mind, the FTD
not change their behavior when a previously rewarded patients performed poorly, although this was for both
stimulus was no longer rewarded, but had no difficulty physical and mental state stories. Verb production
in transferring a rule learnt on one set of exemplars to a overall was impaired relative to controls, and is reso-
different, but related set of stimuli. In the same study, nant with findings from studies mentioned earlier that
the authors used the Cambridge Gamble task to assess suggest a disproportionate impairment of verbs relative
risk-taking behavior. In this task, bets are placed on to nouns in FTD (Bak et al., 2001; Cappa et al., 1998;
the likely position of yellow tokens hidden under one Rhee et al., 2001). On a test of eye gaze preference,
of two boxes on the screen. Patients with FTD showed several of the patients persisted in selecting their own
true risk taking behavior in that they bet a larger pro- personal favorites as the target. In a similar vein, we
portion of their accumulated winnings on the outcome. have observed patients who use idiosyncratic patterns
They did not, however, perform less accurately than in their use of rating scales (e.g., 10, 9, 8, 7, etc. for
controls in this task, even though their deliberation successive items). Such eccentricity highlights one of
times were significantly longer, a feature seen in other the potential confounds in testing this particular group.
patients with orbitofrontal lesions (Rogers et al., 1999). The role of executive function in performing some of
Their risk-taking was not just a reflection of impulsive these tasks is controversial. In a recent study (Lough
behavior, as bets had to be placed in a manner that et al., 2005), performance on both the Hayling and Brix-
minimized this possibility. More recent work by our ton tasks was unrelated to the performance of FTD
group has shown that patients with FTD are significantly patients on a mental state cartoon interpretation task.
impaired on a task requiring concurrent visual discrimi- In fact, it appeared that executive function played a sup-
nations and that, unlike controls, their performance is portive role in processing of cartoons and story vignettes
not enhanced by the addition of a genuine, and otherwise rather than being directly involved in mental state repre-
motivating, monetary reward (A Graham, unpublished sentation. Executive impairments, however, have the
data). potential to mask more specific deficits in the theory
of mind (Snowden et al., 2003), particularly if they are
28.3.11. Theory of mind severe. Furthermore, it seems that the ability to process
social rule violations is related to executive function
Demonstration of abnormalities in the ventromedial measures; despite this, knowledge of social rules them-
aspect of the frontal lobes in FTD prompted interest selves is unaffected (Lough et al., 2005). The ability to
in the possibility that aspects of social cognition such judge the severity of rule violations (moral versus
as the Theory of Mind (ToM) might be impaired in conventional) is compromised in FTD, with patients
these patients. In a comparison with normal controls, tending to judge all social violations equally severely.
and MMSE-matched AD patients, Gregory et al.
(2002), showed that in FTD both first and second order 28.3.12. Personality measures
ToM was significantly worse than in controls, and that
an even greater proportion of FTD patients were Poor recognition of emotional responses in others, and a
impaired on the faux pas task. On the faux pas task, lack of empathy, are frequently seen in FTD, and yet
patients made a number of errors, with false positive there are relatively few studies which attempt to quan-
and false negative endorsements, as well as inappropri- tify these aspects of impaired social functioning in this
ately inferring that a faux pas had been caused inten- disease. On the interpersonal adjectives scale (IAS), a
tionally. In contrast, performance by patients with AD caregiver-based rating of interpersonal functioning,
was compromised by memory impairment as indicated FTD patients show increased submissiveness, possibly
by their scores on control questions. as a function of increased apathy. Patients with semantic
In an interesting study, Snowden et al. (2003) dementia, by contrast, were rated as more cold-hearted
showed that FTD patients were more likely than con- instead, with only a marginal decrease in social domi-
trols or Huntington’s disease patients to make errors nance (Rankin et al., 2003). Both groups maintained
534 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
their dysfunctional personality styles more rigidly than Keane et al., 2002; Lough et al., 2005). There are, how-
comparable patients with Alzheimer’s disease. A subse- ever, inconsistent results when this type of test is applied
quent study reported a correlation between the volume longitudinally, with AD, but not FTD, patients showing
of right orbitofrontal cortex with ‘agreeableness’ deteriorating performance (Lavenu and Pasquier, 2005).
(Rankin et al., 2004). In two cross-sectional studies, fear recognition was also
Disintegration of the sense of self, as reflected in impaired (Rosen et al., 2004b; Lough et al., 2005). A test
changes in behavior, dress, or religious ideas, was of identifying vocal emotion yielded similar results:
reported in six patients with right-sided frontotemporal angry and sad voices were poorly identified, but there
hypoperfusion on SPECT imaging. This occurred were additional deficits in recognizing the sounds of
despite the patients’ retained knowledge of their own happiness or surprise (Keane et al., 2002).
premorbid personality traits (Miller et al., 2001). Many
other socially inappropriate behaviors have been linked
28.4. Progressive nonfluent aphasia
to right-predominant frontotemporal dysfunction in
FTD patients (Mychack et al., 2001). It is worth bear- 28.4.1. Clinical features
ing in mind, however, that whilst the disease process
in FTD is often asymmetric, it is virtually never unilat- Traditionally, the first level of classification of aphasic
eral (Chan et al., 2001; Thompson et al., 2003; Seeley syndromes has been based on whether or not a patient’s
et al., 2005). speech remains fluent, as this is one of the most salient
Other research groups have attempted to use person- differences between the classical fixed-lesion aphasias
ality indices to quantify aspects of the behavioral of Broca and Wernicke types. More recently, fluency
changes in FTD such as impaired insight. On the same has been used as a marker of the heterogeneity within
interpersonal adjectives questionnaire (IAS) as above, progressive aphasia (Snowden et al., 1989; Hodges
self reports of current personality in FTD actually match and Patterson, 1996; Mesulam, 2001), and indeed this
relatives’ accounts of premorbid personality most clo- observation is useful in distinguishing between SD and
sely (Rankin et al., 2005a). Patients tend to exaggerate PNFA in the clinic. The distinction is not, however, as
their positive qualities, whilst minimizing any negative straightforward as it might seem. One problem, as we
ones, and have worst insight for areas of their personal- shall see, is that SD patients sometimes show dysfluency
ity that have undergone the most significant change. related to their word-finding impairment, and conver-
sely that PNFA patients may at times produce at least
28.3.13. Empathy some stretches of fluent speech. Another is that different
observers use different—and not always explicit—
Two studies have assessed the characteristic indiffer- criteria to define the terms. The description ‘non-fluent’
ence to the emotional concerns of others using the may refer to any or all of abnormally slow speech in
interpersonal reactivity index (IRI), a four factor struc- terms of words per minute, an excessive length of
ture reflecting cognitive (perspective-taking and fan- time between utterances, disordered speech rhythm or
tasy) and emotional (empathic concern and personal melody, effortful articulation, hesitation due to word-
distress) aspects of empathy (Davis, 1980; Lough finding difficulty, or other speech abnormalities.
et al., 2005; Rankin et al., 2005b). In frontal or beha- Though each of these features is useful in describing
vioral variant patients, perspective-taking (Lough an individual patient’s speech, none is reliably present
et al., 2005; Rankin et al., 2005b) and empathic con- in PNFA, and not all are reliably absent in SD. In other
cern (Lough et al., 2005) were impaired relative to words, a description based on fluency alone is neither
age-matched controls. Semantic dementia patients precise nor powerful enough to discriminate between
were impaired across all four factors, reflecting a more the two syndromes.
profound cognitive and emotional indifference to The most frequent initial symptom in PNFA is of
others. Interestingly, patients with AD were not word-finding difficulty, though a substantial minority
impaired on these measures, suggesting a defined of cases do not report this at the first consultation. Other
neural substrate for empathy, and not just brain degen- possible presenting complaints include hesitant speech,
eration per se as the source of its deterioration. difficulty constructing a sentence, or difficulty with
writing (Table 28.3). Behavioral features similar to
28.3.14. Emotional recognition and regulation those of bv-FTD may occur (Kertesz et al., 2003b;
Hodges et al., 2004), sometimes reported as a change
The ability of patients with FTD to recognize facial in personality, but these typically appear later (if at all)
emotional expressions in others is particularly impaired and almost never dominate the clinical picture. The
for anger, sadness, and disgust (Lavenu et al., 1999; onset is gradual, and usually precedes presentation to
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 535

Case Study 28.2


A 75-year-old retired clerk presented to the Memory in spontaneous speech as well as on formal tests of
Clinic with an 18-month history of speech problems. naming, and often resorted to stereotyped phrases such
He complained of hesitancy, word-finding difficulty, as ‘it’s all right.’ His ability to understand complex
and distorted speech. There were no complaints sug- syntax had also deteriorated. He continued to make
gestive of autobiographical or day-to-day memory use of nonverbal means of communication, even
impairment, and he continued to pursue hobbies such learning new gestures, and his semantic and visuospa-
as gardening without difficulty. No personality change tial abilities were still intact. He died seven years after
was reported, and he had insight into his problems. His the onset of his symptoms.
