You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/297698569

Personality of Clown Doctors. An exploratotory study

Article  in  Journal of Individual Differences · February 2016


DOI: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000187

CITATIONS READS

18 526

1 author:

Alberto Dionigi
Federazione Nazionale Clowndottori
38 PUBLICATIONS   350 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

RISU - Rivista Italiana di Studi sull'Umorismo View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Alberto Dionigi on 22 November 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Volume 37 / Number 1 / 2016

Volume 37 / Number 1 / 2016

Journal of

Individual
Differences

Editor-in-Chief
Journal of Individual Differences

André Beacuducel

Associate Editors
Philip J. Corr
Sam Gosling
Jürgen Hennig
Philipp Y. Herzberg
Aljoscha Neubauer
Thomas Rammsayer
Willibald Ruch
Stefan Schmukle
Astrid Schütz
Andrzej Sekowski
Jutta Stahl
Martin Vocarek

jid_55-0-41-3_58-60_positiv.indd 3 09.12.2015 11:14:30


Contents
Original Articles Gymnasts and Orienteers Display Better Mental Rotation Performance 1
Than Nonathletes
Mirko Schmidt, Fabienne Egger, Mario Kieliger, Benjamin Rubeli,
and Julia Schüler

Conscientiousness Is the Most Powerful Noncognitive Predictor 8


of School Achievement in Adolescents
Barbara Dumfart and Aljoscha C. Neubauer

Perception of Emotional Expressions in Adults: The Role 16


of Temperament and Mood
Chit Yuen Yi, Matthew W. E. Murry, and Amy L. Gentzler

The Relationships Between the Dark Triad, the Moral Judgment Level, 24
and the Students’ Disciplinary Choice: Self-Selection, Indoctrination,
or Both?
Annika Krick, Stephanie Tresp, Mirijam Vatter, Antonia Ludwig,
Michael Wihlenda, and Martin Rettenberger

Social Support, Emotional Intelligence, and Posttraumatic Stress 31


Disorder Symptoms: A Mediation Analysis
Nicole L. Hofman, Austin M. Hahn, Christine K. Tirabassi,
and Raluca M. Gaher

Rank-Order Consistency and Profile Stability of Self- and Informant- 40


Reports of Personal Values in Comparison to Personality Traits
Henrik Dobewall and Toivo Aavik

Personality of Clown Doctors: An Exploratory Study 49


Alberto Dionigi

Validation and Revision of a German Version of the Balanced Measure 56


of Psychological Needs Scale
Andreas B. Neubauer and Andreas Voss

Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1)


Journal of
Individual
Differences

Your article has appeared in a journal published by Hogrefe Publishing.


This e-offprint is provided exclusively for the personal use of the authors. It may not be
posted on a personal or institutional website or to an institutional or disciplinary repository.

If you wish to post the article to your personal or institutional website or to archive it
in an institutional or disciplinary repository, please use either a pre-print or a post-print of
your manuscript in accordance with the publication release for your article and our
‘‘Online Rights for Journal Articles’’ (www.hogrefe.com/journals).
Author’s personal copy (e-offprint)

Original Article

Personality of Clown Doctors


An Exploratory Study
Alberto Dionigi
Department of Education, Cultural Heritage and Tourism, University of Macerata, Italy

Abstract. In recent years, both professional and volunteer clowns have become familiar in health settings. The clown represents a peculiar
humorist’s character, strictly associated with the performer’s own personality. In this study, the Big Five personality traits (BFI) of 155 Italian
clown doctors (130 volunteers and 25 professionals) were compared to published data for the normal population. This study highlighted specific
differences between clown doctors and the general population: Clown doctors showed higher agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness, and
extraversion, as well as lower neuroticism compared to other people. Moreover, specific differences emerged comparing volunteers and
professionals: Professional clowns showed significantly lower in agreeableness compared to their unpaid colleagues. The results are also
discussed with reference to previous studies conducted on groups of humorists. Clowns’ personalities showed some peculiarities that can help to
explain the facility for their performances in the health setting and that are different than those of other groups of humorists.

