You are on page 1of 9

INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD, JANUARY–FEBRUARY 2014,

VOL. 49, NO. 1, 139–147

Research Report
Stuttering inhibition via altered auditory feedback during scripted telephone
conversations
Daniel Hudock† and Joseph Kalinowski‡
†Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Division of Health Sciences, Idaho State University,
Pocatello, ID, USA
‡Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, College of Allied Health Sciences, East Carolina University,
Greenville, NC, USA
(Received November 2012; accepted July 2013)

Abstract
Background: Overt stuttering is inhibited by approximately 80% when people who stutter read aloud as they hear
an altered form of their speech feedback to them. However, levels of stuttering inhibition vary from 60% to 100%
depending on speaking situation and signal presentation. For example, binaural presentations of delayed auditory
feedback (DAF) and frequency-altered feedback (FAF) have been shown to reduce stuttering by approximately
57% during scripted telephone conversations.
Aims: To examine stuttering frequency under monaural auditory feedback with one combination of DAF with FAF
(COMBO-2) and two combinations of DAF with FAF (COMBO-4) during scripted telephone conversations.
Methods & Procedures: Nine adult participants who stutter called 15 local businesses during scripted telephone
conversations; each condition consisted of five randomized telephone calls. Conditions consisted of (1) baseline
(i.e. non-altered feedback), (2) COMBO-2 (i.e. 50-ms delay with a half octave spectral shift up), and (3) COMBO-
4 (i.e. 200-ms delay and a half octave spectral shift down in addition to the COMBO-2). Participants wore a
supra-aural headset with a dynamic condenser microphone while holding a receiver to their contralateral ear when
making telephone calls.
Outcomes & Results: Stuttering was significantly reduced during both altered auditory feedback (AAF) conditions
by approximately 65%. Furthermore, a greater reduction in stuttering was revealed during the COMBO with four
effects (74%) as compared with the COMBO with two effects (63%).
Conclusions & Implications: Results from the current study support prior research reporting decreased stuttering
under AAF during scripted telephone conversations. Findings that stuttering was significantly reduced to a greater
extent under the COMBO with four effects condition suggest that second signals reduce stuttering along a
continuum. Additionally, findings support prior research results of decreased stuttering frequency under AAF
during hierarchically difficult speaking situations. Clinical application of these findings may be that people who
stutter can use specific software or smartphone applications that produce second speech signals to inhibit stuttering
frequency effectively during telephone conversations.

Keywords: stutter, stuttering, fluency, stammer, stammering, telephone.

What this paper adds?


What is already known on the subject?
Previous research has shown that stuttering can be immediately and effectively reduced during the perception of
second speech signals. This reduction in stuttering by hearing a second speaker, or one’s own voice altered then
feedback to them as they speak varies in level of reduction depending on speaking situation and signal presentation.
Perception of second speech signals during hierarchically difficult speaking situations maintains significant reductions
in overt stuttering behaviours. Second signals that are more speech-like with additional gestural information offer
greater reductions in stuttering frequency.

Address correspondence to: Daniel Hudock, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Division of Health Sciences, Idaho State
University, 921 South 8th Avenue, Mail Stop 8116, Pocatello, ID 83209–8116, USA; e-mail: Hudock@isu.edu
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online  C 2013 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists

DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12053
140 Daniel Hudock and Joseph Kalinowski

What this paper adds?


The current study demonstrated that monaural presentation of AAF significantly reduced stuttering frequency during
hierarchically difficult situations. This is the first time that combinations of altered feedback have been shown to
offer differential effects from single signal presentations. Multiple combinations of AAF exhibited more robust
stuttering reductions than one combination of DAF with FAF. Results indicate that reductions in stuttering during
the perception of second speech signals occur along a continuum that is dependent on situational and signal factors.

