You are on page 1of 12

Research Article

Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 Published online: July 3, 2019


DOI: 10.1159/000499426

A Psycholinguistic Framework for


Diagnosis and Treatment Planning of
Developmental Speech Disorders
Hayo Terband a Ben Maassen b Edwin Maas c
     

a Utrecht Institute of Linguistics-OTS, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands; b Centre for Language
   

and Cognition (CLCG) and University Medical Centre, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands;
c Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
 

Keywords treatment planning holds important advantages, offering


Speech development · Speech disorders · Differential direct leads for treatment aimed at the underlying impair-
diagnosis · Treatment planning ment, tailored to the specific needs of the individual and ad-
justed to the developmental trajectory.
© 2019 The Author(s)
Abstract Published by S. Karger AG, Basel

Background: Differential diagnosis and treatment planning


of developmental speech disorders (DSD) remains a major Introduction
challenge in paediatric speech-language pathology. Differ-
ent classification systems exist, in which subtypes are differ- Speech constitutes the primary channel of human so-
entiated based on their theoretical cause and in which the cial interaction; yet speaking can be considered the most
definitions generally refer to speech production processes. complex skill humans perform. Although most children
Accordingly, various intervention methods have been devel- acquire speech relatively automatically and with little dif-
oped aiming at different parts of the speech production pro- ficulty, some children struggle with the acquisition of
cess. Diagnostic classification in these systems, however, is speech production skills and require sustained and inten-
primarily based on a description of behavioural speech sive treatment [1]. Children with speech disorders are at
symptoms rather than on underlying deficits. Purpose: In increased risk of social-emotional and behavioural prob-
this paper, we present a process-oriented approach to diag- lems [2, 3], as well as of delayed development of language,
nosis and treatment planning of DSD. Our framework com- literacy, and other academic skills [4]. These issues may
prises two general diagnostic categories: developmental de- limit employment and occupational opportunities in
lay and developmental disorder. Within these categories, adulthood [5, 6]. Accurate diagnostic methods and effec-
treatment goals/targets and treatment methods are formu- tive intervention programmes are thus of crucial impor-
lated at the level of processes and rules/representations. tance to limit the short- and long-term impact of devel-
Conclusion: A process-oriented approach to diagnosis and opmental speech disorders (DSD) on the individual.

© 2019 The Author(s) Hayo Terband


Published by S. Karger AG, Basel Utrecht Institute of Linguistics-OTS, Utrecht University
Trans 10, Room 1.24
E-Mail karger@karger.com This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-
NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (CC BY- NL–3512 JK Utrecht (The Netherlands)
www.karger.com/fpl E-Mail h.r.terband @ uu.nl
NC-ND) (http://www.karger.com/Services/OpenAccessLicense).
Usage and distribution for commercial purposes as well as any dis-
tribution of modified material requires written permission.
Unfortunately, differential diagnosis and treatment the complexity of this system due to the different levels of
planning of paediatric speech disorders remains prob- causation involved, and the underspecification of the re-
lematic. Existing systems of diagnostic classification lation between levels, form serious obstacles to clinical
comprise a set of theoretically grounded categories (defi- use.
nitions) that are associated with a symptom complex and The MDD [10, 11] is based on Stackhouse and Wells’
start from the assumption that the categories can be dif- [12] Psycholinguistic Framework, a modular psycholin-
ferentiated by a diagnostic marker. A number of different guistic model of speech production and development.
systems for the classification of paediatric speech disor- The categorisation consists of (sub)groups that are based
ders have been proposed throughout the years [for an on the psycholinguistic level of the presumed core deficit,
overview, see 7], in which subtypes are differentiated the processing level that would be affected:
based on their distal or proximal cause. The predominant • Phonological disorder (PD; phonemic level)
systems at the moment are Shriberg’s Speech Disorders • Phonological delay
Classification System (SDCS) [8, 9] and Dodd’s Model of • Consistent
Differential Diagnosis (MDD) [10, 11]. The SDCS is a • Inconsistent
classification based on the presumed aetiological back- • Phonetic articulation disorder (PAD; phonetic level)
ground of the speech impairment and consists in its most • Developmental apraxia of speech (DAS; motor level;
recent form of three main categories divided into eight planning and programming)
subcategories: In contrast to the SDCS, the MDD thus revolves around
• Speech delay (SD; distal causes) the proximal cause of the different disorders (the process-
• Genetic ing level) and it does not make claims about their possible
• Otitis media more distal causes (neurobiology or aetiology). Although
• Psychosocial this system is consistent in terms of definitions and level
• Motor speech disorders (MSD; proximal causes) of description (the psycholinguistic level of processing),
• Apraxia (childhood apraxia of speech; CAS) this is only the case for the main categories, whereas sub-
• Dysarthria (developmental dysarthria; DD) categories of PD are defined based on symptomatology.
• Not otherwise specified (MSD-NOS) Apart from these specific issues, both classification
• Residual speech errors systems suffer the same problem in that the relation be-
• /s/ tween different levels of causation, in particular the psy-
• /r/ cholinguistic and the behavioural level, remains under-
The basic idea behind the SDCS is that there are identifi- specified. Diagnostic markers that are both specific and
able causes and that there are one-to-one relationships to sensitive have not yet been identified for all categories
specific speech symptoms [9]. A fundamental problem of that are differentiated (e.g., PD, CAS/DAS, and MSD-
the SDCS is that the classification system is inconsistent NOS). Regardless of the theoretical basis, the definitions
in specifying the levels of explanation across categories. of the disorders in these systems refer to speech produc-
In principle, the categorisation is based on the presumed tion processes. A variety of intervention methods for
aetiological background [8, 9], but the two subcategories helping children with DSD have been developed, aiming
of residual speech errors are purely symptomatic descrip- at different parts of the speech production process, such
tions at the behavioural level. Additionally and more im- as lemma selection, sequencing speech sounds and sylla-
portantly, the other subcategories of SD and MSD repre- bles, and planning/programming and execution of artic-
sent factors on different levels of explanation, i.e., genetic, ulatory movements. Clear criteria for determining which
otological, neurological, and psychological. Multiple fac- treatment is the most suitable for an individual child,
tors and multiple levels could be involved in a single case, however, are lacking. Thus, whereas the definitions of the
and their exact role and weight in causing the speech different disorders tend to refer to (hypothesised) under-
symptoms remains unclear. Moreover, the clinical char- lying deficits or causes, the clinical procedures for differ-
acterisations of the diagnostic categories consist of a ter- ential diagnosis are not aimed at these definitions [see
minology that refers to speech production processes, but also, e.g., 13–17, where similar conclusions were present-
how the aetiology is related to the processing deficit is not ed with respect to the taxonomy of adult dysarthria]. The
specified. Thus, the SDCS can be characterised as a hybrid current diagnostic instruments consist of tests that mea-
classification system that has the ambition to encompass sure knowledge and complex skills, such as sentence for-
all speech sound disorders. In its current state, however, mulation, vocabulary, picture and colour naming, and

Process-Oriented Diagnosis of DSD Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 217