spontaneous speech was poorly articulated, with short A sample of his speech is given in Table 28.2, pre-
phrases and many phonological errors. Word-finding sumed target words are italicized; line breaks indicate
difficulty was obvious and led to frequent pauses; pauses in speech.
speech melody and rhythm were abnormal even on
occasions when there was no particularly marked Table 28.2
word-finding difficulty. He was frustrated by his
Sample of speech in PNFA patient
inability to communicate, and used gesture to supple-
ment speech where possible. er nides (nine days) [holding up nine fingers]
He performed poorly on simple tests of naming, And [points to the window]
phonological processing, and syntactic comprehen- an air oh nd (aeroplane)
sion. His digit span was reduced, as was his fluency have flow and er mornd
in word production from an initial letter cue (P) as well bandelenz (?) and er the
as from a semantic category (animals). He processed when we came out a coach [pointing down]
and took ed us all round
written words accurately in tasks requiring nonverbal
er hohdel (hotel)
output, but he made errors in reading aloud. He
three days and er [holds up three fingers]
showed good recall of both verbal and nonverbal we er coach er two days [holds up two fingers]
material, performed well on a nonverbal semantic and aspleep (asleep) and [holds up five fingers]
task, and his visuospatial abilities were preserved. oat five days
Two years later, his speech had deteriorated uz er uz like [laughs, mimes sleeping]
further, and was now only partially intelligible, with it’s alright [gives ‘thumbs up’ sign]
very frequent phonetic errors occurring in short,
telegraphic utterances. He was profoundly anomic

clinic by two or three years. Most PNFA patients present laconic rather than dysphasic, a pattern known as
in their sixties; the diagnosis is unusual outside the ages ‘dynamic aphasia’ (Costello and Warrington, 1989;
of 50 to 75. No sex bias has been demonstrated (West- Esmonde et al., 1996). Other patients are closer to classi-
bury and Bub, 1997; Kertesz et al., 2003b; Gorno- cal nonfluent aphasia, and show effortful, labored speech
Tempini et al., 2004; Knibb et al., 2005), and there are with distorted rhythm and melody. Still others show
no known risk factors other than age. There is some- largely normal runs of speech in between prolonged
times a family history of early-onset dementia, but only word-finding pauses.
rarely is there a clear-cut Mendelian inheritance pattern. Phonetic paraphasias are a characteristic, though not
The number of words uttered per minute is not always ubiquitous, feature of PNFA. Many patients make spon-
low in PNFA, although the length of individual sentences taneous speech errors which are clearly phonetically
or utterances is almost universally reduced. Some related to the target, for example ‘spoot’ for ‘spoon.’ In
patients fill in the gaps in their online speech using stereo- others, these occur more frequently in naming or repeti-
typed, overlearned phrases. In others, initiating speech tion tasks. Patients are usually aware of their errors, and
becomes more difficult; they prefer to listen rather than may attempt to self-correct, sometimes producing suc-
to speak, and give mostly monosyllabic answers when cessively better approximations (conduit d’approche):
asked a direct question. Such a patient may appear ‘elective, elecrit, electry’ for ‘electricity.’ Patients only
536 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
Table 28.3 whose speech is distorted and nonfluent, should be
assessed for other possible diagnoses, such as bulbar
Clinical features of PNFA (not all present in every case)
motor neuron disease (MND). Handwriting may also
Clinical features be affected nonlinguistically by the co-occurrence of
upper-limb apraxia with PNFA, as in the syndrome of
word-finding difficulty in spontaneous speech corticobasal degeneration (CBD) (see below).
in picture naming tests
speech production difficulty initiating speech
difficulty disordered articulation 28.5. Semantic dementia
phoneme substitution in spontaneous speech
errors on repetition, especially of 28.5.1. Clinical features
nonwords
disordered grammar in spontaneous speech The first complaint in SD is almost always of isolated
in syntactic comprehension word-finding difficulty. However, this is so insidious in
tests onset, and speech is otherwise so well preserved, that
on repetition of complex other symptoms have usually evolved by the time the
sentences patient presents to clinic. Among these are difficulty
nonlinguistic deficits executive impairment understanding spoken words, deterioration in spelling,
upper-limb and/or orofacial and difficulty recognizing faces (Table 28.4). Behavioral
apraxia
features are commoner and occur earlier in SD than in
relatively unimpaired word comprehension
PNFA, and include irritability, stereotyped behaviors,
knowledge of items they cannot
name
imaging left perisylvian atrophy
Table 28.4
Clinical features of SD (most present in most cases)
rarely complain of difficulty understanding speech, but
Clinical features
testing very often uncovers problems in sentence com-
prehension. The deficit is in the understanding of syntax, word-finding difficulty in spontaneous speech
rather than of individual words. For example, the passive in picture naming tests
construction in the sentence ‘A lion was attacked by a unable to give information
tiger’ may be misinterpreted as active voice, causing about the items
the patient to assign the roles of attacker and victim the word comprehension alienation du mot (‘what’s a
wrong way round. Closed-class words such as preposi- hobby?’)
tions may also be misunderstood. By contrast, frank in word-picture matching tests
speech errors overuse of generic words
grammatical errors in speech are a less common feature,
circumlocution
and are less common in PNFA than in nonfluent aphasia
coordinate semantic errors
due to fixed lesions (Graham et al., 2004). They do occur difficulty with uncommon in reading and writing
in some patients, however, and consist of omission or irregular words in verb inflection
misuse of inflections or grammatical words, or abnormal other semantic on picture–picture matching
word order: ‘She’s in the washing, er, doing the wash- impairments tests
ing;’ ‘Well, woman cleaning;’ ‘Cupboard. Plate.’ on sound–picture matching tests
Impairments of reading and writing also occur as part face recognition
of the PNFA syndrome. Surface dyslexia has been behavioral changes bizarre food combinations or
reported (Watt et al., 1997), and may be commoner than food fads
previously thought (Knibb and Hodges, 2005), although stereotyped behaviors
inflexibility, irritability
it is unusual for a PNFA patient to complain of difficulty
loss of empathy
with reading. In fact, reading a passage of text aloud
relatively unimpaired syntactic comprehension
usually elicits speech which is more fluent and less para- speech articulation, phonology,
phasic than when the patient speaks spontaneously, and grammar
although reading text aloud is more difficult and error- imaging striking anterior temporal lobe
prone than reading the same component words as single atrophy
items (Patterson et al., 2005). Grammatical deficits are usually asymmetric, most often
sometimes more apparent in writing than in speech. A left worse than right
patient who can write fluently and grammatically, but
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 537

Case Study 28.3


A 54-year-old care assistant in a nursing home pre- as many animals as possible, she could think of only
sented to the Memory Clinic with a 12-month history five in a minute, but she was better at generating words
of ‘loss of memory for words,’ gradual in onset and from an initial letter. Her conversational comprehen-
progressive. She first noticed a problem when she sion was good, and she could follow complex com-
had difficulty naming the contents of the food trolley mands easily provided that they were composed of
at work. She had also noticed difficulty in remember- common nouns and verbs; but her understanding of
ing the names of friends and family members. There less common individual words was impaired. She
was no problem with day-to-day or autobiographical could read aloud fluently, but regularized the pronun-
memory. Her family reported no behavioral changes, ciation of words with an irregular spelling-to-sound
and she was functioning normally in terms of practical correspondence (e.g., pint pronounced to rhyme with
skills and daily living. Physical neurological examina- ‘mint,’ gauge pronounced ‘gorge,’ etc.). Her nonver-
tion was normal. She was fully orientated, and rapidly bal memory was normal, but word-list learning and
learned her way around the ward where she had been other measures of verbal memory were impaired.
admitted for investigation. Her conversation was flu- With progression of the illness, her speech became
ent and superficially normal, with intact speech almost empty of meaningful words, dominated by
rhythm and melody, accurate articulation, and correct ‘thing,’ ‘do,’ ‘go’ and similar words. Later still, she
pronunciation and grammar. However, her complaint was only able to speak in stereotyped expressions—
of word-finding difficulty was borne out in her ‘special place,’ ‘those bits.’ She developed impair-
speech, which was characterized by extensive circum- ment on nonverbal tests of semantic knowledge, and
locution and a tendency to use generic words such as became unable to demonstrate the use of household
‘thing.’ objects, although her level of function at home
She was severely impaired in naming tasks, and her remained good for a long time. She died 14 years after
errors were semantically related to the target (‘horse’ the onset of her symptoms and had MND-type Ubiqui-
for ‘elephant’). When asked to generate the names of tin pathology.