have reported statistically equivalent reductions in stut-


Introduction
tering frequencies during oral reading conditions when
Stuttering is a speech disorder overtly characterized by using DAF delays of 25, 50 and 75 ms (Kalinowski and
syllable repetitions, phoneme prolongations and postu- Stuart 1996), and 50, 200 and 400 ms (Dayalu 2004).
ral fixations. These overt speech disruptions are immedi- They have also reported similar results with FAF spec-
ately and dramatically reduced from 60% to 100% when tral shifts from one-half octave downward to one-half
people who stutter (PWS) perceive a second speech sig- octave upward in one-quarter octave increments (Stu-
nal that is presented in conjunction with their own art et al. 1996) and one full octave shift downward to
speech output (Hargrave et al. 1994, Hudock et al. 2011, one full octave shift upward in half octave increments
Kalinowski et al. 1993, Macleod et al. 1995). Second (Hargrave et al. 1994). Additionally, reductions in stut-
speech signals are speech signals that are presented to the tering frequencies have been noted in single DAF or FAF
speaker during their on-going speech productions. For presentations as compared with combined DAF and
example, when a second speaker verbally reads or recites FAF (Macleod et al. 1995). During oral reading, stut-
content with a person who stutters (PWS), stuttering tering frequency is typically reduced by between 60%
is typically reduced to the extent of being non-existent. and 80% with DAF, FAF or combined DAF and FAF
Another form of second speech signals is using one’s conditions, across the specific tested delays or frequency
own speech to produce a second altered signal using a shifts.
digital signal processor that feeds the generated signal Support from previously mentioned studies and ex-
back to the speaker. Altered auditory feedback (AAF) amination of other factors and situations using second
uses one’s own speech to produce an altered speech sig- speech signals have lead to clinical application of sec-
nal by feeding the speaker’s production into a digital ond speech signals for reducing stuttering. Researchers
signal processor that generates a second temporally and have developed portable devices that offer binaural and
or spectrally altered signal. Two examples of these sig- monaural DAF, FAF or DAF with FAF. It should be
nals are delayed auditory feedback (DAF) and frequency noted that binaural signal presentation is more effec-
altered feedback (FAF). DAF is a delayed representa- tive at reducing stuttering than monaural presentation
tion of one’s speech that is fed back to them; FAF is (Stuart et al. 1997). To the best of the authors’ knowl-
a spectrally shifted representation with minimal delay. edge, this is the only study that specifically examined
DAF (Kalinowski et al. 1993, Kalinowski and Stuart AAF in binaural and monaural presentations. The au-
1996) and FAF (Armson et al. 1997, Howell et al. 1987, thors did report a significant difference between bin-
Kalinowski et al. 1993, Macleod et al. 1995, Stuart et al. aural versus monaural presentations, but for practical
1996) are effective at reducing stuttering frequency by purposes of hearing listeners, reducing environmental
between 60% and 80%. Signals that are self-generated distraction and decreasing the conspicuousness of de-
and externally altered offer the benefit of not relying on vices, the assistive products are typically implemented
a second speaker to be present. with monaural presentations. The portability of these
Researchers have extensively examined second sig- devices offers many opportunities for clinical applica-
nals that reduce stuttering. Some of these signals in- tion across many settings with increased fluency. Re-
clude masking noise (Kalinowski et al. 1993), pure-tones searchers have employed second speech signals dur-
(Saltuklaroglu and Kalinowski 2006), DAF (Kalinowski ing monologues, conversations, verbal presentations and
et al. 1993, Kalinowski and Stuart 1996), FAF (Armson during telephone conversations. For example, Armson
et al. 1997, Howell et al. 1987, Kalinowski et al. 1993, et al. (1997) reported a 74% reduction in stuttering
Macleod et al. 1995, Stuart et al. 1996), and combined frequency with FAF using a one-half octave downward
DAF and FAF (Hargrave et al. 1994). Stuttering is typ- shift during oral reading to audiences of two, four and
ically reduced by approximately 40% when PWS per- 15 people, with no effect for audience size. Additionally,
ceive masking noise and pure-tones, and by 80% when Zimmerman et al. (1997) examined the effect of
perceiving signals that are more speech-like. Researchers DAF and FAF on stuttering frequency during scripted
Stuttering inhibition during scripted telephone conversations 141
telephone conversations. Stuttering frequency was re- ined during the study, but reported participant numbers
duced by approximately 57% during AAF conditions ranging from one to eight. The researchers did not re-
and did not reveal significant difference between DAF port why the final number of participants was five and
and FAF. not eight.
For PWS, the telephone is often described as one Participants in Bray and James (2009) were asked to
of their most feared and avoided situations (Georgieva make three telephone calls (i.e. one to a person they were
