You are on page 1of 19

Scholarly Article

Journal of Service Research


2022, Vol. 0(0) 1–19
Service Provider to the Rescue: How Firm © The Author(s) 2022
Article reuse guidelines:
Recovery of Do-It-Yourself Service Failure sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/10946705221111347
journals.sagepub.com/home/jsr
Turns Consumers from Competitors to
Satisfied Customers

Matthew J. Hall1  and Jamie D. Hyodo2 

Abstract
While consumers frequently attempt to resolve their own consumption problems (i.e., do-it-yourself (DIY)), they are often
unsuccessful and subsequently turn to a professional. In the present research, we consider DIY failure as a form of service failure
(SF) and demonstrate that experiencing DIY service failure (DIY SF) influences consumer evaluations of subsequent firm recovery.
This occurs because consumers who experience DIY SF gain greater understanding of the task (i.e., learning) through their failed
attempt. This learning promotes increased appreciation of the recovering service provider’s ability, ultimately resulting in greater
satisfaction with the recovery offering. We further identify mindset as a moderator of this effect, wherein those with a growth
mindset are more likely to learn from failure and appreciate the abilities of the recovering service provider. By highlighting DIY SF as
a novel form of SF, we demonstrate the importance of understanding customers’ prior experiences with the focal consumption
problem and its solution, and of training front-line employees to better manage these customers. We test our theory across four
studies using lab and field data, and close by discussing theoretical and managerial implications.

Keywords
do-it-yourself service failure, service failure and recovery, do-it-yourself, mindset, learning

Driven by increased access to information, tools, and other annually on professional home improvement services alone
resources, consumers are increasingly trying to resolve their after unsuccessfully attempting a DIY solution (Centrica 2017).
own consumption problems (Bosari 2012). Such instances are Because many DIY activities are service-oriented in nature,
broadly referred to as do-it-yourself (DIY). Do-it-yourself oc- we employ services theory to better understand service provider
curs when amateur consumers aggregate various resources to offerings following consumers’ DIY failures. Specifically, we
engage in creation, repair, reconstruction, or maintenance ac- apply a service failure (SF) lens to this subset of DIY activities
tivities (Kusnetsov and Paulos 2010; Williams 2004; Wolf and and refer to this phenomenon as DIY service failure (DIY SF).
McQuitty 2011) that would more typically be performed by a Through this lens, a firm’s follow-up offering could be con-
professional and sourced from the marketplace (Bonvoisin, sidered a form of service recovery. While extensive research has
Jahnavi, and Prendeville 2017; Murphy 2017; Nagel, Cronin, considered the implications of SF and recovery for firms (e.g.,
and Utecht 2018). As such, DIY represents competition to firms Hess, Ganesan, and Klein 2003; McCollough, Berry, and Yadav
(Bitner et al. 1997). This is particularly relevant to service 2000), this work generally assumes the customer has engaged in
providers, as DIY activities span domains including appliance an exchange relationship with a firm at the time of SF, and that
repair, plumbing and electrical, landscaping, home improve- this relationship persists through firm recovery (i.e., as a static
ment and repair, car maintenance, and more (Wolf and consumer-firm dyad; Grégoire and Mattila 2021). With DIY SF,
McQuitty 2013).
Notably, DIY only represents competition for service pro-
viders to the extent it is successful, and many consumers fail 1
Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR, USA
2
when attempting DIY. Further, following DIY failure, con- Western University, London, ON, Canada
sumers often turn to a professional service provider to complete
Corresponding Author:
the unresolved production task (Wolf and McQuitty 2011). Matthew J. Hall, Oregon State University, 2751 SW Jefferson Way, Corvallis,
Anecdotally, this is a substantial phenomenon. For example, in OR 97331, USA.
the United Kingdom, consumers spend an estimated £34 billion Email: matthew.hall@oregonstate.edu
2 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

however, the customer has not engaged in exchange with a firm effort during a DIY SF experience (i.e., more DIY effort) are
when failure occurs, meaning the recovering firm cannot be held more satisfied with firm recovery. Central to this effect is
responsible for the failure, nor expected to atone for it through consumer learning, as those who exert greater effort attempting
recovery—two underlying premises of SF research. This a DIY solution are more likely to increase their understanding of
highlights a need for new theory to better understand DIY SF. the task and its requirements. This learning enhances consumer
In the present research, we suggest experiencing DIY SF will appreciation for the service provider’s abilities, increasing
systematically influence consumer perceptions of a firm’s satisfaction with firm recovery. Consistent with theory on
subsequent (recovery) offering. We leverage theory on DIY, SF, learning and effort, we find consumer mindset moderates these
and learning to demonstrate that consumers who exert more effects, with growth-minded consumers more likely to learn

Table 1. Review of Extant DIY Research.

DIY Contexts Studied or DIY Antecedents


Author(s) Definition/Conceptualization Identified as DIY

Wolf (2016) Activities in which amateur, untrained Home repair, cooking, decoration, Consumer ability to aggregate task-related
individuals learn how to do specialized crafting, clothing customization, information
tasks makeup, and IT/computers
Wolf and Activities in which individuals engage raw Remodeling, car repair, plumbing, Lack of marketplace product availability/
McQuitty materials/components to produce, woodworking, electrical, quality, economic benefits,
(2011; 2013) transform, or reconstruct material masonry, painting, and appliance customization, and identity
possessions, including those drawn from repair or installation enhancement
the natural environment
Williams (2004) Activities in which customers complete Painting, electric, carpentry, HVAC, Customization and identity factors among
tasks rather than outsourcing the labor construction, plumbing, and affluent consumers. More economic-
insulation based factors for less affluent
consumers
Watson and Activities in which consumers are actively Plumbing, crafting, painting, and Consumers must acquire skills and
Shove (2008) and creatively engaged in integrating and HVAC improve them through practice
transforming complex arrays of material
goods
Vannini and When consumers symbolize and exercise Home repair and improvement, Desire to express oneself and the
Taggart (2014) knowledge capital, lifestyle choices, and gardening convenient utilization of money-saving
autonomous control over possessions skills. DIY is also often a social activity
and their personalization (“doing it with others”; DIWO)
Nagel, Cronin, When consumers decide to self-produce a Baking, construction, painting, auto DIY (or the make/buy decision) is a
and Utecht solution rather than hiring a service repair, tax preparation, moving function of the resources (skills, money,
(2018) provider services, and flooring etc.) the customer is able to aggregate
Murphy (2017) The performance of a task by oneself Home repairs, moving, cooking, Consumers primarily engage in DIY to
instead of hiring an outside party to cleaning, and auto repair conserve resources
perform it
Moisio, Arnould, Activities involved in self-producing/ Home improvement White-collar men use DIY to feel blue-
and Gentry improving tangible elements of the home collar. Blue-collar men use DIY to save
(2013) money
Kuznetsov and Any creation, modification, or repair of Building custom computers and Driven by desire to share ideas, learn, and
Paulos (2010) objects without the aid of a paid software engage creativity, as well as economic
professional value
Khademi-Vidra Activities, services, and products Crafting, home renovation and Rational (economic), identity-related (self-
and Bujdosó implemented by one’s own hands rather decoration expression), and necessity-based
(2020) than under industrial circumstances (customization) motives
Gelber (1997) When non-professionals carry out tasks Home repair, plumbing, furniture DIY is a way for men to reclaim their
traditionally done by professionals repair, and general home masculinity in the home
maintenance
Bonvoisin, Galla, A method of building, modifying, or Product design/creation Consumers use home-based production
and Prendeville repairing things without the direct aid of to avoid makerspace-based or mass
(2017) experts or professionals production
Alhashem, When consumers take full responsibility for Construction, baking, carpentry, Motivated by enjoyment, fun, cost savings,
Moraes, and conception and production of their own and electrical self-sufficiency, identity, and learning.
Szmigin (2020) products and services Also by desire to engage with family and
friends
Hall and Hyodo 3

from failure, and thus more likely to report greater satisfaction their needs (Khademi-Vidra and Bujdosó 2020; Williams 2004;
with the recovery offering. Wolf and McQuitty 2011).
By identifying the link between consumers’ DIY SF expe- While successful DIY represents competition for service
riences and their evaluations of firm recovery, we respond to providers, not all DIY attempts succeed (Wolf and McQuitty
Grégoire and Mattila’s (2021) call to examine SF and recovery 2011). These failed DIY attempts often lead consumers to seek
beyond the static customer-firm dyad. By doing so, we highlight professional offerings to address their unresolved consumption
a novel SF and recovery phenomenon and also explicate novel problems. To our knowledge, no research has explicitly con-
implications of DIY activities for marketers. We next review sidered the potential impact of DIY failure on firm outcomes—a
research on DIY and SF, highlighting its implications for DIY gap we address in the present research. We focus on service-
SF. We then outline our hypotheses, which we test across four oriented DIY activities and propose DIY failure represents a
studies. type of SF, wherein the consumer’s attempt to generate a DIY
service solution has fallen short of their expectations (Hoffman
and Bateson 1997). We further propose that when consumers
seek firm offerings to complete the unresolved task, such so-
Conceptual Background lutions thus represent a type of service recovery. Collectively,
Internal Exchange and DIY we refer to these as DIY service failure (DIY SF) and recovery.
We next draw on SF and recovery literature to highlight how this
Marketing research is largely tasked with understanding ex- prior work aligns with and diverges from DIY SF.
change (Bagozzi 1979). Such exchange can be broadly cate-
gorized as either external or internal. External exchange occurs
SF and Recovery
when consumers seek solutions to consumption problems via
marketplace offerings, while internal exchange occurs when Failure is generally recognized as inevitable in the production of
consumers satisfy needs on their own as an alternative to services. As such, researchers have invested substantial efforts
marketplace options (Lusch, Brown, and Brunswick 1992). For investigating SF and recovery (e.g., De Matos, Henrique, and
example, a consumer needing to paint their home or renovate a Rossi 2007; Hess, Ganesan, and Klein 2003; Maxham and
bathroom could hire a professional painter or contractor (i.e., Netemeyer 2002; McCollough, Berry, and Yadav 2000; Smith,
external exchange), or could attempt these tasks themselves Bolton, and Wagner 1999). Research in this space generally
(i.e., internal exchange). aligns with one of two models to understand consumer re-
In recent years, internal exchange has become more popular, sponses to SF. The first is a relationship-based trust and
largely due to increased consumer access to tools, information, commitment model (e.g., Morgan and Hunt 1994), which
and resources (Bosari 2012). Such internal exchange activities considers how the firm’s violation of consumer trust (via failure)
are often colloquially referred to as DIY. While marketers and leads consumers to believe the firm should atone for such vi-
consumers alike use the term DIY to refer to a broad set of olations. The second model focuses on causal attributions in
consumer activities, many conceptualizations and definitions of which consumers assign blame for failure in varying magni-
DIY exist in the literature (see Table 1). Despite these many tudes depending on features of the SF experience (e.g., Hess,
conceptualizations, the literature largely agrees on a few factors. Ganesan, and Klein 2003; Van Vaerenbergh et al. 2014). The
First, DIY involves amateur consumers aggregating resources foremost of these attributions is the causal locus of failure (i.e.,
(e.g., equipment, skills, and materials) to engage in creation, whether the customer or firm caused the failure; Choi and
reconstruction, maintenance, or repair activities (Bonvoisin, Mattila 2008). To date, SF research has primarily focused on
Jahnavi, and Prendeville 2017; Kusnetsov and Paulos 2010; cases in which the firm is held responsible for failure. In these
Nagel, Cronin, and Utecht 2018; Watson and Shove 2008; Wolf cases, SF shifts customer expectations for subsequent firm
and McQuitty 2011). Second, consumers who engage in DIY interactions, thus informing evaluations of firm recovery (via an
eschew marketplace solutions—they are aware of professional expectation-disconfirmation process; Oliver 1980), as well as
solutions but opt to engage in DIY as an alternative to mar- evaluations of the overall SF and recovery experience (De
ketplace exchange (Lusch, Brown, and Brunswick 1992; Matos, Henrique, and Rossi 2007).
Murphy 2017; Williams 2004). Third, relative to similar phe- While both traditional SF and DIY SF feature failure and
nomena like self-servicing (e.g., Meuter et al. 2000) or crafting recovery experiences, traditional SF differs from DIY SF in
(Campbell 2005), DIY activities generally require greater effort, important ways. First, traditional SF research situates failure and
involvement, and resources (Wolf and McQuitty 2011; 2013). recovery within a static customer-firm dyad (Grégoire and
Relatedly, the literature highlights that the decision to engage in Mattila 2021), meaning failure and recovery are connected
DIY is complex and multifaceted, as consumers engage in DIY through shared relationship structure (both failure and recovery
to save time and money (Khademi-Vidra and Bujdosó 2020; occur within external exchange), shared actors (the customer
Williams 2004; Wolf and McQuitty 2011), test their skills and firm stay consistent from failure to recovery), and shared
(Watson and Shove 2008), maintain or enhance identity context (failure and recovery center on the same consumption
(Moisio, Arnould, and Gentry 2013; Wolf and McQuitty 2011), problem). In the case of DIY SF, however, the connection
or customize production when marketplace options do not meet between the failure and recovery events is limited to the shared
4

