You are on page 1of 19

VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive Impact of Intellectual Disability on Families


Jan Blacher
University of California, Riverside
Bruce L. Baker
University of California, Los Angeles

Abstract
Understanding positive, as well as negative, impact of a child with mental retardation will
lead to a more balanced view of families and disability. In two studies we examined parents’
perceived positive impact of a child with MR/DD. Study 1 involved 282 young adults
with severe mental retardation; Study 2 involved 214 young children with, or without,
developmental delays. In both studies, positive impact was inversely related to behavior
problems. Moreover, positive impact moderated the relationship between behavior prob-
lems and parenting stress. Also, main and moderating effects of positive impact differed
by parent ethnicity. Latina mothers reported higher positive impact than Anglo mothers
did when the child had MR/DD. These findings are discussed in the context of cultural
beliefs.

The idea that a child with mental retardation even a lack of conceptual clarity as to what is
could impact his or her family positively is a rel- meant by positive impact or related terms. We
atively new one. Only in the past decade or two propose three levels at which one could view, and
have researchers considered ‘‘positive impact of assess, the positive impact of a child on his or her
the child’’ worthy of empirical study (Hastings & family. The first is a view that positive impacts
Taunt, 2002; Summers, Behr, & Turnbull, 1989). can be imputed by the absence of negative ones.
By contrast, for over half a century, published re- In this ‘‘low negative’’ view, positive impacts
search has been focused on negative impacts of would be inferred if the parent reported low scores
children with mental retardation on their families on measures of adverse well-being. The second is
(Blacher & Baker, 2002). Although a more bal- that despite their child’s disability, parents expe-
anced view of families and disability is certainly a rience many of the same joys of child-rearing that
conceptual step forward, there is still little well- are experienced by families of children without a
controlled research on positive impact (Helff & disability. In this ‘‘common benefits’’ view, posi-
Glidden, 1998). In the present paper we report on tive impacts would be demonstrated by evidence
two studies of the positive impact of disability, that parents of a child with disabilities report the
assessing families of young children and young same types and extent of positive experiences re-
adults. We examined parents’ perception of both lated to childrearing that are reported by parents
positive and negative impact on the family in re- of children without disabilities. The third is a view
lationship to characteristics of the child (disability that because of the disability the family experi-
status, behavior problems), the parents (mother ences unique benefits not necessarily experienced
vs. father), and the family’s cultural heritage (An- by parents of children without disabilities. In this
glo vs. Latino). ‘‘special benefits’’ view, positive impacts would be
The disability field lacks theoretical models demonstrated by observations of benefits that are
that fully address this idea of positivity. There is not found in families without a child who has a

330 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

disability. These three perspectives often are con- family dynamics, and opportunities to learn new in-
fused in the literature. formation.
The first view of positive impact, low nega- Although reports from such studies have
tive, is reflected in arguments that one way to con- some heuristic value in identifying themes that
ceptualize positive perceptions is by the absence might be special benefits, any conclusions must
of stress, depression, or other forms of negative be tempered not only because of the positive bias
impact (Taunt & Hastings, 2002). To validate this in the way questions have been asked, but also
view, one would need to show that low scores on because, without adequate control groups, we can-
measures of negative impact relate to high scores not know whether these are, indeed, special ben-
on separate measures of positive impact; this rare- efits. It is important to determine whether these
ly has been done. Although such measures are perceived benefits are unique to disability or are
likely to be modestly negatively correlated, par- also reported by parents raising typically devel-
ents may simultaneously report negative and pos- oping children, as well as the extent to which both
itive perceptions. A two-factor model of caregiv- groups reported such benefits. In one of the few
ing appraisal proposed in the gerontology field studies to contrast parents of children with and
(Lawton, Moss, Kleban, Glicksman, & Ravine, without disabilities, Margalit and Ankonina
1991) and further developed with aging parents of (1991) found that parents of children with dis-
adult children with intellectual disabilities (Pruch- abilities exhibited more negative affect but that
no, Patrick, & Burant, 1996) views positive and positive affect did not differ between the groups.
negative emotional states not as polar opposites The conceptual confusion surrounding posi-
but, rather, as partially independent and as having tive impact is increased by two additional domains
different antecedents. In the present studies we considered in the literature. One is the focus in
examined the relationship between a measure of some studies on positive personality dispositions in
positive impact and typical measures of negative the parent, such as optimism, empowerment, per-
impact. ceived competence, self-mastery, or resilience (Has-
The second, common benefits, view has re- tings and Taunt, 2002; Singer & Powers, 1993).
ceived little specific attention, whereas many studies These personal qualities are important to study
have purported to address the third, special benefits because they may buffer the impact of child chal-
view (Hastings & Taunt, 2002; Sandler & Mistretta, lenges on parents’ well-being (Baker, Blacher, &
1998; Stainton & Besser, 1998). Unfortunately, Olsson, 2005). Yet, they do not measure, or even
study methods usually have confused the two views. necessarily reflect, the positive impact of the
For example, Stainton and Besser, through inter- child; indeed, they may be traits that the parent
views with 17 families, asked only the question: possessed before even having children. There is
‘‘What are the positive impacts you feel your son ample nondisability-related literature to suggest
or daughter with an intellectual disability has had that positive ways of thinking can lead to better
on your family?’’ From qualitative analysis of re- mental health outcomes for the individual (Bower
sponses, the authors advanced nine themes (e.g., & Segerstrom, 2004; Seligman, Steen, Park, & Pe-
source of joy and happiness, source of family unity terson, 2005).
and closeness, source of personal growth and The other, and related, view of positive im-
strength). Sandler and Mistretta (1998) administered pact is found in studies that are focused on effec-
a survey to 50 parents of adults with mental retar- tive strategies for coping with childrearing, such
dation and broadly assessed positive attitudes. These as cognitive coping (Turnbull et al., 1993) or ac-
were endorsed by a high percentage of respondents commodation (Gallimore, Hoots, Weisner, Gar-
and clustered in themes of emotional adjustment nier, & Guthrie, 1996). Here, too, these authors
(e.g., confidence, fulfillment), attitudes toward the did not speak directly about how the child posi-
adult child (e.g., feelings of happiness), positive fam- tively impacts the family, although they accentu-
ily impact (e.g., closeness among members), and per- ated family strengths that may contribute to pos-
sonal growth (e.g., compassion). Similarly, Hastings itive outcomes. Several researchers have combined
and Taunt (2002) asked parents to report on the assessments of positive perceptions and coping
positive impact of the child with the disability on strategies. Hastings and his colleagues assessed
the siblings and on the extended family. Parent re- three domains of positive perceptions (Happiness
sponses included having increased sensitivity and and Fulfillment, Strength and Family Closeness,
tolerance, a changed perspective on life, improved Personal Growth and Maturity) of 41 mothers of