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
phoneme inventory. Classification comprises the assign- rological architecture at the second, neurobiological level.
ment of a diagnostic category based on a behavioural de- The brain does not develop according to an exact prede-
scription and symptomatology. This classification proce- termined blueprint, but is continuously adapting to bio-
dure does not provide sufficient direct information about logical and behavioural (environmental) circumstances.
the underlying processes and does not allow specification An example would be the communicative activity level of
of the gradual involvement of different speech produc- an infant. More active infants tend to elicit more commu-
tion processes. nicative responses from the carer than do more passive
At the genetic level, a variety of abnormalities have infants. The third level is the cognitive and sensorimotor,
been linked with developmental speech and language dis- or psycholinguistic, level describing the processes that
orders (e.g., mutation of FOXP2 [18, 19], mutation of underlie the fourth, behavioural level.
GRIN2A [20], and microdeletion of BCL11A [21]). Al- As we have argued before, core impairments at differ-
though the progress in this area is promising, the associ- ent levels of speech development and in different parts of
ated symptomatology is highly heterogeneous and often the speech production chain cannot be clearly distin-
encompasses speech and non-speech motor functions, guished from each other at the behavioural symptom lev-
expressive and receptive language functions, and cogni- el alone, but they need an understanding of the complex
tive functions such as short-term memory and sequenc- interactions between causation levels [31–34]. Many ex-
ing more in general [18, 22–26]. Direct links between amples have shown that different disorders may show
genotype and phenotype have yet to be established [25, similar symptoms [17, 35], but computer simulations
26]. Also at the neurobiological level, few specific under- have also exemplified that a specific underlying deficit
lying deficits have been established for speech disorders can produce symptoms on (apparently) different levels or
other than perhaps those affecting peripheral sensory and domains [36]. Thus, behavioural symptoms are often
motor systems. The link between particular neurobiolog- multi-interpretable at the cognitive and neurobiological
ical findings (e.g., atypical development of the left corti- level. For example, specific phoneme substitution errors
cobulbar tract [27] and a thinner corpus callosum [28]) (behavioural) can often be analysed in phonological
and cognitive and sensorimotor processes and behav- terms as feature substitutions or in speech-motor terms
ioural performance remains to be explicated. Further- as articulatory simplifications or as sequencing errors (all
more, in typical clinical settings, such information about three cognitive). Likewise, specific deficits at the neuro-
the underlying genetic or neurobiological pathology is biological or cognitive level, if they can be determined at
rarely available. Thus, although research on the genetic all, can result in a variety of behavioural symptoms. Thus,
and neurobiological underpinnings of DSD may hold diagnostic classification requires assessments at different
promise for our understanding and, in the long term, for levels of causation and some description of the interac-
clinical purposes, at present such research does not pro- tion between levels.
vide insights or specific suggestions to speech-language A good example of this basic challenge is the history of
pathologists for diagnosis or for treatment planning for the dispute on the core definitions of CAS/DAS. The
speech therapy [29]. In contrast, a focus on the underly- comprehensive 2007 ASHA Technical Report [37] noted
ing psycholinguistic processes has clear implications for that “[w]hereas some of the definitions of CAS reviewed
diagnosis and treatment, as we outline further below. by the Committee view the core problem as one of plan-
The fundamental diagnostic problem is that different ning and programming (cognitive level) the spatiotem-
levels of causation in interaction underlie the speech dis- poral properties of movement sequences (behavioural
order, and that at none of these levels in isolation, spe- level) underlying speech sound production, others pro-
cific and sensitive diagnostic markers for diagnostic clas- pose that the deficit extends to representational-level
sification can be found. Due to the interactions between (cognitive) segmental and/or suprasegmental units in
levels, there is a large overlap of symptomatology between both input processing and production” (p. 4). The ASHA
categories and a large heterogeneity within categories. Technical Report has had the effect of enhancing consen-
According to the 2004 model of Bishop and Snowling sus on its definition of CAS as a “core impairment in plan-
[30], four levels of causation, or four levels of aggregation, ning and/or programming spatiotemporal parameters of
can be distinguished that are involved in developmental movement sequences” (pp. 3–4) and, currently, research-
disorders. The first, aetiological level concerns the genet- ers seem to have agreed on this description of processes
ic constitution of the individual and environmental fac- or proximal causes underlying CAS. Thus, the report
tors, which together determine the unfolding of the neu- clearly makes a choice here at the cognitive and senso-

218 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 Terband/Maassen/Maas


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
rimotor level. The basic problem is how to develop test
procedures such that processing and representational Conceptualisation

deficits can be distinguished. Note that all test procedures Preverbal massage Parsed speech
make use of behavioural measures, not only assessment
of symptoms, but also cognitive tests. The latter are based
Grammatical Grammatical
on behavioural assessments under strict experimental encoding decoding
and test administration procedures; also functional brain
Lemma
imaging tests make use of presenting the subject with a
Surface structure Lexicon
task to elicit behaviour such that brain activity can be in-
emexel lexeme
terpreted.
The high variability and broad spectrum of symptoms Phonological Phonological
encoding decoding
in DSD form a major challenge for clinical management
and research. Effective differential diagnosis therefore re-
quires a theoretically grounded process-oriented ap- Motor planning Internal
self-monitoring
proach focusing on clearly defined underlying deficits
Audiotory
(whether cause or processes) rather than classification Motor Feedback processing
programming
based on symptoms. Instead of searching for homoge- and memory
Somatosensory
neous groupings of overt speech symptoms and subse- processing
quently trying to identify a common cause (whether Motor execution
proximal or distal) one should start with what can be External self-monitoring
clearly defined. Although the symptomatology and aeti- Overt speech
ology are not completely clear, there is a solid theoretical
basis that allows us to precisely describe specific core
Fig. 1. Adult model of speech processing [33, 34] (adapted from
problems in terms of psycholinguistic processes [33, 34]. Levelt [39, 51], Van der Merwe [40, 50], and Guenther [45, 53]),
To identify underlying deficits, one must thus start with displaying the sensorimotor and memory functions involved in
a model of the cognitive and sensorimotor operations in- speech production and perception.
volved, from which specific hypotheses of speech symp-
toms are derived [33, 34] [see also 12, 38]. A focus on
processes instead of cause (aetiology) has the advantage different models show important similarities and overlap
that it provides direct information for treatment. In this [see also, e.g., 31, 33, 34, 38, 47].
paper, we present a theoretically based framework for Similar to any model of complex motor performance,
process-oriented diagnosis and treatment planning of all models of speech processing first and foremost adopt
DSD. a cascade-style hierarchy of control in which the output
of one process forms the input of the next processing lev-
el [e.g., 39–46, 48–52]. Leaving out all the details of dis-
An Integrated Psycholinguistic Model of Speech pute and differences in scope between models, speech
Processing production is preceded by a language process of sentence
formulation, in which lemmata are retrieved from the
The starting point of our framework is an integrated lexicon and inflected and sequenced in a grammatical
model of the cognitive and sensorimotor functions in- phrase to match intended concepts, and stored in a short-
volved in speech production and perception (Fig. 1). A term memory buffer (grammatical encoding) [e.g., 39,
variety of models of speech processing have been present- 51]. Speech production models then start with the word
ed over the years [e.g., 39–46]. Each model has its own forms (lexemes) retrieved from the lexicon, which forms
specific approach, scope and theoretical basis and the sci- the input for phonological encoding, in which the senso-
entific discussion on which model gives the best account rimotor targets that constitute the speech sounds or syl-
of all the different speech phenomena traditionally re- lables are selected and sequenced in a phonological phrase
volves around the differences between models. Whereas of linguistic/symbolic units, and stored in a short-term
the scientific endeavour tends to focus on what we do not memory buffer [e.g., 39–41, 49–51].
know, what we do know is far more important for clinical The next stage, motor planning, comprises the selec-
practice. From this perspective, it can be noted that the tion and sequencing of the articulatory movement goals