Table 2. Review of Research on Consumer-Attributed Production Failure.

Description of Outcome for


Form of Customer Involvement Exchange Exchange Outcome for Exchange New, Post-
Customer in Failed Production Examples and/or Relationship at Partner Customer-centric Partner at the Time of failure Exchange
Involvement Authors Activities Contexts of Study Time of Failure? Post- failure Outcomes Failure Partner

Co-production Bendapudi and Customer is involved in Assembling/designing Yes N/A N/A Participating (vs. not) in N/A
Leone (2003) good or service furniture/clothing failure does not
production increase
dissatisfaction
Sugathan and Customers applying Designing a computer Yes Same firm Attributing failure to Those attributing co- N/A
Ranjan resources (e.g., effort, table or bicycle effort (ability) production failure to
(2020) skill, and knowledge) increases (decreases) low effort are more
in the creation of likelihood to try task likely to coproduce in
products and services again the future
Pacheco, Customer participation Meal Yes Same firm If failure is attributed Internal attributions for N/A
Becker, and in service customization, internally, it leads to co-production failure
Brei (2017) specification and planning a workout regret/ increase
delivery disappointment dissatisfaction
Zolfagharian, Customer participation Designing a personal Yes Same firm N/A Higher customer N/A
Felix, and in the workout/diet plan, participation and low
Braun (2018) performance of serving oneself at a efficacy lead to more
various activities in restaurant/bar negative responses to
the production stage co-produced failure
of a service offering
Dong et al. Customer participation Internet setup, using Yes Same firm N/A High efficacy and N/A
(2015) in service production online rental portals internal attributions
and delivery for failure increase
recovery
expectations
Self-service Yi and Kim Firm provides Airport check-in Yes N/A Self-service failure leads N/A N/A
(2017) infrastructure; counters to helping behaviors
customers create the
service
Köcher and Degree to which Ticket kiosks, furniture Yes Same firm N/A Involvement in N/A
Paluch customers are assembly coproduction lessens
(2019) involved in the dissatisfaction
service delivery
process
Zhu et al. Customers obtain Car rental kiosks, ATM Yes Same firm N/A Internal attributions N/A
(2013) services without lead to higher
direct employee recovery
assistance expectations
Collier et al. None provided Self-checkout, sales Yes Same firm N/A Public self-service N/A
(2017) kiosks, and ATMs failure reduces
subsequent self-
service
Journal of Service Research 0(0)

(continued)
Table 2. (continued)

Description of Outcome for


Form of Customer Involvement Exchange Exchange Outcome for Exchange New, Post-
Hall and Hyodo

Customer in Failed Production Examples and/or Relationship at Partner Customer-centric Partner at the Time of failure Exchange
Involvement Authors Activities Contexts of Study Time of Failure? Post- failure Outcomes Failure Partner

Co-creation Heidenreich Joint value creation by Designing custom Yes Same firm Customer involvement Increased participation N/A
et al. (2015) the company and the shoes, online travel in recovery should in production
customer booking match that of the generates
failure dissatisfaction
Sugathan et al. Customers apply Designing clothing or Yes N/A Attributing failure to N/A N/A
(2017) resources (skills, furniture effort/ability leads to
effort, etc.) in the negative affect
creation of products/
services
Prosumption Xie, Bagozzi, Customer creation Preparing a meal Possible, but N/A Failure decreases N/A N/A
and Troye activities that result in not required intentions to try the
(2008) production of same task in the
products they future
consume
Customer Chen (2018) Customer involvement Taking a course, Yes Same firm N/A High efficacy increases N/A
participation in service delivery getting a haircut firm attributions of
failure
Yen, Gwinner, The actions and Actively participating Yes N/A N/A Increased co- N/A
and Su resources supplied by in a college course production
(2004) customers for service participation in
production/delivery increases firm
attributions for
failure
Do-it-yourself Wolf and Engaging components to Maintenance/repair, No N/A Failure leads to negative N/A N/A
(DIY) McQuitty produce or transform home improvement, affect and identity
(2011; 2013) possessions, including landscaping, etc threat; decreases
those drawn from the control and DIY
environment intentions
DIY service Present Failed service is Plumbing, computer No New firm Effort exerted in DIY SF N/A Effort exerted in
failure (DIY research produced by the repair/programming increases learning DIY SF
SF) customer, without increases
any involvement by satisfaction
the firm with firm
recovery
5
6 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

context. These distinctions have important implications re- within external exchange relationships, and consumer-centric
garding the extent to which SF theory can inform DIY SF outcomes following failure in the context of internal exchange.
theory. Most prominently, due to the absence of shared actors, In the present research, we bridge these literature streams by
the customer cannot attribute responsibility for failure to a firm, considering the implications of internal exchange failure for
meaning the recovering firm has not betrayed trust or acted firm-centric outcomes. Specifically, we examine how experi-
unfairly, and thus cannot be expected to atone for failure. encing DIY SF influences consumer evaluations of firm
Consistent with this perspective, research considering the case recovery.
of a firm recovering another firm’s failure (i.e., external re-
covery) found the recovering firm was able to exceed customer
expectations which had been reduced due to SF experienced Theoretical Development: Toward a Theory of DIY SF
with another firm (Allen et al. 2015). Like DIY SF, this is a SF
phenomenon in which the failure and recovery events lack
and Recovery
shared actors. While the failure and recovery events in the case Given DIY SF is defined by an absence of shared actors and
of DIY SF additionally lack shared relationship structure, it is shared relationship structure across the failure and recovery
possible firms may similarly benefit when providing recovery events, the only connection between these events is the context
offerings after DIY SF. (i.e., the focal task). Because of this, we propose DIY SF is most
Despite structural differences between traditional SF and DIY likely to influence evaluations of firm recovery by altering
SF, it remains likely that, as in the case of traditional SF, con- consumer perceptions of said context, specifically regarding the
sumers who experience DIY SF engage in an attributional pro- task itself and consumers’ own inability to complete the task.
cess, and that this process influences subsequent perceptions of Therefore, we propose consumer responses to the DIY SF
related offerings. In the absence of shared actors or relationship experience will influence how they perceive the failed task, and
structure, however, consumers are unable to assign SF respon- therefore the offering of the service provider who ultimately
sibility to the recovering firm. Rather, consumers are limited to completes it.
attributing failure to external factors like the nature of the failed When individuals attempt a task and fail, it is often thought to
task, or to internal factors (which necessitates taking responsibility be a primarily negative experience. While failure is generally
for failure upon themselves; Sugathan and Ranjan 2020). While aversive, it can have silver linings. One positive outcome that
consumers are generally averse to taking responsibility for neg- can result from failure is increased learning about the failed task
ative outcomes (per the self-serving bias; Folkes 1988), research (Kapur 2010). In general, learning is a pleasurable experience
on co-produced SFs suggests consumers sometimes attribute co- (Goetz et al. 2006) that can take the form of either increased
produced SFs internally—attributions that generate feelings of general understanding of the principles related to a task (i.e.,
guilt (Heidenreich et al. 2015) and regret (Pacheco, Becker, and conceptual learning), or of increased ability to complete the task
Brei 2017). Some research finds internal failure attributions re- in the future (i.e., procedural learning; Rittle-Johnson and
duce disappointment and dissatisfaction with the firm (Bendapudi Alibali 1999). In the case of DIY SF, learning is especially likely
and Leone 2003; Pacheco, Becker, and Brei 2017), while other because the initial decision to engage in DIY is often predicated
research has found the opposite (Heidenreich et al. 2015). Col- on the belief that one has the skills needed to complete the
lectively, this highlights the importance and nuance of consumer production activity (Watson and Shove 2008; Xie, Bagozzi, and
responses to failure, and suggests the potential for these responses Troye 2008). Thus, when this assumption is violated by failure,
to influence subsequent service provider evaluations. consumers are likely to reassess what they know about the task,
Table 2 provides an overview of research relating to con- a practice that often results in conceptual learning (Steenhof
sumer participation in failed production offerings. This research et al. 2019). Indeed, learning resulting from DIY SF should
primarily focuses on two phenomena. First, an emerging re- predominantly take the form of conceptual learning given that
search stream examines instances in which consumers attribute consumer abandonment of the task in favor of a professional
some degree of responsibility for co-produced or co-created SFs recovery offering suggests a lack of substantive increases in
to themselves. Because such failures take place within an ex-
ternal exchange relationship between a customer and firm,
research on this topic has primarily considered firm-related
outcomes of these consumer-attributed failures (e.g., recovery
satisfaction, future co-production intentions). A second litera-
ture stream considers the case of DIY failure. Because DIY, by
definition, occurs in the context of internal exchange (i.e., in the
absence of exchange with a firm), research on the topic pri-
marily focuses on consumer-related outcomes of DIY failure
(e.g., negative affect, identity outcomes; Wolf and McQuitty
2011, 2013). Collectively then, research in this space has fo-
cused on identifying outcomes that align with the nature of the Figure 1. Conceptual model of the effect of do-it-yourself service
exchange relationship—firm-centric outcomes following failure failure on consumer responses to firm recovery.
Hall and Hyodo 7