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 331


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

children (aged 4 to 19) with intellectual disability an influence on parents’ positive perceptions.
(Hastings, Allen, McDermott, & Still, 2002). All Many researchers have found that behavior and/
three were positively related to the coping strategy or mental health problems relate to parental well-
of reframing. being, including measures of negative impact and
These various perspectives on ‘‘positive’’ parenting stress, depression, and burden (Baker,
come together in more generic coping models, Blacher, Kopp, & Kraemer, 1997; Baxter, Cum-
where ‘‘positive perception’’ is a core component, ming, & Yiolitis, 2000; Hauser-Cram, Warfield,
particularly as applied to physical stress and health Shonkoff, & Krauss, 2001; McIntyre, Blacher, &
outcomes (e.g., women coping with breast cancer; Baker, 2002). In fact, recently investigators have
Taylor, 1983; Taylor, Lerner, Sherman, Sage, & found that the well-established relationship be-
McDowell, 2000) and psychological distress, such tween intellectual disability and adverse parental
as caregiving for partners with AIDS and bereave- well-being can be accounted for almost entirely
ment (Folkman, 1997). In these instances, positive by child behavior problems (Baker, Blacher,
perceptions serve therapeutically as an adaptive Crnic, & Edelbrock, 2002; Baker et al., 2003;
coping mechanism. Floyd & Gallagher, 1997; Floyd & Phillippe,
In sum, the emerging literature on positive ef- 1993). In the present studies we examine the re-
fects is uncoordinated and inconclusive, with lationship of behavioral challenges to parent-per-
varying conceptions of ‘‘positive,’’ bias in the way ceived positive impact of young children and
questions are posed, the absence of nondisabled young adults.
control families, and a focus on mothers only. A At the family level, a key variable is parent
central problem is the shortage of standardized gender. Most researchers examining the impact of
measures of positive impacts. Measures of parent- disability on parents obtained data from mothers
ing stress or negative impact abound, and, from only, giving an incomplete picture. Fathers usually
the first low negative view discussed above, one have different roles in the family as well as differ-
might consider low scores to be positive. How- ent relationships with the child and different op-
ever, in addition to conceptual limitations (as it is portunities to observe the child’s behavior (Phar-
certainly possible to hold both positive and neg- es, 1996). When both parents have been included
ative perceptions simultaneously), there is a draw- in studies of children with disabilities, mothers
back to many of the measures themselves. The have reported more stress than have fathers in
scales primarily used by family-disability research- practically all families (summarized in Hastings,
ers, such as the widely used Questionnaire on Re- Beck, & Hill, 2005; but see Dyson, 1997). Little
sources and Stress QRS (Holroyd, 1985), the is known, however, about positive perceptions of
QRS-short form (Friedrich, Greenberg, & Crnic, mothers and fathers because most reports of pos-
1983), or the Kansas Inventory of Parental Percep- itive perceptions have involved only mothers (e.g.,
tions (Behr, Murphy & Summers, 1992) have a Hastings et al., 2002; Stainton & Besser, 1998;
strong disability focus and are not appropriate for Taunt & Hastings, 2002). However, Hastings,
use with families who have typical children, pre- Beck, and Hill (2005), using the Positive Contri-
cluding comparisons. In the present studies we butions Scale (Behr et al., 1992) with mothers and
used a measure that includes a positive perception fathers of children with intellectual disability,
scale and is not specific to disability, the Family found that mothers reported slightly, though sig-
Impact Questionnaire FIQ (Donenberg & Baker, nificantly, more positive perceptions of their
1993). Thus, in these two studies we could address child.
the common benefits view of positive impact, but, At the cultural level, although family research
due to the nature of our measure, not the special primarily has involved Euro American families,
benefits view. there is developing interest in examining parent-
Parents’ perception of positive impact is likely ing practices and beliefs about disability cross-cul-
to be related to characteristics of the child but also turally (Lynch & Hanson, 2004; Zuniga, 1998).
must be considered within the broader context of For example, investigators have found less per-
the family and the culture. From among the many ceived burden and more satisfaction (i.e., a posi-
child, family, and cultural variables that might in- tive contribution) from providing care to a child
fluence parental perceptions, we focus here on with intellectual disabilities for African American
three. At the child level, behavioral challenges, compared to Caucasian mothers (Valentine,
apart from cognitive disability, are likely to have McDermott, & Anderson, 1998). Some reasons

332 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

for these positive perceptions include the value child and family: preschool and the transition to
attributed to intergenerational family support adulthood.
(Cox, Marshall, Mandleco, & Olsen, 2003) and Five questions were of primary interest. The
the protective factor of spirituality or religious first two relate to the conflicting views of positive
connectedness (Billingsley, 1992; Rogers-Dulan & impact that we have considered. The others relate
Blacher, 1995). Our analysis of culture contrasts to the relationship of salient child, parent, and
the perceptions of parents in the dominant Unit- cultural attributes to expressed positive impact.
ed States culture, Caucasians (herein referred to as First, how do positive impact and negative
Anglos), and the fastest growing cultural group in impact measures relate? If these measures have a
the country, namely, Latinos. Depression symp- strong negative relationship, the ‘‘low negative’’
toms have been found to be high in Latina moth- view of positive impact garners some support.
ers of low-functioning children with disabilities Conversely, if the measures have only a modest
(Blacher, Shapiro, Lopez, Diaz, & Fusco, 1997). relationship, this is supportive of the ‘‘two-factor’’
However, positive impact reported by Latina view, wherein positive and negative perceptions
mothers of young adults in a previous study was are largely independent. Second, do parents of
also high, and equally so regardless of the child’s children with and without disabilities differ on a
diagnosis (i.e., autism, Down syndrome, cerebral measure of positive impact that is not disability-
palsy, undifferentiated mental retardation) (Blach- specific? To the extent that the groups do not dif-
er & McIntyre, 2006). To further our understand- fer, the findings would support the common ben-
ing of positive perceptions of disability, we must efits view that some positive perceptions reported
consider cultural context (Hodapp, Glidden, & by parents of children with disabilities are not spe-
Kaiser, 2005). We designed the studies in the pre- cial but are shared by parents of children without
sent paper to replicate and extend these initial disabilities. Third, does positive impact relate to
findings, contrasting positive perceptions in the extent of child behavioral challenges? We ex-
mothers and fathers of children with and without amined whether positive perceptions also relate to
disability and considering the buffering role of the extent of child and young adult behavior
cultural group on the relationship between behav- problems. Fourth, does positive impact relate to
ior problems and positive impact. parent gender or to cultural origins? In Study 2
In this paper we report on two studies of par- we examined whether mothers and fathers differ
ents’ perceived positive impact of a child with dis- in their perceptions of positive impact, and in
abilities. These studies are focused on positive par- both studies we examined how positive impact is
enting experiences that parents of children with expressed within families of Anglo and Latino or-
mental retardation may share with parents of typ- igins. Fifth, does positive impact moderate the es-
ically developing children. Data were drawn from tablished relationship between child behavior
two samples of families. The first, obtained from problems and parenting stress? Hastings and
the University of California, Riverside Families Taunt (2002) have suggested a functional perspec-
Project, includes 282 young adults with moderate tive of positive perceptions, in that they aid in
to profound mental retardation; the second, from coping with stressors and thus buffer adverse out-
the Collaborative Family Study at Penn State, comes. There is related evidence that the person-
UCLA, and UC, Riverside, includes 214 young ality trait of optimism was a buffer for mothers,
children with mild to moderate developmental de- whereby child behavior problems had the greatest
lays or typical development. Our intent was not negative effect on well-being for mothers who
to provide direct comparison of the two samples were least optimistic (Baker et al., 2005). Thus, we
(the participants were different in age and level of hypothesized that parents low in perceptions of
functioning). Rather, the two contexts provide an positive impact would be more negatively affected
opportunity to examine the robustness of the phe- by child or young adult behavioral challenges
nomenon of positive impact in families who have than parents with higher positive perceptions.
children with mental retardation and to garner in-
formation about the contribution of specific child
STUDY 1: FAMILIES PROJECT
and parent characteristics in assessment of positiv-
ity. These two samples, with overlapping mea- YOUNG ADULTS
sures, afforded us an opportunity to examine pos- Study 1 was focused on three of the aims (1,
itive impact at two critical times in the life of the 3, 5) identified above. First, we considered the low

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 333


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

negative view by examining the relationship be- stitutional Review Board. Table 1 shows selected
tween measures of well-being and positive percep- demographics by sample (Anglo vs. Latino). Ma-
tions. Second, we examined whether mothers’ per- ternal age averaged 49 years (range ⫽ 31 to 70).
ception of positive and negative impact related to Biological mothers were the primary respondents
the degree of young adult behavior and/or mental in 92.2% of families; thus, we refer to parents as
health problems. Third, we examined the main mothers throughout. The majority were married
and moderator effects of positive impact in the (68.6%) and worked outside of the home (63.1%).
relationship between child behavior problems and The Anglo sample was significantly higher than
maternal well-being. the Latina sample on measures of education, in-
come, and health. The majority of the young
adults were male (56.7%) and still attending
Method
school (58.5%). Although most young adults were
Participants still living in the family home (89.8%), signifi-
Participants were mothers of 282 young adults cantly more (though still few) in the Anglo sample
with moderate to profound mental retardation. were living in outside residences; their parents
For the present study, the mean age of the young were still very involved. The young adult demo-
adults was 20.3 years (range ⫽ 16 to 26). There graphics (e.g., age, ambulation, adaptive behavior,
were 150 Anglo and 132 Latina mothers. A sub- maladaptive behavior, and mental health) were
sample of 103 of these mothers participated in the quite similar across the Anglo and Latina samples.
study reported by McIntyre et al. (2002), who re- The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow,
ported on an earlier wave of data collection. All Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) mean standard scores for
procedures were approved by the University’s In- both samples were about five SDs below the me-

