You are on page 1of 13

Acta Neurol Scand 2001: 103: 367–378 Copyright # Munksgaard 2001

Printed in UK. All rights reserved


ACTA NEUROLOGICA
SCANDINAVICA
ISSN 0001-6314

Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia,


Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia
Bathgate D, Snowden JS, Varma A, Blackshaw A, Neary D. Behaviour D. Bathgate1, J. S. Snowden,
in frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. A. Varma, A. Blackshaw, D. Neary
Acta Neurol Scand 2001: 103: 367–378. # Munksgaard 2001. Department of Neurology, Manchester Royal Infirmary
1
Now at Department of Neurology, Middlesborough
Objectives – The study aimed to increase understanding of behavioural General Hospital
changes in frontotemporal dementia (FTD) and identify features that
best differentiate FTD from Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and cerebrovas-
cular dementia (CvD). Methods – A semi-structured questionnaire was
administered to carers of 30 FTD, 75 AD and 34 CvD patients. Results –
Behavioural changes that strongly discriminated FTD from AD and to a
lesser extent CvD were loss of emotions and insight, selfishness,
disinhibition, personal neglect, gluttony and sweet food preference, Key words: frontotemporal dementia; Alzheimer’s
wandering, motor and verbal stereotypies, loss of pain, echolalia and disease; vascular dementia; behaviour; affect;
mutism. Irritability, hyposexuality and hypersomnia did not discrimi- hyperorality; stereotypies; pain
nate. Emotional, eating and stereotyped behaviours correctly classified J.S. Snowden, Cerebral Function Unit, Department of
95% of patients using regression analysis. Conclusions – Behavioural Neurology, Manchester Royal Infirmary, Manchester
characteristics accurately differentiate FTD from AD and CvD. The M13 9WL, UK
findings highlight the particular importance of affective change in FTD,
and underline the role of the frontotemporal lobes in emotion. Accepted for publication February 16, 2001

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) (1–4) is the most characteristics: alterations in memory, visuospatial
common clinical syndrome of frontotemporal lobar functions and language, and it is these changes in
degeneration. It is associated with a non-Alzheimer instrumental functions that provide the basis for
pathology (5–7), the distinct histological character- clinical diagnosis (12). A diagnosis of cerebrovas-
istics of which have been outlined in the Lund- cular dementia (CvD) relies heavily on the presence
Manchester consensus statement (8). Mutations in of vascular risk factors and on findings on
the tau gene on chromosome 17 have been identified neurological examination (13, 14). The respective
in some familial cases (9, 10). emphasis on cognitive and neurological data in the
The salient characteristics of FTD are profound diagnosis of these two common forms of dementia
alterations in personality and social conduct, and might lead to putative behavioural symptoms being
behavioural alterations constitute core and suppor- overlooked. Indeed, since CvD affects subcortical
tive symptoms laid out in diagnostic criteria (8, 11). white matter, which has strong connections to the
These include decline in social interpersonal con- frontal lobes, it might be anticipated that patients
duct, impaired regulation of personal conduct, with dementia due to subcortical vascular disease
emotional blunting, loss of insight, decline in ought to share some of the behavioural character-
hygiene and grooming, mental rigidity and inflex- istics of FTD.
ibility, distractibility and impersistence, hyperorality Although a number of valuable comparative
and dietary changes, perseverative and stereotyped studies of behaviour in FTD and other dementias
behaviour and utilization behaviour. There is (15–22) have been published, these have typically not
currently little published information about the been designed to address the range of unusual
prevalence of these individual symptoms in FTD. behaviours encountered in FTD. Behavioural fea-
There is also little data regarding diagnostic tures specified in consensus criteria are currently
specificity. In contrast to FTD, Alzheimer’s disease broad, covering a spectrum of potential forms of
(AD) is defined principally in terms of its cognitive behavioural alteration. The symptom of dietary

367
Bathgate et al.

change, for example, could encompass overeating as Neurological examination was normal or disclosed
well as food avoidance, food fads as well as mild akinesia, rigidity or myoclonus. Structural
indiscriminate eating, preference for sweet and for imaging showed atrophy and SPECT imaging
savoury foods. There is currently little data regard- revealed characteristic posterior hemisphere changes
ing the degree of consistency and precise form of in the temporoparietal regions (23). The pattern of
behavioural changes within an FTD cohort. cognitive change mirrored that of more than 80
Behavioural symptoms incorporated within cur- pathologically verified cases of AD seen in this
rent diagnostic criteria might, moreover, not be fully centre. All patients fulfilled NINCDS-ADRDA
comprehensive. Clinical interview with patients’ criteria for AD (12).
relatives sometimes elicits an unusual symptom in In patients with cerebrovascular dementia, neuro-
an individual patient, such as a failure to respond to logical examination disclosed focal signs, suggestive
pain. Whether such anecdotal reports have diag- of cerebrovascular disease such as hemianopia,
nostic significance needs to be determined by lower facial weakness, dysarthria, hemisensory
systematic enquiry. deficit, hemiparesis or extensor plantar response.
The purpose of the study was to explore more All patients exhibited a subcortical pattern of
fully the nature of behavioural changes in classical dementia (24) on neuropsychological examination
FTD and to determine the discriminating value of
and had a Hachinski ischaemia scale score (13)
behavioural characteristics in the differentiation of
greater than 4. Patients fulfilled criteria for vascular
FTD from two common forms of dementia, AD and
dementia (14).
subcortical CvD. The findings ought to provide
Demographic details of patients at the time of
information about the relative importance of
individual symptoms cited in current diagnostic questionnaire administration are shown in Table 1.
criteria as well as extend understanding of beha- Patients were relatively youthful, reflecting the
vioural changes in FTD. pattern of referral to the neurological department
clinic. However, FTD patients were slightly younger
than AD patients (t=2.5, P=0.01), who in turn
were younger than the CvD patients (t=3.4,
Methods
P=0.001). The groups were well matched for illness
Subjects duration at the time of assessment. Mini Mental
The subjects were consecutive referrals to a specialist State Examination (MMSE) (25) scores were lower
clinic for dementia with a clinical diagnosis of FTD, in the AD group than the FTD (t=2.8, P=0.006)
AD or CvD. The diagnosis was based on historical and CvD (t=4.1, P=0.000) groups.
information, neurological examination and neuro-
psychological assessment and supported by findings
on structural (magnetic resonance) and functional Informant-based behavioural questionnaire
(SPECT) imaging. Patients in whom the clinical
diagnosis was equivocal or in whom mixed pathol- A semi-structured questionnaire schedule was con-
ogy was suspected were excluded from the study. structed, covering the following domains: basic and
Subsequent follow-up of subjects has reinforced the social emotions, social behaviour, response to
clinical diagnoses in all cases. sensory stimuli, eating and other oral behaviours,
Patients with FTD exhibited profound break- wandering behaviour, sexuality, sleep pattern, repe-
down in personality and social conduct, in the titive behaviours, compulsions and rituals, environ-
context of physical well-being. Neurological signs mental dependency, memory and spatially-related
were absent or limited to primitive reflexes and/or behaviours, delusions and hallucinations (see
mild akinesia and rigidity. Neuropsychological Appendix).
investigation disclosed executive deficits indicative
of frontal lobe dysfunction in the absence of Table 1. Demographic features
visuospatial impairment and brain imaging revealed
atrophy and impaired function of the frontal and FTD AD CvD
anterior temporal regions. All patients fulfilled
Number 30 75 34
criteria for prototypical FTD (11). Patients with Males 15 36 22
other clinical syndromes of frontotemporal lobar Females 15 39 12
degeneration, that is, semantic dementia and pro- Age 59* 65* 69*
gressive aphasia (11) were not included in the study. Illness duration 4.2 4.7 4.3
MMSE 21 16* 21
Patients with AD exhibited memory impairment
and deficits in perceptuo-spatial skills and/or * Differed significantly from other groups.
language, in the context of physical well-being. MMSE=Mini Mental State Examination.