Process-Oriented Diagnosis of DSD Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 219


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
that would produce these targets [e.g., 40–42, 48, 50], and specific impaired process can lead to problems at the sub-
the adaptation of these goals to the phonetic environment sequent processing level (which may itself be intact). For
(e.g., coarticulation) [40, 50]. During the subsequent example, difficulties in phonological encoding may affect
stage, motor programming, the motor plans are then im- speech motor control processes and thus cause (sensori)
plemented in muscle-specific motor programmes [e.g., motor symptoms [e.g., 49]. Indirect interaction refers to
40, 43, 45, 48, 50, 53], taking into account articulatory adaptive and compensatory mechanisms. If some part of
context, sensory information, and (meta)linguistic re- the system suffers an impairment, the system will try to
quirements (e.g., speech rate, prosody, and prominence) adapt to the deviant circumstances and/or compensate
[40, 50]. Finally, the constructed neural signals that con- for the impediments. One simple way in which the system
stitute the motor programmes are sent to the peripheral can adapt is by slowing down the speech rate. A slow
systems and executed, resulting in the actual movements speech rate is a general characteristic of MSD irrespective
of the articulators (motor execution) [e.g., 40, 43, 45, 48, of the underlying deficit. In many cases, it does not con-
50, 53]. stitute a primary symptom but rather acts as a compensa-
During speaking, these stages form an ongoing process tory mechanism to make the control task easier [17, 59].
that is monitored continuously at several levels. This self- A clear example of adaptation and compensation mecha-
monitoring is based on both internal and external feed- nisms on a functional-cognitive level can be seen in peo-
back (Fig.  1). Internal feedback is used during motor ple with anatomical deformities of the articulatory or-
planning to avoid, for example, that erroneously planned gans, such as glossectomy [60–65] or dental occlusions
speech movements are executed [e.g., 39, 51, 54]. External and prostheses [66–69].
feedback comprises both fast somatosensory and slow au- In the case of an impairment in one of the processes or
ditory monitoring and provides current information representations in children, the interaction between the
about the state and position of the articulatory organs different parts of the system gets an extra dimension. A
(such as position, movement direction, and speed) as in- specific underlying impairment on one level or domain
put for motor programming [e.g., 40, 43–45, 50, 53]. Ex- also affects the development on adjacent levels or domains
ternal feedback is further used to monitor the produced [14, 36, 70–72]. Due to this developmental interaction,
speech on the motor programming, phonological, and the potential influence of primary deficits on adjacent
higher linguistic levels [e.g., 26, 27, 38, 41] and can be processing levels and of adaptive and compensatory
used for ongoing adaptation of articulation and error cor- mechanisms is even stronger. A primary impairment at
rection [e.g., 43–45, 53]. the acoustic-perceptual level can, for example, cause the
The adult speech production system is very robust. phonological representations to be incorrect or under-
The different processes and representations are highly specified. This could in turn lead to a deviant and incom-
overlearned and the system is highly redundant. In case plete phonological system, primarily because it had to
of acquired deafness, for example, speakers continue to learn from degraded input. Similar observations have
be intelligible despite the fact that auditory self-monitor- been described in children with a cleft palate, who often
ing is completely disabled. The situation is different, how- develop specific articulation patterns to compensate for
ever, during speech acquisition in infants and children. their deficit [73–75]. These compensatory articulation
The different cognitive and sensorimotor functions are patterns may persist after the cleft has been surgically re-
not pre-specified in the infant brain, but they develop paired, causing problems in the development of the chil-
gradually into the adult system [55–57]. The different dren’s phonological system [e.g., 73, 74, 76]. Another ex-
functions and representations develop simultaneously ample is the correlational evidence that suggests that poor
and influence each other during development [e.g., 36, motor control in CAS is associated with poor develop-
58]. ment of the lexicon, the phonological system, and audi-
However, also in the adult speech production and per- tory processing [77, 78].
ception system, the different parts of the processing chain As a result of these interdependencies between the dif-
are not fully independent. The mutual dependence be- ferent levels of speech development and different parts of
tween processes mainly expresses itself in the case of dis- the speech production chain, overlap of symptoms in
ruptions. In the adult model, two types of interaction can paediatric speech sound disorder is the rule rather than
be distinguished: direct and indirect. With direct interac- the exception, which has frustrated attempts to find single
tion, we mean that processes are dependent on input re- diagnostic markers for differential diagnosis. This inter-
ceived from other processes. Degraded input from one dependent nature of the developing speech processing

220 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 Terband/Maassen/Maas


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
system means that the idea of finding single diagnostic typical development. The different characteristics of the
markers is fundamentally problematic and that attempts developmental trajectories demand a fundamentally dif-
to do so are unlikely to be fruitful. As Bishop [55] argued ferent approach in terms of treatment and thus impor-
in the context of specific language impairment already tantly serve to direct the choice of the treatment goals/
more than 20 years ago, the neuropsychological principle targets and methods.
of double dissociation in the study of acquired disorders
in adults does not apply to developmental disorders. De- Treatment Goals/Targets at the Level of Processes and
velopmental disorders are characterised by associations Representations
between functions rather than dissociation. Differential The goal of the speech acquisition process is to form
diagnosis and treatment planning therefore require a dif- the different components of the speech production
ferent approach than classification based on overt speech chain as they exist and function in the adult system
symptoms only. Along this line, Maassen [25] presented (Fig. 1). What we propose with the current framework
a multi-level, multi-factorial description of the underly- is that when problems are encountered during speech
ing deficit of CAS. acquisition, the goals/targets for treatment are defined
in terms of these same components. In other words, the
possible treatment goals/targets correspond to the pro-
Process-Oriented Diagnosis and Treatment Planning cesses and rules/representations that are presented in
the speech production model in Figure 1. Leaving the
The multi-factorial nature of developmental disorders language processes aside, the framework differentiates
means that effective diagnosis and treatment planning re- four processing processes and three monitoring pro-
quire a dynamic, process-oriented approach aimed at de- cesses. The framework further contains a set of phono-
scribing the development of underlying processing defi- logical rules and two representations that are used by the
cits to characterise disorders [31, 32, 36]. Based on the different processes. A short overview of the different
integrated model of speech processing presented above components of the framework that form the possi-
(Fig. 1), we propose a framework for process-oriented di- ble  goals/targets for treatment is presented in Table 1
agnosis and treatment planning of DSD (Table 1). Two [83–114].
essential elements of our framework are that it comprises
general diagnostic categories within which specific treat- Hypothesis Testing
ment goals/targets are formulated at the level of process- In this framework, a diagnosis comprises the assess-
es and representations. ment of the two aspects described in the previous para-
graphs: (1) the developmental trajectory as a whole, char-
General Diagnostic Categories acterised by the developmental profile of processes, and
The framework that we propose distinguishes two (2) deducing the underlying processing deficit(s) and,
general diagnostic categories labelled developmental de- from there, identifying treatment targets. Identifying the
lay and developmental disorder. The division between developmental profile reveals whether specific processes
these categories is based on fundamental differences in lag behind, which forms the basis for specifying treatment
the general characterisation of delayed versus deviant targets. A delayed but balanced profile indicates develop-
speech development [79–82]. These differences are infor- mental delay, whereas an unbalanced profile indicates a
mative for both the treatment goals/targets and the choice deviant development [82]. In addition, it needs to be de-
of the treatment method (the design of the treatment pro- termined whether specific deficits underlie the unbal-
gramme; the choice and planning of exercises and activi- anced profile, which is especially important for the choice
ties). In the case of developmental delay, development of treatment method.
follows the typical pattern, but is delayed. The speech dif- Thus, development could be delayed or deviant in dif-
ficulties that the child experiences are not unusual and are ferent ways. Processes and rules/representations can be
also commonly experienced by children with typical de- less accurate, less automated, or slower. Additionally,
velopment, but they are unusual for the age. Develop- speech development could start typically until the onset
ment can also be delayed in the case of developmental of the speech difficulties, for example, due to increased
disorder; however, essential for the latter category is that demands. The assessment and characterisation of the de-
development does not follow the typical pattern. Speech velopmental trajectory require standardised and norm-
difficulties occur that are not usual during any stage in referenced speech tasks, as well as language and oral mo-