Table 1. Study 1: Family/Mother and Young Adult Characteristics by Sample


Anglo Latino
Characteristic Mean/% Mean/% t/⌾2a
Family/Mother
Mean age in years (SD) 48.5 (5.7) 49.5 (8.1) 1.12
Education (% some college) 76.7 28.8 62.94***
Health (% reported good/excellent) 86.6 53.0 36.51***
Maternal status (% biological) 90.7 93.9 .64
Maternal employment (% employed) 68.0 57.6 2.84
Marital status (% married) 75.3 61.4 5.75*
Family Income (% ⱖ $40,000/year) 73.3 19.7 78.76***
Young adult
Mean age in years 20.4 (2.3) 20.2 (2.8) .70
Gender (% male) 57.3 56.1 .01
Ambulation (% ambulatory) 76.0 75.8 .00
School status (% exited school) 41.3 41.7 .00
Residential status (% out–of–home) 16.0 3.8 10.06**
Mean Adaptive Behavior Standard Score 25.6 (10.4) 24.2 (8.8) 1.19
Mean maladaptive behaviorb ⫺13.2 (10.5) ⫺12.7 (10.9) .38
Mean maladaptive behavior (% mod–high, SIB–R) 21.3 25.2 .39
Mean mental healthc 6.8 (5.1) 7.7 (5.7) 1.38
Mental health (% at-risk)c 33.3 41.9 1.65
Note. Anglo n ⫽ 150, Latino n ⫽ 132)
a
t is used when standard deviations are reported. bScales of Independent Behavior–Revised, General Maladaptive Index.
c
Reiss Screen Total.
*p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01. ***p ⬍ .001.

334 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

dian (100). This was a very low-functioning group The Positive Impact scale has six items en-
of young adults. dorsed on a 0 to 3 scale and one item on a 0 to
6 scale. Thus, the scale score can range from 0 to
24. Each item begins, ‘‘Compared to children,
Procedure
Participants were recruited for an ongoing and parents with children the same age as my
longitudinal study through Southern California child’’ followed by, for example: (a) I enjoy the
Regional Centers, agencies that provide case man- time I spend with my child more; (b) My child
agement services to individuals with mental retar- brings out feelings of happiness and pride more;
dation (see Blacher & McIntyre, 2006, as well as (c) My child makes me feel more energetic; and
McIntyre et al., 2002, for details on recruitment, (d) My child makes me feel more confident as a
informed consent, and interviewer training). Data parent. Internal reliability for mothers in the Fam-
were collected during in-home interviews with ilies Project sample (Study 1) was an alpha of .83.
mothers. All interviews were conducted in the pre- Internal reliabilities in the Collaborative Family
ferred language of the family (English or Spanish); Study sample (Study 2) for mothers and fathers at
most interviewers, and all interviewers for the La- child age 3 years were .81 for both. At 4 years,
tina sample, were bilingual and Latino. Families they were each .86; and at 5 years, .86 and .85,
received honoraria for their participation. respectively. Positive Impact also had moderate to
high stability over time, with Pearson rs ranging
from .60 to .75 across ages 3 to 5 years.
Measures An additional measure of parent well-being
The FIQ (Donenberg & Baker, 1993) is a 50- was the Center for Epidemiological Studies-De-
item measure focused on the child’s impact on pression scale CES-D (Radloff, 1977). This is a
the family compared to the impact other children widely used 20-item self-report of depressive
his/her age have on their families. For use with symptoms. Alpha in the present sample was .90.
parents of young adults, all references to ‘‘chil- The Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised
dren’’ were changed to ‘‘young adults.’’ Parents Problem Behavior Scale SIB-R (Bruininks,
endorse items on a 4-point scale ranging from not Woodcock, Weatherman, & Hill, 1996) is an
at all to very much. The FIQ was developed to
8-item instrument yielding the General Maladap-
address three limitations shared by most parent
tive Index, with a possible range of ⫹10 to ⫺74.
stress measures used by family researchers. First,
Levels of severity are delineated in the adminis-
many measures are focused on difficult child be-
tration manual as ⫹10 to ⫺10 ⫽ normal; ⫺11 to
haviors or skill deficits and infer parental stress
⫺20 ⫽ marginally serious; ⫺21 to ⫺30 ⫽ moder-
from these rather than asking parents directly
ately serious; ⫺31 to ⫺40 ⫽ serious; ⫺41 and be-
about the impact their child has on them. Second,
low ⫽ very serious (Bruininks et al., 1996). We
most measures are focused exclusively on the neg-
ative impact of the child on the family without have used a score of ⫺21 and lower to indicate a
also assessing the child’s positive impacts. Third, clinical range on this measure (McIntyre et al.,
most stress measures are applicable only to fami- 2002). The manual provides sufficient evidence
lies with children who have mental retardation/ for reliability (e.g., test–retest reliability r ⫽ .86;
developmental disabilities or only to families with Cronbach alpha ⫽ .80) and validity.
typically developing children. The Reiss Screen for Maladaptive Behavior,
There are six scale scores on the FIQ that 2nd ed. (Reiss, 1994) was obtained from 260 of
measure Negative Impact on Feelings About Par- the 282 families. The Reiss Screen is a 38-item
enting, Social Relationships, Finances, Marriage screening tool used to identify mental health
and Siblings, and Positive Impact on Feelings problems in adolescents and adults across the full
About Parenting. In the present study, we utilized range of mental retardation. Items describe dis-
a Negative Impact composite of the first two sub- crete behavior categories with a 3-point response
scales (20 items) as well as the Positive Impact scale of (0) no problem, (1) problem, and (2) major
scale (7 items). The Negative Impact composite, problem. We have used a total score of 9 or above
which has been used in samples with and without to indicate clinically significant risk of mental ill-
mental retardation, has good reliability and valid- ness, as suggested by Reiss (1994). The manual
ity (Baker, Heller, & Henker, 2000; Baker et al., reports adequate total score internal consistency,
2002; Blacher & McIntyre, 2006). alphas ⬎ .70, and sufficient evidence for validity

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 335


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

based on clinical and nonclinical samples (Reiss, Anglo mothers (Ms ⫽ 17.6 and 12.0, respective-
1994). ly), t(267) ⫽ 9.76, p ⬍ .001 (Blacher & McIntyre,
2006). Here, we examined the relationship of
Results young adult behavioral challenges to negative and
positive impact separately for Latina and Anglo
Positive Impact Inferred From Low-Negative mothers. Three levels of young adult behavior and
We first examined the view that positive im- mental health problems were determined from
pact can be inferred from low scores on measures scores on the Reiss Screen and SIB-R. Young
of negative impact or well-being. Positive impact adults in the nonclinical range on both scales (see
correlated inversely with negative impact, r ⫽ Method) received a score of 0 (n ⫽143). Young
⫺.45, p ⬍ .001. To examine this further, we di- adults in the clinical range on either scale received
vided positive impact and negative impact into a score of 1 (n ⫽ 49), and those in the clinical
thirds and compared them in cross-tabs. Of the range on both scales received a score of 2 (n ⫽
87 cases in the lowest third on negative impact, 53). Figure 1 shows FIQ Negative and Positive Im-
only 49% fell into the highest third on positive pact scores by the Behavior Problems/Mental
impact. Thus, although there was a modest cor- Health Index as well as the significance of differ-
relation between positive and negative impact, if ences. One-way ANOVAs showed that negative
only the low third of negative impact were used impact increased significantly with increasing Be-
for determining positive impact, fully half of the havior Problems/Mental Health scores in both the
sample would have been misclassified. Moreover, Anglo and Latina subsamples. Also, positive im-
positive impact was not significantly correlated pact decreased significantly, especially from low
with depression, r ⫽ ⫺.09. to high Behavior Problems/Mental Health scores,
in both the Anglo and Latina subsamples.
Positive Impact and Young Adult Behavioral
Challenges Positive Impact as a Buffer
We previously reported significantly higher We further considered whether perceiving
positive impact scores for Latina mothers than greater positive impact of the young adult would

Figure 1. Study 1: Anglo and Latina mothers’ reports of negative and positive impacts of young adults’
behavior/mental health problem (BPMH).