368
Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia

Table 2. Affect and social behaviour

Odds ratios (with 95% confidence intervals)


Frequency of symptom (%) for pairwise group comparisons
FTD AD CvD FTD vs AD FTD vs CvD CvD vs AD

Loss of basic emotions 97 39 47 47.6 (4.4–45.0) 21.6 (2.5–187.8) 1.4 (0.7–3.9)


Exaggerated emotional display 37 49 50 0.5 (0.2–1.3) 0.2 (0.1–1.0) 1.2 (0.5–3.1)
Loss of embarrassment 90 47 53 13.5 (3.4–52.5) 16.9 (2.6–109.5) 1.3 (0.5–3.1)
Irritability 59 71 68 0.5 (0.2–1.1) 0.7 (0.2–2.2) 0.9 (0.4–2.3)
Aggression 47 43 50 1.1 (0.5–2.6) 0.7 (0.2–2.2) 1.5 (0.6–3.5)
Insightfulness 23 85 79 0.0 (0.0–0.1) 0.1 (0.0–0.3) 0.6 (0.2–1.9)
Excessive worrying 13 40 39 0.2 (0.1–0.8) 0.2 (0.1–0.9) 0.9 (0.4–2.1)
Selfishness 87 47 62 6.8 (2.1–21.9) 5.2 (1.2–22.1) 1.7 (0.7–4.0)
Disinhibition 73 27 35 8.3 (2.8–24.7) 4.1 (1.2–14.1) 1.7 (0.7–4.5)
Social avoidance 30 20 35 1.5 (0.6–4.1) 0.7 (0.2–2.2) 2.4 (0.9–6.5)
Seeks out social contact 23 23 26 1.0 (0.4–3.0) 1.0 (0.3–3.6) 1.4 (0.5–3.6)
Neglect of hygiene 87 44 50 9.0 (2.7–29.3) 5.7 (1.4–23.3) 1.3 (0.5–3.0)
Loss of interest 90 49 56 8.6 (2.3–31.2) 6.0 (1.4–26.3) 1.3 (0.5–3.1)

Procedure Analysis
The questionnaire was administered to primary Data analyses were carried out using the SPSS-PC
carers of patients, normally the spouse or partner. software package. For each individual item of the
It was emphasized that a ‘‘symptom’’ should Behavioural Questionnaire age and gender adjusted
reflect a notable change from the patient’s odds ratios were calculated for pairwise group
premorbid state and not a long-standing character comparisons, using logistic regression models.
trait. At the beginning of the interview the Items that yielded a significant increase or decrease
informant was asked to recall the onset of the in odds in the group comparisons were entered into a
illness to permit demarcation of the period of stepwise logistic regression analysis to determine the
illness for which questions applied. Great care was degree to which behavioural characteristics sepa-
taken by the interviewer to ensure that each rated FTD from non-FTD.
symptom being probed was understood. The same
illustrative examples were given at each interview,
ensuring consistency of presentation and of Results
information given. Responses for each question Tables 2–8 show the frequency of behavioural
were coded as 1 or 0, representing the presence or changes for the three diagnostic groups.
absence of notable change from the premorbid Frequencies refer to the percentage of the sample
state. in whom the behavioural abnormality was reported
Interviews were conducted by a single interviewer to be present, an abnormality being defined as a
(D.B.), who was blind to diagnosis. (The informant notable change from the patient’s premorbid state.
was advised at the beginning of the interview that the The tables also show odds ratios, adjusted for age
patient’s clinical diagnosis should not be disclosed.) and gender, together with 95% confidence intervals
The interviewer had no clinical contact with the (CI) for pairwise group comparisons. A ratio of 1
patient, either prior to or during the course of the indicates that a behavioural characteristic has not
interview. A second blind interviewer (A.B.) was changed the odds of a patient having the target
present for 19 interviews and coded responses disease (the first disease in the comparison) relative
independently to allow measures of inter-rater to the reference disease (the second disease in the
reliability to be determined. comparison). A ratio greater than 1 indicates an

Table 3. Sensory behaviours

Frequency of symptom Odds ratios (with 95% confidence intervals)


(%) for pairwise group comparisons
FTD AD CvD FTD vs AD FTD vs CvD CvD vs AD

Loss of awareness of pain 41 4 9 15.8 (3.9–64.3) 6.1 (1.3–28.9) 3.0 (0.5–18.4)


Loss of smell 38 17 22 3.2 (0.9–11.5) 1.8 (0.4–8.8) 1.6 (0.5–5.3)
Exaggerated sensory response 33 42 26 0.6 (0.2–1.5) 1.3 (0.4–4.6) 0.5 (0.2–1.2)
Exaggerated heat/cold response 7 20 9 0.3 (0.1–1.3) 0.9 (0.1–9.8) 0.3 (0.1–1.0)

369
Bathgate et al.

Table 4. Eating and vegetative behaviours

Frequency of symptom Odds ratios (with 95% confidence intervals)