Process-Oriented Diagnosis of DSD Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 221


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
Table 1. The processes and rules/representations of the speech production chain that form the possible treatment goals/targets, accom-
panied by examples of assessments and possible specific task comparisons based on these assessments that address these (note that these
are not exhaustive)

Process Rules/ Description Example assessment Example task comparisons


representations methods

Processing
Phonological Phonological Selection and sequencing of Word and non-word Word vs. non-word imitation (e.g.,
encoding representations linguistic/symbolic units that imitation: phoneme a higher segmental error rate in
[e.g., 39–41, 49–51] form goals for speech sounds: inventory and error words indicates poor representation
formation of a phonological analysis [e.g., 83–86] of lexeme)
Phonotactic phrase Word vs. non-word imitation (e.g.,
rules a higher segmental error rate in
non-words indicates either poor
auditory/memory functions or
poor output assembly)
Metrical Word vs. non-word imitation (e.g.,
spell-out a higher rate of stress attern errors
in non-words indicates either poor
auditory/memory functions or poor
output assembly)
Motor planning Phonemic Selection of articulatory Sentence, word, and More vs. less complex movements
[e.g., 40–42, 48, 50] mappings movement goals non-word imitation: and context (e.g., higher segmental
error analysis and error and deletion rates in more
coarticulation complex movement sequences;
[e.g., 87–90] lack of differentiation in anticipatory
coarticulation between consonant
contexts)
Motor Systemic Implementation in Sentence, word, and More vs. less complex movements
programming mapping muscle-specific motor non-word imitation: and context (e.g., higher
[e.g., 40, 43, 45, programmes consistency of repeated inconsistency in longer utterances
48, 50, 53] productions [e.g., and more complex
91–94] Adaptation to movement sequences [e.g.,
external circumstances consonant clusters])
(biteblock [95, 96]; lip
tube [97])
Motor execution Transmission of neural signals Diadochokinesis: More vs. less complex movements
[e.g., 40, 43, 45, to peripheral systems and maximum repetition (e.g., /tata/ vs. /sasa/; difficulties in
48, 50, 53] transformation into rate, rate variability, sensorimotor tuning cause higher
coordinated muscle activity phonation variability in /sasa/)
duration [e.g., 98–103]
Monitoring
Internal Phonemic Detection of incorrectly Word and non-word Early vs. late/no word uniqueness
self-monitoring mappings planned sounds or movements imitation: error point; high vs. low lexical
[e.g., 39, 51, 54] repair [e.g., 104, 105] neighbourhood density; elayed vs.
Delayed auditory normal auditory eedback (e.g.,
feedback absence of uniqueness point or
[e.g., 106, 107] neighbourhood density effects on
error repairs indicates poor preverbal
monitoring)
External Systemic Ad hoc adaptation of Auditory feedback Masking vs. no masking (e.g.,
self-monitoring – mapping articulation and error masking and reduced vowel contrast in masking
auditory [e.g., 40, and phonemic correction perturbation indicates poor phonemic mappings)
43–45, 50, 53] mappings [108–112]
External Systemic Information about the actual Somatosensory Masking vs. no-masking
self-monitoring – mapping state of the articulatory feedback masking and e.g., increased variability in masking
somatosensory system, ad hoc adaptation of perturbation [113, 114] indicates poor phonemic mappings)
[e.g., 40, 43–45, 50, 53] articulation and
error correction

222 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 Terband/Maassen/Maas


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
tor tasks [115, see also 116].1 To a large extent, these in- ing used successfully in experimental studies [108, 110,
struments are available (albeit not for all languages). 126, 127]. A particularly nice example is the recent study
Identifying the processes involved requires an experi- by Geronikou and Rees [127], who used a battery of
mental research methodology that has become available speech production and perception tasks to specify – for
only recently, and still needs to be further developed and each child individually – the underlying speech process-
refined and subsequently implemented in clinical instru- ing difficulties in four 4.5- to 5.5-year-old children with a
ments. In essence, the approach follows the principal PD with very similar speech error patterns. The speech
ideas of Stackhouse and Wells [12, 121], later advocated output tasks included picture naming, word repetition,
by Baker et al. [38], and is very similar in spirit to the Psy- and non-word repetition, while the perception tasks in-
cholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in cluded non-word discrimination and mispronunciation
Aphasia (PALPA) [122]. The approach comprises a reit- detection. All children showed similar problems in the
erative process of hypothesis testing by means of objec- speech production tasks, but the results showed specific
tive measurements of speech output in systematically var- differences in the performance on the perception tasks for
ied tasks under systematically varied conditions. Differ- these specific target sounds. Two of the children had dif-
ent speaking tasks and speaking conditions put different ficulties in the mispronunciation detection task but not
demands on different components of the speech produc- in non-word discrimination, leading the authors to infer
tion model (Table 1). In the right combination, this en- that for these children the problem resided in the phono-
ables a demonstration of the processes involved. Based on logical representations of the target sounds. The other 2
a specific test result, a hypothesis about the speech im- children had no difficulty with either mispronunciation
pairment is formulated or adjusted, which is then evalu- detection or non-word discrimination, suggesting that
ated by means of a subsequent test or condition. Eventu- the target sounds were well specified in the phonological
ally, after a number of steps, this leads to a profile charac- representations but not in the motor representations
terising which processes and rules/representations of the (phonemic mappings in the terminology of the present
speech production chain are involved. Ultimately, a spe- paper). As the authors concluded, this makes a funda-
cific diagnosis is established by deducing the underlying mental difference for intervention [127].
deficit(s) based on this profile, in combination with the The example above, as well as the PALPA [122], can be
characterisation of the impairment. A concrete treatment characterised as a neuropsychological approach, com-
plan can then be drafted to target (or circumvent) the im- prising the administration of a test battery and deducing
pairment, taking into account other relevant aspects such from the profile which underlying process(es) is/are dis-
as age, severity, and individual characteristics and per- rupted. Such a comprehensive speech profile is the first
sonal interests. step towards a process-oriented diagnosis in which un-
First steps in such profiling of speech production and derlying deficits are identified. The next step is to direct-
perception characteristics based on an extensive assess- ly manipulate speech processes, for instance, by speeding
ment battery have been successfully made in research ap- up, masking noise, auditory perturbation, distorting kin-
plications for diagnostic group assignment for some time aesthetic feedback, or short learning tasks (brief diagnos-
[123–125]. Similarly, model-based approaches compris- tic therapies). Rather than having to interpret a particular
ing detailed comparisons of different tasks and/or condi- speech profile, which relies on a matched comparison
tions to specify the underlying speech processing difficul- group, in these varying conditions the subject is his or her
ties involved in children with speech impairment are be- own control. The required experimental methods are
available and are being further developed and fine-tuned
in research studies. Slowly but certainly, they are becom-
1 Although there is debate about the potential utility of non-speech oral mo-
ing available for use in clinical practice.
tor tasks [98, 99, 116–120], they represent an important first step in differ-
ential diagnosis investigating the anatomy and (neuromuscular) functional-
ity of the oral motor system. The necessity of non-speech oral motor tasks
directly follows from classic definitions of DSD. CAS, for example, is defined Conclusions
as “a neurological childhood (pediatric) speech sound disorder in which the
precision and consistency of movements underlying speech are impaired
in the absence of neuromuscular deficits (e.g., abnormal reflexes, abnormal In this paper, we presented a model of speech process-
tone)” [37, p. 3; emphasis added]. A non-speech oral motor examination ing that can serve as a framework for a process-oriented
offers insight into possible neuromuscular or structural anatomical impair-
ments. Furthermore, Thoonen et al. [98, 99] have shown that diadochokine- approach to diagnosis and treatment planning of DSD.
sis tasks are discriminative between CAS and DD. This process-oriented approach holds important advan-