336 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

moderate, or buffer, the strong relationship be- Conclusions


tween behavior/mental health problems and Study 1 findings indicate that within this sam-
mothers’ well-being. We conducted a regression ple of young adults with moderate to profound
on the FIQ Negative Impact score with the whole mental retardation, mothers’ perceptions of posi-
sample. In Step 1, we entered, as control variables, tive impact were highest when young adults did
the two demographic variables that correlated sig- not have behavior and/or mental health problems
nificantly with negative impact: young adult gen- high enough to be in the clinical range. Moreover,
der and Vineland Standard Score. These account- even after accounting for young adult behavioral
ed for 7.8% of the variance in negative impact. In challenges, mothers with higher perceptions of
Step 2, we entered the Reiss Screen Total score, positive impact reported less stress (main effect).
which accounted for 20.7% additional variance, F Perceptions of positive impact had a somewhat
(change) ⫽ 69.69, p ⬍ .001. In Step 3, we entered greater buffering effect when behavioral challeng-
the Positive Impact score, which accounted for es were high (moderation effect). Although Latina
12.5% additional variance, F (change) ⫽ 50.68, p mothers reported higher positive impact scores
⬍ .001. In Step 4, after converting the Reiss Total than Anglo mothers, positive impact related sim-
and Positive Impact to z scores, we entered the ilarly to young adult behavior and/or mental
product of these two z scores as a test for mod- health problems in both cultural groups.
eration; although this accounted for only 1.3%
additional variance, it was statistically significant,
F (change) ⫽ 5.21, p ⫽ .02. Higher positive im-
STUDY 2: COLLABORATIVE FAMILY
pact somewhat buffered the increase in stress that STUDY YOUNG CHILDREN
accompanies greater young adult mental health In this study we investigated all five aims
problems. This is shown graphically in Figure 2. identified above at a much earlier point in the
We repeated this analysis separately within the family lifespan. In this younger sample, there was
Anglo and Latina subsamples; positive impact ac- a typically developing contrast group, and data
counted for significant additional variance in were obtained from mothers and fathers. First, we
both, but the interaction did not reached signifi- examined the low negative view and by contrast-
cance in either. ing views of parents of children with and without

Figure 2. Study 1. Positive impact as moderator of the relationship between young adult mental disorder
and parenting stress. Square, high; triangle, moderate; and circle, low positive impact.

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 337


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

delay, the common benefits view. Next, we ex- the research center. General selection criteria were
amined whether parents’ perception of positive that the child be between 30 and 40 months of
and negative impact related to child problem be- age and not be diagnosed with autism. Participat-
haviors, to parent gender, and to culture. Finally, ing children were classified as developmentally de-
we examined the main and moderator effects of layed (n ⫽ 82), borderline (n ⫽ 10), or nondelayed
positive impact in the relationship between child (n ⫽ 122). Delayed group families were recruited
behavior problems and parent well-being for both primarily through community agencies that serve
mothers and fathers. persons with developmental disabilities. Further
selection criteria were that the child score between
30 and 75 on the Bayley Scales of Infant Devel-
Method
opment II (see Measures). Borderline group chil-
Participants dren had Bayley II scores of 76 to 84. Delayed
Participants were 214 families, recruited to and borderline children were combined in the
participate in a longitudinal study of young chil- present analyses and designated as ‘‘delayed.’’
dren from ages 3 to 5 years, with samples drawn Nondelayed group families were recruited primar-
from Southern California and Central Pennsyl- ily through local preschools and daycare programs
vania. This Collaborative Family Study is based that serve the same catchment areas as the agen-
at three universities: Penn State University, Uni- cies that support delayed group children. Further
versity of California, Los Angeles, and University selection criteria were that the child (a) score 85
of California, Riverside. All procedures were ap- or above on the Bayley II and (b) not have been
proved by the Institutional Review Boards of the born prematurely or have had any developmental
three universities involved. The present sample disability.
was comprised of all families for whom data were Table 2 shows demographics separately for
available on the primary measures across child the children with developmental delays and those
ages 3, 4, and 5; this constitutes 92% of the orig- with no delays and their parents. Child age at in-
inally recruited sample. take averaged 35.2 months (SD ⫽ 2.9) and the
School and agency personnel mailed bro- majority were boys (59%). Most children in the
chures describing the study to families who met delayed group had not received a specific diag-
selection criteria, and interested parents phoned nosis; when there was one, the most frequent was

Table 2. Study 2: Child and Family Demographics by Delay Status


Delayed No delay
(n ⫽ 92) (n ⫽ 122)
Variable Mean/% Mean/% t/⌾2a
Child
Mean age at testingb (SD) 35.6 (2.9) 34.9 (3.2) 1.73
Gender (% boys) 66.3 53.3 3.16
Mean BSID II: MDIc 60.3 (13.0) 104.2 (11.6) 25.97***
Parent and family
Marital status (% married) 80.4 88.5 2.10
Mean mother age (years) 32.5 (6.2) 34.1 (5.6) 1.93
Mean mother education (grade) 14.3 (2.4) 15.8 (2.5) 4.30***
Mother employment (%) 48.9 60.7 2.47
Mother race (% Caucasian) 58.7 66.4 1.02
Mean father education (grade)d 14.0 (2.7) 15.7 (2.9) 4.22***
Family income (%$50K⫹) 41.3 57.9 5.08*
a
ts are used when standard deviations are reported. bIn months. cBayley Scales of Infant Development, Mental Devel-
opment Index. dNs ⫽ 80 for the delayed group and 116 for the nondelayed group, respectively.
*p ⬍ .05. ***p ⬍ .001.

338 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

Down syndrome or cerebral palsy. Most mothers of 15. Bayley (1993) reported high short-term
(85%) were married because recruitment had ini- test–retest reliability for the MDI, r ⫽ .91.
tially focused on intact families. Mother’s race/ Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale IV (Thorndike,
ethnicity was predominantly Caucasian (63%) or Hagan, & Sattler, 1986). Classification of children
Latina (21%). Mothers and fathers averaged about as delayed or not delayed at 60 months was based
15 years of school, and 51% of families had an on the 60-month administration of the Stanford
annual income of $50,000 or more. Binet, administered in an assessment session at
The two status groups did not differ on the the clinic.
child attributes shown in Table 2. except on Bay- Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1½–5 CBCL
ley II scores. On parent and family attributes, the (Achenbach, 2000). This new version of the widely
highest grade completed in school was higher for used CBCL is aimed at the preschool years. It has
mothers and fathers in the nondelayed group as 99 items that indicate child problems, listed in
was family income. However, because parent ed- alphabetical order (from ‘‘aches and pains without
ucation and family income were unrelated to the medical cause’’ to ‘‘worries’’) and one ‘‘other’’
variable of interest namely, positive impact item. The respondent indicates, for each item,
these were not covaried in analyses. whether it is not true (0), somewhat or sometimes true
(1), or very true or often true (2), now or within the
past 2 months. We utilized only total problem
Assessment Procedure
scores; these are converted to T scores with a
The data examined in this study were ob-
mean of 50 and an SD of 10. Total score alphas
tained in two ways. The initial measures of child
for the present sample at the initial assessment
developmental level and problem behaviors were
were .94 for both mothers and fathers. For some
obtained at a home intake assessment session con-
analyses, behavior problem groups were deter-
ducted when the child was between 30 and 40
mined from parents’ CBCL Total T scores follow-
months of age. Prior to this session, parents had
ing Achenbach’s (1991) suggested cut-offs. These
completed a telephone intake interview with staff
were designated as low (T score ⬍ 60, indicating
and had received an informed consent form. Two
nonclinical range) and high (T score ⱖ 60, indi-
trained research assistants visited the family to re-
cating borderline or clinical range).
view procedures, obtain informed consent, and
administer the Bayley II to the child. Mother, and
father if present, completed a demographic ques- Results
tionnaire and the Child Behavior Checklist
(CBCL, see discussion below). The CBCL was ob- Positive Impact Inferred From Low Negative
tained again at home assessment sessions when We examined the view that positive impact
the child was 48 and 60 months of age. The FIQ can be inferred from low scores on measures of
(see Study 1) was part of a packet completed dur- well-being. At child age 36 months, positive im-
ing each annual assessment. This packet also in- pact correlated inversely with negative impact, r
cluded the CES-D (see Study 1); alpha for the ⫽ ⫺.38, p ⬍ .001. Positive impact and negative
present sample was .89. Families were paid an impact were each divided into thirds and com-
honorarium for participation. pared in cross-tabs. Of the 75 cases in the lowest
third on negative impact, 48% fell into the highest
third on positive impact. Here, too, although
Measures
there was a modest correlation between positive
Bayley Scales of Infant Development II (Bayley,
and negative impact, if only the low third of neg-
1993). Classification of children as delayed or not
ative impact were used for determining positive
delayed at 36 and 48 months was based on the
impact, about half of the sample would have been
36-month Bayley II. This is a widely used assess-
misclassified. Positive impact was not significantly
ment of mental and motor development in chil-
correlated with depression, r ⫽ ⫺.08.
dren aged 1 to 42 months. It was administered in
the child’s home, with the mother present. In
most cases, there was a primary examiner and an Positive Impact by Child Delay Status and
assistant. Only mental development items were Behavior Problems
administered; the Mental Development Index Child behavior problems were designated as
(MDI) is normed with a mean of 100 and an SD low (nonclinical range, see Method) or high (bor-