one’s ability to complete the task. Thus, we propose consumers learn from failed tasks. One factor that largely determines these
who experience DIY SF and subsequently seek a firm recovery outcomes is consumer mindset. Mindset is a continuous con-
offering are most likely to experience learning in the form of struct pertaining to an individual’s lay theories regarding the
increased general understanding of the task (Feather 1967; extent to which traits like ability and intelligence are malleable
Kapur 2010) and of one’s own limitations in ability or versus fixed, and the extent to which these views influence self-
knowledge that impeded success (DeCaro and Rittle-Johnson regulatory processes and outcomes (Burnette et al. 2013; Dweck
2012; Kulhavy 1977). While minimal skill acquisition might and Leggett 1988). On one end of this continuum are growth-
occur following DIY SF, we use the term learning to refer to a minded individuals, who believe traits like ability and intelli-
general increase in understanding of the principles related to the gence can be improved through experience and effort. On the
failed task. other end of this continuum are those with a fixed mindset, who
To this point, we have largely discussed DIY SF in binary believe abilities and knowledge are more stable.1
terms (i.e., failure vs. non-failure). However, a consumer who Because mindset influences one’s views of improvement and
moves on from a task after a five-minute attempt is unlikely to ability, growth- and fixed-minded individuals respond very
respond in the same manner as a consumer who spends an entire differently to failure. In general, growth-minded individuals are
day working prior to realizing failure. This suggests DIY SF highly motivated to enhance their knowledge and abilities,
experiences exist on a continuum of the effort consumers exert meaning they view failure as an opportunity to better understand
prior to experiencing failure and seeking firm recovery—we the failed task, which facilitates improvement (Dweck and
refer to this as DIY effort. Do-it-yourself effort has direct im- Leggett 1988). Those with a growth mindset also view effort
plications for learning from DIY SF. For example, higher DIY as a necessary component of learning and improvement
effort may involve multiple attempts to complete the task, likely (Burnette et al. 2013). As such, when growth-minded con-
resulting in the consumer testing various strategies (Kapur sumers exert greater effort during a failed attempt, they are less
2010). As consumers exert more DIY effort, they should be- likely to fixate on negative outcomes associated with failure
come more familiar with the components of the task itself, and (Senko and Harackiewicz 2005). Instead, they focus on un-
with the necessary skills required to complete it—skills the derstanding why this level of effort was insufficient, which
consumer should now realize they do not possess (Bloom further increases learning (Dweck 2006). Beyond learning from
1974). Alternatively, rapidly abandoning a task is unlikely to the attempt itself, when growth-minded individuals fail, they
generate learning because it entails minimal experience with the also report increased interest in (and thereby reflection on) the
task. Thus, when consumers experience DIY SF, we propose failed task (Burns and Gentry 1998). This increased interest,
those who exert greater DIY effort prior to failure will realize combined with the desire to learn, means growth-minded in-
greater learning. dividuals are more likely to pay attention to and acknowledge
Why might the learning consumers realize from their DIY SF the abilities of those who can complete the failed task (Smiley
experiences matter to service providers? When one better un- et al. 2016). Ultimately, these factors suggest that when DIY SF
derstands a task and the abilities needed to complete it, this often is followed by firm recovery, there will be a positive effect of
promotes recognition of the abilities of others who can complete DIY effort on satisfaction among growth-minded consumers.
the task—particularly when one lacks said abilities (Feather and Conversely, fixed-minded individuals find failure highly
Simon 1971). Thus, when consumers experience DIY SF, those aversive because they believe failure signals low ability, which
who realize greater learning about the failed task should better cannot be easily improved (Burnette et al. 2013). Fixed-minded
appreciate the ability and knowledge of the service provider individuals are thus motivated to distance themselves from
who ultimately resolves the task (Tsiros, Mittal, and Ross 2004; failed tasks by attributing failure to external sources (e.g., bad
Specht, Fichtel, and Meyer 2007). Because consumers tend to luck or situational circumstances) to avoid undermining self-
be more satisfied when interacting with more competent and efficacy (Dweck and Leggett 1988). While those with a fixed
knowledgeable service providers (e.g., Price and Arnould mindset may acknowledge a gap between their own ability and
1999), we propose that when DIY SF occurs, consumers that required to complete a failed task, they are not motivated to
who have exerted greater DIY effort, and thus better understand close this gap because they do not believe ability can be
the task and its requirements, will report greater satisfaction substantially improved (Burns and Gentry 1998). As such,
with the firm’s recovery offering (Figure 1). failure often results in fixed-minded individuals losing interest
in the failed task, minimizing its perceived importance, and
H1: When DIY SF occurs, consumers who exerted greater often dissociating from the task entirely (Senko and
DIY effort will be more satisfied with subsequent firm Harackiewicz 2005; Smiley et al. 2016). Thus, compared to
recovery. their growth-minded counterparts, fixed-minded consumers
H2: This effect will be mediated by learning about the task should not only be less likely to learn from their DIY SF ex-
and the skills required to complete it. periences, but should also be less likely to acknowledge the
recovering provider’s skills or be interested in their recovery
Our theory proposes that learning from failure is central to offering. In sum, the effect of DIY effort on satisfaction with
the effect of DIY effort on satisfaction with firm recovery. Even firm recovery should be attenuated among fixed-minded con-
beyond effort, however, consumers vary in their likelihood to sumers (Figure 1).
8 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

H3: The effect of DIY effort on satisfaction with firm re- experience (all items on a 5-point scale; 1 = very dissatisfied, 5 =
covery will be moderated by mindset, such that the effect will very satisfied). The survey was designed so the “overall service
be stronger for growth-minded consumers than for fixed- experience” measure was a reflective measure of the other items.
minded consumers. Given our model predicts increased satisfaction as a function of
increased perceptions of the provider’s abilities (i.e., compe-
tence as opposed to warmth), we use the initial “quality of
Overview of Studies service provided” measure as our dependent variable and
We present four studies to test our hypotheses. Study 1 employs control for timeliness and friendliness.2
a field study that measures DIY effort and mindset to test our We were permitted to append two items to the CHC’s
basic interaction model. Study 2 employs a novel paradigm to standard survey. To assess DIY effort, participants completed
test these effects in a lab experiment featuring a volitional DIY one item: “How much effort did you exert trying to resolve your
activity, while also testing our proposed learning-based process. computer-related problem yourself prior to seeking CHC ser-
Study 3 provides additional process-based evidence, while also vices?” (0 = did not attempt, 1 = low effort, 5 = high effort; M =
examining how learning and satisfaction with firm recovery 3.26, SD = 1.43). Six participants indicated they did not attempt
influence consumers’ downstream DIY and loyalty intentions. a DIY solution. Because these individuals did not experience
Importantly, we also manipulate mindset in Study 3 to provide DIY SF, they were excluded from our analyses, resulting in a
additional causal evidence for our theoretical model. Lastly, final sample of 169 participants. To assess mindset, participants
Study 4 concludes by leveraging our theoretical model to design indicated their agreement with, “People have a certain amount
and test a service provider intervention that maximizes customer of ability and you can’t do much to change it” (from Chiu et al.
satisfaction with firm recovery following DIY SF. 1997; 1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree; reverse-coded to
reflect growth mindset; M = 3.53, SD = 1.22).3
Study 1: Field Evidence
Results
In Study 1, we partnered with a real service organization to
provide an initial test of our proposed theoretical framework. We conducted a regression of service quality as a function of
Consistent with the suggestions of Van Heerde et al. (2021), this effort and mindset (both standardized), and their interaction,
study was thus designed to establish initial ecological validity with service timeliness and provider friendliness as covariates
for our theory. Specifically, we partnered with the Computer (all reported coefficients are standardized). This model revealed
Help Center (CHC) at a large university to test the effect of DIY a significant main effect of DIY effort (β = .079, SE = .042,
effort and mindset on customer satisfaction with firm recovery t(163) = 2.17, p = .032), but no main effect of mindset (β = .044,
in the context of technology repair. The CHC offers a broad SE = .042, t(163) = 1.20, p = .232). Importantly, the hypoth-
range of technology services and repairs, both of which are esized interaction was observed (β = .075, SE = .038, t(163) =
domains in which DIY activities are common (Kuznetsov and 2.09, p = .038). To probe this interaction, we tested the effect of
Paulos 2010; Wolf 2016). Discussions with CHC technicians DIYeffort on service quality for those with more growth (+1SD)
confirmed customers often attempt DIY solutions prior to versus fixed ( 1SD) mindsets. There was a positive effect of
seeking CHC services, with efforts ranging from simply re- DIY effort on perceived quality for more growth-minded par-
starting one’s computer or researching solutions online to ticipants (β = .187, SE = .061, t(163) = 3.08, p = .003), but not
completely removing hardware components. Consistent with for more fixed-minded participants (β = .01, SE = .066, t(163)
our conceptualization of DIY, these initial consumer attempts =.15, p = .875). A Johnson-Neyman test revealed a significant
involve customers aggregating skills, knowledge, and other effect of DIY effort on perceived quality for those with a
resources to engage in creation, repair, or maintenance mindset above 3.42 on the 5-point scale ( .09 SD; 51.5% of
activities—activities that occur in lieu of seeking a marketplace participants). Do-it-yourself effort did not negatively predict
offering. However, when their own solution fell short of ex- perceived quality at any level of mindset.
pectations, they turned to the CHC to resolve their problem.
This collectively suggests many CHC customers have indeed
experienced DIY SF—experiences that involve a wide range of
Discussion
DIY effort—and the CHC offering represents a recovery This study provided initial support for our framework using
offering. field data, thus establishing ecological validity. It confirmed
consumers not only exert varying levels of effort in their DIY SF
experiences, but also that they seek firm recovery after DIY SF.
Method Consistent with our model, consumers who experienced DIY
CHC customer feedback surveys were collected over a 2-month SF and subsequently sought a firm recovery offering were more
period (n = 175). The standard CHC survey assessed customer satisfied with the recovery offering when they had exerted more
satisfaction with the quality of service provided, friendliness of effort in their failed DIY attempt (supporting H1). Also as
the technician, and timeliness of the service experience. Next, it predicted, this effect was moderated by mindset, as it was
measured customer satisfaction with the overall service stronger for growth-minded consumers than for fixed-minded
Hall and Hyodo 9