and changes in eating behavior (Snowden et al., 2001; with a word to the extent of saying, ‘Hobby, hobby. . . I
Thompson et al., 2003). SD and PNFA share a similar should know what a hobby is, but I can’t remember.’
profile in terms of age, sex, and family history (see This has been called alienation du mot (Poeck and Luz-
comments above.) zatti, 1988), and is a very specific sign of SD. Usually,
Spontaneous speech may appear normal to the casual however, comprehension impairment must be specifi-
listener, even at quite advanced stages of the disease. cally elicited, by saying a word to the patient and asking
Early on, however, the trained ear should notice a shift them to point to a picture, or to define the word in as
towards the use of general, high-level category terms much detail as possible. The definitions produced may
when the context requires a specific word, for example be frankly inaccurate, but more typically they are vague
‘place’ instead of ‘hospital’ or ‘creature’ instead of as in ‘a big one, I’ve got one of those,’ or simply
‘goat’; frank within-category semantic errors (‘dog’ overgeneralized and lacking in specific details.
for ‘goat’) may also be seen. Circumlocution is another The progressive loss of vocabulary leads to parallel
common strategy—‘play music with it’ for ‘violin,’ or deficits on tests of word production and word compre-
‘you see them outside, it goes around’ for ‘goat.’ All hension, and errors occur on corresponding items in
of these naming errors become more noticeable on for- each test (Lambon Ralph and Howard, 2000). This also
mal naming tests. Hesitation while searching for a word applies to surface dyslexia, another core feature of the
is less prominent in SD than in PNFA, although the SD syndrome. The pronunciation of many English
anomia is much more profound, and semantic errors words is predictable from their spelling (e.g., ‘cord,’
are typical. ‘loot,’ or ‘mint’), while for others it is not (e.g., ‘word,’
Impairment of single-word comprehension is often ‘foot,’ or ‘pint’). In surface dyslexia, irregular words
equally striking. Occasionally a patient loses familiarity are pronounced as if they had a typical spelling–sound
538 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
relationship. Similarly, although the grammatical struc- common, and it is important in the clinical history to
ture of speech is preserved, over-regularization errors distinguish this from a failure to recognize a familiar
frequently occur when SD patients are asked to trans- person. There seems to be a specific problem with face
form a sentence in present tense into the past: e.g., processing in many SD patients, a progressive proso-
‘Today I fall on the stairs’ ! ‘Yesterday I falled on pagnosia, and this is more common in the right-predo-
the stairs.’ minant variant. We have observed a right-dominant SD
patient emerge from the clinic testing room out into the
28.5.2. Behavioral features waiting room where (as she knew) her husband awaited
her; but she went up to a stranger in the waiting room to
The behavioral and personality changes associated tell him that she was ready to go home.
with bv-FTD are common in SD, at presentation as A recent study found a number of clinical features
well as later in the illness, but the emphasis is different. which were associated with either left- or right-
Impaired social functioning results from a combination predominant cases at the time of first presentation to
of emotional withdrawal, depression, disinhibition, the clinic (Thompson et al., 2003). The features which
apathy, and irritability. Changes in eating behavior, were significantly more common in left-predominant
such as the development of a sweet tooth, are common, SD were both language-related, namely word-finding
but usually there is a restriction of food preferences, or difficulty and impaired comprehension. By contrast,
bizarre food choices, rather than the overeating often right-predominant cases showed a higher prevalence
seen in bv-FTD. Loss of physiological drives is com- of person recognition problems, social awkwardness,
mon and includes poor appetite, weight loss, and and poor insight into their condition. Perhaps because
decreased libido. New religiosity and eccentricity of of this, right-predominant patients were more likely
dress is also reported (Edwards-Lee et al., 1997). The to have lost their jobs before presenting to clinic.
right temporal variant, which has only one-third the
prevalence of left sided cases, seems to be more con-
28.6. Neuropsychological tests in SD and PNFA
vincingly associated with behavioral disturbance than
the left (Edwards-Lee et al., 1997; Perry et al., 2001; 28.6.1. Language
Thompson et al., 2003; Seeley et al., 2005), but cases
28.6.1.1. Naming
are seldom, if ever, purely unilateral. In a recent study,
after an average of three years, the symptoms that were Most aphasic patients, whatever the nature of their syn-
not present initially have generally emerged, whether drome or lesion, show some impairment on tests of pic-
they be behavioral or semantic (Seeley et al., 2005). ture or object naming. This includes both PNFA and
Compulsions are a prominent, but delayed feature, SD patients, but these groups differ in their error pat-
and reflect the predominant temporal lobe involved. terns. PNFA patients obtain mid-range scores, and their
This study suggested that with left-predominant SD, errors are more often phonetic approximations than
visual objects such as coins or buttons are likely to failures of retrieval (Mendez et al., 2003; Weintraub
become the target stimulus, while in the right-sided et al., 1990). They can usually demonstrate knowledge
variant, the focus is on letters, words, and symbols of the items they cannot name (Gorno-Tempini et al.,
(e.g., word puzzles, and writing notes to doctors), 2004), either by giving definitions or properties of the
although we have observed exceptions to these general item, by miming its shape or use, or by pointing cor-
rules. Clockwatching and an intense interest in jigsaws rectly to the item given a particular property (‘which
are very common (Thompson et al., 2002). Compul- one of these has a nautical connection?’). On the other
sions evolve approximately 5 to 7 years after the initial hand, all but the very earliest SD patients score very
symptoms, and are often accompanied by disinhibition, poorly on simple naming tests. Their errors are often
prosopagnosia, and altered food preference. Strong ‘don’t know’ responses; they may also make semanti-
visual compulsions may result in accusations of sho- cally related errors, either at an inappropriately generic
plifting and theft. Lack of empathy is a feature that level (‘animal’ for ‘horse’), or from within the same
appears to be more common as a later feature in dis- category (‘piano’ for ‘harp’) (Snowden et al., 1992;
ease, although it may be seen at presentation if this is Hodges et al., 1995). They have much more limited
delayed. Mental inflexibility can be extreme and pro- knowledge about the items that they fail to name.
voke marked behavioral fluctuations in response to
changes in the immediate environment. 28.6.1.2. Noun generation
Deficits in person recognition frequently occur at Again, both PNFA and SD affect the ability to generate
some stage in the disease (Thompson et al., 2003). words, either from a specified initial letter or from a par-
Inability to retrieve a person’s name is extremely ticular category. Normal subjects can produce an
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 539
average of 18 animal names or 15 words beginning with 28.6.1.5. Reading and writing
‘F’ in a minute, with 10 as a lower limit. In SD, category The surface dyslexia of SD is elicited using reading
fluency drops dramatically, often to just a handful of tests with irregular words, such as the NART (National
words, while letter fluency (at least early in the disease) Adult Reading Test; Nelson and Willison, 1991) or the
is relatively spared—this dissociation is characteristic ‘Surface List’ (Patterson and Hodges, 1992). Other-
of SD, and reflects the underlying impairment of seman- wise, tests of reading and writing are of little specific
tic knowledge. PNFA tends to affect both varieties diagnostic value in these conditions.
of fluency to a similar extent, and the words produced
often continue to show a reasonably wide variation as 28.6.2. Memory in PNFA and SD
in normal speakers; in SD, by contrast, the instances pro-
duced are restricted to the most common, high-frequency From a clinical perspective, there is often a striking dis-
words (e.g., ‘cat,’ ‘dog,’ and ‘horse’ for animals). sociation between language and everyday memory
functions in both PNFA and SD. Indeed, diagnostic cri-
28.6.1.3. Repetition teria for progressive aphasic syndromes have generally
Impairment at repeating spoken words back to the demanded relatively preserved everyday (episodic)
examiner is a reliable feature of PNFA, and errors may memory (Neary et al., 1998). However, demonstrating
be observed here before they become obvious in sponta- this preservation using standard tests is fraught with dif-
neous speech. Long words with complex or repeated ficulty. Understanding test instructions can sometimes
sequences of consonants are particularly difficult (‘epis- be a problem in PNFA as well as in SD, owing to im-
copal,’ ‘hippopotamus’). The phonological structure of pairments of sentence and single-word comprehension
the word is distorted by phoneme omissions, transposi- respectively. More dramatically, tests requiring spoken
tions or insertions. Often the patient will make multiple output are likely to be affected by anomia and other pro-
false starts, coming to a halt in midword each time. duction impairments. For this reason, patients may score
Repetition of long nonwords is even more difficult, as better (relative to control ranges) on tests of recognition
it relies more specifically on phonological working than recall.