Keywords: clown doctors, personality profile, Big Five, volunteers, professionals

Humor is an important part of life. Numerous studies have consistent with research conducted on male professional
been conducted in order to understand why people laugh, British cartoonists (Pearson, 1983), who were found to be
and theories have been proposed to explain the mechanisms high on Psychoticism suggesting a link between artistic cre-
of humor (for a review, see Ruch, 2008). Research has ativity and Psychoticism. The psychological profile of pro-
mainly dealt with individual differences in the appreciation fessional comedians was also compared to clowns’ profiles
of humor by most people while the psychological character- in a Rorschach inkblot test (Fisher & Fisher, 1981). Come-
istics of comic performers, such as cartoonists, humor writ- dians showed more references to themes of good and evil
ers, stand-up comedians, and clowns, have only marginally and a lower perception of self-worthiness and made a
been investigated. In particular, studies have been con- higher number of negative remarks about themselves.
ducted on humorists aimed at investigating whether they According to these results, an unusual personality structure
have particular personality traits that differ from those of may help to explain the facility for comedians’ perfor-
the others and whether there are differences with amateur mances and be helpful in making people laugh. As Psych-
or less effective humor producers. Generally, a distinction oticism is closely related to creativity (Eysenck, 1995), the
in personality traits depending upon being a creator (e.g., high P-scorer is more capable of coming up with unusual,
writers) or an interpreter (e.g., actors) was highlighted incongruous, and wittier punch lines (Köhler & Ruch,
(Kogan, 2002). 1996).
The psychological research in this field has mainly Research on the personality of humorists has mainly
focused on comedians’ personality, which revealed a spe- focused on comedians, although clowns – as pranksters,
cific pattern. In an early study, following a psychoanalytical jesters, jokers, harlequins, and mythologized tricksters –
approach based both on projective tests and analyses of have been around for quite a long time and have attracted
dreams, comedians were found to be sad, depressive, much popular interest. The clown is, by definition, a curi-
despondent, and angry (Janus, 1975). When investigating ous figure that projects itself on a different wavelength than
gender differences, male comedians were more introverted that of societies’ status quo (Dionigi, Ruch, & Platt, 2014).
than females, who were found to be vivacious, frenetic, and The clown is a performer who acts foolishly and childishly
hypomanic (Janus, Bess, & Janus, 1978). When compared in order to elicit positive emotions, and it is strictly associ-
to the general population, both professional and amateur ated with the performer’s own personality, physical body,
comedians showed significantly lower conscientiousness, and subjectivity (Lecoq, 2011). This personality is often
extraversion, agreeableness, and higher openness compared also reflected in a specific costume, as adopting a costume
to college students (Greengross & Miller, 2009). Interest- frequently helps to construct and define a new role (Miller,
ingly, comedians and actors were also found to score higher Jasper, & Hill, 1991). Differently from an actor, a clown
in Psychoticism with respect to the rest of society’s norms does not play a character and develops this based on the
(Ando, Claridge, & Clark, 2014). These findings are performer’s physical and psychological characteristics

Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1):49–55


DOI: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000187
Author’s personal copy (e-offprint)
50 A. Dionigi: Clown Doctor’s Personality