consumers (supporting H3). Given the field setting of this study, Measures. After the TA completed the task, participants re-
it is possible external factors (e.g., the varying nature of the ported satisfaction with the TA’s services (three 7-point bipolar
tasks) influenced these effects. Study 2 thus employs a lab items; displeased/pleased, negative/positive, dissatisfied/satisfied;
setting to better control for these potential externalities. α = .95) and completed a mindset scale (4 items; adapted from
Chiu et al. 1997): People have a certain amount of ability and
you can’t do much to change it (R); You can learn new things
Study 2: Experimental Replication but you can’t change your basic ability level (R); No matter
The primary purpose of Study 2 was to establish internal how much ability you have, you can always change it quite a
validity for our theoretical model. We thus employed a con- bit; You can always substantially change how much ability you
trolled lab simulation of a DIY activity to standardize the DIY have (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree; higher scores
task and recovery experiences. In this paradigm, participants indicate a growth mindset; M = 5.10, SD = 1.02; α = .87). To
were asked to complete a task in Microsoft Excel. Consistent assess general learning related to the task, participants com-
with our conceptualization of DIY, participants initially at- pleted two items: I learned something new; I learned some-
tempted the task on their own. In so doing, they aggregated thing valuable (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree; r =
resources (knowledge, time, and information) to attempt the .54). Last, participants reported Excel experience (little ex-
task. They could also expend resources at any time to outsource perience = 1; significant experience = 7; M = 4.17, SD = 1.30)
the task to a professional. Upon failing the task, participants and demographics. Excel experience predicted DIY effort (r =
were required to pay the professional to complete the task (i.e., .217, p = .012), but our results are insensitive to its inclusion as
provide a recovery offering). An important benefit of this a covariate (see Web Appendix D).
paradigm was that it allowed for volitional exertion of varying
levels of DIY effort prior to seeking the professional’s services.
A second purpose of this study was to test our proposed Results
learning-based process (H2).
Satisfaction. We estimated a regression of satisfaction as a
function of DIY effort and mindset (both standardized), and
Method their interaction (all reported coefficients are standardized).5
This model revealed a main effect of DIY effort (β = .267, SE =
Participants and design. Undergraduate participants (n = 152)
.093; t(131) = 3.19, p = .002), no main effect of mindset (β =
completed this study for extra course credit. Seventeen par-
.112, SE = .092; t(131) = 1.35, p = .178), and a significant
ticipants successfully completed the assigned task. Because
interaction (β = .418, SE = .081; t(131) = 5.21, p < .001).
they neither experienced DIY SF nor required the profes-
Replicating findings from Study 1, a spotlight analysis indi-
sional’s assistance, they were removed from the data set. This
cated a positive effect of DIY effort on satisfaction for more
left a final sample of 135 participants (Mage = 21.0 years;
growth-minded participants (+1SD; β = .718, SE = .135;
54.8% female).
t(131) = 5.32, p < .001), while no effect was observed for more
fixed-minded participants ( 1SD; β = .127, SE = .110;
Procedure. At individual lab stations, participants read they
t(131) =1.15, p = .252). A Johnson-Neyman test revealed a
had 5 minutes to complete a Microsoft Excel task using the
significant positive effect of effort on satisfaction for those
VLOOKUP formula. Participants were provided with a pre-
with a mindset above 4.78 on the 7-point scale ( .28 SD;
populated spreadsheet and a countdown timer was displayed.
62.2% of participants). A negative effect of effort on satis-
They received instructions about how to determine when the
faction was observed for those with a mindset below 3.78
task was complete, but were given few other details (see Web
( 1.28 SD; 12.6% of participants; Figure 2).
Appendix A). Participants could not continue until the task
was completed. They were told that, as a bonus, they were
being given five raffle tickets toward an Amazon gift card. Learning. The same regression model with learning as the de-
Those who completed the task without assistance could pendent variable revealed main effects of both DIY effort (β =
submit all five tickets to the raffle. Alternatively, they could .249, SE = .095; t(131) = 2.61, p = .010) and mindset (β = .194,
pay a teaching assistant (a hypothesis-blind confederate) four SE = .094; t(131) = 2.06, p = .041), qualified by the expected
tickets to help them complete the task at any time. Thus, akin interaction (β = .243, SE = .083; t(131) = 2.92, p = .004).
to real DIY tasks, participants were incentivized to self- Consistent with our theory, a spotlight analysis revealed a
produce a solution to conserve resources. Participants had positive effect of DIY effort on learning for more growth-
access to the internet as a support resource during the study. If minded participants (+1SD; β = .465, SE = .132; t(131) =
participants could not complete the task in the allotted 3.52, p < .001), but not for more fixed-minded participants
5 minutes, they were instructed to seek help from the TA. Our ( 1SD; β = .011, SE = .117; t(131) =.09, p = .927). The
independent variable was the level of effort exerted before positive effect of DIY effort on learning was significant for those
disengaging from the task, operationalized as the time (in with a mindset above 4.82 on the 7-point scale ( .27 SD; 67.7%
seconds) participants spent working on the task prior to of participants). No significant negative effect of DIY effort on
seeking a TA solution (M = 497.66, SD = 202.96).4 learning was observed at any level of mindset.
10 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

Figure 2. Study 2 result: The effect of do-it-yourself effort and mindset on satisfaction and learning.

We next tested mediation (Hayes 2017, model 8, 10,000 Method


bootstrapped samples) with DIY effort as the independent
variable, mindset the moderator, learning the mediator, and Participants and design. Participants from Amazon’s Mechanical
satisfaction the dependent variable. This revealed an indirect Turk (Mturk; n = 404) completed a 2 (DIY Effort: High vs.
effect of the interaction on satisfaction, through learning (index Low) x 2 (Mindset: Growth vs. Fixed) experiment for financial
of moderated mediation = .107, SE = .054; CI95 = [.007, .217]). compensation. Twenty-one participants were removed prior to
For more growth-minded participants (+1SD), DIY effort analysis for failing an instructional manipulation check
positively predicted learning, which in turn increased satis- (Oppenheimer, Meyvis, and Davidenko 2009), leaving a final
faction (indirect effect = .206, SE = .090; CI95 = [.042, .391]). sample of 383 participants (Mage = 41.9 years; 59.9% female).
There was no indirect effect for more fixed-minded
participants ( 1SD; indirect effect = .005, SE = .040; Procedure. Participants began by reading a news article about
CI95 = [ .085, .077]; Figure 2). the nature of ability, which served as our mindset manipulation
(adapted from Kray and Haselhuhn 2007). In the growth
mindset condition, participants read an article titled “Skills and
Discussion Ability are Changeable and Can Be Developed,” which con-
Study 2 employed greater design control while testing our sisted of expert accounts supporting the malleability of an in-
model in a paradigm that allowed for volitional DIY effort dividual’s general level of skill and ability. Participants in the
and seeking professional recovery. Consistent with Study 1, fixed mindset condition read an article titled “Skills and Ability,
this study demonstrated that effort exerted in DIY SF pos- Like Plaster, Are Fairly Stable Over Time.” This article con-
itively predicted satisfaction with firm recovery (H1). This sisted of expert accounts discussing general ability and skills as
effect was again stronger for more growth-minded con- not easily improved through effort or experience. See Web
sumers (H3). Study 2 also provided initial mediation evi- Appendix A for complete stimuli.
dence supporting learning as the process underlying our After the mindset manipulation, participants were guided
identified effects (H2). We next focus on providing stronger through a visualization exercise in which they imagined their
evidence of causality. kitchen drainpipe was leaking. Those in the high (low) DIY
effort condition read that they spent an hour (10 min) attempting
a DIY solution. However, this attempt was ultimately unsuc-
cessful, leading them to call a professional plumber. All par-
Study 3: Additional Process Evidence ticipants concluded by reading the plumber arrived the next day
Study 3 employs a controlled experiment manipulating DIY and fixed the leak with no issues.
effort and mindset to enhance causal interpretability. Further,
this study provides insights into downstream outcomes of DIY Measures. Participants first reported their satisfaction with the
SF and recovery, specifically with regard to consumers’ future plumber’s recovery offering (three 7-point bipolar items; an-
intentions to attempt related DIY tasks versus re-hire the re- chors as Study 2; α = .96). Next, participants reported learning,
covering provider. Lastly, Study 3 builds on prior findings by failure attributions, service provider warmth and competence,
testing the effects of DIY SF and recovery in a more traditional and affect (order of these measures was randomized).6 Learning
DIY context. Specifically, we test our framework in the context was measured using four items. The first asked, “Assuming you
of plumbing repair, which is a domain recognized as being had this experience, how much would you have learned about
common for consumers to engage in DIY activities (Wolf and fixing a leaking drain pipe” (1 = very little; 7 = a great deal). The
McQuitty 2011, 2013). other items asked the extent of agreement with the following
Hall and Hyodo 11