(%) for pairwise group comparisons
FTD AD CvD FTD vs AD FTD vs CvD CvD vs AD

Preference for sweet foods 50 19 26 4.6 (1.7–12.0) 2.2 (0.7–7.2) 1.3 (0.5–3.6)
Preference for savoury foods 0 3 0 – – –
Food fads 17 19 21 0.8 (0.3–13.6) 0.6 (0.1–2.6) 1.1 (0.4–3.2)
Loss of discrimination 46 18 18 4.0 (1.5–11.0) 4.5 (1.2–17.4) –
Overeats 70 23 35 7.8 (2.8–21.4) 3.6 (1.1–11.9) 1.9 (0.7–4.9)
Eats continually if food present 43 7 15 13.0 (3.7–46.1) 6.2 (1.4–26.4) 1.8 (0.5–7.3)
Steals food from others’ plates 30 1 9 29.8 (3.3–266.8) 2.8 (0.6–13.7) 11.6 (1.1–121.3)
Seeks out food 33 5 9 7.4 (2.0–27.2) 3.1 (0.6–16.2) 1.4 (0.3–7.3)
Crams food 50 1 18 80.6 (9.2–707.2) 4.9 (1.3–18.5) 12.3 (1.4–110.1)
Increased alcohol consumption 13 11 0 1.2 (0.3–4.4) – –
Increased smoking 100# 17 27 – – –
Oral exploration of objects 13 3 15 6.6 (1.1–39.4) 0.8 (0.2–3.8) 6.1 (1.1–34.7)
Wandering 67 25 38 6.7 (2.5–17.8) 3.2 (1.0–10.6) 2.1 (0.8–5.2)
Pacing 60 24 35 4.4 (1.7–11.1) 2.7 (0.8–8.6) 1.7 (0.7–4.3)
Hyposexuality 58 52 48 1.2 (0.5–3.1) 0.9 (0.2–3.0) 1.1 (0.4–2.6)
Hypersexuality 19 15 6 1.3 (0.4–4.5) 3.1 (0.4–22.3) 0.5 (0.1–2.8)
Hyposomnia 20 19 21 1.0 (0.3–2.9) 1.0 (0.2–4.0) 1.3 (0.4–3.7)
Hypersomnia 47 36 35 1.2 (0.5–3.0) 2.0 (0.6–6.5) 1.2 (0.5–3.1)
#
There were only 3 smokers in the FTD cohort so that the 100% increase in smoking in that group needs to be viewed with caution.

increase in odds for the target disease and less than embarrassment was almost universal in FTD, but
1 a decrease in odds. Ratios that differed from 1 not in other groups. FTD patients were more likely
with 95% confidence were considered significant and than other patients to be selfish, showing a lack of
are in bold. sympathy with and empathy for others. They were
also more likely than other patients to exhibit
disinhibited behaviours, to show a lack of concern
Affect and social behaviour for personal hygiene and a general loss of interest in
A loss of the capacity to demonstrate basic emotions activities. The presence of these behavioural features
was virtually ubiquitous in FTD (Table 2), whereas it significantly increased the likelihood of a diagnosis
was reported in less than half of patients with other of FTD compared to both AD and CvD. There were
dementias. Loss of emotion greatly increased the no differences between the groups in the tendency
odds of FTD relative to both AD and CvD. A either to avoid or seek out social contact.
subsequent analysis of individual emotional states Insightfulness, determined by the patients’
(not shown in the table) indicated that the loss of demonstration of anxiety, concern or loss of
affect in FTD was not confined to a single emotion. confidence, when confronted by tasks that they
Loss of specific emotions significantly increased the can no longer accomplish, was present less often in
odds of FTD compared to AD: loss of happiness FTD than in other groups. The presence of
(odds ratio=5.3, CI=2.0–14.3), sadness (odds insightfulness greatly decreased the odds of a patient
ratio=17.6, CI=5.5–56.7), fear (odds ratio=12.4, having FTD compared to both AD and CvD.
CI=4.4–35.2), anger (odds ratio=14.1, CI= Similarly, the tendency to worry excessively
4.4–45.0), surprise (odds ratio=7.2, CI=2.8–19.0) decreased the likelihood of FTD compared to AD
and disgust (odds ratio=13.3, CI=4.6–39.0). and CvD.
Comparisons between FTD and CvD also revealed Irritability and aggression were present in a
a greater likelihood of loss of specific emotions in relatively high proportion of patients with FTD.
FTD: for happiness (odds ratio=3.8, CI=1.1– 13.7), However, they were present also in other groups,
sadness (odds ratio=30.0, CI=4.6–159.0), fear and group comparisons revealed no significant
(odds ratio=22.1, CI=4.0–122.6), anger (odds effects. Affective and social behavioural symptoms
ratio=15.7, CI=3.1–79.0), surprise (odds ratio= did not significantly influence the odds of CVD
7.3, CI=2.0–26.4) and disgust (odds ratio=9.0, compared to AD.
CI=2.4–33.8). Comparisons between AD and CvD
revealed no influence of emotion on likelihood of
diagnosis. Sensory behaviours
Affective change in FTD patients included A lack of appropriate response to painful stimuli
changes in social emotions. Loss of feelings of was reported in approaching half of the FTD group

370
Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia

Table 5. Repetitive behaviours, compulsions and rituals

Frequency of symptom Odds ratios (with 95% confidence intervals)


(%) for pairwise group comparisons
FTD AD CvD FTD vs AD FTD vs CvD CvD vs AD

Simple motor stereotypies 60 39 26 2.8 (1.1–6.9) 3.1 (1.0–9.8) 0.6 (0.2–1.4)


Complex motor routines 27 5 6 4.8 (1.2–18.7) 2.6 (0.4–15.7) 1.9 (0.3–12.4)
Paces fixed route 29 3 3 15.9 (3.1–80.0) 10.8 (1.2–96.8) 1.5 (0.1–18.5)
Verbal stereotypies 50 16 21 5.6 (2.1–15.5) 4.0 (1.1–14.1) 1.2 (0.4–3.5)
Verbal perseverations 17 3 5 2.5 (0.6–11.0) 6.9 (0.4–115.4) 0.4 (0.0–4.8)
Repetitive themes 30 24 32 1.3 (0.5–3.4) 0.6 (0.2–1.9) 1.4 (0.6–3.6)
Counts objects 13 4 15 2.8 (0.5–14.5) 0.5 (0.1–2.7) 5.5 (1.2–25.7)
Aligns objects 37 24 21 2.1 (0.8–5.4) 2.9 (0.8–10.7) 0.6 (0.2–1.8)
Adherence to daily routine 13 15 6 0.9 (0.2–3.2) 3.5 (0.4–28.9) 0.3 (0.1–1.4)
Arranges belongings in same way 7 12 3 0.6 (0.1–3.1) 2.4 (0.1–56.1) 0.2 (0.0–1.4)
Overconcern with cleanliness 3 8 0 0.4 (0.0–3.8) – –
Clockwatching 27 13 21 2.2 (0.7–6.7) 1.2 (0.3–4.7) 1.5 (0.5–4.4)
Excessive checking 13 19 24 0.6 (0.2–2.2) 0.4 (0.1–2.1) 1.2 (0.4–3.3)
Excessive attention to detail 17 9 15 1.8 (0.5–6.3) 1.3 (0.3–6.0) 1.8 (0.5–6.8)
Upset if routine disrupted 27 15 24 2.1 (0.7–6.0) 1.2 (0.3–4.5) 1.5 (0.6–4.8)
Needs to do things immediately 40 25 38 1.9 (0.8–4.9) 1.1 (0.4–3.6) 2.0 (0.8–4.9)
Unusual toileting routines 30 8 3 5.2 (1.5–17.8) 9.8 (1.1–89.4) 0.3 (0.0–2.4)
Superstitious rituals 20 1 3 14.7 (1.6–139.1) 5.6 (0.5–58.9) 5.9 (0.3–123.4)