memory, which is typically impaired in PNFA. The On a more fundamental level, a specific impairment of
Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition (Gathercole verbal memory is common, particularly in SD. This def-
and Baddeley, 1996) is useful in eliciting subtle deficits. icit affects performance on tests of word-list learning
These problems are also reflected in a low digit span, such as the RAVLT (Rey Auditory–Verbal Learning
and in difficulty with repeating sentences or following Test), as well as recall of a narrative. As the phonological
a three-part command. On the other hand, SD patients and semantic representations of words are disordered in
mostly find word repetition easy, and show a striking PNFA and SD respectively, such a deficit is not surpris-
dissociation between production of a word and ing, and does not necessarily imply an impairment of
knowledge of its meaning. anterograde memory systems. Tests which use exclu-
sively nonverbal material, such as recall of the Rey com-
28.6.1.4. Comprehension tasks plex figure, are more useful in this regard, provided that
Impairments of comprehension of single words and of the patient does not have an impairment of visuospatial
syntax are characteristic of SD and PNFA respectively, processing, which sometimes accompanies PNFA as part
and each may occur in the absence of the other. Word of its overlap with the corticobasal syndrome (Graham
comprehension deficits are tested by word–picture et al., 2003a; Kertesz and Munoz, 2003).
matching, or by asking the patient to define a word, as It is difficult to find a single ‘memory’ test which is
described above. Because the underlying problem in reliably normal in all cases, and so clinical judgment is
SD is a breakdown of conceptual knowledge, item con- crucial: a patient who complains mainly of symptoms
sistency is seen between tests of word production (or of anterograde episodic memory deficits is more likely
retrieval) and comprehension. For the same reason, SD to be suffering from Alzheimer’s disease, but a degree
patients also show impairment on nonverbal tests of of impairment on language-dependent tests without
semantic association, such as the Pyramids and Palm such symptoms does not rule out a diagnosis of PNFA
Trees Test (Howard and Patterson, 1992); a low score or SD.
here is quite specific for SD. Simple syntactic compre-
hension tests are also described above, but for formal 28.6.3. Executive function in PNFA and SD
testing the TROG is particularly suitable, as it uses very
common words which both PNFA and SD patients are Impaired executive function has rarely been discussed
likely to understand in isolation. in the context of progressive aphasia, although it is
540 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
frequently cited as a defining impairment of the bv-FTD blurred the distinct cognitive profiles of these disorders,
group (Neary et al., 1998). By implication, then, it might and presumably contributed in some part to the conflict-
be taken as a relative exclusion criterion for PNFA or ing results. Furthermore, FTLD often exhibits hemi-
SD, but little published work has addressed this issue. spheric asymmetry, and at least one study has
One study (Nestor et al., 2003) found that a group of demonstrated differential performance in left and right
10 PNFA patients (selected on the basis of reduced predominant FTD (Boone et al., 1999).
and distorted speech and impaired syntactic comprehen- Studies with prospective assessment of consecu-
sion) showed impairment on the WCST compared with tively enrolled subjects are rare, but should be encour-
controls, although a group of AD patients were almost as aged. As this is a field that is still dynamic, it is difficult
impaired. Our own experience is that executive impair- to standardize test batteries across studies, and group
ments are commoner in PNFA than in SD, but further sizes are likely to remain small. Pathological confirma-
investigation is needed. tion should be obtained wherever possible to reduce
any bias resulting from clinical diagnosis, and postmor-
28.7. Interpretation of neuropsychological test tem series should aim to recruit widely, and not just
performance patients with classical clinical features (Fig. 28.1). Care
should be taken to avoid circularity whereby inclusion
The interpretation of neuropsychological test scores, criteria are used to discriminate the disease from
either routine or experimental, warrants a degree of others. Lastly, even the pathologists, as final arbiters,
caution. Significant heterogeneity exists in the perfor- have something to learn in this evolving field (Halliday
mance of FTD patients; group effects may not be repre- et al., 2002).
sentative of individual cases. There is also frequent Patients with FTD are often impaired on standard neu-
overlap in performance with either controls or patients ropsychological test batteries relative to controls (Binetti
with other neurodegenerative disorders, particularly et al., 2000). Whilst these tests are sensitive, however,
Alzheimer’s disease. Conflicting results are often they are often not specific in discriminating between
obtained across different studies, and can be ascribed FTD and other neurodegenerative diseases. We now
to a number of methodological issues. discuss what is known about such discrimination.
Matching of patients across disease categories is one
significant issue. The most common method of matching 28.8. Differential diagnosis
uses performance on the Mini-Mental State Examination
(MMSE) to control for differences in disease severity. See Table 28.5 for a summary.
This is not particularly satisfactory as the MMSE does
not weight different cognitive domains equally. Other 28.8.1. Alzheimer’s disease
studies (Kramer et al., 2003; Diehl et al., 2005) have used
Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scores to match Since most studies in FTD compare patients to normal
patients, but as illustrated in one study (Rosen et al., controls or to those who have Alzheimer’s disease,
2004a), this measure emphasizes functional disability, many of the issues relevant to this section have already
which can be inflated by marked behavioral disturbance, been discussed. Age of onset in the FTD syndromes is
and may not truly reflect an equivalent pathological typically a decade earlier than in AD (Hodges et al.,
burden across disease groups. Failure to distinguish 2003) although this may reflect an ascertainment bias
FTD from the language variants (PNFA and SD) has with many studies using a cutoff of 65 or 70 years.

Fig. 28.1. Imaging findings associated with frontotemporal dementia syndromes (A) behavioral variant FTD with marked fron-
tal atrophy and ventricular enlargement; (B) progressive nonfluent aphasia showing left perisylvian atrophy; and (C) semantic
dementia with anterior temporal lobe pathology, worse on the left. The images are shown in radiological convention with the
left side of the patient on the right side of the image.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 541
Table 28.5 Focal variants of AD with marked dysexecutive fea-
tures have been described (Johnson et al., 1999), but
Other diagnoses to consider in suspected FTD
these patients have a coexisting typical AD profile, with
Category Diseases memory and visuospatial impairment. In our Cambridge
experience, non-AD patients with this pattern typically
Neurodegenerative Alzheimer’s disease evolve into other dementia syndromes such as progres-
Corticobasal degeneration sive supranuclear palsy (PSP), or have vascular demen-
Progressive supranuclear palsy tia. Similarly, patients with proven FTD pathology have
FTD with motor neuron disease occasionally been shown to have the anterograde mem-
Vascular dementia
ory disturbance usually associated with Alzheimer’s
Dementia with Lewy bodies
(Caine et al., 2001; Hodges et al., 2004; Graham et al.,
Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease
Huntington’s disease 2005). In practice, diagnostic confusion is less common
Psychiatric Adult Asperger’s syndrome than might be imagined, as in bv-FTD it is the beha-
Affective disorders vioral disturbance that dominates the clinical picture.
Schizophrenia An inventory such as the CBI readily distinguishes the
Miscellaneous Frontal tumor symptom complexes that are typical of these disorders,
Hypothyroidism and highlights the memory disturbance in cases that
Vasculitis might otherwise be diagnosed as bv-FTD.
Vitamin deficiencies AD pathology occasionally causes PNFA. This may
be indistinguishable from the syndrome caused by
FTD-spectrum pathologies (Pogacar and Williams,
1984; Green et al., 1990; Kempler et al., 1990; Benson
The very high prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease over and Zaias, 1991; Karbe et al., 1993; Galton et al.,
the age of 75 is likely to obscure the detection of 2000; Li et al., 2000; Clark et al., 2003; Godbolt et al.,
FTD in patients presenting in this age group. Because 2004), at least on strictly linguistic criteria. A more fre-
AD pathology is very common in the elderly popula- quent presentation, however, is of prominent phonologi-
tion, atypical clinical presentations are seen relatively cal impairments and/or dysfluency in the context of
frequently, especially in specialist clinics. episodic memory impairment or other cognitive defi-
Personality change, unconcern, and socially inap- cits, and AD may be diagnosed confidently in this situa-
propriate behavior typically distinguish FTD from the tion. Care must be exercised in interpreting the results of
anterograde memory disturbance and topographical tests of verbal memory, tests with complicated instruc-
disorientation characteristic of Alzheimer’s disease. tions, or tests which rely on spoken or written answers,
Stereotypic behavior, changes in eating preference or as language impairment may cause falsely low scores
hyperorality and emotional blunting also reliably separate and suggest global impairment in a purely aphasic
the groups (Bozeat et al., 2000; Rosen et al., 2002a). patient. Even when the language syndrome is relatively
Failure to assess behavioral characteristics results in the pure, AD pathology is very likely when the onset occurs
incorrect classification of many FTLD subjects as AD. at over 70 years of age.