statements: I gained greater understanding of the requirements Mediation model 1—mediation of learning on satisfaction with
to successfully complete this task; I learned skills required to recovery. To test hypothesis 2, we conducted a moderated
better attempt this task in the future; I gained knowledge re- mediation analysis (Hayes 2017, model 8, 10,000 bootstrapped
quired to better attempt this task in the future (1 = strongly samples) with DIY effort as the independent variable, mindset
disagree; 7 = strongly agree; 4-item α = .96). the moderator, learning the mediator, and satisfaction the de-
Participants also reported downstream behavioral intentions. pendent variable. This resulted in a significant indirect effect of
Specifically, they were asked to imagine encountering a similar the interaction on satisfaction, through learning (index of
future problem with a different sink in their home, and to in- moderated mediation = .098, SE = .056; CI95 = [.002, .221]).
dicate the likelihood (1 = very unlikely; 7 = very likely) they Replicating findings from Study 2, DIY effort led to increased
would attempt to fix this new issue themselves, would call the learning and satisfaction within the growth mindset condition
same plumber, or would call a different plumber. They also (indirect effect = .089, SE = .040; CI95 = [.024, .175]). However,
reported their likelihood to engage in these behaviors in the case no indirect effect was observed within the fixed mindset con-
of an unrelated and more difficult plumbing issue (i.e., if a pipe dition (indirect effect = .008, SE = .037; CI95 = [ .089, .063]).
burst in their ceiling).7 Lastly, participants completed manip-
ulation checks for DIY effort (In the scenario you read, how Mediation model 2—extension to downstream outcomes. We then
much effort did you exert toward solving the problem; 1 = very examined the effects of our model on downstream consumer
little to no effort; 7 = substantial effort) and mindset (4 items intentions. This consisted of testing four mediation models,
from Study 2; α = .96), and provided demographics. one for each of the four following downstream behavioral
intention measures: intent to attempt DIY for a similar future
plumbing problem, intent to attempt DIY for a different
Results future plumbing problem, intent to re-hire the same service
Manipulation checks. An ANOVA of perceived effort as a professional for a similar future plumbing problem, and
function of DIY effort, mindset, and their interaction revealed intent to re-hire the same service professional for a different
only a main effect of the effort manipulation (F(1, 379) = 68.72, future plumbing problem. These models (Hayes 2017; model
p < .0001; other effect Fs < .4). The same ANOVA with the 83; 10,000 bootstrapped samples) each tested DIY effort as
mindset measure as the dependent variable revealed only a main the independent variable, mindset the moderator, learning
effect of the mindset manipulation (F(1, 379) = 77.88, p < and satisfaction as serial mediators (in that order), and each of
.0001; other effect Fs < 1.10). Both the mindset and effort the behavioral intention measures in their own model as the
manipulations were thus supported. dependent variable. There was no significant indirect effect of
the interaction on intentions to attempt a DIY solution for a
Satisfaction. An ANOVA of satisfaction as a function of DIY similar plumbing problem (index of moderated mediation =
effort, mindset, and their interaction revealed a marginal main .014, SE = .013; CI95 = [ .046, .002]; no indirect effect in
effect of DIY effort (F(1, 379) = 2.99, p = .085; ηp2 = .008), a either mindset condition). However, there was a significant
main effect of mindset (F(1, 379) = 5.87, p = .016; ηp2 = .015), negative indirect effect on intentions to try a DIY solution for
and the expected interaction (F(1, 379) = 11.93, p < .001; ηp2 = a different plumbing problem (indirect effect = .026, SE =
.031). Follow-up simple effects revealed a positive effect of .018; CI95 = [ .070, .007]). This indirect effect was ob-
DIY effort on satisfaction for participants in the growth mindset served for those in the growth mindset condition (indirect
condition (MHigh = 6.44, SD = .73; MLow = 5.66, SD = 1.54; F(1, effect = .024, SE = .014; CI95 = [ .057, .004]), but not in
379) = 14.03, p < .001; ηp2 = .036). There was no effect for the fixed mindset condition (indirect effect = .002, SE = .010;
participants in the fixed mindset condition (MHigh = 5.56, SD = CI95 = [ .017, .026]). We also observed a positive indirect
1.78; MLow = 5.82, SD = 1.56; F(1, 379) = 1.43, p = .233; ηp2 = effect of the interaction on intentions to re-hire the same
.004). provider to complete a similar future task (index of mod-
erated mediation = .044, SE = .027; CI95 = [.001, .106]), with
Learning. An ANOVA of learning as a function of DIY effort, an indirect effect observed in the growth condition (indirect
mindset, and their interaction revealed a marginal main effect of effect = .040, SE = .019; CI95 = [.010, .083]), but not the fixed
effort (F(1, 379) = 3.27, p = .072; ηp2 = .007), a main effect of condition (indirect effect = .004, SE = .017; CI95 = [ .041,
mindset (F(1, 379) = 13.52, p < .001; ηp2 = .033), and the .028]). A positive indirect effect of the interaction was also
expected interaction (F(1, 379) = 4.04, p = .045; ηp2 = .011). observed when considering intentions to re-hire the plumber
Follow-up simple effect contrasts revealed a significant positive for a different plumbing task (index of moderated mediation
effect of DIY effort on learning in the growth mindset condition = .039, SE = .024; CI95 = [.023, .093]), with a significant
(MHigh = 4.95, SD = 1.41; MLow = 4.26, SD = 1.86; F(1, 379) = indirect effect observed in the growth condition (indirect
7.61, p = .006; ηp2 = .019), but no effect in the fixed condition effect = .035, SE = .017; CI95 = [.009, .076]), but not the fixed
(MHigh = 3.93, SD = 1.81; MLow = 3.97, SD = 1.89; F(1, 379) = condition (indirect effect = .003, SE = .015; CI95 = [ .034,
.02, p = .888; ηp2 = .0001). .025]).
12 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

Discussion Method
Study 3 built on our prior studies in multiple ways. First, by Participants and design. Prolific participants (n = 852) were
manipulating DIY effort and mindset it added causal inter- assigned to a 2 (DIY Effort: High vs. Low) x 2 (Mindset: Fixed
pretability to our prior findings. Second, it provided additional vs. Growth) x 2 (Intervention: Instruction vs. Control) between-
support for our proposed learning-based process. Third, this participants experiment. Sixty-seven participants failed an in-
study tested downstream intentions related to both the task and structional manipulation check (Oppenheimer, Meyvis, and
the recovering provider. These downstream tests suggested an Davidenko 2009), resulting in a final sample of 785 (Mage =
interesting story. While we did not observe an indirect effect of 41.0 years; 46.2% female).
DIY effort and mindset on consumer intentions to attempt a
similar DIY task in the future, we did see a negative indirect Procedure and measures. Participants read the same mindset
effect of the same model on intentions to attempt a different (but manipulations and DIY scenarios as in Study 3. However, when
domain-consistent) DIY task. Further, we observed a positive the plumber arrived, those in the instruction (control) condition
indirect effect on intentions to re-hire the same provider for both read the plumber explained what he was doing (chatted idly with
similar and different future plumbing tasks. Collectively, these them) while he worked (see Web Appendix A). After the DIY
suggest that strong firm recovery of DIY SF shifts consumers SF experience and recovery interaction, participants reported
away from future DIY efforts and instead engenders loyalty to satisfaction with the plumber’s offering (three items as Study 3;
the service provider. While it may seem counterintuitive that a α = .98). Participants then reported learning (four items; α =
consumer who has previously attempted DIY would shift away .98), service provider warmth and competence, and mindset
from future DIY after learning more about the task, recall we using the same measures as Study 3.8 Lastly, participants re-
theorize the predominant form of learning derived from DIY SF ported demographics.
takes the form of gaining general understanding about the task,
and not necessarily skill acquisition. Thus, while consumers had
an increased general understanding about the task and the skills
Results
needed to complete it, such learning did not directly encourage
future DIY attempts. Rather, this learning seemed to instead Satisfaction. We first conducted a 3-way ANOVA with satis-
result in loyalty to the provider. These results suggest DIY SF faction as the dependent variable. This revealed a marginal main
has implications for customer-firm relationships beyond just effect of DIY effort (F(1, 778) = 3.76, p = .053; ηp2 = .005), and
customer satisfaction with the immediate recovery interaction. significant main effects of mindset (F(1, 778) = 5.77, p = .017;
While not reported in the manuscript proper, we additionally ηp2 = .007) and the intervention (F(1, 778) = 52.33, p < .001; ηp2
measured participants’ attributions for failure, affective re- = .063). There was a significant 2-way interaction between
sponses, and perceptions of service provider warmth and effort and the intervention (F(1, 778) = 6.49, p = .011; ηp2 =
competence in this study. These exploratory measures provided .008), a marginal interaction between mindset and the inter-
initial evidence suggesting that learning was heightened when vention (F(1, 778) = 3.79, p = .052; ηp2 = .005), and no in-
consumers attributed failure to a lack of ability (rather than to teraction between effort and mindset (F(1, 778) = .08, p = .781;
external factors such as bad luck), and that the effect of learning ηp2 = .0000). Importantly, all effects were qualified by a sig-
on satisfaction was a function of heightened perceptions of nificant 3-way interaction (F(1, 778) = 10.61, p = .001; ηp2 =
service provider competence. Initial evidence also suggested .013).9
our focal findings cannot be explained by negative affect. Please To examine the effectiveness of the instruction intervention,
see Web Appendix C for further details. we conducted follow-up contrast analyses examining the 2-way
interaction between the effort and intervention conditions
within each mindset. Within the growth mindset condition, there
Study 4: Firm-Provided Intervention was a significant interaction (F(1, 778) = 16.98, p < .001; ηp2 =
Studies 1–3 demonstrated that DIY effort positively predicts .019). Replicating findings from Studies 1–3, growth-minded
satisfaction with firm recovery, and that this effect is participants in the control condition (without the intervention)
strongest among growth-minded consumers. While this were more satisfied with firm recovery when they had exerted
provides firms with a potential opportunity, it also means greater DIY effort (MHigh = 6.43, SD = .85; MLow = 5.77, SD =
those who exert less effort during their DIY SF experiences 1.57; F(1, 778) = 13.90, p < .001; ηp2 = .016). However, when
may be less satisfied with firm recovery because they have not instruction was provided, growth-minded participants were
learned as much via their DIY attempt. In Study 4, we test more satisfied when they had exerted low DIY effort (MHigh =
whether a learning-based intervention during recovery en- 6.39, SD = 1.24; MLow = 6.76, SD = .44; F(1, 778) = 4.37, p =
hances outcomes for these consumers. This study thus tests .037; ηp2 = .005). Looked at another way, growth-minded
mediation by moderation (Spencer, Zanna, and Fong 2005). participants who exerted low DIY effort were more satisfied
Of practical importance, we also consider the impact of this upon receiving instruction (MInstruction = 6.76, SD = .44; MControl
proposed learning-based intervention on more fixed-minded = 5.77, SD = 1.57; F(1, 778) = 31.64, p < .001; ηp2 = .036).
consumers. Alternatively, within the fixed mindset condition, there was no
Hall and Hyodo 13

Figure 3. Study 4 result: The effect of the 3-way interaction on satisfaction and learning.

interaction between DIY effort and the intervention on satis- greater DIY effort (MHigh = 4.54, SD = 1.73; MLow = 2.79, SD =
faction (F(1, 778) = .25, p = .617; ηp2 = .0002). For fixed- 1.83; F(1, 778) = 73.59, p < .001; ηp2 = .086). However, growth-
minded participants, the instruction-based intervention equally minded participants who received instruction reported equal
enhanced satisfaction for those who had exerted high DIY effort learning regardless of DIY effort (MHigh = 6.10, SD = 1.25;
(MInstruction = 6.66, SD = .54; MControl = 5.78, SD = 1.58; F(1, MLow = 6.22, SD = .81; F(1, 778) = .32, p = .574; ηp2 = .0004).
778) = 24.04, p < .001; ηp2 = .028) and low DIY effort Considered from a different perspective, among growth-minded
(MInstruction = 6.40, SD = 1.12; MControl = 5.64, SD = 1.89; F(1, participants who exerted low DIY effort, those who received
778) = 17.94, p < .001; ηp2 = .021; Figure 3). instruction reported greater learning (MInstruction = 6.22, SD =
.81; MControl = 2.79, SD = 1.83; F(1, 778) = 291.89, p < .001; ηp2
Learning. We next conducted a 3-way ANOVA with learning as = .273). There was also a significant interaction between DIY
the dependent variable. This revealed main effects of all three effort and the intervention within the fixed mindset condition
conditions (FEffort(1, 778) = 54.36, p < .001; ηp2 = .065; (F(1, 778) = 4.15, p = .042; ηp2 = .001). However, among fixed-
FMindset(1, 778) = 30.85, p < .001; ηp2 = .038; FIntervention(1, minded participants, the intervention significantly increased
778) = 711.45, p < .001; ηp2 = .478). There were also 2-way learning in both the high (MInstruction = 6.01, SD = .90; MControl =
interactions between effort and the intervention (F(1, 778) = 3.37, SD = 1.58; F(1, 778) = 165.03, p < .001; ηp2 = .175) and
36.22, p < .001; ηp2 = .044) and between mindset and the low (MInstruction = 5.62, SD = 1.38; MControl = 2.39, SD = 1.60;
intervention (F(1, 778) = 4.55, p = .033; ηp2 = .006), but not F(1, 778) = 251.40, p < .001; ηp2 = .244) DIY effort conditions
between effort and mindset (F(1, 778) = .43, p = .512; ηp2 = (Figure 3).
.001). Importantly, all effects were qualified by a significant 3-
way interaction (F(1, 778) = 9.76, p = .002; ηp2 = .012). Mediation analyses. To empirically test our learning-based
We then examined the 2-way interaction contrasts between process, we conducted a moderated, moderated mediation
the DIY effort and intervention conditions within each mindset. analysis (Hayes 2017; model 13, 10,000 bootstrapped samples)
Within the growth mindset condition, there was a significant 2- with DIY effort as the independent variable, the mindset and
way interaction (F(1, 778) = 42.14, p < .001; ηp2 = .051). intervention conditions as moderators, learning the mediator,
Consistent with prior findings, growth-minded participants in and satisfaction the dependent variable. This revealed an in-
the control condition learned more when they had exerted direct effect of the 3-way interaction on satisfaction through
14 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