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 339


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

Table 3. Positive Impact Reported by Mothers and Fathers of Young Children at Ages 3, 4, and 5
Years by Delay Status (DS) and Behavior Problems (BP)
Delayed No delay
Parent/
Child agea Low BP Hi BP Low BP Hi BP F (DS) F (BP) F (DS ⫻ BP)
Mothers
3 15.7 14.3 16.4 15.0 0.72 2.62 0.00
4 16.3 13.1 16.3 12.9 0.02 14.52*** 0.01
5 15.2 14.7 16.5 12.7 0.15 5.48* 3.22
Fathers
3 17.7 15.2 17.2 14.2 0.80 11.27*** 0.14
4 16.4 14.5 17.1 15.6 1.03 3.72⫹ 0.05
5 16.7 14.2 16.8 14.0 0.00 6.69** 0.02
Child age in years.
a

⫹p ⬍.10. *p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01. ***p ⬍ .001.

derline/clinical range) at each assessment. Sepa- Neither the time (child age) main effect nor the
rate 2 (behavior problem level) ⫻ 2 (delay status) Parent Gender ⫻ Time interaction was significant.
ANOVAS were conducted on positive impact, for
mothers and fathers, at each assessment (child age Positive Impact by Culture
3, 4, and 5 years). Table 3 shows mean scores and We have seen in the whole sample that par-
F values for delay status, behavior problems, and ents of children with developmental delays and
the interaction. Delay status had no relationship those with children without delays reported nearly
to positive impact in any analysis, a finding that identical positive impact. This finding, however,
is considered further in our examination of cul- could mask a difference between cultural groups.
ture. Clinical-level child behavior problems, how- Table 4 shows means for Anglo and Latino par-
ever, were associated with lower positive impact
in every comparison; for mothers and fathers, the
Table 4. Positive Impact at Child Ages 3, 4, and
relationship was statistically significant in four of
5 Years by Parent and Culture
the six ANOVAs. The Delay Status ⫻ Behavior
Problems interaction term was not significant in Parent Age 3 Age 4 Age 5
any analysis.
Mother 15.6 15.7 15.4
Father 16.8 16.4 16.3
Positive Impact by Parent Gender t (paired) 2.96 1.49 2.00
There was a moderate relationship between df 189 181 177
mother and father positive impact scores at each P .003 ns .047
of the three assessments, rs ⫽ .31, .32, and .43,
respectively, all ps ⬍ .001. Correlations of positive Culture
impact scores across assessments were moderately Anglo mother 15.1 15.3 15.0
high for mothers and for fathers, ranging from .60 Latina mother 17.4 16.4 16.5
to .75, all ps ⬍ .001. Positive impact in the com- t (independent) 2.90 1.23 1.66
bined sample by parent gender (mother, father) is df 178 175 178
shown in Table 4; at each time-point, fathers ex- P .004 ns .098
pressed greater positive impact than did mothers. Anglo father 16.4 15.9 15.6
A repeated measures ANOVA on positive impact Latino father 17.7 18.0 18.3
by parent gender (mother, father) and child age
t (independent) 1.24 2.75 3.20
(3, 4, and 5), showed a significant main effect for
df 154 149 148
parent gender (Ms for mothers and fathers ⫽
P ns .008 .002
15.39 and 16.31, respectively, F ⫽ 5.90, p ⫽ .016.

340 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

ents across the three time points, with consistently or interaction effects of time approached signifi-
higher scores for Latino parents. The relationship cance in mother or father analyses.
between delay status and positive impact was an-
alyzed further within these two cultural subgroups
that corresponded to those in Study 1. Because Positive Impact as Moderator of Child
mothers and fathers in some families had different Behavior Problems and Parenting Stress
ethnicities, we ran analyses separately for each par- We have found a strong relationship be-
ent. Two repeated measures ANOVAs were run tween child behavior problems and both moth-
on Positive Impact ⫻ Delay Status (delays, no de- ers’ and fathers’ perceived negative impact, or
lays) and ethnicity (Anglo, Latino) across child parenting stress, at every assessment point. We
ages 3, 4, and 5. Latina mothers (n ⫽ 44) reported further examined whether positive impact had a
higher positive impact than did Anglo mothers (n moderator effect on this relationship, through re-
⫽ 135), but the difference did not reach statistical gression analyses on the FIQ Negative Impact
significance. There was, however, a significant in- score at each assessment. For each analysis, we
teraction between culture and delay group, first identified demographic variables that corre-
F(1, 173) ⫽ 4.23, p ⫽ .04. Figure 3 shows the lated with negative impact and entered these con-
marginal means. In the no delay group, expression trol variables as Step 1. Step 2 was behavior prob-
of positive impact was about the same for Anglo lems (CBCL Total score) and Step 3 was FIQ
and Latina mothers. In the developmental delay Positive Impact. For Step 4, following the same
group, Anglo mothers expressed lower positive procedures as in Study I, we converted the CBCL
impact than did Anglo mothers in the no delay and Positive Impact scores to z scores and en-
group, whereas Latina mothers expressed higher tered the product of these two z scores as a test
positive impact than did Latina mothers in the no for moderation. For mothers at child age 3, these
delay group. variables accounted for 56.1% of the variance in
Figure 3 also shows results for fathers, which negative impact. The control variables (child IQ,
were highly similar to those for mothers. Latino child gender, and mother education) accounted
fathers reported marginally more positive impact for 10.1%, F(3, 209) ⫽ 7.85, p ⬍ .001, behavior
than did Anglo fathers, F(1, 138) ⫽ 3.69, p ⫽ problems accounted for an additional 37.4%,
.057. There was a similar interaction between cul- F(change, 1, 208) ⫽ 147.95, p ⬍ .001, and pos-
ture and delay group, although only marginally itive impact accounted for an additional 6.7%,
significant, F(1, 138) ⫽ 3.68, p ⫽ .057. No main F(change, 1, 207) ⫽ 30.40, p ⬍ .001. The inter-

Figure 3. Study 2. Delay status and positive impact in Anglo and Latino families.

