You are on page 1of 3

Machine Translated by Google

Brain & Language 161 (2016) 1–3

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Brain & Language

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l

Editorial

The cerebellum and language: Persistent themes and findings


Julie A. Fiez
Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, Learning Research and Development Center, and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, 3939 O’Hara Street, University of
Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA

article info

Article history: This special issue brings together a set of articles that focus on the cerebellum and language. Contributors
Available online 20 September 2016 were specifically invited from relative newcomers to this research topic, as a way to draw attention to
perspectives and findings that might otherwise be overlooked. This editorial provides an overview of the
issue from a historical context that draws upon more than 25 years of research on the linguistic cerebellum.

2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

In the early 1990s, the contributions of the cerebellum to cogni et al., 2014; Marien & Manto, 2016; Stoodley & Stein, 2013). As one
tion and language became an emergent topic in the cognitive neu would expect, these reviews are predominantly authored by indi
roscience literature. As one metric of this phenomenon, Fiez (1996) viduals with a long history of research on the linguistic cerebellum.
graphed the growth of the relevant literature from a near-zero In contrast, the contributions to this special issue were invited from
baseline in 1980 to an accumulated body of more than 100 publi relative newcomers to the topic, as a way to feature new ideas and
cations by 1995. Interest was sparked by speculative review arti findings that might otherwise be overlooked. Contribu tors includes
cles that applied motor theories of cerebellar function to cognitive researchers with a long-standing interest in the cere bellum who
and language processing, using a small body of neu roanatomical, have more recently concentrated on studies of language processing,
neurophysiological, and neuropsychological findings for support as well as those with a long-standing interest in language whose
(e.g., Dow, 1974; Ito, 1993; Keele & Ivry, 1990; Leiner, Leiner, & findings have led them to become interested in the cerebellum. The
Dow, 1986, 1987; Roland, 1993; Schmahmann, 1996; Watson, resulting issue provides a current perspective on theoretical themes
1978). At roughly the same time, the emergence of func tional and empirical findings that have persisted across 25 years of
neuroimaging as a new tool was yielding surprising evidence of research on the non-motor functions of the cerebellum.
cerebellar activation linked to cognitive and linguistic task demands
(Petersen, Fox, Posner, Mintun, & Raichle, 1989). In the subsequent One persistent theme is the idea that the cellular structure and
years, interest in the linguistic cerebellum has contin ued. Indeed, connectivity of the cerebellum follows general organizational prin
searching the PubMed database using the terms ‘‘cere bellum” and ciples, and therefore it may perform a common type of computa tion
‘‘language” now identifies more than 2000 scientific publications across motor and non-motor domains. In line with this idea, Ito
each year. (1993) was an early proponent of the idea that the cerebellum makes
The growth of research on the cerebellum and language is use of internal models of a current state to make predictions about
impressive. However, it pales in contrast to the more than 20,000 a future state, so that ongoing motor (or potentially cogni tive)
publications identified by the terms ‘‘brain” and ‘‘language” appearing performance can be dynamically optimized. In this issue, the
in 2015 alone. Given this difference in scientific focus, it should continuation of this broad idea can be seen in the review by
come as no surprise that the contributions of the cerebel lum to Argyropoulos (2016) and Moberget et al. (2016), both of which
language (and cognition more generally) remain something of an consider the role of the cerebellum in linguistic prediction. In the
enigma (Marien et al., 2014). Nonetheless, significant pro gress has linguistic literature, prediction is broadly conceptualized as the use
been made: Current research now focuses on how – not if -- the of a current linguistic state to predict a future state (Pickering &
cerebellum contributes to language processing. Garrod, 2013). For instance, an internal representation of a partially
Excellent reviews of the accumulated literature on the cerebel completed sentence might be used to predict the likely syntactic and
lum and language, including those in an edited book on the topic, semantic features of an upcoming word. The concepts of prediction
are available for the interested reader (e.g., Buckner, 2013; Marien generation and prediction error have clear analogies to the concepts
of internal models and sensorimotor prediction error in the motor
E-mail address: fiez@pitt.edu literature. Extending these ideas into

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.09.004
0093-934X/ 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Machine Translated by Google