learning (index of moderated, moderated mediation = .041, contribute to this space by demonstrating the value of a
SE = .015; CI95 = [ .071, .014]). This was driven by a learning-based model. Prior service research demonstrates that
positive effect of effort on satisfaction, through learning, for consumers value learning during recovery as a means of better
those in the growth mindset and control condition (indirect understanding their role in service interactions (Dong, Evans,
effect = .223, SE = .044; CI95 = [.141, .313]). However, there and Zou 2008). Our findings extend this work by demonstrating
was no indirect effect in the growth/intervention condition that consumers also value learning about the focal consumption
(indirect effect = .015, SE = .020; CI95 = [ .056, .021]), problem and the skills needed to complete it. By highlighting
indicating the intervention enhanced learning and satisfaction their shared context, a learning-based model allows for appli-
regardless of prior effort. cation of SF and recovery theory to inform the otherwise
discrete consumer experiences of DIY SF and firm-based re-
covery offerings. We suggest future research would benefit from
Discussion employing a consumer learning lens to better understand how
Study 4 demonstrated that, while DIY SF influenced evalua- and when instruction is an effective recovery strategy.
tions of firm recovery as a function of learning, consumers can Our findings also contribute to extant DIY literature. By
learn in ways beyond their DIY SF experiences. Specifically, highlighting that failed DIY attempts often precede consumer
service providers can facilitate learning at the time of service interactions with the marketplace, we identify novel conse-
delivery, producing similar satisfaction as that associated with quences of DIY failure. Prior research on DIY failure has
effort-derived learning. This provides added evidence for our primarily focused on consumer-centric outcomes (i.e., negative
theoretical model by directly manipulating learning. Impor- affect and reduced DIY intentions; Wolf and McQuitty 2013;
tantly, we did not observe deleterious effects of the instruction Xie, Bagozzi, and Troye 2008). We build on this work by
intervention among those who already learned through DIY highlighting how DIY failure can additionally increase con-
effort. Further, we observed positive effects of firm-provided sumer understanding of the task (i.e., learning), and extend it by
instruction for fixed-minded individuals regardless of DIY ef- identifying firm-related consequences of DIY failure (i.e.,
fort. This suggests that while these fixed-minded consumers do satisfaction with firm recovery). We further identify and elab-
not derive learning from DIY SF, they nonetheless value in- orate on a boundary condition (i.e., mindset) and process (i.e.,
struction provided during recovery. learning) that collectively determine consumer responses to
DIY failure. Lastly, we draw on the varying theories of DIY in
the literature to put forth an integrated conceptualization of DIY.
General Discussion In particular, we adopt prior perspectives regarding the con-
Across four studies, we demonstrate that consumers’ DIY ex- sumer activities involved in DIY, while also highlight the im-
periences affect service provider outcomes. Specifically, when portance of conceptualizing DIY as a phenomenon rooted in
consumers exert greater effort in a failed DIY attempt (DIY SF), internal exchange (Lusch, Brown, and Brunswick 1992). Thus,
they realize greater learning about the task, which in turn we provide a broad framework for future DIY research to assist
promotes increased satisfaction with firm recovery. We further scholars focused on understanding DIY as a market-
demonstrate this effect is moderated by mindset, wherein independent phenomenon.
growth-minded consumers are more likely to learn from the
DIY SF experience and to recognize the abilities of the re-
covering provider. These results are supported by both field and
Managerial Implications
lab data, and across multiple service-relevant DIY domains. Today’s consumers are increasingly turning to DIY solutions
given increased access to DIY tools, resources, and information
(Bosari 2012). As this trend continues, service providers in
Theoretical Implications
many industries should recognize many of their customers
This research makes multiple contributions to theory. We first employ firm services after having unsuccessfully attempted a
contribute to the SF literature by expanding the SF paradigm DIY solution. This is critical given our findings that firm of-
beyond the “static customer-firm dyad” (Grégoire and Mattila ferings following DIY SF take on the role of service recovery,
2021). Consistent with Grégoire and Mattila’s call to action, which substantially increases the stakes of successful im-
prior research suggests firms can benefit when recovering from plementation. Our findings suggest successful recovery after
other firms’ failures (i.e., external recovery; Allen et al. 2015). DIY SF can result in enhanced customer satisfaction with the
We employ a similarly extended perspective of the customer service offering by virtue of greater appreciation of the skills
journey to demonstrate firms can also recover SFs that occur in required to provide the service. This is somewhat analogous to
the absence of customer-firm exchange. In the case of DIY SF, the service recovery paradox (De Matos, Henrique, and Rossi
given the recovering firm did not play a role in the failure 2007), in that more positive post-failure outcomes are realized
experience, neither models of fairness and justice (e.g., Tax, than might have been anticipated. This suggests firms should
Brown, and Chandrashekaran 1998), nor models where re- seek to understand their customers’ prior experiences with the
covery expectations are a function of failure attributions (e.g., task prior to the service interaction. For example, service
Hess, Ganesan, and Klein 2003) are appropriate. We thus providers might look for evidence that the customer attempted
Hall and Hyodo 15

and failed a DIY solution, or might ask the customer directly behaviors indicative of a growth mindset. With appropriate
whether such an attempt was made. Learning a customer ex- training, service providers might detect cues indicating an in-
perienced DIY SF will enable providers to focus on validating dividual’s mindset, which could allow them to better tailor their
the customer’s attempt and highlighting positive outcomes that offering to individual customer needs.
can be realized through such efforts (e.g., learning and
improvement).
Second, our work has implications for how service providers
Limitations and Future Directions
interact with customers. In the context of a firm recovering While the present research has significant implications for
another firm’s failures, Allen et al. (2015) recommend training theory and practice, it is not without limitations. First, due to
front-line employees to listen to customers and make gestures of limitations imposed by our partner organization, our field study
goodwill. Our work similarly suggests value in training front- findings employed a measure of our dependent variable that
line employees, but highlights the importance of training them differed from those in our controlled experimental studies.
to support customer learning during service provision. Thus, our Future research to more extensively test our theory in field
findings support Dong et al. (2008), who highlight how service settings would thus be valuable. Second, the present research
providers can benefit from providing instruction during the does not account for how varying motivations for DIY en-
service offering. While Dong et al. suggest instruction should gagement might influence consumers’ responses to DIY SF and
primarily take the form of enhancing customers’ role clarity, our recovery. The decision to engage in DIY is a complex function
findings suggest such instruction should also help customers of situational and individual factors (Williams 2004; Wolf and
understand the complexity of the focal task and the high level of McQuitty 2011), meaning it is possible certain DIY antecedents
skill required to complete it. Importantly, we identify little risk may affect the likelihood that consumers will learn from DIY
of this type of intervention for customers who had experienced SF. Similarly, certain tasks may be inherently less interesting or
DIY SF, regardless of DIY effort or mindset. This suggests firms consumers may be less likely to derive value from learning
operating in industries in which customers might attempt DIY about them. For example, consumers may derive less value from
can benefit from training employees to provide information at a learning about a task they do not anticipate encountering in the
level that enables customers to learn broadly about the task. future (vs. one they are more likely to encounter again). Future
Doing so presents an opportunity for service providers to en- research is needed to better understand the relationship between
hance their value proposition. While providers may harbor DIY motivations, DIY SF, and service provider outcomes.
concerns that educating customers could negatively affect future Third, the present research assumes the firm successfully im-
sales by increasing future DIY engagement, our results suggest plements the recovery offering. Better understanding con-
this may not be the case. In Study 3, those who reported in- sumers’ responses to a failed recovery offering following DIY
creased satisfaction with firm recovery after DIY SF indicated SF also warrants exploration, as this might represent new type of
they were more likely to seek future services from the same double deviation. Double deviations occur when a firm’s re-
provider if faced with a similar problem. We acknowledge the covery offering fails to meet customer expectations after SF
preliminary nature of this particular finding, however, and (Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault 1990), evoking strongly negative
suggest firms carefully consider what type of instruction to consumer responses.
provide. For example, firms might consider enhancing cus- Additional opportunities for future research also emerge
tomers’ general understanding of the focal task as opposed to when considering our findings in relation to prior research on
providing skill-enhancing instruction. Alternatively, firms DIY outcomes. For example, at first glance, our findings may
might benefit from teaching customers how to complete lower- appear to run counter to the seminal work on DIY by Wolf and
margin jobs themselves, which could in turn promote loyalty for McQuitty (W&M; 2011). Specifically, while our focal finding is
higher-margin jobs in the future. that consumers respond positively to firm offerings following
Lastly, because mindset moderates our focal effect, service DIY SF, W&M alternatively suggest consumers realize pri-
providers may worry about identifying this implicit customer marily negative outcomes following failed DIY attempts (e.g.,
trait. However, we offer encouragement to firms in this regard. they experience disappointment and frustration). Notably, our
Mindset is broadly predicted by culture—for example, the supplemental analyses support the notion that consumers expe-
majority of Americans lean toward a growth mindset (Dweck rience negative affect following DIY SF (see Web Appendix C).
2006)—meaning service providers might determine the pre- However, we extend this finding by demonstrating that it is
vailing mindset of their target markets. Further, it may be stronger among fixed-minded consumers. W&M further suggest
possible for front-line employees to identify a customer’s negative affect stemming from DIY failure can carry over to in-
mindset during service interactions. In anecdotal support of this fluence consumers’ perceptions of service provider offerings (p.
point, CHC employees from Study 1 reported commonly 165). While our findings suggest negative carryover may indeed
hearing customers say things like, “I’m just not a computer emerge among fixed-minded consumers (see Web Appendix C),
person” or “I always struggle with technology”—phrases we also highlight how the learning consumers realize from DIY SF
suggestive of a fixed mindset. Other customers were reported as can outweigh negative affective responses to failure. Thus, it is
asking questions related to the task, showing keen interest in the possible W&M’s respondent, whose quotation supported this
solution, or asking for advice related to future problems— point, had more fixed-minded views, thereby preventing them
16 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