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 341


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

action term, indicating moderation, accounted pact accounted for significant variance in negative
for an additional 1.9%, F(change; 1, 206) ⫽ 8.77, impact beyond child behavior problems at child
p ⫽ .003. In the final model, behavior problems, ages 3, 4, and 5 years, all ps ⬍ .001. Positive im-
p ⬍ .001, positive impact, p ⬍ .001, the inter- pact was also a significant moderator at ages 4 and
action term, p ⬍ .01, and child IQ, p ⬍ .05, were 5; the interaction term was significant at p ⬍ .001.
all significant. At 36 months, however, the interaction was not
At child ages 4 and 5 years, results for mother significant.
negative impact were highly similar, with 58.4%
and 65.3% of the variance in negative impact ac-
counted for. The control variables (in these anal-
Discussion
yses, child IQ and maternal health) accounted for We examined the positive impact of a child
12.9% and 15.7%, respectively, both ps ⬍ .001. with an intellectual disability on mothers and fa-
Behavior problems accounted for an additional thers in two samples of families: those with (a)
38.2% and 37.9%, both F(changes), p ⬍ .001. Pos- preschool children (with borderline, mild, or
itive impact accounted for an additional 4.2% and moderate intellectual disabilities or without intel-
7.3%, both F(changes), p ⬍ .001. The interaction lectual disabilities) and (b) young adults with se-
term, indicating moderation, accounted for an ad- vere intellectual disabilities. We proposed three
ditional 3.0% at age 4, F change (1, 199) ⫽ 14.37, ways one could conceptualize positive impact: as
p ⬍ .001, and 4.4% at age 5, F change (1, 208) ⫽ low negative outcomes, as common benefits (the
26.49 p ⬍ .001. All of the interactions reported same child-rearing benefits enjoyed by parents of
evidenced the same pattern of effect as that shown children without disabilities), and as special ben-
in Figure 4, which illustrates the moderation effect efits (derived because of the disability).
at age 5 graphically, with the main and moderator Our first question was, Can positive impact
variables centered (Aiken & West, 1991). be inferred from low scores on measures of neg-
We repeated these three regression analyses ative impact or well-being? Findings from these
for fathers, with very similar results. Positive im- two very different samples indicated that inferring

Figure 4. Study 2. Positive impact moderates the relationship between child behavior problems and
parenting stress (mothers, at child age 3 years). Square, high; triangle, moderate; and circle, low positive
impact.

342 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

positive impact from low negative impact would we focused on parent gender and cultural back-
be wrong as often as it would be correct, thus ground. Within the preschool-age sample, reports
providing little support for the low negative per- by mother and fathers of their perceptions of pos-
spective. Moreover, positive impact was unrelated itive impact of parenting were not appreciably dif-
to another measure of well-being, depression, in ferent, although fathers reported significantly
both samples. Although low scores on measures higher scores. As noted earlier, Hastings, Kor-
of psychopathology are, of course, desirable, one shoff‘ et al. (2005), using a different assessment of
must be cautious in interpreting these as related positive perceptions, found that mothers reported
to disability. Even when parents of children with slightly higher scores. To date, perceptions of pos-
and those without disability differ on these mea- itive impact cannot be said to systematically differ
sures, the difference is primarily related to child by parent gender.
behavior problems, not the disability per se (Baker Regarding culture, we sought to replicate and
et al., 2002). Also, as we have seen here, low neg- extend our previous finding in the sample of
ative scores do not strongly predict high scores on young adults with severe disability: that Latina
a more direct measure of positive impact. mothers reported much higher positive impact
Our second question was, Would there be than did Anglo mothers. We contrasted Latino
group differences on a measure designed to assess and Anglo subsamples from the young child sam-
common benefits? To address this question, we ple and replicated this finding with both mothers
utilized a Positive Impacts scale that is equally ap- and fathers. Within the whole young child sam-
propriate for parents of children with and without ple, we had found no relationship between delay
disability. In Study 2, parents of preschool-aged status and positive impact. However, an impor-
children with developmental delays or no delays tant difference was masked by combining Latino
differed significantly on a measure of negative im- and Anglo parents because there was a significant
pact, or stress. We had previously found that in relationship between culture and delay group.
this sample positive impact at child age 3 years With typically developing children, Anglo and La-
did not differ between families of delayed and typ- tino mothers and fathers reported about the same
ically developing children (Baker et al., 2002), and extent of positive impact. However, with devel-
in the present analyses we found no delay status opmentally delayed children, Anglo mothers and
differences at ages 4 and 5 years as well. The ex- fathers both reported lower positive impact (as ex-
pression of common benefits of parenting across pected), but Latino mothers and fathers both re-
families with and without disability, despite dif- ported higher positive impact. Although this De-
ferences in negative impact, lends support to the lay Status ⫻ Culture interaction was of only bor-
two-factor model of caregiving appraisal (Pruchno derline statistical significance, these results were
et al., 1996) and tempers the exclusively negative consistent with the very large cultural group dif-
perspective that has characterized earlier literature ference in the sample of young adults, all of
on families and disability. whom had developmental disabilities.
Our third question of interest was, Do par- To understand Latino families’ increase in
ents’ reports of positive impact vary by child be- positive views in the face of disability, consider
havioral challenges? In the present studies, par- this expression ‘‘No hay mal que por bien no ven-
ents’ reports of positive impact were significantly ga,’’ which translates ‘‘there is nothing bad out of
inversely related to young adult behavior/mental which good cannot come’’ (Zuniga, 1992, p. 151).
health problems and to child behavior problems This positive expression reflects cultural beliefs re-
at all three assessments. This is consistent with lated to disability, family, and religion. For ex-
other findings, noted above, that child behavioral ample, Latina and non-Latina mothers may hold
challenges accounted for more variance in nega- some different beliefs about what are considered
tive indicators of parental well-being than did dis- acceptable child-rearing practices. Juarez (1985)
ability status in the present samples (Baker et al., noted that, in some Latino families, it might be
2002, 2003; McIntyre et al., 2002) and others deemed acceptable for a nondelayed preschooler
(Floyd & Gallagher, 1997; Poehlmann, Clements, to drink from a baby bottle or a preteen to still
Abbeduto, & Farsad, 2005). sit in her or his mother’s lap. Thus, when the son
Our fourth question was, Which factors be- or daughter has a disability, especially a grown
yond the child might contribute to positive per- ‘‘child’’ of age 20 or more whose caregiving needs
ceptions of parenting? To address this question, resemble those of a young child, the daily care

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 343


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

burden may be perceived as less stressful or drain- theless, there is potential usefulness for including
ing for Latino families than for Anglo ones. In- this aspect in parent education or intervention
deed, a lack of push for independence of growing programs, particularly where there is an emphasis
children is characteristic of some cultures, even on cognitions about parenting.
more so when there is a child or young adult with Our final question was, Do parents’ percep-
a severe disability (Rueda, Monzo, Shapiro, Go- tion of positive contributions of the child with
mez, & Blacher, 2005). Religious belief systems disability buffer the established relationship be-
also may incline Latina mothers toward positive tween child behavior problems and parenting
reframing, providing families with a spiritual stress? In both samples, positive impact had a
framework for understanding the disability and main effect relationship to parenting stress; moth-
for providing hope (Zuniga, 1998). ers and fathers who perceived greater positive im-
Latina mothers may report more positive im- pact reported lower parenting stress. Moreover,
pact of their child with disabilities on parenting, there were significant Positive Impact ⫻ Behav-
in part because of the importance of the parent– ioral Challenge interactions in the young adult
child relationship in Latino families and the sample and in the preschool sample for mothers
mother’s own views of her child’s characteristics. at all three time points and for fathers at two of
One interpretation of mother’s positive percep- three time points. Thus, when child-rearing chal-
tions reflects attribution theory. In an earlier study lenges were lower, there was little relationship be-
of 149 Latina mothers of children with develop- tween positive views of parenting and experienced
mental disabilities, Chavira, Lopez, Blacher, and stress. However, when child-rearing challenges
Shapiro (2000) found that most mothers view were higher, parents who held the least positive
their child as not responsible for his or her be- views of parenting reported the greatest stress. Al-
havior problems. By not attributing cause to the though studies have repeatedly linked child be-
child, mothers avoid the formulation of negative havioral challenges to lowered parental well-being,
emotions. Another interpretation reflects the em- there is always considerable unexplained variance
beddedness of the child in the family, and Latina in the well-being variables. The present finding,
mothers’ great pride and responsibility in their that perceptions of positive impact moderate the
knowledge of their child. Thus, as reported in relationship between child challenge and parental
Rueda et al. (2005), Latinas often feel that they stress, is particularly important because it was
are better able to make child-related decisions manifested in two large samples quite different in
than are professionals. This empowerment is like- age and level of disability. This finding relates to
ly very satisfying and positive. Furthermore, Lati- a larger body of literature on positive thinking.
na mothers often perceive their child as more Several researchers have found that positivism in
competent than do professionals who assess or the form of perceptions of positive meaning lead
provide services to the child. In speaking about to better outcomes in health domains, such as
her young adult son who functioned at the level breast cancer (Bower, 2005; Bower & Segerstrom,
of a 6-year-old, one mother said: ‘‘He is a very 2004). Examining mechanisms, Taylor, Lerner,
handsome young man and he is attending the Sherman, Sage, and McDowell (2003) found that
Easter Seals and his functional level is basically a forms of positive illusions or beliefs actually re-
moderate mental retardation, very intelligent’’ duced cardiovascular responses to stress and pro-
(Rueda et al., 2005, p. 7). duced lower baseline levels of cortisol.
Given the empirical evidence of successful in- Models of stress and coping pertaining di-
terventions for depression that have been formu- rectly to families and disability posit that the re-
lated from a positive psychology perspective (Se- lationship between child stressors and parental
ligman et al., 2005), the possibility of building on well-being is affected by resources (e.g., income,
Latina mothers’ positive perceptions in interven- social support) and cognitions (e.g., McCubbin &
tion programs for parents who have children with Patterson, 1983). Although some investigators
disabilities is compelling. We note the impor- have examined the moderating role of resources
tance, though, of documenting and considering (e.g., Suarez & Baker, 1997), there is a need to
culturally relevant variables because Latina moth- study further the personality characteristics that
ers have reported some alienation in their expe- parents bring with them to parenting, as well as
riences with service delivery systems (Shapiro, attitudes concerning parenting, as possible buffers
Monzo, Rueda, Gomez, & Blacher, 2004). None- of child-rearing challenges. Recently, reported re-