2 J. A. Fiez / Brain & Language 161 (2016) 1–3

the linguistic domain, it becomes possible to see how consistent patterns of syntactic speech to learn audio-visual associations, potentially through the use of inner speech.
structure and semantic associations could be used as a basis for linguistic predictions via
the use of internal models that involve the cerebellum (Argyropoulos, 2016). Semantic processing is another aspect of language function for which there is strong
evidence of cerebellar involvement. Indeed, some of the earliest evidence for a linguistic
In the motor literature, internal models are presumed to sup port both dynamic motor cerebellum came from a neuroimaging study that revealed greater activation in the right
coordination as well as motor learning (Ito, 2008; Thach, Goodkin, & Keating, 1992). The lateral cerebellum when individuals were asked to say aloud a semantically related verb
general idea is that the cerebellum is involved in detecting discrepancies between in response to a visually presented noun, as compared to merely reading the visually
predicted and actual states, so that prediction error signals can be used to drive the presented noun aloud (Petersen et al., 1989). In the current issue, Lesage et al. (2016)
learning that improves future performance. Some of the early evidence extending this demonstrate a connection between the cerebellum and language learning for a related
idea to the domain of language came from a single-case study of an individual with focal task involving vocabulary learning. Other articles in this issue help to bound the role of
injury to the right lateral cerebellum. He exhibited abnormal learning on word association the cerebellum in semantic processing. Moberget et al. (2016) find that individuals with
tasks, such as a verb generation task involving the repeated production of a related verb resections of cerebellar tissue that spare the right lateral cere bellum exhibit the typical
for each of a set of nouns (Fiez, Petersen, Cheney, & Raichle, 1992). In the current issue, neuroimaging signatures of semantic prediction, a result that accords with their prior
Lesage, Nailer, Miall, and Language (2016) use neuroimaging to relate brain activity localization of semantic prediction based on imaging neurotypical subjects. In his review,
during language learning to later knowledge gains. They find evidence that the recruitment Argypoulus (2016) makes an important distinction between associative versus categorical
of cerebellar, but not cerebral, regions during learning correlates with a subsequent semantic relationships, with damage to the cerebellum affecting associative semantic pro
measure of knowledge consolation. The results highlight the use of neuroimaging as a cessing but leaving categorical semantic knowledge largely or fully intact. Thus, similar to
tool for uncovering the circuits that support adaptive plasticity within the language system. the conclusions of Fernandez et al. (2016) for reading, the cerebellum appears to have a
modulatory effect on cerebral regions involved in semantic processing. To harken back to
ideas in the motor literature, ‘‘the cerebellum may be viewed as an in-line corrective
filter” (Llinas, 1974), as opposed to the originator of cognitive or motor activity.

Another theoretical strand within the cerebellum and cognition literature has been the
idea that it plays a crucial role in timing and sequencing dynamics. Again, this idea has
its roots in the motor lit erature, where it has been clear for decades that cerebellar dys
function impairs motor coordination and other timing-related aspects of motor function
(Llinas, 1974). Ivry and Keele (1989) extended these ideas into the cognitive domain, by
finding that individuals with cerebellar damage exhibit impairments in both the production In closing, over the past 25 years there have been persistent themes about the
and perception of temporal sequences. In this issue, Schwatze and Kotz (2016) bring underlying nature of the linguistic cerebellum and accumulated research findings that
together the proposed roles of the cerebellum in prediction and timing, by focusing on point towards productive avenues for continued research. Relative newcomers to the
temporal structure in speech perception. They argue that the temporal struc ture of study of the linguistic cerebellum contributed the articles in this special issue. The resulting
speech, at multiple and distinct levels, is itself a source of information that can be used to fresh perspectives and novel data move the field forward while retaining a connection to
dynamically enhance speech per ception. Moreover, they speculate about the potential longstanding ideas.
role of the cerebellum in using synchrony as a mechanism for coordinating frontal and
temporal regions within the cerebral cortex in service of speech perception. The result is
a very modern twist on a long standing idea.

References

Argyropoulos, G. P. D. (2016). The cerebellum, internal models and prediction in ’non-


motor’ aspects of language: A critical review. Brain and Language, 161, 4– 17.