1994, Leith and Timmons 1983). Speaking on the tele- comfortable speaking to, one to a person they were not
phone to a naı̈ve listener is typically ranked highest on comfortable speaking to, and one to a retailer asking for
their hierarchy of difficult speaking situations (Blood- a brochure) over the span of 1 month, and to record the
stein and Bernstein-Ratner 2007). Stuttering frequency three telephone calls. Recorded telephone calls during
has even been shown to increase during this difficult the first month created the first baseline, and recorded
situation relative to conversations held within clinical telephone calls made during the second month were
settings (Ladouceur et al. 1982). To date, however, only the second baseline. Two weeks after the second base-
a limited number of studies have examined stuttering line, researchers visited the participants to train them on
frequency during telephone conversations. The major- using a telephone feedback device (VA609 TAD) that
ity of these studies used clinicians and researchers, and offered a choice between two separate combinations of
briefed listeners or family members during timed con- DAF with FAF. The device was a handheld telephone
versational exchanges (Craig et al. 1996, Stager et al. with monaural signal presentation. The first combina-
1995). To evaluate stuttering during telephone conver- tion presented a 56-ms delay with a 304 Hz upward
sations, some researchers may call clients or have clients shift in frequency, while the second preset had a 90-
call family members or staff from clinical settings (Craig ms delay with a 530 Hz upward shift in frequency. If
et al. 1996, Stager et al. 1995). However, using these the researchers mean that the spectrum was shifted up
methods to evaluate true fluency levels during gener- by the listed frequency amount, it is similar to one-
alization may be problematic. If the PWS speaks to quarter octave and half octave shift up, respectively. It
someone who is familiar with stuttering, the listener was not reported what preset the five (or eight) partic-
may not portray typical responses during the exchange. ipants used during the telephone conversations. Bray
In addition, if participants remain in comfortable envi- and James (2009) only reported the descriptive statistics
ronments where they may have performed similar tasks for average stuttering collapsed across telephone calls;
numerous times, their level of fluency may not be a true 8.62% for baseline 1, 8.42% for baseline 2, and 4.82%
representation. Procedures may be further adjudicated for the experimental condition. Findings from the study
by not having participants use scripted texts, as some of indicated a 50% reduction in stuttering. However, due
the most frequently employed compensatory strategies to the concerns listed above, results should be considered
by PWS are circumlocutions and substitutions to avoid with caution.
saying words that they fear stuttering on. Such com- The current study was designed to explore differ-
pensatory strategies often occur to the extent of being ences between one combination of DAF with FAF, and
almost reflexively produced to avoid saying words with two combinations of DAF with FAF. Although signifi-
anticipated difficultly. By substituting words or altering cant differences between DAF, FAF, and combined DAF
word order, PWS may exhibit the illusion of being more with FAF have not been consistently reported in the
fluent than they actually are. literature, both authors experience self-reported addi-
Only a limited number of studies have examined tional benefits when using multiple combinations of
stuttering episodes of PWS during telephone con- DAF with FAF as compared with single effects or one
versations with unfamiliar listeners (Bray and James combination of effects. Also, participants were required
2009, Chambers 2009, Zimmerman et al. 1997). to place telephone calls to unfamiliar persons using
Zimmerman et al. (1997) had nine adult PWS call busi- scripted conversations. Similar procedures and scripts as
nesses on a binaural headset while reading scripted texts Zimmerman et al. (1997) were used to reduce partic-
during baseline, DAF and FAF conditions. DAF was set ipants’ use of avoidances, circumlocutions and substi-
at a 50-ms delay; FAF had a downward shift of one- tutions. It was hypothesized that differences from base-
half octave. They reported 60% and 55% reductions for line to both conditions and between one signal with
DAF and FAF, respectively, with no statistically signifi- two effects to one signal with four effects would be re-
cant differences between DAF and FAF. Bray and James vealed (similar to the authors’ experiences) when using
(2009) examined stuttering frequencies and attitudes of a difficult speaking situation (scripted text to unfamiliar
participants who stuttered during two baseline phases communication partners) to elicit increased frequency
and one experimental phase of a study. However, there of stuttering. Additionally, the use of monaural signal
were some limitations with their study. For example, the presentations in the current study enabled researchers
authors claimed that only five participants were exam- to compare the results with Bray and James (2009)
142 Daniel Hudock and Joseph Kalinowski
and to a lesser extent with Zimmerman et al. (1997), mixer via line inputs. The mixed signal was then sent to