344 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

search with the Study 2 sample found that the Baker, B. L., Blacher, J., Kopp, C. B., & Kraemer,
personality trait of dispositional optimism mod- B. (1997). Parenting children with mental re-
erated this relationship for mothers (Baker et al., tardation. International Review of Research in
2005). The present findings also support the mod- Mental Retardation, 20, 1–45.
el of cognitions as moderators of child challenges Baker, B. L., Heller, T. L., & Henker, B. (2000).
on parental outcome. However, it is notable that Expressed emotion, parenting stress, and ad-
in the Study 1 sample, positive views of parenting justment in mothers of young children with
and dispositional optimism were not significantly behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology
related. and Psychiatry, 41, 907–915.
Finally, we note that longitudinal perspectives Baker, B. L., McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J., Crnic,
on positive impact of disability are lacking in the K., Edelbrock, C., & Low, C. (2003). Pre-
literature. In the present study, positive impact school children with and without develop-
was stable across the preschool years. Poehlmann mental delay: Behavior problems and parent-
et al. (2005) have suggested that by the time a ing stress over time. Journal of Intellectual Dis-
child with intellectual disability reaches adoles- ability Research, 47, 217–230.
cence, parents have had more time to reflect on Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Develop-
the son’s or daughter’s positive attributes and to ment Second Edition: Manual. San Antonio:
develop more positive perceptions. We note that Psychological Corp.
within the disability groups of our studies report- Behr, S. K., Murphy, D. L., & Summers, J. A.
ed here, Anglo mothers’ positive impact mean (1992). User’s manual: Kansas Inventory of Pa-
score was 14.5 in the 3-year old sample and 12.0 rental Perceptions (KIPP). Lawrence: University
in the young adult sample, whereas Latina moth- of Kansas, Beach Center on Families and Dis-
ers had almost identical scores (Ms ⫽ 17.7 and ability.
17.6 in the two samples, respectively). These sam- Billingsley, A. (1992). Black families in white Amer-
ples, of course, differed in many respects, so al- ica. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
though these means do not support Poehlmann Blacher, J., & Baker, B. L. (2002). The best of
et al.’s hypothesis of more positive perceptions AAMR. Families and mental retardation: A col-
over time, the question can only adequately be lection of notable AAMR journal articles across
addressed longitudinally and in different cultural the 20th century. Washington, DC: American
contexts. Association on Mental Retardation.
Blacher, J., & McIntyre, L. L. (2006). Syndrome
specificity and behavioural disorders in young
References adults with intellectual disability: Cultural dif-
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child ferences in family impact. Journal of Intellectual
Behavior Checklist 4–18. Burlington: Universi- Disability Research, 50, 184–198.
ty of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. Blacher, J., Shapiro, J., Lopez, S., Diaz, L., & Fus-
Achenbach, T. M. (2000). Manual for the Child co, J. (1997). Depression in Latina mothers of
Behavior Checklist 1½–5. Burlington: Univer- children with mental retardation: A neglected
sity of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. concern. American Journal on Mental Retarda-
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regres- tion, 101, 83–96.
sion: Testing and interpreting interactions. New- Bower, J., Meyerowitz, B. E., Desmond, K. A.,
bury Park, CA: Sage. Bernaards, C. A., Rowland, J., & Patricia, A.
Baker, B. L., Blacher, J., & Olsson, M. (2005). Pre- (2005). Perceptions of positive meaning and
school children with and without develop- vulnerability following breast cancer predic-
mental delay: Behaviour problems, parents’ tors and outcomes among long-term breast
optimism, and well being. Journal of Intellectual cancer survivors. Annals of Behavioral Medi-
Disability Research, 49, 575–590. cine, 29, 236–245.
Baker, B. L., Blacher, J., Crnic, K. A., & Edel- Bower, J. E., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2004). Stress
brock, C. (2002). Behavior problems and par- management, finding benefit, and immune
enting stress in families of three-year-old chil- function: Positive mechanisms for interven-
dren with and without developmental delays. tion effects on physiology. Journal of Psycho-
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, somatic Research, 56, 9–11.
433–444. Bruininks, R. H., Woodcock, R. W., Weather-