but was rare in other groups (Table 3). Loss of pain Only 3 (10%) FTD patients, 11 (32%) CVD
response significantly increased the odds of FTD patients and 18 (24%) of AD patients smoked
compared to both AD and CvD. A loss of sense of cigarettes prior to the onset of illness. Odds ratios
smell was numerically more frequent in FTD. An were not calculated for change in smoking habits,
exaggerated or inappropriate response to sensory because this behavioural feature could potentially be
stimuli was reported in a proportion of patients in all documented in so few cases. The figure of 100%
groups. A change in response to heat and cold was increase in smoking in FTD is based on 3 patients
reported numerically most often in AD. However, only so should be viewed with caution. Nevertheless,
none of these behavioural features altered the odds the figure is consistent with the view that hyperoral
of diagnosis to a significant degree. behaviour in FTD may extend beyond the realm of
food consumption. Oral exploration of inanimate
objects was relatively rare although it increased
Eating and oral behaviours
diagnostic odds for both FTD and CvD compared
to AD.
Alterations in eating behaviour were reported more
frequently in FTD than in other dementias
(Table 4). An altered preference for sweet foods, Other vegetative behaviours
and a range of gluttonous behaviours including Wandering and pacing were reported more fre-
indiscriminate eating, overeating, eating continually quently in FTD than in other groups (Table 4).
while food remains present, stealing food from Wandering significantly increased the diagnostic
others’ plates, seeking out food and cramming food odds of FTD compared to AD and CvD. Pacing
increasd the odds of FTD compared to AD and to a increased the odds of FTD compared to AD. Loss of
slightly lesser extent CvD. interest in sexual activity and increased somnolence

Table 6. Environmental dependency

Frequency of symptom Odds ratios (with 95% confidence intervals)


(%) for pairwise group comparisons
FTD AD CvD FTD vs AD FTD vs CvD CvD vs AD

Hoarding 57 41 24 1.6 (0.7–4.0) 4.0 (1.2–13.5) 0.6 (0.2–1.4)


Touches or handles objects 40 23 26 2.1 (0.8–5.5) 1.5 (0.5–5.0) 1.2 (0.5–3.4)
Echolalia 33 4 12 14.6 (3.4–63.0) 5.0 (1.1–22.0) 2.6 (0.5–13.6)
Echopraxia 3 1 6 2.6 (0.1–45.7) 0.6 (0.0–8.2) 6.5 (0.5–78.3)
Utilization phenomena 0 0 0 – – –
Reads aloud notices 33 21 18 1.7 (0.6–4.5) 1.6 (0.4–6.3) 0.9 (0.3–2.6)

371
Bathgate et al.

Table 7. Cognitively-mediated behaviours

Frequency of symptom Odds ratios (with 95% confidence intervals)


(%) for pairwise group comparisons
FTD AD CvD FTD vs AD FTD vs CvD CvD vs AD

Mislays objects 53 89 76 0.2 (0.1–0.5) 0.4 (0.1–1.4) 0.5 (0.1–1.4)


Lost in familiar surroundings 23 41 45 0.5 (0.2–1.5) 0.4 (0.1–1.6) 1.4 (0.6–3.4)
Disoriented in own home 7 31 24 0.2 (0.0–0.8) 0.2 (0.0–1.4) 0.8 (0.3–2.0)
Fails to recognize objects 17 19 12 0.8 (0.3–2.7) 1.8 (0.4–8.8) 0.6 (0.2–2.1)
Difficulty locating objects 7 56 15 0.0 (0.0–0.2) 0.4 (0.1–2.7) 0.2 (0.1–0.5)
Uses wrong words 23 45 35 0.4 (0.1–0.9) 0.3 (0.1–1.2) 0.7 (0.3–1.8)
Mutism 23 5 3 6.0 (1.5–23.4) 33.0 (2.1–523.3) 0.6 (0.1–6.5)

were common features of FTD, but they occurred cantly alter diagnostic odds. Clockwatching
too in other dementias. Alterations in sexual and occurred in a quarter of FTD patients and a
sleep behaviours did not significantly alter diagnos- tendency to align objects in one third. Although
tic odds in any group comparison. numerically more frequent than in other groups the
increase in odds for FTD was non-significant. A
tendency to count objects significantly increased the
Repetitive behaviours, compulsions and rituals odds for CvD compared to AD. No other repetitive,
Repetitive behaviours were common in the FTD compulsive or ritualistic behaviours altered diag-
group (Table 5). Simple motor stereotypies, com- nostic odds for CvD compared to AD.
prising vocalizations such as grunting or humming,
facial movements such as lip smacking or limb
movements such as hand rubbing or foot tapping, Environmental dependency
were reported in almost two thirds of the sample, Hoarding was a common feature in FTD (Table 6),
and significantly increased the odds of FTD but it had diagnostic value only in comparison with
compared to AD and CvD. The odds of FTD CvD, since hoarding was a relatively common
compared to AD were also increased by the presence feature also of AD. Echolalia, present in one third
of complex repetitive motor routines. Repetitive of FTD patients, significantly increased the odds of
routines in FTD included the tendency to sing the FTD compared to AD and to a lesser extent CvD.
same song or recite the same rhyme over and over One third of FTD patients showed a tendency to
again, to clap the same rhythm repetitively or dance read public notices aloud, although the increased
the same dance. The most commonly reported odds for FTD compared to AD and CvD was not
individual behaviour, present in 25% of FTD significant. Frank utilization behaviour was not
patients, was rhythm clapping. Other repetitive reported in any patient.
behaviours that significantly increased the odds for
FTD compared to AD and (in most cases) CvD were
pacing a fixed route, producing the same word or Cognitively-mediated behaviours
phrase repetitively (verbal stereotypy), engaging in Behaviours relating to memory and spatial dysfunc-
unusual toileting routines and the presence of tion were numerically less common in FTD than in
superstitious rituals, such as avoiding standing on other dementias, particularly AD (Table 7). The
cracks between paving stones. Frankly obsessive tendency to mislay objects, to be disoriented within
features, such as repeated checking and excessive the home and to have difficulty locating objects
attention to detail, were rare and did not signifi- placed in front of them significantly decreased the