who used binaural presentation. The aim of the current a Rolls RA68b headphone-amplifying mixer where the
study was to examine the effect of monaural presen- line stereo plug for the Sennheiser headset was inserted.
tations from one combination of DAF with FAF and Frequency ranges for the broadcast quality headset were
two combinations of DAF with FAF on stuttering fre- 8–25 000 Hz.
quency during scripted telephone conversations. It was Specific delay and frequency settings for the com-
hypothesized that stuttering frequency would signifi- bination with two effects (COMBO-2) condition were
cantly decrease during both conditions, and that differ- DAF – 50 ms with FAF – plus one-half octave. Set-
ential effects would be noted when comparing the two tings for the COMBO with four effects (COMBO-
effect combinations and four effect combinations. 4) condition were DAF – 200 ms with FAF minus
one-half octave in addition to the COMBO-2 settings.
These settings were chosen in consideration of prior re-
Method search (Hargrave et al. 1994, Macleod et al. 1995, Stuart
Participants et al. 1997). Signals were markedly different, yet in-
dividually perceptible via self-report from participants.
Nine native English speaking adults who stutter includ- Hargrave et al. (1994) reported equivocal reductions
ing eight males and one female, mean age of 35.1 years in stuttering frequency between half and one octave
(SE = 5.3, range = 21–72), with no self-reported history positive and negative frequency shifts during reading
of concomitant speech, language, cognitive, uncorrected conditions. Similarly, under DAF, researchers reported
visual or hearing deficits participated in the study. Par- equivocal reductions in stuttering frequency between
ticipants met inclusionary criteria of greater than 5% 50 and 400-ms delays (Kalinowski and Stuart 1996).
stuttering during informal assessment of spontaneous Macleod et al. (1995) reported equivocal reductions in
speech. Prior to experimental procedures, informed con- stuttering frequency when comparing DAF with a 50-
sent (approved by the University and Medical Center ms delay, FAF minus one-half octave shift and a combi-
Institutional Review Board, East Carolina University) nation of these two signals. Finally, increased reductions
was obtained from all participants. have been noted with binaural compared with monaural
signal presentations (Stuart et al. 1997). The preceding
studies all report approximately between 70% and 80%
Instrumentation and stimuli
reductions in stuttering frequency during oral reading
Scripts from Zimmerman et al. (1997) were utilized. tasks. Furthermore, general experimental designs for the
Some scripts were altered in order to increase so- current study were similar to Zimmerman et al. (1997)
cial relevance for 2012 (see appendix A). Condi- who used binaural feedback conditions of 50 ms DAF,
tion sequences and telephone scripts were random- one-half octave negative shift down FAF and baseline
ized by assigning individual numerical values to each non-altered feedback during scripted telephone con-
then randomly generating number sequences using versations. It should be noted that current procedures
http://www.randomizer.org/. Participants spoke into a with monaural signal presentations enabled researchers
Sennheiser HMD 281 Pro 64 Ohm monaural supra- to capture unaltered downstream audio from the partic-
aural headset with a unidirectional dynamic microphone ipants without recording the speaker on the other end
(frequency ranges = 50–13 500 Hz) positioned 2.5 cm of the phone.
directly in front of their mouths. Downstream audio was
received by a Radial JS-3 XLR splitter that delivered the
Procedures
throughput, unamplified, stereo signal via XLR to 3.5
mm cable into an iMac 10.1 Intel Duo Core computer, The researcher verbally briefed participants prior to sign-
which used Audacity 1.3.14 Beta software to capture ing informed consent documents. Prior to beginning the
the audio signal of the speaker at 44.1 kHz. The Radial experiment, participants were requested not to use flu-
JS-3 delivered two amplified stereo signals into a Mackie ency enhancing strategies and to talk in their natural way
Micro Series 1202 twelve-channel mixer via XLR con- of speaking during all conditions. Volume was adjusted
nections. From there two lineout stereo signals went to the participants’ designated most comfortable listen-
from the mixer into two separate left mono RCA inputs ing level while being able to perceive both combinations
in Yamaha DSP-1 digital signal processors (DSP). The of signals at approximately the same loudness. Partic-
first DSP generated a second signal with a 50-ms delay ipants called a total of 15 local businesses requesting
with positive half-octave shift up. The second DSP gen- information and items (for a complete list of scripts, see
erated a second signal with a 200-ms delay with negative appendix A). During each condition participants made
half-octave shift down. Mono-unbalanced signals from five consecutive calls. In order to reduce any possible
the two DSPs were then fed into a Rolls Mix Max RM81 carryover effects researchers had participants produce
Stuttering inhibition during scripted telephone conversations 143
Table 1. Raw data with mean and standard errors (SE) for total spoken and total stuttered syllables as a function of AAF condition