When the presence of social conduct disorder, hyperoral- Similar comments apply to SD, except that it is
ity and akinesia are combined with absence of amnesia unusual for AD pathology to mimic SD precisely. Occa-
and perceptual disturbance, up to 93% of the FTLD and sionally it may produce a forme fruste of SD (a fluent
97% of the AD patients are correctly classified (Rosen aphasia without the usual attendant features). Semantic
et al., 2002b). Although apathy is more frequent in memory impairment is a common component of a glo-
FTD, it is not a particularly helpful discriminating feature. bal AD syndrome, but this is usually easily distinguished
Abnormal perception and praxis are more discrimi- from pure SD by the predominance of episodic memory
natory for AD than amnesic symptoms. Impaired spatial impairment and other deficits. In a recent pathologi-
perception with spatial disorientation, inability to loca- cally confirmed series of 18 patients, only 2 patients
lize objects, or failure on spatial tasks such as tracking, who were clinically diagnosed with SD were eventually
copying line drawings or counting dots, dramatically shown to have AD (Davies et al., 2005a).
increases the probability of AD in an individual patient
(Varma et al., 1999). Although there is substantial over- 28.8.2. Corticobasal degeneration
lap in the performance of FTD and AD patients, assess-
ment of the type of errors made (i.e., perseveration, Corticobasal degeneration (CBD) was originally
or concrete interpretation of sentences) may help described as an atypical Parkinsonian syndrome, with
distinguish the two groups (Thompson et al., 2005). limb apraxia, myoclonus, cortical sensory loss, and
542 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
the alien limb phenomenon (Rebeiz et al., 1968). More comprehension. Bulbar features, including dysarthria,
recently, language impairment has been recognized as are also common, and may hamper assessment of lin-
part of the syndrome, and impairments in phonological guistic function. These deficits may precede the onset
processing have been demonstrated in patients with the of the motor features, but it is usual for the PSP syn-
clinical features of CBD but no symptomatic aphasia drome to appear later in the illness. The possibility of
(Graham et al., 2003b). The term ‘CBD’ refers to a PSP should be considered in a patient presenting with
set of pathological findings as well as a clinical pattern; a progressive dynamic aphasia, and the physical signs
this association has largely stood the test of time, but a should be sought both at presentation and at follow-up.
number of patients who were diagnosed with unequivo- Apathy and disinhibition are prominent in PSP
cal PNFA in life have had CBD pathology at postmor- (Aarsland et al., 2001), which is in keeping with the
tem (Kertesz and Munoz, 2002; Ferrer et al., 2003). medial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neuronal loss
Kertesz et al. (2000a) highlighted the frequency of seen in this disease. However, the picture is not the
reports of behavioral disturbance in the case reports of florid disruption in social functioning that is seen more
CBD. This consists primarily of apathy, disinhibition, typically in FTD. In a recent analysis (Bak, unpub-
perseveration, and inattention, although as usual these lished data), there was no difference in the summed
deficits are not present in every subject. On the Neuro- behavioral scores on the Cambridge Behavioural
psychiatric Inventory (NPI), depression was the only Inventory (CBI) across different stages of disease in
feature that was endorsed more frequently in CBD than PSP. This implies that it is not simply the motor dys-
in FTD (Cummings and Litvan, 2000), although apathy function that leads to a lower reporting of behavioral
(40%), irritability (20%), anxiety (15%), and disinhibi- disruption in PSP. Conversely, a recent report demon-
tion (15%) were not uncommon. How these features strated FTD pathology with ubiquitin inclusions in
relate to underlying depression is unclear, but it is likely three cases with a distinct PSP phenotype (Paviour
to be a significant factor in their presence. Depression is et al., 2004). Extrapyramidal features dominated the
fairly uncommon in bv-FTD, as mentioned earlier. A clinical picture, but one patient had significant disinhi-
further study suggests that behavioral features are bition, another presented in his mid-seventies, and a
marked in FTD from the start of the illness, and their fre- third developed fasciculations suggestive of motor
quency does not alter significantly, whereas in CBD, neuron disease.
they accumulate gradually (Marczinski et al., 2004).
A patient with CBD may show the classical motor 28.8.4. Frontotemporal dementia with motor
features, language features, behavioral disturbance, or neuron disease (FTD-MND)
all three (Graham et al., 2003a). Deficits may also
develop sequentially. A diagnosis of CBD should there- Frontotemporal dementia with motor neuron disease
fore be considered in a case of suspected PNFA or bv- (FTD-MND) usually presents in the sixth decade, and
FTD, especially in the presence of visuospatial impair- is more common in men. Behavioral and cognitive
ments or apraxia (in copying either a geometric design changes invariably precede motor symptoms by 6 to
or a meaningless hand position). The aphasia is likely 12 months. Bulbar features including dysphagia and
to affect phonology predominantly, and naming may dysarthria are particularly common. The FTD-MND
be relatively spared. syndrome is unusual for the fact that delusions and
indeed hallucinations have been described (Bak and
28.8.3. Progressive supranuclear palsy Hodges, 2001). These phenomena are not part of the
usual FTD spectrum, but the remainder of the beha-
The classical features of progressive supranuclear palsy vioral and cognitive features are indistinguishable from
(PSP) are disruptions of motor function, including axial classical FTD, with disinhibition being particularly
rigidity, postural instability with a tendency to fall back- common (Neary et al., 2000).
wards, bradykinesia, and a supranuclear vertical gaze Progressive aphasia is a common feature of this
palsy (Richardson et al., 1963). However, language syndrome (Bak et al., 2001). The aphasia may appear
may also be affected. Full-blown PNFA with paraphasia before the onset of motor features, and is usually a typical
and dysgrammatism has been reported (Mochizuki PNFA. Semantic deficits appear to be less frequent (Bak
et al., 2003), and speech apraxia may also occur (Sakai and Hodges, 2001; 2004). Progression is rapid (Bak and
et al., 2002), but the most frequent language impairment Hodges, 2004), and prognosis is poor—the median
is difficulty in initiating output. This manifests as survival of FTD-MND patients in a recent series was just
‘dynamic aphasia’ (Esmonde et al., 1996), with a 2 years from symptom onset (Hodges et al., 2003). If
marked and progressive reduction in speech output and physical signs of MND are not present at the first presen-
shortened phrases, but otherwise normal speech and tation to clinic, they usually appear within a year.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 543
28.8.5. Vascular dementia particularly rapid deterioration should prompt concern
about prion disease. A few cases of Creutzfeldt–Jakob
Although one might expect vascular dementia affecting disease presenting with progressive aphasia have been
subcortical regions to affect the frontal lobes in a simi- reported (Shuttleworth et al., 1985; Yamanouchi et al.,
lar manner to frontotemporal dementia, this does not 1986; Mandell et al., 1989), though the association is
appear to be the case. In fact, the behavioral symptoms controversial (Turner et al., 1996). Other cognitive and
that distinguish FTD from vascular dementia are fairly physical features developed within weeks or months of
similar to those that distinguish the disorder from AD onset in these cases, and the progression of the aphasia
(Snowden et al., 2001). Vascular risk factors, stepwise was also very rapid. Huntington’s disease is occasion-
decline and the presence of ischemic lesions are char- ally suggested by an appropriate family history. Irrit-
acteristic of vascular dementia (Roman et al., 1993). ability is common in the early stages, and early
It has never been reported to cause progressive aphasia. cognitive changes include impaired attention and
impaired executive dysfunction (Ho et al., 2003).