Table 8. Hallucinations and delusions

Frequency of symptom Odds ratios (with 95% confidence intervals)


(%) for pairwise group comparisons
FTD AD CvD FTD vs AD FTD vs CvD CvD vs AD

Suspiciousness 30 44 50 0.6 (0.2–1.5) 0.4 (0.1–1.2) 1.2 (0.5–2.8)


Delusions of theft 10 25 35 0.4 (0.1–1.4) 0.2 (0.1–1.0) 1.3 (0.5–3.4)
False belief that people in home 10 20 21 0.6 (0.1–2.3) 0.5 (0.1–2.8) 1.1 (0.4–3.1)
Misidentification phenomena 30 41 29 0.6 (0.2–1.5) 0.9 (0.3–3.2) 0.7 (0.3–1.9)
Visual hallucinations/illusions 7 23 15 0.3 (0.1–1.2) 0.5 (0.1–3.1) 0.6 (0.2–1.8)
Auditory hallucinations/illusions 10 8 12 1.0 (0.2–4.7) 0.9 (0.1–5.9) 2.0 (0.5–8.4)

372
Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia

odds of FTD compared to AD. A difficulty in the were correctly classified as non-FTD with an
spatial location of objects also decreased the odds of accuracy of 96%. The single variable of insightful-
CvD compared to AD. Use of wrong words, ness alone effectively classified 79% of FTD patients
reported more commonly in AD than FTD, and 83% of non-FTD. High levels of classification
decreased the odds of FTD compared to AD. By accuracy could be achieved using other variable
contrast, mutism, reported most commonly in FTD, combinations, the common feature being that they
increased the odds of FTD compared to both AD included variables relating to a) emotion, b) altered
and CvD. eating habits and c) repetitive behaviours.

Hallucinations and delusions


Suspiciousness, delusions of theft, the false belief of Discussion
the presence of others in the home, misidentification In FTD behavioural change is the overriding clinical
phenomena and visual hallucinations and illusions symptom. A number of behavioural symptoms show
were numerically less common in FTD patients than very high sensitivity to FTD, being present in at least
in other groups (Table 8). However, these relatively 50% of cases. These include loss of basic emotions
small differences did not significantly alter diagnos- (e.g. fear) and social emotions (e.g. embarrassment),
tic odds. together with selfishness, disinhibition, irritability,
neglect of personal hygiene, loss of interest/apathy,
Inter-rater reliability gluttony, altered preference for sweet foods, wan-
dering, pacing, hyposexuality, motor and verbal
There was high agreement between the two raters in
stereotypies, and hoarding. The most sensitive
determining the presence or absence of symptoms
feature, being present in virtually all patients, is
(mean Kappa statistic 0.97).
the loss of basic emotions, representing the capacity
to demonstrate happiness, sadness, fear, anger,
Effect of age and gender on symptoms surprise and disgust. Also present in virtually all
Examination of patterns of behavioural change in patients is a failure to show embarrassment and a
younger compared to older patients within a group general loss of interest in activities. Selfishness and
(<mean vs >mean) and in men compared to neglect of personal hygiene are reported in 87% of
women did not reveal notable differences in pattern patients.
of behavioural change as a function of age or gender. Behaviours that are sensitive to FTD, as judged by
their prevalence of at least 50%, also typically show
significant diagnostic specificity. Those common
Logistic regression analysis: FTD v non-FTD behaviours all increase the diagnostic odds of
Items that had yielded a significant increase or FTD compared to AD and CvD, with the exception
decrease in odds ratio in the pairwise group of irritability and hyposexuality which occur with
comparisons were entered into a stepwise logistic relatively equal frequency in all three groups.
regression analysis. Where variables were obviously There are a number of additional behaviours that,
inter-correlated (e.g. loss of emotion, loss of capacity although present in only a proportion of FTD
for sadness) only the variable whose odds ratio patients, have diagnostic value. Loss of awareness of
showed the largest difference from 1 was included, to pain was reported in 41% of FTD patients, yet only
reduce the degree of multicollinearity. The criterion rarely in other groups. In most cases, this loss had
for entry of variables was set at 0.05 and for removal come to light as a result of dramatic incidents:
of variables set at 0.1. Five variables entered the patients had for example, stepped into a scalding
equation in the following order: insightfulness, bath and shown no withdrawal reaction, leading to
pacing a fixed route, loss of emotion, food cramming burns requiring medical attention. A number of
and difficulty locating objects. The numerical size of eating-related behavioural changes increase the odds
the parameter estimate at step 1 was x2.9, standard of FTD in addition to the general features of
error 0.5, and at step 5 estimates ranged from 4.7 to overeating and sweet food preference. These
x4.7, standard error 1.2–1.9. Insightfulness and a included a tendency to continue eating for as long
difficulty locating objects contributed negatively to a as food is present, to steal food from others’ plates
diagnosis of FTD, whereas loss of emotion, food and to cram food into the mouth. These features
cramming, pacing a fixed route contributed posi- may to some extent reflect environmental depen-
tively. Using a classification cut-off of 0.5, these five dency: stimulus-driven behaviour that cannot be
variables resulted in an overall accuracy of classifi- overridden by virtue of the impairment in goal-dir-
cation of 95%. FTD patients were correctly classified ected, purposive behaviour engendered by frontal
with an accuracy of 90% and AD and CvD patients lobe dysfunction. However, the tendency in a third