NAF COMBO-2 COMBO-4


Spoken Stuttered Spoken Stuttered Spoken Stuttered
Participant syllables syllables syllables syllables syllables syllables
1 233 41 231 10 351 19
2 222 32 221 12 232 4
3 422 125 243 37 279 39
4 218 67 255 33 255 33
5 248 82 236 39 206 20
6 236 54 251 17 246 13
7 264 84 292 36 363 29
8 188 33 155 5 166 1
9 352 104 508 39 407 15
Means 264.7 69.1 265.8 25.3 278.3 19.2
SE 24.8 10.8 32.6 4.7 26.5 4.2
Note: NAF, non-altered feedback; COMBO-2 = DAF (50-ms delay) with FAF (half octave shift up); COMBO-4 = COMBO-2 and DAF (200-ms delay) with FAF (half octave shift
down).

2-min spontaneous conversation samples without audi- stuttered syllables by the total spoken syllables per con-
tory feedback between the three conditions. dition. Stuttering was defined as part-word repetitions,
phoneme prolongations and postural fixations. Intra-
Speech rate analysis and inter-rater reliabilities were obtained from a ran-
domized 10% of the data. A certified speech–language
As longer DAF settings have been demonstrated to pathologist trained in stuttering evaluation performed
decrease speech rates, researchers calculated partici- the inter-rater reliability using Cohen’s kappa syllable-
pants’ speech rates (i.e. syllables per second) in order to by-syllable agreements. Kappa values for intra-rater anal-
account for the fact that a 200-ms delay was only used ysis were 0.87 and were 0.54 for inter-rater analysis. Val-
during the COMBO-4 condition. To obtain an accurate ues above 0.41 represent moderate agreement and values
speech rate calculation, researchers used only fluently greater than 0.75 represent excellent agreement (Viera
produced syllables that were at least two syllables distant and Garrett 2005).
from any stuttering episode and contained a minimum The numbers of spoken and stuttered syllables as
of three contiguous syllables. One recording did not a function of feedback condition for each participant
meet these criteria—in this instance, researchers used are presented in table 1. The proportions of spoken
a derived average from the participant’s other four and stuttered syllables as a function of feedback condi-
recordings during the same condition. Mean syllable tion for each participant are presented in figure 1 and
rates and their standard errors were 4.6 (SE = 0.5), table 2. Prior to inferential statistical analysis partici-
4.1 (SE = 0.5), and 4.4 (SE = 0.6) syllables/s for the pants’ proportion of stuttering values were transformed
non-altered feedback, COMBO-2 and COMBO-4 into arcsine units. This commonly used transforma-
conditions, respectively. A one-factor analysis of tion reduces endpoint weighting of proportional data
variance (ANOVA) was utilized to examine differences sets (Viera and Garrett 2005). Two one-factor ANOVA
in speech rate as a function of condition. There were were utilized to examine differences in the proportion
no statistically significant differences between syllable of stuttered syllables as a function of condition and to
rates [F(2, 16) = 3.29, p = 0.06, η2p = 0.29]. examine differences in speech rate as a function of con-
dition. A significant effect of condition was revealed
Results [F(2, 16) = 152.35, p < 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.95]. Post hoc
Prior to analysing stuttering frequencies, the re- t-tests with Bonferroni corrections revealed differ-
searcher orthographically transcribed participants’ ences between NAF and COMBO-2 (p < 0.05) and