Process-Oriented Diagnosis of DSD Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 223


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
tages, in that it offers direct leads for treatment aimed at tics and treatment planning [31–33]. The step from a be-
the specific underlying impairment and adjusted during havioural to a process-oriented diagnostic and treatment
the course of the speech disorder. Although the diagnos- planning means, in the words of Baker et al. [38], “refram-
tic procedure will be more time-consuming than the cur- ing the ways in which we understand children’s commu-
rent procedures, a more targeted treatment tailored to the nication problems” (p. 700) [see also 33, 34, 40, 50, 121].
specific needs of the individual promises to be more ef-
fective and efficient. Moreover, we believe that current
and future technological advances in diagnostic instru- Acknowledgements
mentation, such as automated test administration and
automated processing and analyses of test performance, This work was supported by the Netherlands Organization for
Scientific Research (NWO-VENI grant 275-89-016 awarded to
will partly eliminate these drawbacks.
H.T.), and the National Institutes of Health (National Institute of
Possibly more problematic is that this process-orient- Deafness and Other Communication Disorders grant K01-
ed approach requires a different way of thinking by clini- DC010216 awarded to E.M.).
cal practitioners [12, 33, 34, 38, 40, 50, 121]. Speech ther-
apists are generally trained to focus on speech output
skills, to think and work according to a framework of di- Statement of Ethics
agnostic classification based on behavioural symptoms
only and plan treatment according to the corresponding The authors have no ethical conflicts to disclose.
protocol. A change to a practice of hypothesis testing in a
psycholinguistic framework requires a different educa-
tion and training to provide speech therapists with the Disclosure Statement
theoretical background and the clinical skills to utilise
and interpret a process-oriented instrument for diagnos- The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References
  1 Campbell TF. Functional treatment outcomes review and critical evaluation of current clas- Disordered Speech. Oxford: Oxford Univer-
in young children with motor speech disorders. sification systems. Int J Lang Commun Dis- sity Press; 2004. pp. 1–28.
In: Caruso A, Strand E, editors. Clinical Man- ord. 2013 Jan;48(1):25–40. 15 McNeil MR, Ballard KJ, Duffy J, Wambaugh
agement of Motor Speech Disorders in Chil-  8 Lewis BA, Freebairn LA, Hansen AJ, Stein JL. Apraxia of Speech Theory, Assessment,
dren. New York: Thieme; 1999. pp. 385–96. CM, Shriberg LD, Iyengar SK, et al. Dimen- Differential Diagnosis and Treatment: Past,
  2 Conti-Ramsden G, Botting N. Social difficul- sions of early speech sound disorders: a factor Present and Future. In: Van Lieshout P, Maas-
ties and victimization in children with SLI at analytic study. J Commun Disord. 2006 Mar- sen B, Terband H, editors. Speech Motor
11 years of age. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2004 Apr;39(2):139–57. Control in normal and disordered speech: Fu-
Feb;47(1):145–61.   9 Shriberg LD, Fourakis M, Hall SD, Karlsson ture developments in theory and methodol-
  3 van Daal J, Verhoeven L, van Balkom H. Be- HB, Lohmeier HL, McSweeny JL, et al. Exten- ogy. Rockville (MD): American Speech-Lan-
haviour problems in children with language sions to the Speech Disorders Classification guage-Hearing Association; 2017.
impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007 System (SDCS). Clin Linguist Phon. 2010 Oct; 16 McNeil MR, Pratt SR, Fosset TR. The differ-
Nov;48(11):1139–47. 24(10):795–824. ential diagnosis of apraxia of speech. In:
  4 McCormack J, McLeod S, McAllister L, Har- 10 Dodd B, editor. Differental Diagnosis and Maassen B, Kent R, Peters HF, et al., editors.
rison LJ. A systematic review of the associa- Treatment of Children with Speech Disorder. Speech Motor Control in Normal and Disor-
tion between childhood speech impairment 2nd ed. London: Whurr; 2005. dered Speech. Oxford: Oxford University
and participation across the lifespan. Int J 11 Dodd B. Differential Diagnosis of Pediatric Press; 2004. pp. 389–414.
Speech Lang Pathol. 2009;11(2):155–70. Speech Sound Disorder. Curr Dev Disord 17 Weismer G, Kim Y. Classification and Tax-
  5 Felsenfeld S, Broen PA, McGue M. A 28-year Rep. 2014;1(3):189–196. onomy of Motor Speech Disorders: What are
follow-up of adults with a history of moderate 12 Stackhouse J, Wells B. Children’s speech and the Issues? In: Maassen B, Van Lieshout P,
phonological disorder: educational and occu- literacy difficulties: a psycholinguistic frame- editors. Speech motor control: New develop-
pational results. J Speech Hear Res. 1994 Dec; work. Wiley; 1997. ments in basic and applied research. Oxford:
37(6):1341–53. 13 Boutsen F. Prosody: an organizing principle Oxford University Press; 2010. pp. 229–42.
 6 Snowling MJ, Adams JW, Bishop DV, Sto- for neurogenic speech disorders. Asia Pac J 18 Morgan A, Fisher SE, Scheffer I, Hildebrand
thard SE. Educational attainments of school Speech Lang Hear. 2008;11(2):81–7. M. FOXP2-related speech and language dis-
leavers with a preschool history of speech- 14 Kent RD. Models of speech motor control: orders. In: Adam MP, Ardinger HH, Pagon
language impairments. Int J Lang Commun implications from recent developments in RA, Wallace SE, Bean LJH, Stephens K, et al.,
Disord. 2001 Apr-Jun;36(2):173–83. neurophysiological and neurobehavioral sci- editors. GeneReviews® [Internet]. Seattle
 7 Waring R, Knight R. How should children ence. In: Maassen B, Kent R, Peters HF, et al., (WA): University of Washington, Seattle;
with speech sound disorders be classified? A editors. Speech Motor Control in Normal and 2016 [updated 2017 Feb 2].