373
Bathgate et al.

of FTD patients to seek out food, even when it is not stress or anxiety if routines are disrupted. Moreover,
visibly present, suggests that environmental depen- despite the presence of verbal and motor stereotypies
dency cannot be a sufficient explanation, and and repetitive routines, patients typically show a
suggests that more fundamental change to satiety notable absence of concern for personal hygiene,
mechanisms may also be present. The altered and do not show the repetitive checking behaviours
preference for sweet foods present in 50% of patients and excessive attention to detail in carrying out tasks
supports the notion of fundamental dietary changes, that is characteristic of OCD. It seems reasonable to
independent of environmental dependency. The assume that the repetitive behaviours in these
importance of dietary changes in FTD has been patients arise as a consequence of the loss of the
highlighted by others (26). supervisory role of the frontal lobes to implement
Verbal stereotypies, comprising repetitive use of effective goal-directed, purposive and novel beha-
the same word or phrase, and motor stereotypies, viours and to inhibit existing programs of behaviour.
such as grunting, arm rubbing or foot tapping are Whether there are distinct mechanisms underlying
common in FTD. Repetitive, stereotyped motor complex and simple forms of repetitive behaviour is
behaviours may constitute complex routines as well worthy of further exploration. It has been suggested
as simple motor mannerisms. These take a variety of (27) that complex repetitive routines may result from
forms. For example, one patient, on entering a room aberrant motor plans, and more elementary beha-
would tap out the same rhythm on each of the four viours to disinhibited motor programs.
walls before being seated. Another sang Ave Maria Subjects were consecutive referrals to a neuro-
repetitively. Another counted every step made with logical clinic, the rationale being to include as rep-
his left foot and multiplied this by two every 100 resentative a sample as possible of each diagnostic
steps to announce how far he had walked. Another group. The groups were not therefore precisely
wrote a continuous commentary of all conversations matched for gender and age. However, odds ratios
and activities on any available scraps of paper. were age and gender adjusted, so that putative
Another would empty her wardrobe of clothes daily, effects of age and gender are controlled. Moreover,
carry the clothes to another part of the room, where examination of behavioural profiles across groups as
she would lay them out on the floor, following which a function of age and gender suggests that they
she would return them into their correct place in the contribute little to behavioural differences.
wardrobe. The most commonly reported individual Traditionally, comparative studies of dementia
behaviour was rhythm clapping, followed by tune attempt to match for overall dementia severity.
singing or humming. Although less common than However, measures of overall severity are necessa-
simple motor stereotypies these complex routines are rily artificial, since patients with different dementing
important diagnostically because of their rarity in illnesses break down along differing rather than
other dementias. Similarly, although wandering and common dimensions of impairment. Nevertheless,
pacing occur in other dementias, it is specifically in there is good evidence that the group differences in
FTD that patients may pace a fixed rather than this study cannot be accounted for by notional
random route, giving the pacing a more repetitive, differences in overall dementia severity. Patients
stereotyped quality than occurs in AD or CvD. were well matched with respect to duration of
Stereotyped behaviours in FTD may have a super- symptoms. Since the disorders have relatively
stitious, ritualistic quality, such as refusal to walk on comparable time courses this suggests that patients
the cracks between paving stones. One man, for were at an equivalent stage of illness. Moreover, the
example, always stepped on to flagstones with his presence of dissociations in the findings rules out a
left foot. general explanation in terms of severity. Whereas
A number of previous studies have highlighted the many behavioural symptoms were more common in
association between repetitive and compulsive FTD, cognitively-based behaviours relating to
behaviours and frontotemporal dementia (22, memory and spatial dysfunction were more
27–29), providing support for the role of frontal– common in AD. They were more likely to mislay
striatal circuitry in compulsive behaviours (30). The objects, to be disoriented in the home and to have
extensive range of behaviours that fall within this difficulty in the spatial location of objects in the
rubric has been well highlighted (27). Although some immediate environment. In the realm of language,
patients with FTD have been regarded as fulfilling echolalia and mutism were more common in FTD
criteria for obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) and the use of wrong words (verbal and phonemic
(27–29) the majority of patients in this study would paraphasias) more common in AD. Such dissocia-
not do so. Compulsive acts do not appear to be tions suggest that differences are genuinely disease-
linked to intrusive thought processes as in OCD and related and not artefacts of overall severity. The
in most patients there is no indication of induced dissociation between aphasic symptoms and mutism

374
Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia

reinforces the view that mutism in FTD reflects the (16, 31) in keeping with our own findings, have not.
patients’ adynamia and loss of generational cap- Some studies have reported verbal outbursts to be
abilities secondary to frontal lobe dysfunction rather more common in FTD (22) whereas the present
than a primary aphasia. study suggests that aggressive behaviour is no more
It is generally accepted that psychotic symptoms common in FTD than in AD or CvD. Assessment
are relatively rare in FTD, and some studies have methodology may play a part in accounting for some
demonstrated a lower incidence compared to AD disparities. Depression, for example, has been
(16, 17, 19). In the present study psychiatric reported to be more common in FTD than AD
symptoms of hallucinations and delusions were (17). However, it has been pointed out (32) that the
numerically less common in FTD than in other rating scale employed in that study incorporates
groups although, because of the relatively low apathy as a component of depression, thereby falsely
incidence across all groups, they did not alter elevating depression scores in FTD patients, in
significantly diagnostic odds. whom apathy and loss of volition are prominent
The largest differences in symptom frequency features. It is unlikely that methodological factors
occurred, as expected, between FTD and AD. It had are a sufficient explanation for lack of consensus. It
been anticipated that CvD might share some would appear from the present study that con-
behavioural similarities with FTD on the basis of ventional neuropsychiatric symptoms, such as irrit-
disruption to subcortical-frontal circuitry. Never- ability, while they may have a place in the
theless, an important finding of the study is that characterization of FTD, are less reliable indicators
behavioural features that strongly differentiate FTD of diagnosis than are other more unusual beha-
from AD also typically distinguish FTD from CvD, viours, which are uniquely associated with FTD.
albeit with reduced magnitude. By contrast, with few In this study, the initial selection of patients was
exceptions, those behaviours do not distinguish AD based on a combination of historical, neurological,
from CvD. Such findings indicate the importance of neuropsychological and neuroimaging data, to
those behavioural symptoms both in the character- avoid the potential for circularity inherent in a
purely behavioural diagnosis. Moreover, the beha-
ization of FTD and its differentiation from other
vioural features explored in this study extend beyond
dementias.
those outlined in current criteria for FTD, and are
The features that emerged from the logistic
not specified in AD and CvD criteria. The
regression analysis that best discriminate FTD
demonstration of highly significant behavioural
from other dementias were loss of basic emotions,
differences, derived from ‘‘blind’’ interview, rein-
food cramming, pacing a fixed route, an absence of
forces the view that behavioural symptoms are
difficulty in locating objects and an absence of
critical to the diagnosis of FTD and may be more
insightfulness. The symptom of insightfulness, as reliable than cognitive test scores in differentiating
operationally defined in this study, referred not to FTD from other dementias (21, 33).
the capacity to report symptoms of illness explicitly The features that best discriminate FTD from
but rather to the implied awareness of difficulty, other dementias fall into three principle domains: i)
indicated by patients’ demonstration of distress, affect, ii) oral behaviours and iii) repetitive, stereo-
anxiety, loss of confidence or withdrawal when typed behaviours. Reduction in the capacity to
confronted by tasks that they are unable to perform. demonstrate primary emotions, in particular, was a
It was found in a previous study (15) that virtually ubiquitous feature of FTD, even in the
insightfulness determined in this way strongly early stages of the disorder. An additional salient
discriminated between FTD and AD, whereas feature identified by the present study was a loss of
conventional measures of insight, requiring explicit appropriate response to painful stimuli. In FTD,
report of symptoms, did not. The data from this pathological change is primarily cortical in early
study support the earlier data in showing that disease, although limbic structures such as the
insightfulness as defined by patients’ emotional hippocampus and amygdala are affected later in
response when confronted by difficulties signifi- the course (4). The precise mechanisms underlying
cantly differentiated FTD from other dementias: patients’ unusual behaviours and the particular role
AD and CvD patients showed a degree of insightful- of the frontal and temporal lobes and of limbic
ness whereas FTD patients did not. structures in contributing to behavioural changes
Previous comparative studies of neuropsychiatric remains to be explored.
symptoms in FTD such as irritability, aggression,
depression, and anxiety, have not yielded entirely
uniform results. Some studies found significant
differences in the incidence of irritability between References
FTD and AD patients (17, 19) whereas other studies 1. GUSTAFSON L. Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer

375
Bathgate et al.

type. II. Clinical picture and differential diagnosis. Arch entiated using a brief battery of tests? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
Gerontol Geriatrica 1987;6:209–23. 1997;12:375–83.
2. NEARY D, SNOWDEN JS, NORTHEN B, GOULDING PJ. Dementia of 22. MENDEZ MF, PERRYMAN KM, MILLER BL, CUMMINGS JL.
frontal lobe type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988;51: Behavioural differences between frontotemporal dementia
353–61. and Alzheimer’s disease: a comparison on the BEHAVE-AD
3. MILLER BL, CUMMINGS JL, VILLANUEVA-MEYER J et al. Frontal rating scale. Int Psychogeriatr 1998;10:155–62.
lobe degeneration: clinical, neuropsychological and SPECT 23. NEARY D, SNOWDEN JS, SHIELDS RA et al. Single photon
characteristics. Neurology 1991;41:1374–82. emission tomography using 99m Tc-HMPAO in the inves-
4. SNOWDEN JS, NEARY D, MANN DMA. Frontotemporal lobar tigation of dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987;
degeneration: frontotemporal dementia, progressive aphasia, 50:1101–9. .
semantic dementia. New York: Churchill Livingstone, 1996. 24. CUMMINGS JL. Subcortical dementia. New York: Oxford
5. BRUN A. Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. I. University Press, 1990.
Neuropathology. Arch Gerontol Geriatrica 1987;6:193–208. 25. FOLSTEIN MF, FOLSTEIN SE, MCHUGH PR. Mini-mental state. J
6. MANN DMA, SOUTH PW. The topographic distribution of Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–98.
brain atrophy in frontal lobe dementia. Acta Neuropath 26. MILLER BL, DARBY AL, SWARTZ JR, YENER GG, MENA I.
1993;85:334–40. Dietary changes, compulsions and sexual behaviour in
7. MANN DMA, SOUTH PW, SNOWDEN JS, NEARY D. Dementia of frontotemporal degeneration. Dementia 1995;6:195–9.
frontal lobe type; neuropathology and immunohisto- 27. AMES D, CUMMINGS JL, WIRSHING WC, QUINN B, MAHLER M.
Repetitive and compulsive behaviour in frontal lobe degen-
chemistry. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1993;56:605–14.
erations. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994;6:100–13.
8. The Lund and Manchester groups. Clinical and neuropatho-
28. TONKOLOGY JM, SMITH TW, BARREIRA PJ. Obsessive-compul-
logical criteria for frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol
sive disorders in Pick’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin
Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;57:416–18.
Neurosci 1994;6:176–80.
9. HUTTON M, LENDON CL, RIZZU P et al. Association of missense
29. MENDEZ MF, PERRYMAN KM, MILLER BL, SWARTZ JR,
and 5k-splice-site mutations in tau with the inherited dementia
CUMMINGS JL. Compulsive behaviours as presenting symp-
FTDP-17. Nature 1998;393:702–5. toms of frontotemporal dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry
10. POORKAJ P, BIRD TD, WIJSMAN E et al. Tau is a candidate gene Neurol 1997;10:154–7.
for chromosome 17 frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 30. RUBIN RT, HARRIS GJ. Obsessive-compulsive disorder and the
1998;43:815–25. frontal lobes. In: MILLER BL, CUMMINGS JL, eds. The Human
11. NEARY D, SNOWDEN JS, GUSTAFSON L et al. Frontotemporal frontal Lobes: Functions and Disorders. New York: The
lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Guilford Press, 1999; 522–36.
Neurology 1998;51:1546–54. 31. MILLER BL, IKONTE C, PONTON M et al. A study of the Lund-
12. MCKHANN G, DRACHMAN D, FOLSTEIN M et al. Clinical Manchester research criteria for frontotemporal dementia:
diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS- clinical and single-photon emission CT correlations.
ADRDA Working group under the auspices of the Neurology 1997;48:937–42.
Department of Health and Human Service Task Force on 32. LEVY ML, CUMMINGS JL, FAIRBANKS LA et al. Apathy is not
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1984;34:939–44. depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998;10:314–19.
13. HACHINSKI VC, ILIFF LD, ZILHKA E et al. Cerebral blood flow in 33. KERTESZ A, DAVIDSON W, FOX H. Frontal behavioral
dementia. Arch Neurol 1975;32:632–7. . inventory: diagnostic criteria for frontal lobe dementia. Can
14. ROMAN GC, TATEMICHI TK, ERKINJUNTTI T. Vascular dementia: J Neurol Sci 1997;24:29–36.
diagnostic criteria for research studies. A report of the
NINDS-AIREN international workshop. Neurology 1993;
43:250–60.
15. BARBER R, SNOWDEN JS, CRAUFURD D. Frontotemporal Appendix: Manchester Behavioural Questionnaire
dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: retrospective differentia-
tion using information from informants. J Neurol Neurosurg The appendix lists behavioural symptoms and
Psychiatry 1995;59:61–70. provides a broad definition. For the sake of economy,
16. LEVY ML, MILLER BL, CUMMINGS JL, FAIRBANKS LA, CRAIG A. it does not specify the precise wording adopted in the
Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal dementias. interview. Although the semi-structured nature of the
Behavioural distinctions. Arch Neurol 1996;53:687–90.
17. LOPEZ OL, GONZALEZ MP, BECKER JT, REYNOLDS CF, test instrument allowed for re-phrasing and provision
SUDILOVSKY A, DEKOSKY ST. Symptoms of depression and of examples, to ensure that behavioural symptoms
psychosis in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal demen- were appropriately understood, wording of questions
tia. Exploration of underlying mechanisms. Neuropsychiatry was standardized as far as possible to optimize
Neuropsychol Behavioural Neurol 1996;9:154–61. consistency of administration.
18. CHERRIER MM, MENDEZ MF, PERRYMAN KM, PACHANA NA,
MILLER BL, CUMMINGS JL. Frontotemporal dementia versus
vascular dementia: differential features on mental status
examination. J Am Geriatr Soc 1997;45:579–83. Affect and social behaviour behaviour
19. ROZZINI L, LUSSIGNOLI G, PADOVANI A, BIANCHETTI A,
TRABUCCHI M. Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal demen- Loss of basic Loss of the capacity to show a)
tia. Arch Neurol 1997;54:350. emotions happiness, b) sadness, c) fear, d)
20. SJÖGREN M, WALLIN A, EDMAN A. Symptomatological anger, e) surprise and f) disgust
characteristics distinguish between FTD and vascular demen-
Exaggerated Inappropriate or exaggerated
tia with a dominant frontal lobe syndrome. Int J Geriatr
Psychiatry 1997;12:656–61. emotional displays of a) happiness, b)
21. GREGORY CA, ORRELL M, SAHAKIAN B, HODGES JR. Can display sadness, c) fear, d) anger, e)
frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease be differ- surprise and f) disgust