audio-recorded telephone conversations to obtain total COMBO-2 to COMBO-4 (p < 0.05). Therefore, both
syllable production counts. This was done since conver- COMBO-2 and COMBO-4 conditions had signifi-
sations slightly varied due to the responses of the clerks cantly lower proportions of stuttered syllables than the
and employees. The researcher then analysed conversa- non-altered feedback condition. The COMBO-4 con-
tions for stuttered syllables and calculated proportional dition had significantly lower proportions of stuttered
values for each condition by dividing the number of syllables than the COMBO-2 condition.
144 Daniel Hudock and Joseph Kalinowski
significantly more effective at reducing stuttering than
the COMBO-2 condition.
Findings that stuttering was reduced during both
AAF conditions supports prior research results of sig-
nificantly reduced stuttering under AAF during difficult
speaking situations (Armson et al. 1997, Bray and James
2009, Zimmerman et al. 1997). Interestingly, the level
that AAF reduces stuttering appears to vary by speaking
situation, and signal presentation. The monaural presen-
tation of COMBO-4 reduced stuttering to the greatest
extent and COMBO-2 reduced stuttering to greater ex-
tents as compared with Zimmerman et al. (1997) or
Bray and James (2009). Binaural signal presentation
in Zimmerman et al. (1997) reported 60% and 55%
reductions in stuttering frequency for DAF and FAF,
respectively. Bray and James (2009) reported approxi-
mately 50% reductions using monaural presentations
of DAF with FAF. First, a comparison should be made
between the previous two studies. Zimmerman et al.
(1997) used binaural single-effect presentations and re-
Figure 1. Mean proportions stuttering with standard errors as a ported larger reductions than Bray and James (2009)
function of AAF condition. NAF = non-altered feedback, COMBO who used a monaural presentation with combined ef-
with two = DAF (50 ms delay) with FAF (1/2 octave shift up),
COMBO with four = COMBO with two signal, and DAF (200 ms
fects. The current study used a monaural presentation
delay) with FAF (1/2 octave shift down). Error bars represent plus with two and four combinations of effects and reported
one standard error of the mean. the largest reduction in stuttering. Although COMBO-
4 had the largest effect, COMBO-2 still reduced stutter-
ing to greater extents than either other study. Further-
Table 2. Proportion of stuttering as a function of condition
more, the current study and Zimmerman et al. (1997)
Participant NAF COMBO-2 COMBO-4 used scripts, whereas Bray and James (2009) did not.
1 0.18 0.04 0.05 Larger reductions in stuttering from Zimmerman et al.
2 0.14 0.05 0.01 (1997) and the current study as compared with Bray and
3 0.30 0.15 0.14 James (2009) may have occurred due to conversation re-
4 0.31 0.13 0.13 quirements. In other words, participants may have read
5 0.33 0.17 0.10
6 0.23 0.07 0.05
over the scripts prior to the clerk answering the tele-
7 0.32 0.12 0.08 phone or content construction may require additional
8 0.18 0.03 0.01 effort as compared with reading prepared texts.
9 0.30 0.08 0.07 Due to the findings that COMBO-4 reduced stut-
Means 0.254 0.093 0.069 tering to greater extents than COMBO-2, and that all
SE 0.024 0.017 0.016
AAF conditions reduced stuttering to greater extents
Note: NAF, non-altered feedback; COMBO-2 = DAF (50-ms delay) with FAF (half than reported in Zimmerman et al. (1997) or Bray and
octave shift up); COMBO-4 = COMBO-2 and DAF (200-ms delay) with FAF (half