224 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 Terband/Maassen/Maas


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
19 Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Win- opments in theory and methodology. Rock- namics Application) manual. New Haven
chester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, et al. A func- ville (MD): American Speech-Language- (CT): Haskins Laboratories, Inc.; 2007.
tional genetic link between distinct develop- Hearing Association; 2016. 49 Goldrick M, Blumstein SE. Cascading activa-
mental language disorders. N Engl J Med. 34 Terband H, Maassen B, Maas E. Klinisch fo- tion from phonological planning to articula-
2008 Nov;359(22):2337–45. rum: Een procesgerichte aanpak van differen- tory processes: evidence from tongue twisters.
20 Turner SJ, Mayes AK, Verhoeven A, Mandel- tiaaldiagnose en therapieplanning bij spraa- Lang Cogn Process. 2006;21(6):649–83.
stam SA, Morgan AT, Scheffer IE. GRIN2A: kontwikkelingsstoornissen. Stem-, Spraak- 50 Van der Merwe A. A theoretical framework
an aptly named gene for speech dysfunction. en Taalpathologie. 2016;21:1–31. for the characterization of pathological speech
Neurology. 2015 Feb;84(6):586–93. 35 Maassen B, Nijland L, Terband H. Develop- sensorimotor control. In: McNeil MR, editor.
21 Peter B, Matsushita M, Oda K, Raskind W. De mental models of Childhood Apraxia of Clinical Management of Sensorimotor Speech
novo microdeletion of BCL11A is associated Speech. In: Maassen B, Van Lieshout P, edi- Disorders. 2nd ed. New York: Thieme Medi-
with severe speech sound disorder. Am J Med tors. Speech motor control: New develop- cal Publishers Inc; 2009. pp. 3–18.
Genet A. 2014 Aug;164A(8):2091–6. ments in basic and applied research. Oxford: 51 Levelt WJ, Roelofs A, Meyer AS. A theory of
22 Peter B, Wijsman EM, Nato AQ Jr, Matsush- Oxford University Press; 2010. pp. 243–58. lexical access in speech production. Behav
ita MM, Chapman KL, Stanaway IB, et al.; 36 Terband H, Maassen B, Guenther FH, Brum- Brain Sci. 1999 Feb;22(1):1–38.
University of Washington Center for Mende- berg J. Auditory-motor interactions in pedi- 52 Tilsen S. A dynamical model of hierarchical
lian Genomics. Genetic candidate variants in atric motor speech disorders: neurocomputa- selection and coordination in speech plan-
two multigenerational families with child- tional modeling of disordered development. J ning. PLoS One. 2013 Apr;8(4):e62800.
hood apraxia of speech. PLoS One. 2016 Apr; Commun Disord. 2014 Jan-Feb;47:17–33. 53 Guenther FH, Perkell JS. A neural model of
11(4):e0153864. 37 ASHA. Childhood Apraxia of Speech [techni- speech production and its application to stud-
23 Newbury DF, Fisher SE, Monaco AP. Recent cal report]. American Speech-Language- ies of the role of auditory feedback in speech.
advances in the genetics of language impair- Hearing Association. 2007. Available from: In: Maassen B, Kent R, Peters HF, et al., edi-
ment. Genome Med. 2010 Jan;2(1):6. www.asha.org/policy. tors. Speech Motor Control in Normal and
24 Newbury DF, Monaco AP. Genetic advances 38 Baker E, Croot K, McLeod S, Paul R. Psycho- Disordered Speech. Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni-
in the study of speech and language disorders. linguistic models of speech development and versity Press; 2004. pp. 29–50.
Neuron. 2010 Oct;68(2):309–20. their application to clinical practice. J Speech 54 Nooteboom SG, Quené H. Self-monitoring
25 Maassen B. Developmental models of child- Lang Hear Res. 2001 Jun;44(3):685–702. for speech errors: two-stage detection and re-
hood apraxia of speech. In: Bahr RH, Silliman 39 Levelt WJ. Speaking: From intention to artic- pair with and without auditory feedback. J
ER, editors. Routledge Handbook of Commu- ulation. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press; 1989. Mem Lang. 2017;95:19–35.
nication Disorders. New York (NY): Rout- 40 Van der Merwe A. A theoretical framework 55 Bishop DV. Cognitive neuropsychology and
ledge; 2015. pp. 124–33. for the characterization of pathological speech developmental disorders: uncomfortable
26 Ziegler W, Ackermann H. Subcortical contri- sensorimotor control. In: McNeil MR, editor. bedfellows. Q J Exp Psychol A. 1997 Nov;
butions to motor speech: phylogenetic, devel- Clinical Management of Sensorimotor Speech 50(4):899–923.
opmental, clinical. Trends Neurosci. 2017 Disorders. New York: Thieme Medical Pub- 56 Karmiloff-Smith A. The tortuous route from
Aug;40(8):458–68. lishers Inc; 1997. pp. 1–25. genes to behavior: a neuroconstructivist ap-
27 Morgan AT, Su M, Reilly S, Conti-Ramsden 41 Bohland JW, Bullock D, Guenther FH. Neural proach. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2006
G, Connelly A, Liégeois FJ. A Brain Marker representations and mechanisms for the per- Mar;6(1):9–17.
for Developmental Speech Disorders. J Pedi- formance of simple speech sequences. J Cogn 57 Karmiloff-Smith A, Scerif G, Ansari D. Dou-
atr. 2018 Jul;198:234–239.e1. Neurosci. 2010 Jul;22(7):1504–29. ble dissociations in developmental disorders?
28 Luders E, Kurth F, Pigdon L, Conti-Ramsden 42 Klapp ST. Reaction time analysis of two types Theoretically misconceived, empirically du-
G, Reilly S, Morgan AT. Atypical Callosal of motor preparation for speech articulation: bious. Cortex. 2003 Feb;39(1):161–3.
Morphology in Children with Speech Sound action as a sequence of chunks. J Mot Behav. 58 Nip IS, Green JR, Marx DB. The co-emer-
Disorder. Neuroscience. 2017 Dec;367:211–8. 2003 Jun;35(2):135–50. gence of cognition, language, and speech mo-
29 Morgan A, Bonthrone A, Liégeois FJ. Brain 43 Kröger BJ, Kannampuzha J, Neuschaefer-Ru- tor control in early development: a longitudi-
basis of childhood speech and language disor- be C. Towards a neurocomputational model nal correlation study. J Commun Disord.
ders: are we closer to clinically meaningful of speech production and perception. Speech 2011 Mar-Apr;44(2):149–60.
MRI markers? Curr Opin Pediatr. 2016 Dec; Commun. 2009;51(9):793–809. 59 Duffy JR. Motor Speech Disorders: Sub-
28(6):725–30. 44 Hickok G. Computational neuroanatomy of strates, Differential Diagnosis, and Manage-
30 Bishop DV, Snowling MJ. Developmental speech production. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012 ment. St Louis (MO): Mosby; 2005.
dyslexia and specific language impairment: Jan;13(2):135–45. 60 Fletcher SG. Speech production following
same or different? Psychol Bull. 2004 Nov; 45 Guenther FH. A neural network model of partial glossectomy. J Speech Hear Disord.
130(6):858–86. speech acquisition and motor equivalent 1988 Aug;53(3):232–8.
31 Maassen B, Terband H. Process-oriented di- speech production. Biol Cybern. 1994; 72(1): 61 Skelly M, Spector DJ, Donaldson RC, Brodeur
agnosis of Childhood and adult Apraxia of 43–53. A, Paletta FX. Compensatory physiologic
Speech (CAS & AOS). In: Redford MA, edi- 46 Goldstein LM, Fowler CA. Articulatory pho- phonetics for the glossectomee. J Speech Hear
tor. Handbook of Speech Production. Malden nology: A phonology for public language use. Disord. 1971 Feb;36(1):101–14.
(MA): Wiley-Blackwell; 2015. In: Schiller NO, Meyer AS, editors. Phonetics 62 Morrish EC. Compensatory articulation in a
32 Terband H, Maassen B. Spraakontwikkel- and Phonology in Language Comprehension subject with total glossectomy. Br J Disord
ingsstoornissen: van symptoom- naar proces- and Production: Differences and Similarities. Commun. 1988 Apr;23(1):13–22.
diagnostiek. Logopedie en Phoniatrie. 2012; Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter; 2003. pp. 159– 63 Morrish L. Compensatory vowel articulation
7-8:229–34. 207. of the glossectomee: acoustic and videofluo-
33 Terband H, Maassen B, Maas E. Toward a 47 Price CJ, Crinion JT, Macsweeney M. A gen- roscopic evidence. Br J Disord Commun.
model of pediatric speech sound disorders erative model of speech production in Broca’s 1984 Sep;19(2):125–34.
(SSD) for differential diagnosis and therapy and Wernicke’s areas. Front Psychol. 2011 64 Barry WJ, Timmermann G. Mispronuncia-
planning. In: Van Lieshout P, Maassen B, Ter- Sep;2:237. tions and compensatory movements of
band H, editors. Speech Motor Control in 48 Nam H, Browman C, Goldstein L, Rubin P, tongue-operated patients. Br J Disord Com-
normal and disordered speech: Future devel- Proctor M, Saltzman E. TADA (TAsk Dy- mun. 1985 Apr;20(1):81–90.