376
Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia

Loss of Fails be appropriately embar- Crams food Crams and stores food in mouth
embarrassment rassed for self or others without swallowing
Irritability More irritable than formerly Increased Drinks more alcohol than for-
Aggression Verbally or physically aggres- alcohol merly
sive or threatening consumption
Insightfulness Reacts to difficulties by becom- Increased Smokes a) more, b) repetitively.
ing upset, distressed, anxious, smoking c) Seeks out cigarettes
losing confidence or withdrawn Oral Puts inanimate objects in mouth
Excessive Worries excessively about little exploration
worrying things of objects
Selfishness More self-centred, shows lack of Wandering Wanders from home or needs
consideration for others supervision to stop wandering
Disinhibition Laughs inappropriately, makes Pacing Paces home
jokes in bad taste and inap- Hyposexuality Diminished interest in sexual
propriate comments. Acts in activity
disinhibited manner in public Hypersexuality Increased interest in sexual
Social avoidance Avoids social contact with activity, sexual preoccupations
people Hyposomnia Sleeps less than formerly
Seeks out Seeks other people, follows Hypersomnia Sleeps more than formerly
social contact people around
Neglect of Less concerned about cleanli-
hygiene ness. Needs to be prompted to Repetitive behaviours, compulsions and rituals
wash and change clothes Simple motor a) vocalizations (e.g. grunting,
Loss of interest Shows a general loss of interest stereotypies humming)
in hobbies and leisure activities b) facial movements (e.g. lip
smacking, sniffing)
c) upper limb (e.g. hand rub-
Sensory behaviours bing, scratching, picking)
Loss of Shows no awareness of painful d) lower limb (e.g. foot tapping)
awareness of sensations such as heat, sharp Complex a) oral (e.g. reciting same nur-
pain objects, knocks, falls motor routines sery rhyme, singing same song)
Loss of smell Sense of smell lessened b) upper limb (e.g. clapping
Exaggerated Inappropriate or exaggerated same rhythm)
sensory response response to pain, touch, noise, c) dancing complex dance rou-
odours and tastes tine
Exaggerated Inappropriate or exaggerated Paces fixed route Stereotyped pacing, rhythmical,
heat/cold response to heat and cold same direction
response Verbal Has favoured word or phrase
stereotypies that keeps repeating
Verbal Repeats words or phrases imme-
Eating and vegetative behaviours perseverations diately after uttering them
Preference Altered food preference in Repetitive themes Continually repeats or sticks to
for sweet foods favour of sweet foods the same theme in conversation
Preference for Altered food preference in Counts objects Constantly counts things
savoury foods favour of savoury foods Aligns objects Needs constantly to order/align
Food fads More selective and picky than things
formerly Adherence to Sticks to a strict timetable for
Loss of Less selective about what eats. daily routine ordinary daily activities
discrimination Eats indiscriminately Arranges Always arranges belongings in a
Overeats Eats more than formerly belongings in certain way
Eats continually Would carry on eating indefi- same way
if food present nitely if food present Overconcern More/overly concerned about
Steals food Actively takes food from the with cleanliness being neat and clean
from others’ plates of others Clockwatching Constantly watches and worries
plates about the time
Seeks out food Actively looks for extra food, Excessive Preoccupied with checking
e.g. raids the biscuit tin checking things such as lightswitches

377
Bathgate et al.

Excessive Unnecessary attention to detail Lost in Difficulty finding way in own


attention to when carrying out tasks familiar neighbourhood
detail surroundings
Upset if Upset if timetable is interfered Disoriented Difficulty finding way within the
routine disrupted with and activities prevented in home home
Needs to do If has an idea to do something, Fails to Sees objects but does not know
things has to do it immediately recognize objects what they are
immediately Difficulty Slow to find objects placed in
Unusual Eccentric toileting and groom- locating objects front of them
toileting routines ing habits Uses wrong Substitutes inappropriate word
Superstitious Behavioural rituals or supersti- words for correct one
rituals tions, such as avoiding standing Mutism Has stopped talking altogether
on cracks between paving stones

Environmental dependency
Hoarding Collects up, hides or hoards
Hallucinations and delusions
objects
Touches or Touches, picks up and examines Suspiciousness Has become more suspicious/
handles objects objects, puts in pocket less trusting
Echolalia Copies words or sentences Delusions of Has believed possessions to be
uttered by others theft stolen
Echopraxia Imitates actions carried out by False belief Believes other people to be
others that people living in the home
Utilization Uses or mimics use of objects in in home
phenomena environment Misidentification Assumed familiar people to be
Reads Reads aloud notices in street or phenomena someone else
notices aloud public buildings Visual Sees visions, such as people or
hallucinations/ animals
illusions
Cognitively-mediated behaviours Auditory Hears noises or voices not
Mislays objects Puts objects down and cannot hallucinations/ experienced by others
find them illusions

378
Copyright of Acta Neurologica Scandinavica is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like