(Peacock, 2009). Clowns have appeared in a large variety of Neurotic individuals are more prone to experience anx-
environments, and, in the last three decades, many more iety, worry about life events, and evaluate themselves more
have been integrated into health settings. This practice critically (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Furthermore, anxious
was first started by Michael Christensen in 1986, a profes- individuals are more susceptible to focusing on potential
sional clown who set up the first Clown Care Unit (Dionigi, threats in their environment (Eysenck & Derakshan,
Flangini, & Gremigni, 2012). 2011). High neuroticism in performers leads to stronger
The art of clinic clowning is rooted both in the figures stage fright (Steptoe et al., 1995). As clown doctors are
of fools and jesters and in shamans and folk healers. In required to stay in tune with patients and manage their
many cultures, people believe that fools, clowns, and trick- and others’ emotions in order to perform well and accom-
sters possess magical powers, enabling healing (Grinberg, plish their task, they were predicted to score low on neurot-
Pendzik, Kowalsky, & Goshen, 2012). Although the figures icism as compared to the general non-clown population.
of the circus clown and of the clown doctors are often Finally, as an increasing number of people practice as
linked, fundamental differences exist between them. These clown doctors, they include a wide variety of professional
differences lie in the goal and methodology of clowning uti- backgrounds (Koller & Gryski, 2008). Currently, it is com-
lized in a health setting. The main aim of circus and street mon to find both professional and volunteer clown doctors
clowns is to entertain and amuse audiences, while the pur- in hospitals (Dionigi, Ruch, & Platt, 2014), so differences in
pose of clown doctors is to assist patients in the healing pro- personality traits between volunteers and professionals were
cess, distracting them from painful procedures, reducing also investigated.
anxiety triggered by hospitalization, and enhancing their
moods (Dionigi, Sangiorgi, & Flangini, 2014; Grinberg
et al., 2012; Pendzik & Raviv, 2011). To accomplish this Aim of the Study
goal, clown doctors must be bright performers and talented
humorists, yet be empathetic and attentive to patients’ At the time of this writing, there is no reported research
needs. Moreover, they must have high emotional intelli- about personalities of clowns working in health settings.
gence as their purpose is to modify the moods of patients The purpose of this study was to investigate the psycholog-
and change their emotional states to positive ones (Dionigi ical characteristics of a sample of Italian clown doctors,
et al., 2012; Warren & Spitzer, 2013). both professionals and volunteers, and to compare findings
Research has been carried out on a variety of other with published normative data. Due to the characteristics
humorists and social groups, as yet no empirical studies required to work as a clown in health settings, it was
have been done on the personality traits of clown doctors. expected that clown doctors would score higher in agree-
Therefore, to further understand the effectiveness of these ableness, extraversion, conscientiousness, and openness
clowns in their dual roles as healers and humorists, it is and lower in neuroticism when compared to the normal
of interest to establish what the typical psychological char- population. Moreover, as friendliness and sociability go
acteristics of clown doctors are. The purpose of this study is along with the need to help it was expected that volunteers
to investigate the personalities of a sample of clown doc- score higher in agreeableness compared to professional
tors, using the five-factor model of personality (FFM) clowns.
(McCrae & Costa, 2003). The hypothesis was that the per-
sonality profiles of clown doctors are significantly different
from the general non-clown population.
As performing artists, clowns show similarities in per-
sonality when compared to actors and comedians as clown Method
doctors perform in front of crowds to elicit positive emo-
tions. High extroversion is associated with the desire for Participants
being the center of social attention (Nettle, 2006), so clown
doctors were expected to score high on extroversion. The total sample consisted of 155 clown doctors (40 males,
Clown doctors, similar to comedians, tune their acts to 115 females) varying in age between 21 and 66 years
the crowd’s reactions (Greengross & Miller, 2009). They (M = 37.99, SD = 10.25). Participants were well-educated
must also put the patients’ needs first and cooperate with adults (0.6% primary school, 5.8% low secondary school,
colleagues, healthcare staff, and patients’ relatives. There- 41.9% upper secondary school, 52.3% university). With ref-
fore, clown doctors were predicted to score high on erence to marital status, 86 were not married, 51 were mar-
agreeableness. ried or cohabiting, 15 were divorced, and three were
Previous studies have shown that creative people widowed. Participants had different levels of experience
are low on conscientiousness and high on openness in the art of clowning in health settings (M = 5.51 years,
(Greengross & Miller, 2009; Nettle, 2006; Nowakowska, SD = 3.07, range = 0–13 years).
Strong, Santosa, Wang, & Ketter, 2005). Creativity is
required in clown doctors as well; however, since they per-
form in peculiar settings, such as hospitals, they must man- Instruments
age their emotions and be self-disciplined. Clown doctors
were thus expected to score high on both conscientiousness Participants completed a short demographic questionnaire
and openness. and the Big Five Inventory (BFI; John & Srivastava, 1999)

Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1):49–55 Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing


Author’s personal copy (e-offprint)
A. Dionigi: Clown Doctor’s Personality 51

to assess the Big Five dimensions of personality. The BFI participants (female = 56.9%, M = 34.80, SD = 14.53,
consists of 44 short-phrase items, rated on a 5-point scale range 18–80 years old) recruited to study the psychometric
(1 = disagree strongly to 5 = agree strongly). The items assess properties of the Italian adaptation of the BFI (Ubbiali
the core traits that define each Big Five domain. The BFI et al., 2013). The clown doctor scores on the scales showed
items are assigned to five scales measuring Extraversion good to acceptable reliabilities (Cronbach’s a: Openness to
(E; 8 items), Agreeableness (A; 9 items), Conscientiousness experience: .80; Conscientiousness: .79; Extraversion: .77;
(C; 9 items), Neuroticism (N; 8 items), and Openness to Agreeableness: .70; Neuroticism: .75). Comparisons
experience (O; 10 items). Prior researchers have presented between means, standard deviations of BFI traits, as well
adequate evidence of the reliability and validity of the BFI as correlations with age, and education are reported in
scales (John & Srivastava, 1999). In this study, the Italian Table 1.
version translated and validated by Ubbiali, Chiorri, Each dimension of the FFM was compared to the values
Hampton, and Donati (2013) was used. of the Italian population (Ubbiali et al., 2013) using a one-
sample t-test (Table 1). A significant difference in each
concept of the Big Five was found (all p < .001), with a
Procedures large effect size in O (d = 0.86) and medium effect size
in A (d = 0.67), N (d = 0.66), E (d = 0.46), C (d = 0.42).
Data was collected in an online survey, and the study was Thus, the sample of clown doctors investigated can gener-
conducted among a broad sample of Italian clown doctors. ally be described as more extraverted, agreeable, conscien-
Participants were recruited via e-mails sent to the coordina- tious, open, and low in neuroticism compared to the general
tors of Italian clown care units asking them to forward to population. In the clown doctors’sample, a positive correla-
clown doctors belonging to each unit. The e-mail contained tion (r = .19, p < .05) emerged between age and agreeable-
a link to the survey created on Survey Monkey and an ness while older clown doctors scored lower in neuroticism
explanation of the aim of the study. All participants were (r = .16, p < .05). No significant correlations were found
guaranteed anonymity. The sample that fully completed between the five personality traits and education.
the questionnaire was composed of 155 clown doctors. An independent-samples t-test was also conducted to
All statistical procedures were performed using the software compare the scores on the Big Five dimensions of male
package SPSS (21.0, IBM Inc., New York, NY, USA). and female clown doctors (Table 2).
Female clowns scored higher in neuroticism compared
to males (Mmales = 2.37, SD = 0.66; Mfemales = 2.69,
SD = 0.64); t(153) = 2.76, p = .06, with a moderate effect
Results size (d = 0.49), while no other significant gender-related
differences emerged. In their study, Ubbiali et al. (2013)
Comparison Between the Clown Doctor computed a point-biserial correlation, showing a significant
Sample and the General Population relationship between genders of Italian population and spe-
cific dimensions of the Big Five: Females were found to be
Comparison data for the FFM personality traits were taken significantly higher in Agreeableness and Neuroticism
from a large Italian general sample composed of 1,023 compared to males.

Table 1. BFI scale means, standard deviations, associations with age and education of clown doctors and italian
population
Clown doctors General population
E A C N O E A C N O
Min 2.25 2.44 1.89 1.00 2.50 1.00 1.89 1.33 1.00 1.00
Max 4.88 5.00 5.00 4.50 5.00 5.00 5.00 5.00 5.00 5.00
M 3.63 4.09 3.90 2.61 4.18 3.32 3.72 3.62 3.09 3.67
SD 0.61 0.51 0.60 0.66 0.51 0.74 0.59 0.74 0.78 0.67
t-test values (df = 154)
t 6.88*** 8.94*** 5.78*** 9.17*** 12.64***
d 0.46 0.67 0.42 0.66 0.86
Associations with age and education
Age .06 .19* .11 .16* .08 .00 .09** .26** .18** .06*
Education .05 .13 .01 .05 .07 .01 .03 .10** .14** .08**
Notes. E = Extraversion; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness; N = Neuroticism; O = Openness; Min/Max = minimum/
maximum; M = Mean; SD = Standard Deviation; df = degree of freedom; d = Effect size; Number of Clown Doctors = 155 (%female =
74.20, M =37.99, SD = 10.25 range 21–66 years old). Source of general population data: Ubbiali et al. (2013), n = 1023 (%female =
56.9, M = 34.80, SD = 14.53, range 18–80 years old). *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. All tests were two-tailed.

Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1):49–55


Author’s personal copy (e-offprint)
52 A. Dionigi: Clown Doctor’s Personality

Table 2. Independent t-test for gender differences in five factor model personality traits in clown doctors
Females Males
M SD M SD t Effect size
Extraversion 3.64 0.62 3.70 0.58 0.54 .10
Agreeableness 4.09 0.49 4.08 0.57 0.18 .02
Conscientiousness 3.91 0.58 3.88 0.66 0.26 .05
Neuroticism 2.69 0.64 2.37 0.66 2.76** .49
Openness 4.19 0.51 4.16 0.49 0.29 .06
Notes. Number of female clown doctors = 115; number of male clown doctors = 40; Effect size = d. **p < 0.01 (two-tailed).

Differences Between Volunteers and gender] revealed a significant group difference for
and Professionals Agreeableness as volunteer clowns scored higher than pro-
fessionals, F(1, 151) = 6.29, p < .01, and a moderate effect
In order to assess differences between clowns, two sub- size ( f = .57). ANCOVA revealed no main effects of
groups were established: Volunteer and professional clown gender, F(1, 151) = .02, p = .88, and a main effect of
doctors. The volunteer group was composed of participants age, F(1, 151) = 5.67, p < .05.
whose clown activity does not represent their main job and
who are not paid for it. The professional group includes par-
ticipants who reported to do this activity as a job and who
receive money for it. The volunteer group was composed of Discussion
130 clown doctors (31 males and 99 females; M = 38.15,
SD = 10.61, range = 21–66 years old) while the profes- This is the first study aimed at investigating personality
sional group included 25 people (9 males and 16 females; characteristics of clown doctors; the data for this study
M = 37.16; SD = 8.28; range = 27–62 years old). shows that this sample of clown doctors showed peculiar
In the next step, the Big Five dimensions of volunteer characteristics that distinguish them from the general Italian
and professional clowns were compared. Due to the small non-clown population. Clown doctors score higher in Extra-
sample of professional clowns, the assumption of normality version, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness
of all five dimensions of the Big Five was examined using to experience, and lower in Neuroticism compared to pub-
normal probability plots. No apparent deviations from nor- lished normative data of the populace. These results con-
mality emerged. Following, Levene’s homogeneity of vari- firm what was suggested by theoretical manuscripts
ance test was conducted: The variances of the Big Five written in this area (Dionigi et al., 2012; Warren & Spitzer,
dimensions of the two groups were not different from each 2013).
other (Extraversion: F(1, 153) = 0.32, p = .57; Agreeable- The clown doctor is a comic character required to be
ness: F(1, 153) = 0.26, p = .61; Conscientiousness: creative in order to improvise according to what is found
F(1, 153) = 1.61, p = .21; Neuroticism: F(1, 153) = 3.19, in the healthcare setting. Extraversion is related to the flu-
p = .77; Openness to experience: F(1, 153) = 0.95, ency component of creativity (Eysenck, 1995) and humor,
p = .33). Therefore, it was safe to continue with the creativity, and extraversion have been found to be positively
ANOVA, controlling for both age and gender. Results are correlated (Koppel & Sechrest, 1970). Extraverts are char-
presented in Table 3. acterized by traits such as being sociable, lively, active,
Table 3 shows the comparisons among volunteer and assertive, tending to enjoy human interactions, and being
professional clown doctors groups based on the Big Five enthusiastic, talkative, assertive, and gregarious. In the field
personality. An ANCOVA [between-subjects factor: of humor, extraverts (as compared to introverts) are more
professionalism (volunteer, professional); covariates: age cheerful, less serious, and able to produce a higher quantity

Table 3. Comparisons between volunteer and professional clown doctors


Volunteer clowns Professional clowns
M SD M SD F Effect size
Extraversion 3.67 0.61 3.60 0.58 0.53 .12
Agreeableness 4.13 0.51 3.85 0.48 6.29** .57
Conscientiousness 3.94 0.61 3.71 0.51 2.83 .41
Neuroticism 2.60 0.68 2.62 0.50 0.10 .03
Openness 4.18 0.51 4.21 0.49 0.16 .06
Notes. Number of volunteers = 130; number of professionals = 25; Effect size = f. **p < 0.01 (two-tailed).

Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1):49–55 Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing


Author’s personal copy (e-offprint)
A. Dionigi: Clown Doctor’s Personality 53

of humor content (Köhler & Ruch, 1996). As clowns the performance. Clown doctors who were better able to
expose themselves with the intention of being the laughing differentiate between their persona and their clown persona
stock for others, probably extraversion may provide respon- were less interfered by external cues and internal thoughts;
siveness toward the interpersonal reward of being the center they experienced less anxiety before and during the activity
of an audience’s attention (Nettle, 2006). (Dionigi, Ruch, & Platt, 2014).
Clown doctors show high Agreeableness compared to The results of the present study also shed light on how
other people. This dimension is related to the ability to groups of humorists may differ. A cheerful temperament is
be sensitive toward others’ needs, compassionate, and coop- involved in the sense of humor and it goes along with high
erative. Moreover, other public figures who want to be extraversion, emotional stability, agreeableness, and open-
loved and appreciated, such as actors and politicians, are ness, and low conscientiousness (Ruch & Köhler, 1998).
high on this dimension (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Consiglio, Compared to this, clowns are relatively similar with the
Picconi, & Zimbardo, 2003; Nettle, 2006). Agreeableness exception that they are higher (and not lower) in conscien-
represents the core dispositional trait contributing to proso- tiousness. In particular, volunteers are high on both agree-
cial behaviors as agreeable individuals are altruistic, ableness and conscientiousness. Clowns thus seem to be
straightforward, trusting, softhearted, modest, and compli- very different though from the witty professionals and witty
ant (McCrae & Costa, 2003). Researchers found significant amateurs. Cartoonists (Pearson, 1983) and those able to
positive relations between agreeableness and volunteering, provide funnier punch lines are high in P (Köhler & Ruch,
as people scoring high on Agreeableness are more likely 1996) and stand-up comedians are higher in schizotypy
to comply with requests from others (Carlo, Okun, Knight, (Ando et al., 2014). Likewise, stand-up comedians in
& de Guzman, 2005). In the present study, volunteers Greengross and Miller’s (2009) study were significantly
scored higher than professionals in agreeableness. One pos- lower in both Conscientiousness and Agreeableness. High
sible explanation can be that volunteers are more altruistic, P is an indicator of low Agreeableness and low Conscien-
modest, and compliant compared to their paid colleagues tiousness, and here we can assume that witty people and
(Liao-Troth, 2005). clowns are on the opposite poles of the Psychoticism
The sample of clown doctors was also high on Consci- dimensions. However, clowns (in this study), comedians
entiousness compared to normative data. Clowns are (Greengross, Martin, & Miller, 2012; Greengross & Miller,
required to attend rigorous training before entering the 2009), and witty people share higher openness to experi-
health setting and must be aware of their role as well as ence. In their study, Ruch and Köhler (1998) showed that
of the risks related to a wrong approach to patients or to the ability to produce witty punch lines goes along with
a lack of competence. As conscientious individuals are able high openness and low seriousness. The clowns in the pres-
to manage emotions, be self-disciplined, and focus on goal ent study were higher in Extraversion and lower in Neurot-
achievement, it is not surprising that clown doctors score icism, which is again different from the witty. Comedians
high on this trait. were lower in Extraversion (Ando et al., 2014; Greengross
As predicted, clown doctors have been found to be more & Miller, 2009; Greengross et al., 2012).
open to experience than the general population. This find- The results of this study showed that the sample of clown
ing is consistent with those of previous studies that showed doctors examined possess specific personality characteris-
that other creative groups are also high on Openness to tics in comparison to Italian non-clown population. Signifi-
experience (Nettle, 2006). Clown doctors are required to cant differences were found between volunteers and
be curious and possess a high imagination as they must look professionals. Although these findings shed light on a topic
at the world in a different, unconventional, and imaginative that previous authors have only addressed theoretically, some
way where (almost) anything is possible and permitted limitations to extending the findings to other clown doctors
(Dionigi, Ruch, & Platt, 2014). Furthermore, Openness to need to be acknowledged. The study was conducted on a
experience has consistently been positively related to crea- sample of Italian clown doctors, and further research is
tivity in general, and clown doctors need to continually needed to confirm these results for other cultures and nation-
adjust their way to approach patients in order to make their alities. The relatively small sample of professional clown
work evolve. doctors further limits the findings, and replications on larger
Finally, clown doctors scored low in Neuroticism. This samples are required. A comparison was not possible with
result is not surprising, as neuroticism is made up of traits published normative data where study samples had been sep-
like anxious, depressed, guilt feelings, low self-esteem, irra- arated into subgroups by age and gender, which prevented
tional, and shyness; that is not representative of the clown’s verification of personality differences using these criteria.
profile. Moreover, neuroticism was found to be negatively Future studies will include a control group, instead of com-
correlated with cheerfulness (Köhler & Ruch, 1996). paring results with published data, in order to address this
Studies on other creative people like poets and writers show concern, as well as to investigate possible differences in per-
that they are high in this dimension. However, differently sonalities of clinic versus circus clowns.
from clown doctors, they do not have to perform their Finally, as personality is a complex phenomenon, fur-
creation on stage (Nowakowska et al., 2005). Research con- ther research should explore specific personality traits that
ducted on comedians showed no differences in neuroticism can play a role in clinic clowning, such as narcissism and
compared with the rest of society (Greengross & Miller, emotional intelligence. Narcissists support their self-image
2009). This finding is consistent with what has been through feedback and admiration received from others
recently found about the ability to remain in the role during (Atlas & Them, 2008), which may lead to feelings of

Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1):49–55


Author’s personal copy (e-offprint)
54 A. Dionigi: Clown Doctor’s Personality

anxiety after perceived negative feedback. These individu- ability of professional stand-up comedians compared to
als may fail to develop appropriate strategies for dealing college students. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and
with intense emotions. On the other hand, emotional intel- the Arts, 6, 74–82. doi: 10.1037/a0025774
Greengross, G., & Miller, G. F. (2009). The Big Five personality
ligence has been indicated as important for clown doctors, traits of professional comedians compared to amateur
since individuals scoring high on emotional intelligence are comedians, comedy writers, and college students. Personal-
better able to use, manage, understand, and pay attention to ity and Individual Differences, 47, 79–83. doi: 10.1016/
emotions (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2004). j.paid.2009.01.045
Grinberg, Z., Pendzik, S., Kowalsky, R., & Goshen, Y. (2012).
Drama therapy role theory as a context for understanding
Acknowledgments medical clowning. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 39, 42–51.
doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2011.08.005
Janus, S. S. (1975). The great comedians: Personality and other
The study was conducted during the research stay of the factors. American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 35, 169–174.
author at the University of Zürich: the author is grateful doi: 10.1007/BF01358189
to Willibald Ruch, Jennifer Hofmann and Tracey Platt for Janus, S. S., Bess, B. E., & Janus, B. R. (1978). The great
the precious help and assistance received. Many thanks to comediennes: Personality and other factors. The American
Carlo Chiorri, Chiara Manfredi and Giulia Casu for the Journal of Psychoanalysis, 38, 367–372. doi: 10.1007/
BF01253595
helpful hints and comments. John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big five trait taxonomy:
History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A.
Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory
and research (pp. 102–138). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
References Kogan, N. (2002). Careers in the performing arts: A psycho-
logical perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 14, 1–16.
Ando, V., Claridge, G., & Clark, K. (2014). Psychotic traits in doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ1401_1
comedians. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 204, 341–345. Köhler, G., & Ruch, W. (1996). Sources of variance in current
doi: bjp-bp.113.134569 sense of humor inventories: How much substance, how much
Atlas, G. D., & Them, M. A. (2008). Narcissism and sensitivity method variance? Humor, 9, 363–398. doi: 10.1515/
to criticism: A preliminary investigation. Current Psychol- humr.1996.9.3-4.363
ogy, 27, 62–76. doi: 10.1007/s12144-008-9023-0 Koller, D., & Gryski, C. (2008). The life threatened child and the
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Consiglio, C., Picconi, L., & life enhancing clown: Towards a model of therapeutic
Zimbardo, P. G. (2003). Personalities of politicians and clowning. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative
voters: Unique and synergistic relationships. Journal of Medicine, 5, 17–25. doi: 10.1093/ecam/nem033
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 849. doi: 10.1037/ Koppel, M. A., & Sechrest, L. (1970). A multitrait-multimethod
0022-3514.84.4.849 matrix analysis of sense of humor. Educational and
Carlo, G., Okun, M. A., Knight, G. P., & de Guzman, M. R. T. Psychological Measurement, 30, 77–85. doi: 10.3102/
(2005). The interplay of traits and motives on volunteering: 00346543046003407
Agreeableness, extraversion and prosocial value motivation. Lecoq, J. (2011). The moving body (Le corps poetique):
Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1293–1305. Teaching creative theatre. London, UK: Methuen.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.08.012 Liao-Troth, M. A. (2005). Are they here for the long haul? The
Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO effects of functional motives and personality factors on the
personality inventory (NEO-PI-R and NEO five-factor psychological contracts of volunteers. Nonprofit and Volun-
inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: tary Sector Quarterly, 34, 510–530. doi: 10.1177/
Psychological Assessment Resources. 0899764005279513
Dionigi, A., Flangini, R., & Gremigni, P. (2012). Clowns in Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). TARGET
hospitals. In P. Gremigni (Ed.), Humor and health promotion ARTICLES: ‘‘Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and
(pp. 213–227). New York, NY: Nova Science. implications’’. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 197–215.
Dionigi, A., Ruch, W., & Platt, T. (2014). Components and doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02
determinants of the shift between own persona and the clown McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2003). Personality in
persona: A hierarchical analysis. European Journal of adulthood: A five-factor theory perspective. New York,
Humour Research, 1, 58–80. NY: Guilford Press.
Dionigi, A., Sangiorgi, D., & Flangini, R. (2014). Clown Miller, K. A., Jasper, C. R., & Hill, D. R. (1991). Costume and
intervention to reduce preoperative anxiety in children and the perception of identity and role. Perceptual and Motor
parents: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Health Skills, 72, 807–813.
Psychology, 19, 369–380. doi: 10.1177/1359105312471567 Nettle, D. (2006). Psychological profiles of professional actors.
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Creativity as a product of intelligence and Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 375–383.
personality. In D. H. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.008
International handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. Nowakowska, C., Strong, C. M., Santosa, C. M., Wang, P. W., &
231–247). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Ketter, T. A. (2005). Temperamental commonalities and
Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in differences in euthymic mood disorder patients, creative
attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Dif- controls, and healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disor-
ferences, 50, 955–960. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019 ders, 85, 207–215. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.11.012
Fisher, S., & Fisher, R. L. (1981). Pretend the world is funny and Peacock, L. (2009). Serious play: Modern clown performance.
forever: A psychological analysis of comedians, clowns, and Bristol, UK: Intellect Books.
actors. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Pearson, P. (1983). Personality characteristics of cartoonists.
Greengross, G., Martin, R. A., & Miller, G. F. (2012). Person- Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 227–228.
ality traits, intelligence, humor styles, and humor production doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(83)90030-2

Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1):49–55 Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing


Author’s personal copy (e-offprint)
A. Dionigi: Clown Doctor’s Personality 55

Pendzik, S., & Raviv, A. (2011). Therapeutic clowning and Date of acceptance: May 12, 2015
drama therapy: A family resemblance. The Arts in Published online: February 29, 2016
Psychotherapy, 38, 267–275. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2011.08.005
Ruch, W. (2008). Psychology of humor. In V. Raskin (Ed.), The
primer of humor research (pp. 17–100). Berlin, Germany:
Mouton de Gruyter.
Ruch, W., & Köhler, G. (1998). A temperament approach to
humor. In W. Ruch (Ed.), The sense of humor: Explorations
of a personality characteristic (pp. 203–230). Berlin, Alberto Dionigi
Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.
Steptoe, A., Malik, F., Pay, C., Pearson, P., Price, C., & Win, Z.
(1995). The impact of stage fright on student actors. British Department of Education
Journal of Psychology, 86, 27–39. doi: 10.1111/j.2044- Cultural Heritage and Tourism
8295.1995.tb02544.x University of Macerata
Ubbiali, A., Chiorri, C., Hampton, P., & Donati, D. (2013). Piazzale Luigi Bertelli (Contrada Vallebona)
Psychometric properties of the Italian adaptation of the Big 62100 Macerata
Five Inventory (BFI). Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata, Italy
266, 37–48. Tel. +39 05 4169-7122
Warren, B., & Spitzer, P. (2013). Smiles are everywhere: Fax +39 05 4160-0386
Integrating clown-play into healthcare practice. London, E-mail alberto.dionigi@unimc.it
UK: Routledge.

Ó 2016 Hogrefe Publishing Journal of Individual Differences 2016; Vol. 37(1):49–55

View publication stats

You might also like