28.8.6. Dementia with Lewy bodies
28.11. Conclusion
The presence of delusions, hallucinations, and extra-
pyramidal features are uncommon in bv-FTD unless it Symptoms in frontotemporal lobar dementia are related
is associated with motor neuron disease. Thus, it is sel- to the underlying structures involved in the pathological
dom that DLB provokes diagnostic confusion. Simi- process, and not the nature of the pathology itself. There
larly, the presence of visuospatial impairment in FTD are reliable symptom clusters which define syndromes,
would make one reconsider the diagnosis. There is a but there are several language and behavioral features
single case report of progressive aphasia with cortical that show significant overlap between the different var-
Lewy body disease, but this case also had dysarthria iants of FTLD. Future work needs to continue to deline-
and developed rapidly into an MND syndrome. Even ate the relevant neural substrates of the disturbed
as part of a broader syndrome, it is unusual for cognition in these patients. As the nature of the disease
language to be impaired in these disorders. becomes clearer, it is likely that the disease will be
recognized in more patients, particularly those over the
28.9. Psychiatric diagnoses age of 65 who have previously been given a diagnosis
of Alzheimer’s disease. Despite substantial progress in
Patients with bv-FTD frequently arrive via the psychia- delineating the features of this disease, there remains
trist’s office, but when they do not, the effects of mood no effective treatment, either symptomatic or curative.
disturbance, either mania or depression, should be con-
sidered. The older patient who presents with a new mood Acknowledgements
disorder in the absence of something similar in earlier
years should arouse more suspicion than someone who Dr. C. Kipps is supported by the Wellcome Trust
has been chronically depressed for years. Similarly, schi- (Grant No. 073580) and Prof. J. Hodges by the UK
zophrenic presentations tend to be in a younger age-group Medical Research Council.
than the 50–60 year olds most commonly affected by
FTD syndromes. In taking the history, it is vital to make References
every attempt to verify that there is a change in function- Aarsland D, Litvan I, Larsen JP (2001). Neuropsychiatric
ing with regards to behavior. Imaging may be of use, symptoms of patients with progressive supranuclear palsy
although in bv-FTD there may be little or no abnormality and Parkinson’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci
of structural scans many years into the disease (Davies 13: 42–49.
et al., 2005b). This is not the case with SD or PNFA Bak TH, Hodges JR (2001). Motor neurone disease, dementia
however. One should also consider the possibility of adult and aphasia: Coincidence, co-occurrence or continuum? J
Asperger’s syndrome and be aware of the potential for Neurol 248: 260–270.
Bak TH, Hodges JR (2004). The effects of motor neurone dis-
dysfunctional marital relationships to paint a colorful
ease on language: Further evidence. Brain Lang 89:
picture of behavioral disruption.
354–361.
Bak TH, O’Donovan DG, Xuereb JH, et al. (2001). Selective
28.10. Other differential diagnoses impairment of verb processing associated with pathologi-
cal changes in Brodmann areas 44 and 45 in the motor neu-
Hypothyroidism, vasculitis, and vitamin deficiencies, rone disease–dementia–aphasia syndrome. Brain 124 :
though unlikely, should be considered. Structural 103–120.
lesions such as a frontal lobe tumor could in principle Barber R, Snowden JS, Craufurd D (1995). Frontotemporal
present as a progressive Broca’s aphasia, and dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: Retrospective
544 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
differentiation using information from informants. J Neu- contribution of standard neuropsychological tests to differen-
rol Neurosurg Psychiatry 59: 61–70. tial diagnosis. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 18: 39–44.
Benson DF, Zaias BW (1991). Progressive aphasia: A case with Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, et al. (2000). The FAB:
postmortem correlation. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol A Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 55:
Behav Neurol 4: 215–223. 1621–1626.
Binetti G, Locascio JJ, Corkin S, et al. (2000). Differences Duncan J, Owen AM (2000). Common regions of the human
between Pick disease and Alzheimer disease in clinical frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands.
appearance and rate of cognitive decline. Arch Neurol Trends Neurosci 23: 475–483.
57: 225–232. Edwards-Lee T, Miller BL, Benson DF, et al. (1997). The tem-
Bishop DVM (1983). The Test for Reception of Grammar poral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 120:
Age and Cognitive Performance Research Centre. Univer- 1027–1040.
sity of Manchester, Manchester. Elfgren C, Passant U, Risberg J (1993). Neuropsychological
Boone KB, Miller BL, Lee A, et al. (1999). Neuropsychologi- findings in frontal lobe dementia. Dementia 4: 214–219.
cal patterns in right versus left frontotemporal dementia. Esmonde T, Giles E, Xuereb J, et al. (1996). Progressive
J Internat Neuropsychol Soc 5: 616–622. supranuclear palsy presenting with dynamic aphasia.
Bozeat S, Gregory CA, Lambon Ralph MA, et al. (2000). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 60: 403–410.
Which neuropsychiatric and behavioural features distin- Ferrer I, Hernandez I, Boada M, et al. (2003). Primary pro-
guish frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal gressive aphasia as the initial manifestation of corticobasal
dementia from Alzheimer’s disease? J Neurol Neurosurg degeneration and unusual tauopathies. Acta Neuropathol
Psychiatry 69: 178–186. (Berl) 106: 419–435.
Broe M, Hodges JR, Schofield E, et al. (2003). Staging dis- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975). Mini-mental
ease severity in pathologically confirmed cases of fronto- state: A practical method for grading the mental state of
temporal dementia. Neurology 60: 1005–1011. patients for clinicians. J Psychiatr Res 12: 189–198.
Brun A, Englund B, Gustafson L, et al. (1994). Clinical and Frisoni GB, Pizzolato G, Geroldi C, et al. (1995). Dementia
neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. of the frontal type: Neuropsychological and [99Tc]-HM-
The Lund and Manchester Groups. J Neurol Neurosurg PAO SPET features. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 8:
Psychiatry 57: 416–418. 42–48.
Caine D, Patterson K, Hodges JR, et al. (2001). Severe ante- Galton CJ, Patterson K, Xuereb JH, et al. (2000). Atypical
rograde amnesia with extensive hippocampal degeneration and typical presentations of Alzheimer’s disease: A clini-
in a case of rapidly progressive frontotemporal dementia. cal, neuropsychological, neuroimaging and pathological
Neurocase 7: 57–64. study of 13 cases. Brain 123: 484–498.
Cappa SF, Binetti G, Pezzini A, et al. (1998). Object and Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD (1996). The Children’s Test of
action naming in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal Nonword Repetition. Psychological Test Corporation, Har-
dementia [see comment]. Neurology 50: 351–355. court Brace.
Chan D, Fox NC, Scahill RI, et al. (2001). Patterns of tem- Glosser G, Gallo JL, Clark CM, et al. (2002). Memory encod-
poral lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s ing and retrieval in frontotemporal dementia and Alzhei-
disease. Ann Neurol 49: 433–442. mer’s disease. Neuropsychology 16: 190–196.
Clark DG, Mendez MF, Farag E, et al. (2003). Clinicopatholo- Godbolt AK, Beck JA, Collinge J, et al. (2004). A presenilin
gic case report: Progressive aphasia in a 77-year-old man. 1 R278I mutation presenting with language impairment.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 15: 231–238. Neurology 63: 1702–1704.
Costello AL, Warrington EK (1989). Dynamic aphasia: The Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, et al. (2004).
selective impairment of verbal planning. Cortex 25: Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary pro-
103–114. gressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 55: 335–346.
Cummings JL, Litvan I (2000). Neuropsychiatric aspects of Graham A, Davies R, Xuereb J, et al. (2005). Pathologically
corticobasal degeneration. Adv Neurol 82: 147–152. proven frontotemporal dementia presenting with severe
Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al. (1994). The Neuropsy- amnesia. Brain 128: 597–605.
chiatric Inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psycho- Graham NL, Bak TH, Hodges JR (2003a). Corticobasal
pathology in dementia. Neurology 44: 2308–2314. degeneration as a cognitive disorder. Mov Disord 18:
Davies RR, Hodges JR, Kril JJ, et al. (2005a). The pathologi- 1224–1232.
cal basis of semantic dementia. Brain 128(9): 1984–1985. Graham NL, Bak T, Patterson K, et al. (2003b). Language
Davies RR, Kipps CM, Mitchell J, et al. (2005b). Progression function and dysfunction in corticobasal degeneration.
in frontotemporal dementia: identifying a benign beha- Neurology 61: 493–499.
vioural variant on MRI. Arch Neurol 63: 1627–31. Graham N, Patterson K, Hodges J (2004). When more yields
Davis MH (1980). A multidimensional approach to indivi- less: Speaking and writing deficits in nonfluent progressive
dual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected aphasia. Neurocase 10: 141–155.
Documents in Psychology 10. Green J, Morris JC, Sandson J, et al. (1990). Progressive
Diehl J, Monsch AU, Aebi C, et al. (2005). Frontotemporal aphasia: A precursor of global dementia? Neurology 40:
dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease: The 423–429.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 545
Gregory CA (1999). Frontal variant of frontotemporal Karbe H, Kertesz A, Polk M (1993). Profiles of language
dementia: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study of neu- impairment in primary progressive aphasia. Arch Neurol
ropsychiatric features. Psychol Med 29: 1205–1217. 50: 193–201.
Gregory CA, Hodges JR (1996). Frontotemporal Dementia: Keane J, Calder AJ, Hodges JR, et al. (2002). Face and emo-
Use of consensus criteria and prevalence of psychiatric fea- tion processing in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia.
tures. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 9: Neuropsychologia 40: 655–665.