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 345


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

man, R. F., & Hill, B. K. (1996). Scales of In- mental retardation services. American Journal
dependent Behavior Revised comprehensive man- on Mental Retardation, 109, 53–62.
ual. Itasca, IL: Riverside. Hastings, R. P., Korshoff, H., Brown, T., Ward,
Chavira, V., Lopez, S. R., Blacher, J., & Shapiro, N. J., Espinosa, F. D., & Remington, B.
S. (2000). Latina mothers’ attributions, emo- (2005). Coping strategies in mothers and fa-
tions, and reactions to the problem behaviors thers of preschool and school-age children
of their children with developmental disabil- with autism. Autism, 9, 377–391.
ities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Hastings, R. P., & Taunt, H. M. (2002). Positive
41, 245–252. perceptions in families of children with de-
Cox, A. H., Marshall, E. S., Mandleco, B., & Ol- velopmental disabilities. American Journal on
sen, S. F. (2003). Coping responses to daily Mental Retardation, 107, 116–127.
life stressor of children who have a disability. Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Shonkoff, J. P.,
Journal of Family Nursing, 9, 397–413. & Krauss, M. W. (2001). Children with dis-
Dyson, L. L. (1997). Fathers and mothers of abilities: A longitudinal study of child devel-
school-age children with developmental dis- opment and parent well-being. Monographs of
abilities: Parental stress, family functioning, the Society for Research in Child Development, 66,
and social support. American Journal on Men- 1–131.
tal Retardation, 102, 267–279. Helff, C. M., & Glidden, L. M. (1998). More pos-
Floyd, F. J., & Gallagher, E. M. (1997). Parental itive or less negative? Trends in research on
stress, care demands, and use of support ser- adjustment of families rearing children with
vices for school-age children with disabilities developmental disabilities. Mental Retardation,
and behavior problems. Family Relations, 46, 36, 457–464.
359–371. Hodapp, R. M., Glidden, L. M., & Kaiser, A. P.
Floyd, F. J., & Phillippe, K. A. (1993). Parental (2005). Siblings of persons with disabilities:
interactions with children with and without Toward a research agenda. Mental Retardation,
mental retardation: Behavior management, 43, 334–338.
coerciveness, and positive exchange. American Holroyd, J. (1985). Questionnaire on Resources and
Stress manual. Unpublished manuscript, Uni-
Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 673–684.
versity of California, Los Angeles, Neuropsy-
Folkman, S. (1997). Positive psychological states
chiatric Institute.
and coping with severe stress. Social Science
Juarez, R. (1985). Core issues in psychotherapy
and Medicine, 45, 1207–1221.
with the Hispanic child. Psychotherapy, 22,
Friedrich, W. N., Greenberg, M. T., & Crnic, K.
441–448.
(1983). A short-form of the Questionnaire on
Lawton, M. R., Moss, M., Kleban, M. H., Glicks-
Resources and Stress. American Journal of Men-
man, A., & Ravine, M. (1991). A two-factor
tal Deficiency, 88, 41–48. model of caregiving appraisal and psycholog-
Gallimore, R., Hoots, J. J., Weisner, T. S., Garnier, ical well-being. Journal of Gerontology: Psycho-
H. E., & Guthrie, D. (1996). Family responses logical Sciences, 46, 181–189.
to children with early developmental disabil- Lynch, E. W., & Hanson, M. J. (2004). Developing
ities II: Accommodation intensity and activity cross-cultural competence (3rd ed.). Baltimore:
in early and middle childhood. American Jour- Brookes.
nal on Mental Retardation, 101, 215–232. Margalit, M., & Ankonina, D. B. (1991). Positive
Hastings, R. P., Allen, R., McDermott, K., & Still, and negative affect in parenting disabled chil-
D. (2002). Factors related to positive percep- dren. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 4, 289–
tions in mothers of children with intellectual 299.
disabilities. Journal of Applied Research in Intel- McCubbin, H. I., & Patterson, J. (1983). Family
lectual Disabilities, 15, 269–275. transition: Adaptation to stress. In H. I.
Hastings, R. P., Beck, A., & Hill, C. (2005). Posi- McCubbin & C. R. Figley (Eds.), Stress and the
tive contributions made by children with an family: Vol. I. Coping with normative transitions
intellectual disability in the family. Journal of (pp. 5–23). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Intellectual Disabilities, 9, 155–165. McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J., & Baker, B. L. (2002).
Hastings, R. P., & Horne, S. (2004). Positive per- Behaviour/mental health problems in young
ceptions held by support staff in community adults with intellectual disability: The impact

346 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

on families. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re- pact of children with an intellectual disability
search, 46, 239–249. on the family. Journal of Intellectual and Devel-
Phares, V. (1996). Fathers and developmental psycho- opmental Disability, 23, 57–70.
pathology. New York: Wiley. Suarez, L. M., & Baker, B. L. (1997). Child exter-
Poehlmann, J., Clements, M., Abbeduto, L., & nalizing behavior and parents’ stress: The role
Farsad, V. (2005). Family experiences associ- of social support. Family Relations, 47, 373–
ated with a child’s diagnosis of fragile X or 381.
Down syndrome: Evidence for disruption and Summers, J. A., Behr, S. K., & Turnbull, A. P.
resilience. Mental Retardation, 43, 255–267. (1989). Positive adaptation and coping
Pruchno, R. A., Patrick, J. M. H., & Burant, C. strengths of families who have children with
(1996). Mental health of aging women with disabilities. In G. H. S. Singer & L. K. Irvin
children who are chronically disabled: Ex- (Eds.), Support for caregiving families: Enabling
amination of a two-factor model. Journal of positive adaptation to disabilities (pp. 27–40).
Gerontology: Social Sciences, 51B, S284–296. Baltimore: Brookes.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self- Taunt, H. M., & Hastings, R. P. (2002). Positive
report depression scale for research in the gen- impact of children with developmental disabil-
eral population. Applied Psychological Measure- ities on their families: A preliminary study. Ed-
ment, 1, 385–401. ucation and Training in Mental Retardation and
Reiss, S. (1994). Reiss Screen for Maladaptive Be- Developmental Disabilities, 37, 410–420.
havior: Test manual (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening
IDS. events: A theory of cognitive adaptation.
Rogers-Dulan, J., & Blacher, J. (1995). African American Psychologist, 38, 1161–1173.
American families, religion, and disability: A Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower,
conceptual framework. Mental Retardation, 33, J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psycholog-
226–238. ical resources, positive illusions, and health.
Rueda, R., Monzo, L., Shapiro, J., Gomez, J., & American Psychologist, 55, 99–109.
Blacher, J. (2005). Cultural models of transi- Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage,
tion: Latina mothers of young adults with de- R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Are self-
velopmental disabilities. Exceptional Children, enhancing cognitions associated with healthy
71, 1–14. or unhealthy biological profiles? Journal of Per-
Sandler, A. G., & Mistretta, L. A. (1998). Positive sonality and Social Psychology, 85, 605–615.
adaptation in parents of adults with disabili- Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M.
ties. Education and Training in Mental Retar- (1986). The Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale,
dation and Developmental Disabilities, 33, 123– Fourth edition: Technical manual. Chicago: Riv-
130. erside.
Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., & Pe- Turnbull, A. P., Patterson, J. M., Behr, S. K., Mur-
terson, C. (2005). Positive psychology pro- phy, D. L., Marquis, J. G., & Blue-Banning,
gress. Empirical validation of interventions. M. J. (Eds.). (1993). Cognitive coping, families,
American Psychologist, 60, 410–421. and disability. Baltimore: Brookes.
Shapiro, J., Monzo, L. D., Rueda, R., Gomez, J. Valentine, D. P., McDermott, S., & Anderson, D.
A., & Blacher, J. (2004). Alienated advocacy: (1998). Mothers of adults with mental retar-
Latina mothers of young children with devel- dation: Is race a factor in perceptions of bur-
opmental disabilities speak out about service dens and gratifications? Families in Society, 79,
delivery systems. Mental Retardation, 42, 37– 577–584.
54. Zuniga, M. E. (1992). Families with Latino roots.
Singer, G. H. S., & Powers, L. E. (1993). Families, In E. W. Lynch & M. J. Hanson (Eds.), De-
disability, and empowerment: Active coping skills veloping cross-cultural competence (pp. 151–179).
and strategies for family interventions. Baltimore: Baltimore: Brookes.
Brookes. Zuniga, M. E. (1998). Families with Latino roots.
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. In E. W. Lynch & M. J. Hanson (Eds.), De-
(1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Cir- veloping cross-cultural competence (2nd ed., pp.
cle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. 209–250). Baltimore: Brookes.
Stainton, T., & Besser, H. (1998). The positive im-

䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 347


VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 330–348 円 SEPTEMBER 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Positive impact of intellectual disability J. Blacher and B. L. Baker

Received 12/22/05, accepted 9/21/06. tigator; Bruce Baker, Jan Blacher, and Craig Edel-
Editor-in-charge: William E. MacLean, Jr. brock, co-principal investigators). We are indebted
to our staff and doctoral students at UC Riverside,
UCLA, and Pennsylvania State University. We
This article was based on activities of the Univer- also acknowledge the helpful feedback from col-
sity of California Riverside Families Project, sup- leagues Steve Lopez and Robert Rueda. We es-
ported by National Institute for Child Health and pecially appreciate the participation of the fami-
Human Development (NICHD) Grant HD 21324 lies who made this research possible. Requests for
(Jan Blacher, principal investigator) and The Col- reprints should be sent to Jan Blacher, Graduate
laborative Family Study, supported by NICHD School of Education, UC Riverside, Riverside,
Grant 34879-1459 (Keith Crnic, principal inves- CA. 92521. E-mail: jan.blacher@ucr.edu

VOLUME 112, NUMBER 5: 348 円 JULY 2007 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Errata
In the July 2007 issue of AJMR, pp. 239–299 (Narrative Development in Adolescents and Young Adults
With Fragile X Syndrome, by Yolanda D. Keller-Bell and Leonard D. Abbeduto), there should be no
middle initial for Leonard Abbeduto.

348 䉷 American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities

You might also like