Process-Oriented Diagnosis of DSD Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 225


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
65 Georgian DA, Logemann JA, Fisher HB. speech from speech delay: introduction. J   94 Maner KJ, Smith A, Grayson L. Influences of
Compensatory articulation patterns of a sur- Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017 Apr;60(4):S1094– utterance length and complexity on speech
gically treated oral cancer patient. J Speech 5. motor performance in children and adults. J
Hear Disord. 1982 May;47(2):154–9. 80 Dodd B, Ttofari-Eecen K, Brommeyer K, Ng Speech Lang Hear Res. 2000 Apr;43(2):560–
66 Knipfer C, Riemann M, Bocklet T, Noeth E, K, Reilly S, Morgan A. Delayed and disor- 73.
Schuster M, Sokol B, et al. Speech intelligibil- dered development of articulation and pho-   95 Nijland L, Maassen B, van der Meulen S. Ev-
ity enhancement after maxillary denture nology between four and seven years. Child idence of motor programming deficits in
treatment and its impact on quality of life. Int Lang Teach Ther. 2018;34(2):87–99. children diagnosed with DAS. J Speech Lang
J Prosthodont. 2014 Jan-Feb;27(1):61–9. 81 Dodd B. Differentiating speech delay from Hear Res. 2003 Apr;46(2):437–50.
67 Hamlet SL, Stone M. Speech adaptation to disorder: does it matter? Top Lang Disord.   96 Namasivayam AK, van Lieshout P, De Nil L.
dental prostheses: the former lisper. J Prosthet 2011;31(2):96–111. Bite-block perturbation in people who stut-
Dent. 1982 May;47(5):564–9. 82 Curtiss S, Katz W, Tallal P. Delay versus devi- ter: immediate compensatory and delayed
68 Hörschgen J, Wisser W, Berger R, Lotzmann ance in the language acquisition of language- adaptive processes. J Commun Disord. 2008
U. The influence of major connectors of par- impaired children. J Speech Hear Res. 1992 Jul-Aug;41(4):372–94.
tial dentures of phonation: an instrumental Apr;35(2):373–83.   97 Ménard L, Perrier P, Aubin JM, Savariaux C,
analysis of speech. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2004 83 Wilshire CE, McCarthy RA. Experimental In- Thibeault M. Compensation strategies for a
May-Jun;56(3):144–56. vestigations of an Impairment in Phonologi- lip-tube perturbation of French [u]: an
69 Laine T, Jaroma M, Linnasalo AL. Relation- cal Encoding. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1996; acoustic and perceptual study of 4-year-old
ships between interincisal occlusion and ar- 13(7):1059–98. children. J Acoust Soc Am. 2008 Aug;124(2):
ticulatory components of speech. Folia Pho- 84 Munson B, Krause MO. Phonological en- 1192–206.
niatr (Basel). 1987;39(2):78–86. coding in speech-sound disorder: evidence   98 Thoonen G, Maassen B, Gabrieëls F, Schreud-
70 Smith A, Goffman L. Interaction of motor from a cross-modal priming experiment. Int er R. Validity of maximum performance tasks
and language factors in the development of J Lang Commun Disord. 2017 May; 52(3): to diagnose motor speech disorders in chil-
speech production. In: Maassen B, Kent R, Pe- 285–300. dren. Clin Linguist Phon. 1999;13(1):1–23.
ters HF, et al., editors. Speech Motor Control 85 Munson B, Baylis AL, Krause MO, Yim D.   99 Thoonen G, Maassen B, Wit J, Gabrieëls F,
in Normal and Disordered Speech. Oxford, Representation and access in phonological Schreuder R. The integrated use of maxi-
UK: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 225– impairment. Lab Phonol. 2010;10:381–404. mum performance tasks in differential diag-
52. 86 Seiger-Gardner L, Brooks PJ. Effects of onset- nostic evaluations among children with mo-
71 Terband H, Maassen B. Speech motor devel- and rhyme-related distractors on phonologi- tor speech disorders. Clin Linguist Phon.
opment in childhood apraxia of speech: gen- cal processing in children with specific lan- 1996;10(4):311–36.
erating testable hypotheses by neurocompu- guage impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 100 Ziegler W. Task-related factors in oral mo-
tational modeling. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2008 Oct;51(5):1263–81. tor control: speech and oral diadochokinesis
2010;62(3):134–42. 87 Bahr R. Differential diagnosis of severe speech in dysarthria and apraxia of speech. Brain
72 Strand EA, McCauley RJ, Weigand SD, disorders using speech gestures. Top Lang Lang. 2002 Mar;80(3):556–75.
Stoeckel RE, Baas BS. A motor speech assess- Disord. 2005;25(3):254–65. 101 Ackermann H, Hertrich I, Hehr T. Oral di-
ment for children with severe speech disor- 88 Nijland L, Maassen B, Van der Meulen S, Ga- adochokinesis in neurological dysarthrias.
ders: reliability and validity evidence. J Speech breels F, Kraaimaat FW, Schreuder R. Coar- Folia Phoniatr Logop. 1995;47(1):15–23.
Lang Hear Res. 2013 Apr;56(2):505–20. ticulation patterns in children with develop- 102 Van Brenk F, Lowit A. The relationship be-
73 Chapman KL. Phonologic processes in chil- mental apraxia of speech. Clin Linguist Phon. tween acoustic indices of speech motor con-
dren with cleft palate. Cleft Palate Craniofac 2002 Sep;16(6):461–83. trol variability and other measures of speech
J. 1993 Jan;30(1):64–72. 89 Nijland L, Maassen B, Van Der Meulen S, Ga- performance in dysarthria. J Med Speech-
74 Konst EM, Rietveld T, Peters HF, Prahl-An- breëls F, Kraaimaat FW, Schreuder R. Plan- Lang Pathol. 2012;20:24–9.
dersen B. Phonological development of tod- ning of syllables in children with develop- 103 Diepeveen S, van Haaften L, Terband H, et
dlers with unilateral cleft lip and palate who mental apraxia of speech. Clin Linguist Phon. al. A standardized protocol for Maximum
were treated with and without infant orthope- 2003 Jan-Feb;17(1):1–24. Repetition Rate assessment in children. Fo-
dics: a randomized clinical trial. Cleft Palate 90 Terband H. Deviant coarticulation in child- lia Phoniatr Logop, under review.
Craniofac J. 2003 Jan;40(1):32–9. hood apraxia of speech (CAS) does not in- 104 Corley M, Brocklehurst PH, Moat HS. Error
75 van Lieshout PH, Rutjens CA, Spauwen PH. clude hyperarticulation. In: Maassen B, Ter- biases in inner and overt speech: evidence
The dynamics of interlip coupling in speakers band H, van Lieshout P, editors. 7th Inter- from tongue twisters. J Exp Psychol Learn
with a repaired unilateral cleft-lip history. J national Conference on Speech Motor Mem Cogn. 2011 Jan;37(1):162–75.
Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002 Feb;45(1):5–19. Control; 2017 July 5-7; Groningen, the 105 Coalson GA, Byrd CT, Kuylen A. Unique-
76 Whitehill TL, Francis AL, Ching CK. Percep- Netherlands. Stem-, Spraak- en Taalpathol- ness Point Effects during Speech Planning in
tion of place of articulation by children with ogie; 2017. Adults Who Do and Do Not Stutter. Folia
cleft palate and posterior placement. J Speech 91 Iuzzini-Seigel J, Hogan TP, Green JR. Speech Phoniatr Logop. 2017;69(5-6):191–208.
Lang Hear Res. 2003 Apr;46(2):451–61. inconsistency in children with childhood 106 Siegel GM, Fehst CA, Garber SR, Pick HL Jr.
77 Crary MA, Landess S, Towne R. Phonological apraxia of speech, language impairment, and Delayed auditory feedback with children. J
error patterns in developmental verbal dys- speech delay: depends on the stimuli. J Speech Hear Res. 1980 Dec;23(4):802–13.
praxia. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1984 May; 6(2): Speech Lang Hear Res. 2017 May; 60(5): 107 Yeni-Komshian G, Chase RA, Mobley RL.
157–70. 1194–210. The development of auditory feedback
78 Marion MJ, Sussman HM, Marquardt TP. 92 Marquardt TP, Jacks A, Davis BL. Token-to- monitoring. II. Delayed auditory feedback
The perception and production of rhyme in token variability in developmental apraxia of studies on the speech of children between
normal and developmentally apraxic chil- speech: three longitudinal case studies. Clin two and three years of age. J Speech Hear
dren. J Commun Disord. 1993 Sep;26(3):129– Linguist Phon. 2004 Mar;18(2):127–44. Res. 1968 Jun;11(2):307–15.
60. 93 Preston JL, Koenig LL. Phonetic variability in 108 Iuzzini-Seigel J, Hogan TP, Guarino AJ,
79 Shriberg LD, Strand EA, Fourakis M, Jakielski residual speech sound disorders: exploration Green JR. Reliance on auditory feedback in
KJ, Hall SD, Karlsson HB, et al. A diagnostic of subtypes. Top Lang Disord. 2011 Apr; children with childhood apraxia of speech. J
marker to discriminate childhood apraxia of 31(2):168–84. Commun Disord. 2015 Mar-Apr;54:32–42.