Buckner, R. L. (2013). The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight


The articles in this issue also draw attention to domains of lan guage function for from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron, 80, 807–816.
which there has been persistent evidence for cerebellar involvement. One domain is Dow, R. S. (1974). Some novel concepts of cerebellar physiology. Mount Sinai Journal
of Medicine, 41, 103–119.
reading. Fawcett, Nicolson, and Dean (1996) observed that children with develop mental
Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., & Dean, P. (1996). Impaired performance of children with
dyslexia exhibit impairments in motor function that are similar to those observed in dyslexia on a range of cerebellar tasks. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 259–263.
individuals with cerebellar dysfunc tion. This led them to propose a cerebellar deficit Fernandez, VG, Juranek, J., Romanowska-Pawliczek, A., Stuebing, K., Willliams, V.
J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2016). White matter integrity of cerrebellar-cortical tracts in
hypothesis of developmental dyslexia. It has been a minor strand within the reading
reading impaired children: A probabilistic tractography study. Brain and Language,
literature, but one that warrants increased attention (Stoodley & Stein, 2013). In this issue, 161, 45–56.
new contributions to this area of research are provided by Fernandez et al. (2016) and Fiez, J. A., Petersen, S. E., Cheney, M. K., & Raichle, M. E. (1992). Impaired non-motor
Moberget et al. (2016). Fernandez et al. tackle this question by using diffusion tensor learning and error detection associated with cerebellar damage. A single case study.
Brain, 115, 155–178.
imaging to contrast the integrity of cerebro-cerebellar connectivity in children with versus Trust, JA (1996). Cerebellar contributions to cognition. Neuron, 16, 13–15.
without a reading impairment. They evidence of enhanced connectivity between the Ito, M. (1993). Movement and thought: Identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum.
anterior cerebellum and reading-related regions in the temporal, parietal, and frontal Trends in Neurosciences, 16, 448–450.
Ito, M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum.
cortices. They conclude the cere bellum has a regulatory effect on cortical pathways for Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9, 304–313.
reading. Ivry, R. B., & Keele, S. W. (1989). Timing functions of the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 1, 136–152.
Keele, S. W., & Ivry, R. (1990). Does the cerebellum provide a common computation for
diverse tasks? A timing hypothesis. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
608, 179–207.
Leiner, H. C., Leiner, A. L., & Dow, R. S. (1986). Does the cerebellum contribute to
mental skills? Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 443–454.
The results from Moberget et al. provide insight into the mecha nisms that might underlie
Leiner, H. C., Leiner, A. L., & Dow, R. S. (1987). Cerebro-cerebellar learning loops in
cerebellar regulation of the reading net work. They report evidence that damage to the apes and humans. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 8, 425–436.
anterior cerebellum reduces the functional activation of auditory cortex and parietal Lesage, E., Nailer, E. L., & Miall, R. C. (2016). Cerebellar BOLD signal during the
acquisition of a new lexicon predicts its early consolidation. Brain and Language, 161,
operculum during silent word reading. To account for these results, they suggest the
33–44.
cerebellum may be involved in the use of inner Llinas, R. (1974). Eighteenth Bowditch lecture. Motor aspects of cerebellar control.
Physiologist, 17, 19–46.
Machine Translated by Google

J. A. Fiez / Brain & Language 161 (2016) 1–3 3

Marien, P., Ackermann, H., Adamaszek, M., Barwood, C. H., Beaton, A., Desmond, J. E., ... Roland, P. E. (1993). Partition of the human cerebellum in sensory-motor activities, learning
Ziegler, W. (2014). Consensus paper: Language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. and cognition. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 20, S75–S77.
Cerebellum, 13, 386–410. Schmahmann (1996). From movement to thought: Anatomic substrates of the cerebellar
Marien , P. , & Cloak , M. (Eds.). (2016). The linguistic cerebellum. Waltham, MA: contribution to cognitive processing. Human Brain Mapping, 4,
Elsevier. 174–198.
Moberget, T., Hilland, E., Andersson, S., Lundar, T., Due-Tonnessen, B. J., Heldal, A., ... Schwatze, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2016). Contributions of cerebellar event-based temporal
Endestad, T. (2016). Patients with focal cerebellar lesions show reduced auditory cortex processing and preparatory function to speech perception. Brain and Language, 161, 28–
activation during silent reading. Brain and Language, 161, 18– 27. 32.
Stoodley, C. J., & Stein, J. F. (2013). Cerebellar function in developmental dyslexia.
Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., & Raichle, M. E. (1989). Positron Cerebellum, 12, 267–276.
emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. Journal of Cognitive Thach, W. T., Goodkin, H. P., & Keating, J. G. (1992). The cerebellum and the adaptive
Neuroscience, 1(2), 153–170. coordination of movement. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 403–442.
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language prediction and Watson, P. J. (1978). Nonmotor functions of the cerebellum. Psychological Bulletin, 85(5),
comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 329–347. 944–967.

You might also like