from realizing the learning opportunity their failure could have Declaration of Conflicting Interests
afforded. Alternatively, this respondent may have had high in- The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
volvement in the DIY domain, which further exacerbates negative the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
affective responses to failure (Schunk 1995). Future research might
consider how identity- and involvement-related factors affect Funding
consumer responses to DIY SF and recovery.
Lastly, we believe our findings in Study 3 regarding down- The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship,
stream consumer intentions provide intriguing opportunities for and/or publication of this article.
future research. When considering future DIY and re-hiring in-
tentions for the same versus dissimilar (but domain-consistent) ORCID iDs
tasks, three of these measures indicated consumers had reduced
Matthew J. Hall  https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0769-6795
DIY intentions and increased loyalty intentions as a function of
Jamie D. Hyodo  https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5441-5410
DIY effort, mindset, learning, and satisfaction. Interestingly, this
runs somewhat counter to previous mindset research, which
suggests learning from failure should increase the likelihood that Supplemental material
growth-minded consumers will attempt the failed task in the Supplemental material for this article is available online.
future (Smiley et al. 2016). In light of this inconsistency, we
conducted supplemental analyses (reported in Web Appendix C).
Notably, when satisfaction was removed from the mediation Notes
model in Study 3 (i.e., learning was the only mediator consid- 1. Mindset is theorized as a continuous construct, meaning con-
ered), we indeed observe a positive effect of learning on future sumers are more accurately described as being more growth- or
DIY intentions, and an attenuation of the effect of learning on fixed-minded. However, for parsimony and consistency with
loyalty intentions. This suggests that satisfaction with the re- extant literature, we use binary terminology to describe mindset
covery offering after DIY SF is a critical component that not only (i.e., we refer to consumers simply as growth- or fixed-minded).
dissuades consumers from attempting DIY in the future, but also 2. Please see Web Appendix C for additional analyses, including
fosters loyalty to the recovering provider. We suggest service results excluding covariates.
providers’ successful recovery offerings leave an outsized im- 3. Growth-minded participants reported exerting marginally more DIY
pression on consumers following DIY SF, as these service effort (r = .131, p = .084). We standardized both mindset and effort to
providers are rescuing consumers from an otherwise unsolvable reduce nonessential collinearity (Dalal and Zickar 2012). All effects
task—metaphorical knights in shining armor sweeping them are still observed when tested in a model using orthogonal predictors
away from future DIY and into a loyal relationship.10 This (Wurm and Fisicaro 2014; see Web Appendix C).
suggests future research might benefit from examining how 4. Mean time was greater than 5 minutes because it reflects time spent
factors like perceived consumer helplessness, perhaps driven by on the task page prior to advancing. Thus, it includes time spent
the timeliness or magnitude of service recovery, might further reading instructions, working on the task, and waiting for the TA to
foster loyalty following DIY SF and recovery. complete the task. Further, despite being instructed to seek TA help
In sum, the present research highlights the importance of firm after 5 minutes, many students continued working beyond this
recovery offerings in response to consumers’ DIY SF experi- point.
ences. We encourage future research to consider the intersection 5. DIY effort and mindset were correlated (r = .296, p < .001), as
of various DIY tasks, consumer responses to failure in these those with a fixed mindset exerted more effort. We standardized
domains, and service provider recovery strategies to better predictors to reduce nonessential collinearity (Dalal and Zickar
understand various consumer- and firm-related outcomes in this 2012). All effects are observed in a model using orthogonal
domain. predictors (Wurm and Fisicaro 2014; see Web Appendix C).
6. Attributions, affective responses, and service provider competence
and warmth were collected for exploratory purposes. Measures and
Acknowledgments
results for these constructs can be found in Web Appendix B and C,
The authors would like to thank Thomas Dotzel, Martin Mende, Lisa respectively. These analyses support and provide additional insight
Bolton, and Karen Page Winterich for their helpful feedback on this into our learning-centric mediation model.
manuscript. The marketing faculty at the Ivey College of Business at 7. No differences were observed in likelihood to call a different
Western University, the Smeal College of Business at Penn State, and plumber. Thus, our analyses focus on intentions to re-attempt DIY
the College of Business at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln were and to re-hire the same plumber. We report additional behavioral
also helpful in developing this research. Lastly, the authors would like intention results in Web Appendix C.
to thank the staff of the Computer Help Center at the University of 8. Perceived service provider competence and warmth were again
Nebraska-Lincoln for their participation and assistance with collecting measured for exploratory purposes. These measures and results,
field data. which are respectively discussed in Web Appendix B and C, again
Hall and Hyodo 17

suggest learning increases satisfaction with recovery by increasing Chen, Chia-Yi (2018), “How Customer Participation Influences Ser-
perceptions of service provider competence. vice Failure Attribution: The Moderating Effect of Self-efficacy,”
9. Contrast analyses examining the two-way interaction between Journal of Service Theory and Practice, 28 (3), 298-314.
effort and mindset on satisfaction and learning within the control Chiu, Chi-yue, Carol Dweck, Jennifer Tong, and Jeanne Fu (1997),
condition revealed the same pattern of results as Studies 1–3 (see “Implicit Theories and Conceptions of Morality,” Journal of
Web Appendix C). Personality and Social Psychology, 73 (5), 923-940.
10. The authors would like to thank the editor for suggesting this Choi, Sunmee and Anna S. Mattila (2008), “Perceived Controllability and
metaphor. Service Expectations: Influences on Customer Reactions Following
Service Failure,” Journal of Business Research, 61 (1), 24-30.
Collier, Joel, Michael Breazeale, and Allyn White (2017), “Giving
References Back the “Self” in Self Service: Customer Preferences in Self-
Allen, Alexis M., Michael K. Brady, Stacey G. Robinson, and Clay M. Service Recovery,” Journal of Services Marketing, 31, 604-617.
Voorhees (2015), “One Firm’s Loss is Another’s Gain: Capital- Dalal, Dev and Michael Zickar (2012), “Some Common Myths about
izing on Other Firms’ Service Failures,” Journal of the Academy Centering Predictor Variables in Moderated Multiple Regression
of Marketing Science, 43 (5), 648-662. and Polynomial Regression,” Organizational Research Methods,
Alhashem, Mohammed, Caroline Moraes, and Isabelle Szmigin 15 (3), 339-362.
(2020), “Use and Social Value in Peer-to-peer Prosumption DeCaro, Marci and Bethany Rittle-Johnson (2012), “Exploring
Communities,” European Journal of Marketing, 55 (1), Mathematics Problems Prepares Children to Learn,” Journal of
193-218. Experimental Child Psychology, 113 (4), 552-568.
Bagozzi, Richard P. (1979), “Toward a Formal Theory of Marketing De Matos, Celso, Jorge Luiz Henrique, and Carlos Alberto Vargas
Exchanges,” In Conceptual and Theoretical Developments in Rossi (2007), “Service Recovery Paradox: A Meta-Analysis,”
Marketing, O.C. Ferrell, eds. Chicago: American Marketing, Journal of Service Research, 10 (1), 60-77.
431-447. Dong, Beibei, Kenneth R. Evans, and Shaoming Zou (2008), “The
Bendapudi, Neeli and Robert P. Leone (2003), “Psychological Im- Effects of Customer Participation in Co-Created Service Re-
plications of Customer Participation in Co-Production,” Journal covery,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 36 (1),
of Marketing, 67 (1), 14-28. 123-137.
Bitner, Mary Jo, Bernard Booms, and Mary Stanfield Tetreault (1990), Dong, Beibei, K. Sivakumar, Kenneth R. Evans, and Shaoming Zou
“The Service Encounter: Diagnosing Favorable and Unfavorable (2015), “Effect of Customer Participation on Service Outcomes:
Incidents,” Journal of Marketing, 54 (1), 71-84. The Moderating Role of Participation Readiness,” Journal of
Bitner, Mary Jo, William T. Faranda, Amy R. Hubbert, and Valarie A. Service Research, 18 (2), 160-176.
Zeithaml (1997), “Customer Contributions and Roles in Service Dweck, Carol (2006), Mindset: The New Psychology of Success. New
Delivery,” International Journal of Service Industry Manage- York, NY: Random House.
ment, 8 (3), 193-205. Dweck, Carol and Ellen Leggett (1988), “A Social-Cognitive Ap-
Bloom, Benjamin S. (1974), “Time and Learning,” American Psy- proach to Motivation and Personality,” Psychological Review, 95
chologist, 29 (9), 682-688. (2), 256-273.
Bonvoisin, Jérémy, Jahnavi Galla, and Sharon Prendeville (2017), Feather, Norman T. (1967), “Valence of Outcome and Expectation of
“Design Principles for Do-it-yourself Production,” Sustainable Success in Relation to Task Difficulty and Perceived Locus of
Design and Manufacturing, 7, 77-86. Control,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7 (4),
Bosari, Jessica (2012), “Growth of the Home Décor Market Shows No 372-386.
Signs of Slowing Down,” Forbes (October 2012). https://www.forbes. Feather, Norman T. and Jerrold G. Simon (1971), “Attribution of
com/sites/moneywisewomen/2012/10/24/growth-of-the-home-decor- Responsibility and Valence of Outcome in Relation to Initial
market-shows-no-signs-of-slowing-down/19a775d919a7 Confidence and Success and Failure of Self and Other,” Journal of
Burnette, Jeni, Ernest O’Boyle, Eric M. VanEpps, Jeffrey M Pollack, Personality and Social Psychology, 18 (2), 173-188.
and Eli J Finkel (2013), “Mind-Sets Matter: A Meta-Analytic Folkes, Valerie S. (1988), “Recent Attribution Research in Consumer
Review of Implicit Theories and Self-Regulation,” Psychological Behavior: A Review and New Directions,” Journal of Consumer
Bulletin, 139 (3), 655-701. Research, 14 (4), 548-565.
Burns, Alvin C. and James W. Gentry (1998), “Motivating Students to Gelber, Steven M. (1997), “Do-it-Yourself: Constructing, Repairing
Engage in Experiential Learning: A Tension-to-Learn Theory,” and Maintaining Domestic Masculinity,” American Quarterly, 49
Simulation & Gaming, 29 (2), 133-151. (1), 66-112.
Campbell, Colin (2005), “The Craft Consumer: Culture, Craft and Goetz, Thomas, Nathan C. Hall, Anne C. Frenzel, and Reinhard Pekrun
Consumption in a Postmodern Society,” Journal of Consumer (2006), “A Hierarchical Conceptualization of Enjoyment in
Culture, 5 (1), 23-42. Students,” Learning and Instruction, 16 (4), 323-338.
Centrica (2017), “Brits Spend Billions on Professional Help to Fix Grégoire, Yany and Anna S. Mattila (2021), “Service Failure and
Their DIY Disasters,” Centrica.com, November 29. https://www. Recovery at the Crossroads: Recommendations to Revitalize the
centrica.com/news/brits-spend-billions-professional-help-fix- Field and its Influence,” Journal of Service Research, 24 (3),
their-diy-disasters 323-328.
18 Journal of Service Research 0(0)