226 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 Terband/Maassen/Maas


DOI: 10.1159/000499426
109 Maas E, Mailend ML, Guenther FH. Feed- 115 ASHA. Speech-language pathology medical 121 Stackhouse J, Wells B. Psycholinguistic as-
forward and feedback control in apraxia of review guidelines: American Speech-Lan- sessment of developmental speech disor-
speech: effects of noise masking on vowel guage-Hearing Association. 2011. Available ders. Eur J Disord Commun. 1993; 28(4):
production. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015 from: www.asha.org/policy. 331–48.
Apr;58(2):185–200. 116 Kent RD. Nonspeech Oral Movements and 122 Kay J, Lesser R, Coltheart M. PALPA: Psy-
110 Terband H, van Brenk F, van Doornik-van Oral Motor Disorders: A Narrative Review. cholinguistic Assessments of Language Pro-
der Zee A. Auditory feedback perturbation Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2015 Nov; 24(4): cessing in Aphasia. Psychology Press; 2009.
in children with developmental speech 763–89. 123 Terband H, Maassen B, van Lieshout P, Ni-
sound disorders. J Commun Disord. 2014 117 Maas E. Speech and nonspeech: what are we jland L. Stability and composition of func-
Sep-Oct;51:64–77. talking about? Int J Speech Lang Pathol. tional synergies for speech movements in
111 Jacks A, Haley KL. Auditory masking effects 2017 Aug;19(4):345–59. children with developmental speech disor-
on speech fluency in apraxia of speech and 118 Staiger A, Schölderle T, Brendel B, Bötzel K, ders. J Commun Disord. 2011 Jan-Feb;
aphasia: comparison to altered auditory Ziegler W. Oral motor abilities are task de- 44(1): 59–74.
feedback. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015 Dec; pendent: a factor analytic approach to per- 124 Nijland L. Speech perception in children
58(6):1670–86. formance rate. J Mot Behav. 2017 Sep-Oct; with speech output disorders. Clin Linguist
112 Daliri A, Wieland EA, Cai S, Guenther FH, 49(5):482–93. Phon. 2009 Mar;23(3):222–39.
Chang SE. Auditory-motor adaptation is re- 119 Weismer G. Philosophy of research in motor 125 Nijland L, Terband H, Maassen B. Cognitive
duced in adults who stutter but not in chil- speech disorders. Clin Linguist Phon. 2006 functions in childhood apraxia of speech. J
dren who stutter. Dev Sci. 2018 Mar; Jul;20(5):315–49. Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015 Jun;58(3):550–
21(2):e12521. 120 Ziegler W, Ackermann H. Neuromotor 65.
113 Loucks TM, De Nil LF. The effects of mas- speech impairment: it’s all in the talking. Fo- 126 Terband H, Spruit M, Maassen B. Speech
seter tendon vibration on nonspeech oral lia Phoniatr Logop. 2013;65(2):55–67. Impairment in Boys With Fetal Alcohol
movements and vowel gestures. J Speech Spectrum Disorders. Am J Speech Lang
Lang Hear Res. 2001 Apr;44(2):306–16. Pathol. 2018 Nov;27(4):1405–25.
114 Namasivayam AK, van Lieshout P, McIlroy 127 Geronikou E, Rees R. Psycholinguistic pro-
WE, De Nil L. Sensory feedback dependence filing reveals underlying impairments for
hypothesis in persons who stutter. Hum Greek children with speech disorders. Child
Mov Sci. 2009 Dec;28(6):688–707. Lang Teach Ther. 2016;32(1):95–110.

Process-Oriented Diagnosis of DSD Folia Phoniatr Logop 2019;71:216–227 227


DOI: 10.1159/000499426

You might also like