145–153. Kempler D, Metter EJ, Riege WH, et al. (1990). Slowly progres-
Gregory C, Lough S, Stone V, et al. (2002). Theory of mind sive aphasia: Three cases with language, memory, CT and
in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia PET data. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 53: 987–993.
and Alzheimer’s disease: Theoretical and practical impli- Kertesz A, Davidson W, Fox H (1997). Frontal behavioral
cations. Brain 125: 752–764. inventory: Diagnostic criteria for frontal lobe dementia.
Gregory CA, McKenna PJ, Hodges JR (1998). Dementia of Can J Neurol Sci 24: 29–36.
frontal type and simple schizophrenia: Two sides of the Kertesz A, Davidson W, McCabe P, et al. (2003a). Behavioral
same coin? Neurocase 4: 1–6. quantitation is more sensitive than cognitive testing in
Gregory CA, Orrell M, Sahakian B, et al. (1997). Can fronto- frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord
temporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease be differentiated 17: 223–229.
using a brief battery of tests? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12: Kertesz A, Davidson W, McCabe P, et al. (2003b). Primary
375–383. progressive aphasia: Diagnosis, varieties, evolution. J Int
Halliday G, Ng T, Rodriguez M, et al. (2002). Consensus Neuropsychol Soc 9: 710–719.
neuropathological diagnosis of common dementia syn- Kertesz A, Martinez-Lage P, Davidson W, et al. (2000a). The
dromes: Testing and standardising the use of multiple diag- corticobasal degeneration syndrome overlaps progressive
nostic criteria. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 104: 72–78. aphasia and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55:
Ho AK, Sahakian BJ, Brown RG, et al. (2003). Profile of cog- 1368–1375.
nitive progression in early Huntington’s disease. Neurol- Kertesz A, Munoz DG (2002). Primary progressive aphasia:
ogy 61: 1702–1706. A review of the neurobiology of a common presentation
Hodges JR, Davies R, Xuereb J, et al. (2003). Survival in of Pick complex. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 17:
frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 61: 349–354. 30–36.
Hodges JR, Davies RR, Xuereb JH, et al. (2004). Clinico- Kertesz A, Munoz DG (2003). Primary progressive aphasia
pathological correlates in frontotemporal dementia. Ann and Pick complex. J Neurol Sci 206: 97–107.
Neurol 56: 399–406. Kertesz A, Nadkarni N, Davidson W, et al. (2000b). The
Hodges JR, Graham N, Patterson K (1995). Charting the pro- Frontal Behavioral Inventory in the differential diagnosis
gression in semantic dementia: Implications for the organisa- of frontotemporal dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 6:
tion of semantic memory. Memory 3: 463–495. 460–468.
Hodges JR, Miller B (2001). The neuropsychology of frontal Knibb JA, Hodges JR (2005). Semantic dementia and pri-
variant frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. mary progressive aphasia: A problem of categorisation?
Introduction to the special topic papers: Part II. Neurocase Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 19 (Suppl1): S7–S14.
7: 113–121. Knibb JA, Xuereb JH, Patterson K, et al. (2005). Clinical and
Hodges JR, Patterson K (1996). Nonfluent progressive apha- pathological characterisation of progressive aphasia. Ann
sia and semantic dementia: A comparative neuropsycholo- Neurol 59(1): 156–165.
gical study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2: 511–524. Kramer JH, Jurik J, Sha SJ, et al. (2003). Distinctive neuro-
Hodges JR, Patterson K, Ward R, et al. (1999). The diffe- psychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, seman-
rentiation of semantic dementia and frontal lobe dementia tic dementia, and Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol
(temporal and frontal variants of frontotemporal 16: 211–218.
dementia) from early Alzheimer’s disease: A comparative Kril JJ, Halliday GM (2004). Clinicopathological staging of
neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology 13: 31–40. frontotemporal dementia severity: Correlation with regio-
Hornak J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, et al. (2003). Changes in emo- nal atrophy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 311–315.
tion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal Lambon Ralph MA, Howard D (2000). Gogi aphasia or
and cingulate cortices. Brain 126: 1691–1712. semantic dementia? Simulating and assessing poor verbal
Howard D, Patterson K (1992). Pyramids and Palm Trees: A comprehension in a case of progressive fluent aphasia.
Test of Semantic Access from Pictures and Words. Thames Cogn Neuropsychol 17: 437–465.
Valley Test Company, Bury St. Edmunds, Suffolk. Lavenu I, Pasquier F (2005). Perception of emotion on faces
Johnson JK, Diehl J, Mendez MF, et al. (2005). Frontotem- in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: A
poral lobar degeneration: Demographic characteristics of longitudinal study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 19:
353 patients. Arch Neurol 62: 925–930. 37–41.
Johnson JK, Head E, Kim R, et al. (1999). Clinical and patho- Lavenu I, Pasquier F, Lebert F, et al. (1999). Perception of
logical evidence for a frontal variant of Alzheimer disease. emotion in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer dis-
Arch Neurol 56: 1233–1239. ease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 13: 96–101.
546 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
Lebert F, Pasquier F, Souliez L, et al. (1998). Frontotemporal behavior in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 56:
behavioral scale. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 12: S11–S15.
335–339. Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. (1998). Frontotem-
Levy ML, Miller BL, Cummings JL, et al. (1996). Alzheimer poral lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnos-
disease and frontotemporal dementias. Behavioral distinc- tic criteria. Neurology 51: 1546–1554.
tions. Arch Neurol 53: 687–690. Neary D, Snowden JS, Mann DM (2000). Cognitive change
Li F, Iseki E, Kato M, et al. (2000). An autopsy case of Alz- in motor neurone disease/amyotrophic lateral sclerosis
heimer’s disease presenting with primary progressive (MND/ALS). J Neurol Sci 180: 15–20.
aphasia: A clinicopathological and immunohistochemical Nelson HE, Willison J (1991). National Adult Reading Test,
study. Neuropathology 20: 239–245. 2nd edn. NFER-Nelson Publishing Co. Ltd.
Lindau M, Almkvist O, Kushi J, et al. (2000). First Nestor PJ, Graham NL, Fryer TD, et al. (2003). Progressive
symptoms—frontotemporal dementia versus Alzheimer’s non-fluent aphasia is associated with hypometabolism
disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 11: 286–293. centred on the left anterior insula. Brain 126: 2406–2418.
Lough S, Kipps C, Triess C, et al. (2005). Social reasoning, Nyatsanza S, Shetty T, Gregory C, et al. (2003). A study of
emotion and empathy in frontal variant frontotemporal stereotypic behaviours in Alzheimer’s disease and frontal
dementia. Neuropsychologia 44: 950–958. and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol
Lund and Manchester Groups (1994). Clinical and neuro- Neurosurg Psychiatry 74: 1398–1402.
pathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Pachana NA, Boone KB, Miller BL, et al. (1996). Comparison
Neurosurg Psychiatry 57: 416–418. of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer’s disease
Mandell AM, Alexander MP, Carpenter S (1989). Creutz- and frontotemporal dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2:
feldt–Jakob disease presenting as isolated aphasia. Neurol- 505–510.
ogy 39: 55–58. Pasquier F, Grymonprez L, Lebert F, et al. (2001). Memory
Marczinski CA, Davidson W, Kertesz A (2004). A longitudi- impairment differs in frontotemporal dementia and Alzhei-
nal study of behavior in frontotemporal dementia and pri- mer’s disease. Neurocase 7: 161–171.
mary progressive aphasia. Cogn Behav Neurol 17: Patterson K, Graham N, Lambon Ralph MA, et al. (2005).
185–190. Progressive non-fluent aphasia is not a progressive form
Mathuranath PS, Nestor PJ, Berrios GE, et al. (2000). A brief of non-fluent (post-stroke) aphasia. Aphasiology 20:
cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s disease 1018–1034.
and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55: 1613–1620. Patterson K, Hodges JR (1992). Deterioration of word mean-
McKhann GM, Albert MS, Grossman M, et al. (2001). Clini- ing: Implications for reading. Neuropsychologia 30:
cal and pathological diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia: 1025–1040.
Report of the Work Group on Frontotemporal Dementia Paviour DC, Lees AJ, Josephs KA, et al. (2004). Frontotem-
and Pick’s Disease. Arch Neurol 58: 1803–1809. poral lobar degeneration with ubiquitin-only-immunoreac-
Mendez MF, Clark DG, Shapira JS, et al. (2003). Speech tive neuronal changes: Broadening the clinical picture to
and language in progressive nonfluent aphasia compared include progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain 127:
with early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 61: 2441–2451.
1108–1113. Perri R, Koch G, Carlesimo GA, et al. (2005). Alzheimer’s
Mendez MF, Perryman KM (2002). Neuropsychiatric fea- disease and frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia.
tures of frontotemporal dementia: Evaluation of consensus A very brief battery for cognitive and behavioural
criteria and review. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 14: distinction. J Neurol 252: 1238–1244.