Hayes, Andrew (2017), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Nagel, Duane M., J. Joseph Cronin, and Richard Utecht (2018),
Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. “Consumption or Prosumption? A Question of Resources,”
Guilford Publications. Journal of Services Marketing, 32 (6), 739-754.
Heidenreich, Sven, Kristina Wittkowski, Matthias Handrich, and Oliver, Richard L. (1980), “A Cognitive Model for the Antecedents and
Tomas Falk (2015), “The Dark Side of Customer Co-Creation: Consequences of Satisfaction,” Journal of Marketing Research,
Exploring the Consequences of Failed Co-Created Services,” 17 (4), 460-469.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 43 (3), 279-296. Oppenheimer, Daniel M., Tom Meyvis, and Nicolas Davidenko (2009),
Hess, Ronald, Shankar Ganesan, and Noreen Klein (2003), “Service “Instructional Manipulation Checks: Detecting Satisficing to
Failure and Recovery: The Impact of Relationship Factors on Increase Statistical Power,” Journal of Experimental Social
Customer Satisfaction,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Psychology, 45 (4), 867-872.
Science, 31 (2), 127-145. Pacheco, Natália Araújo, Larissa Carine Braz Becker, and
Hoffman, Douglas and John Bateson (1997), Essentials of Services Vinı́cius Andrade Brei (2017), “A Light in the Dark: The Benefits
Marketing. Dryden Press. of Co-production in Service Failures,” Journal of Retailing and
Kapur, Manu (2010), “Productive Failure in Mathematical Problem Consumer Services, 34, 95-101.
Solving,” Instructional Science, 38 (6), 523-550. Price, Linda L. and Eric J. Arnould (1999), “Commercial Friendships:
Khademi-Vidra, Anikó and Zoltán Bujdosó (2020), “Motivations and Service Provider–Client Relationships in Context,” Journal of
Attitudes: An Empirical Study on Do-It-Yourself Consumers in Marketing, 63 (4), 38-56.
Hungary,” Sustainability, 12 (2), 517. Rittle-Johnson, Bethany and Martha Wagner Alibali (1999), “Con-
Köcher, Sören and Stefanie Paluch (2019), “‘My Bad’: Investigating ceptual and Procedural Knowledge of Mathematics: Does One
Service Failure Effects in Self-Service and Full-Service Settings,” Lead to the Other?” Journal of Educational Psychology, 91 (1),
Journal of Services Marketing, 33 (2), 181-191. 175-189.
Kray, Laura and Michael Haselhuhn (2007), “Implicit Negotiation Schunk, Dale (1995), “Implicit Theories and Achievement Behavior,”
Beliefs and Performance: Experimental and Longitudinal Evi- Psychological Inquiry, 6 (4), 311-314.
dence,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93 (1), Senko, Corwin and Judith Harackiewicz (2005), “Achievement Goals, Task
49-64. Performance, and Interest: Why Perceived Goal Difficulty Matters,”
Kulhavy, Raymond W. (1977), “Feedback in Written Instruction,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31 (12), 1739-1753.
Review of Educational Research, 47 (2), 211-232. Smiley, Patricia, Katherine Buttitta, Samuel Chung, Valeska Dubon,
Kuznetsov, Stacey and Eric Paulos (2010), “Rise of the Expert Am- and Lilian Chang (2016), “Mediation Models of Implicit Theories
ateur: DIY Projects, Communities, and Cultures,” Proceedings of and Achievement Predict Planning and Withdrawal after Failure,”
the 6th Nordic Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, Motivation and Emotion, 40, 878-894.
295-304. Smith, Amy, Ruth Bolton, and Janet Wagner (1999), “A Model of
Lusch, Robert, Stephen Brown, and Gary Brunswick (1992), “A Customer Satisfaction with Service Encounters Involving Failure
General Framework for Explaining Internal vs. External Ex- and Recovery,” Journal of Marketing Research, 36 (3), 356-372.
change,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 20 (2), Specht, Nina, Sina Fichtel, and Anton Meyer (2007), “Perception and
119-134. Attribution of Employees’ Effort and Abilities: The Impact on
Maxham, James and Richard Netemeyer (2002), “A Longitudinal Customer Encounter Satisfaction,” International Journal of
Study of Complaining Customers’ Evaluations of Service Failures Service Industry Management, 18 (5), 534-554.
and Recovery,” Journal of Marketing, 66 (4), 57-71. Spencer, Steven J., Mark P. Zanna, and Geoffrey T. Fong (2005), “Es-
McCollough, Michale A., Leonard L. Berry, and Manjit S. Yadav tablishing a Causal Chain: Why Experiments are Often More Effective
(2000), “An Empirical Investigation of Customer Satisfaction than Mediational Analyses in Examining Psychological Processes,”
after Service Failure and Recovery,” Journal of Service Research, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89 (6), 845-851.
3 (2), 121-137. Steenhof, Naomi, Nicole N. Woods, Pascal Van Gerven, and Maria
Meuter, Matthew L., Amy L. Ostrom, Robert I. Roundtree, and Mylopoulous (2019), “Productive Failure as an Instructional
Mary Jo Bitner. (2000), “Self-service Technologies: Under- Approach to Promote Future Learning,” Advances in Health
standing Customer Satisfaction with Technology-based Service Sciences Education, 24, 739-749.
Encounters,” Journal of Marketing, 64 (3), 50-64. Sugathan, Praveen and Kumar Rakesh Ranjan (2020), “When Co-
Moisio, Risto, Eric Arnould, and James W. Gentry (2013), “Pro- Production Fails: The Role of Customer’s Internal Attributions
ductive Consumption in the Class-Mediated Construction of and Impression Management Concerns,” Journal of Business
Domestic Masculinity: Do-It-Yourself Home Improvement in Research, 121 (December), 535-548.
Men’s Identity Work,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40 (2), Sugathan, Praveen, Kumar Ranjan, and Avinash Mulky (2017), “An
298-316. Examination of the Emotions that Follow a Failure of Co-Cre-
Morgan, Robert M. and Shelby D. Hunt (1994), “The Commitment- ation,” Journal of Business Research, 78, 43-52.
Trust Theory of Relationship Marketing,” Journal of Marketing, Tax, Stephen, Stephen Brown, and Murali Chandrashekaran (1998),
58 (3), 20-38. “Customer Evaluations of Service Complaint Experiences: Im-
Murphy, Ryan (2017), “The Diseconomies of Do-it-yourself,” The plications for Relationship Marketing,” Journal of Marketing, 62
Independent Review, 22 (2), 245-255. (2), 60-76.
Hall and Hyodo 19

Tsiros, Michael, Vikas Mittal, and William Ross Jr. (2004), “The Role Xie, Chunyan, Richard Bagozzi, and Sigurd Troye (2008), “Trying to
of Attributions in Customer Satisfaction: A Reexamination,” Prosume: Toward a Theory of Consumers as Co-creators of Value,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (2), 476-483. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 36 (1), 109-122.
Van Heerde, Harald, Christine Moorman, C. Page Moreau, and Yen, HsiuJu, Kevin P. Gwinner, and Wanru Su (2004), “The Impact of
Robert W. Palmatier (2021), “Reality Check: Infusing Ecological Customer Participation and Service Expectation on Locus At-
Value into Marketing Research,” Journal of Marketing, 85, 1-13. tributions following Service Failure,” International Journal of
Van Vaerenbergh, Yves, Chiara Orsingher, Iris Vermeir, and Bart Service Industry Management, 15 (1), 7-26.
Larivière (2014), “A Meta-Analysis of Relationships Linking Yi, Youjae and Seo Young Kim (2017), “The Role of Other Customers
Service Failure Attributions to Customer Outcomes,” Journal of during Self-Service Technology Failure,” Service Business, 11
Service Research, 17 (4), 381-398. (4), 695-715.
Vannini, Phillip and Jonathan Taggart (2014), “Do-it-yourself or Do-it- Zhu, Zhen, Cheryl Nakata, K. Sivakumar, and Dhruv Grewal
with? The Regenerative Life Skills of Off-grid Home Builders,” (2013), “Fix or Leave it? Customer Recovery from Self-
Cultural Geographies, 21 (2), 267-285. Service Technology Failures,” Journal of Retailing, 89 (1),
Watson, Matthew and Elizabeth Shove (2008), “Product, Competence, 15-29.
Project and Practice: DIY and the Dynamics of Craft Con- Zolfagharian, Mohammadali, Reto Felix, and Jakob Braun (2018),
sumption,” Journal of Consumer Culture 8 (1), 69-89. “Boundary Conditions of the Effect of Customer Coproduction:
Williams, Colin (2004), “A Lifestyle Choice? Evaluating Motives of The Case of Service Failure,” Journal of Marketing Management,
DIY Consumers,” International Journal of Retail & Distribution 34 (9), 705-731.
Management, 32 (5), 270-278.
Wolf, Christine T. (2016), “DIY Videos on YouTube: Identity and Author Biographies
Possibility in the Age of Algorithms,” First Monday, 21 (6). doi:
Matthew J. Hall, PhD, is an assistant professor of marketing at
10.5210/fm.v21i6.6787
Oregon State University. His research primarily focuses on
Wolf, Marco and Shaun McQuitty (2011), “Understanding the Do-It-
understanding how social and psychological factors influence
Yourself Consumer: DIY Motivations and Outcomes,” AMS
consumers’ perceptions of themselves and their consumption
Review, 1 (3-4), 154-170.
experiences.
Wolf, Marco and Shaun McQuitty (2013), “Circumventing Traditional
Markets: An Empirical Study of the Marketplace Motivations and Jamie D. Hyodo, PhD, is an assistant professor of consumer
Outcomes of Consumers’ Do-It-Yourself Behaviors,” Journal of behavior in the DAN Department of Management and Orga-
Marketing Theory and Practice, 21 (2), 195-210. nizational Studies at Western University (Canada). His work has
Wurm, Lee and Sebastiano Fisicaro (2014), “What Residualizing been published in the Journal of Consumer Research. His re-
Predictors in Regression Analyses Does (and What it Does not search focuses on the effects of consumer morality, emotions,
Do),” Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 37-48. persuasion, and social influence.

You might also like