424–429. Perry RJ, Hodges JR (2000). Differentiating frontal and tem-
Mesulam MM (2001). Primary progressive aphasia. Ann poral variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s
Neurol 49: 425–432. disease. Neurology 54: 2277–2284.
Miller BL, Cummings JL, Villanueva-Meyer J, et al. (1991). Perry RJ, Rosen HR, Kramer JH, et al. (2001). Hemispheric
Frontal lobe degeneration: Clinical, neuropsychological, dominance for emotions, empathy and social behaviour:
and SPECT characteristics. Neurology 41: 1374–1382. Evidence from right and left handers with frontotemporal
Miller BL, Darby AL, Swartz JR, et al. (1995). Dietary dementia. Neurocase 7: 145–160.
changes, compulsions and sexual behavior in frontotem- Pick A (1892). Über die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatro-
poral degeneration. Dementia 6: 195–199. phie zur Aphasie. Prager Medizinische Wochenschrift 17
Miller BL, Seeley WW, Mychack P, et al. (2001). Neuroanat- [translated in Berrios and Girling (1994). Hist Psych 5:
omy of the self: Evidence from patients with frontotem- 542–547.
poral dementia. Neurology 57: 817–821. Pick A (1901). Senile Hirnatrophie als Grundlage von
Mochizuki A, Ueda Y, Komatsuzaki Y, et al. (2003). Progres- Herderescheinungen. Wien Klin Wochenschr 14 [trans-
sive supranuclear palsy presenting with primary progres- lated in Girling and Marková (1995). Hist Psych 6:
sive aphasia—clinicopathological report of an autopsy 533–537.
case. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 105: 610–614. Pick A (1904). Zur Symptomatologie der linksseitigen Schlä-
Mychack P, Kramer JH, Boone KB, et al. (2001). The influ- fenlappenatrophie. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neu-
ence of right frontotemporal dysfunction on social rologie 16: 378–388.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA 547
Pijnenburg YA, Gillissen F, Jonker C, et al. (2004). Initial Rosen HJ, Narvaez JM, Hallam B, et al. (2004a). Neuro-
complaints in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Dement psychological and functional measures of severity in Alz-
Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 302–306. heimer disease, frontotemporal dementia, and semantic
Poeck K, Luzzatti C (1988). Slowly progressive aphasia in dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 18: 202–207.
three patients. The problem of accompanying neuropsy- Rosen HJ, Pace-Savitsky K, Perry RJ, et al. (2004b). Recog-
chological deficit. Brain 111: 151–168. nition of emotion in the frontal and temporal variants of
Pogacar S, Williams RS (1984). Alzheimer’s disease present- frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
ing as slowly progressive aphasia. R I Med J 67: 181–185. 17: 277–281.
Rahman S, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (1999a). Comparative Sakai K, Furui E, Komai K, et al. (2002). [Acquired stuttering
cognitive neuropsychological studies of frontal lobe func- as an early symptom in a patient with progressive supra-
tion: Implications for therapeutic strategies in frontal var- nuclear palsy]. Rinsho Shinkeigaku 42: 178–180.
iant frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Seeley WW, Bauer AM, Miller BL, et al. (2005). The natural
Disord 10: 15–28. history of temporal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neu-
Rahman S, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, et al. (1999b). Specific rology 64: 1384–1390.
cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal Shigenobu K, Ikeda M, Fukuhara R, et al. (2002). The Stereo-
dementia. Brain 122: 1469–1493. typy Rating Inventory for frontotemporal lobar degenera-
Rankin KP, Baldwin E, Pace-Savitsky C, et al. (2005a). Self tion. Psychiatry Res 110: 175–187.
awareness and personality change in dementia. J Neurol Shuttleworth EC, Yates AJ, Paltan-Ortiz JD (1985). Creutz-
Neurosurg Psychiatry 76: 632–639. feldt–Jakob disease presenting as progressive aphasia. J
Rankin KP, Kramer JH, Miller BL (2005b). Patterns of cog- Natl Med Assoc 77: 649–50, 652, 655.
nitive and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar Simons JS, Verfaellie M, Galton CJ, et al. (2002). Recollec-
degeneration. Cogn Behav Neurol 18: 28–36. tion-based memory in frontotemporal dementia: Implica-
Rankin KP, Kramer JH, Mychack P, et al. (2003). Double dis- tions for theories of long-term memory. Brain 125:
sociation of social functioning in frontotemporal dementia. 2523–2536.
Neurology 60: 266–271. Slachevsky A, Villalpando JM, Sarazin M, et al. (2004). Fron-
Rankin KP, Rosen HJ, Kramer JH, et al. (2004). Right and tal assessment battery and differential diagnosis of fronto-
left medial orbitofrontal volumes show an opposite rela- temporal dementia and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol
tionship to agreeableness in FTD. Dement Geriatr Cogn 61: 1104–1107.
Disord 17: 328–332. Snowden JS, Bathgate D, Varma A, et al. (2001). Distinct
Rebeiz JJ, Kolodny EH, Richardson EP Jr (1968). Corticoden- behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and
tatonigral degeneration with neuronal achromasia. Arch semantic dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 70:
Neurol 18: 20–33. 323–332.
Rhee J, Antiquena P, Grossman M (2001). Verb comprehen- Snowden JS, Gibbons ZC, Blackshaw A, et al. (2003). Social
sion in frontotemporal degeneration: The role of gramma- cognition in frontotemporal dementia and Huntington’s
tical, semantic and executive components. Neurocase 7: disease. Neuropsychologia 41: 688–701.
173–184. Snowden JS, Goulding PJ, Neary D (1989). Semantic demen-
Richardson JC, Steele J, Olszewski J (1963). Supranuclear tia: A form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behav Neu-
ophthalmoplegia, pseudobulbar palsy, nuchal dystonia rol 2: 167–182.
and dementia. A clinical report on eight cases of heteroge- Snowden JS, Griffiths HL, Neary D (1996). Progressive lan-
nous system degeneration. Trans Am Neurol Assoc 88: guage disorder associated with frontal lobe degeneration.
25–29. Neurocase 2: 429–440.
Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, et al. (1999). Dissoci- Snowden JS, Neary D, Mann DM, et al. (1992). Progressive
able deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic language disorder due to lobar atrophy. Ann Neurol 31:
amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal 174–183.
damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted nor- Thompson JC, Stopford CL, Snowden JS, et al. (2005). Quali-
mal volunteers: Evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. tative neuropsychological performance characteristics in
Neuropsychopharmacology 20: 322–339. frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neu-
Roman GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al. (1993). Vascu- rol Neurosurg Psychiatry 76: 920–927.
lar dementia: Diagnostic criteria for research studies. Thompson SA, Graham KS, Patterson K, et al. (2002). Is
Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. knowledge of famous people disproportionately impaired
Neurology 43: 250–260. in patients with early and questionable Alzheimer’s dis-
Rosen HJ, Gorno-Tempini ML, Goldman WP, et al. (2002a). ease? Neuropsychology 16: 344–358.
Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and Thompson SA, Patterson K, Hodges JR (2003). Left/right
semantic dementia. Neurology 58: 198–208. asymmetry of atrophy in semantic dementia: Behavioral–
Rosen HJ, Hartikainen KM, Jagust W, et al. (2002b). Utility cognitive implications. Neurology 61: 1196–1203.
of clinical criteria in differentiating frontotemporal lobar Turner RS, Kenyon LC, Trojanowski JQ, et al. (1996). Clini-
degeneration (FTLD) from AD. Neurology 58: cal, neuroimaging, and pathologic features of progressive
1608–1615. nonfluent aphasia. Ann Neurol 39: 166–173.
548 C.M. KIPPS ET AL.
Varma AR, Snowden JS, Lloyd JJ, et al. (1999). Evaluation Weintraub S, Rubin NP, Mesulam MM (1990). Primary pro-
of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria in the differentiation of gressive aphasia. Longitudinal course, neuropsychological
Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. J Neu- profile, and language features. Arch Neurol 47:
rol Neurosurg Psychiatry 66: 184–188. 1329–1335.
Warrington EK, James M (1991). VOSP: The Visual Object Westbury C, Bub D (1997). Primary progressive aphasia: A
and Space Perception Battery Thames Valley Test review of 112 cases. Brain Lang 60: 381–406.
Company. Yamanouchi H, Budka H, Vass K Sr (1986). Unilateral
Watt S, Jokel R, Behrmann M (1997). Surface dyslexia Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease. Neurology 36: 1517–1520.
in nonfluent progressive aphasia. Brain Lang 56: 211–233.

You might also like