octave shift down). James (2009), it may be interpreted that second sig-
nals effect stuttering along a continuum that depends
on situational and signal factors. For example, Armson
et al. (1997) reported 75% reductions in stuttering fre-
Discussion
quency under FAF during oral reading to audiences of
Generalfindings that AAF reduced stuttering frequency two, four and 15—with no effect for audience size.
during telephone conversations support prior research Similarly, researchers consistently report larger stutter-
results (Bray and James 2009, Zimmerman et al. 1997). ing frequency reductions under AAF during oral reading
In the current study, stuttering frequency was signif- as compared monologues or conversations (Antipova
icantly reduced an average of 72% during both al- et al. 2008, Armson et al. 2006, Armson and Keifte
tered feedback conditions relative to baseline—it was 2008, Armson and Stuart 1998). Under AAF stutter-
reduced 63% in the COMBO-2 and by 74% during ing is typically reduced between 70% and 80% during
the COMBO-4 conditions. This is the first study to oral reading and from 35% to 60% during monologues
reveal differences in stuttering between varied combi- and conversations. Further support for the interpre-
nations of AAF. Lastly, the COMBO-4 condition was tation of second signals reducing stuttering along a
Stuttering inhibition during scripted telephone conversations 145
continuum comes from the fact that white noise and Clinical implications
pure tones reduce stuttering less than signals that are
Results from this study demonstrate that perceiving
more speech like (Kalinowski et al. 1993, Saltuklaroglu
second speech signals with combinations of effects re-
and Kalinowski 2006). Second, non-speech signals typ-
duces stuttering frequency during hierarchically difficult
ically reduce stuttering between 20% and 40% during
communication situations. In addition to reductions in
oral reading—speech signals or signals that are speech-
stuttering under AAF, impact and improvement
like typically reduce stuttering by between 60% and
during difficult speaking situations may be indi-
100%. It may be interpreted that speech and speech-
cated in questionnaire responses from PWS, sug-
like signals possess temporal and spectral characteristics
gesting that they would use and recommend
that are similar to the speech signal and therefore reduce
using AAF during difficult speaking situations
stuttering to a greater extent than non-speech signals.
(Chambers 2009, Pollard et al. 2009, Stuart
Using Max’s inverse internal model of sensori-motor
et al. 2006, Unger et al. 2012). Similarly, many re-
control the current findings may be adequately explained
searchers note the benefits of AAF during difficult speak-
(Max, Guenther, Gracco, Ghosh & Wallace, 2004). Max
ing situations for PWS (Antipova et al. 2008, van Borsel
et al. (2004) suggest that stuttering may occur due to;
et al. 2003). Other clinical applications can be general-
1) an insufficient preparatory model (efference copy)
ized to PWS using software or smartphone applications
for motor output or that the efference model, sent for
that offer various combinations of second speech signal
comparison to feedback controllers, does not match ex-
effects for them to hear as the speak on the phone. It
ecuted actions, or 2) that PWS have an overreliance
is beneficial to have the altered signal only presented
on afferent, sensory, feedback for online corrections. As
to the PWS, as they would likely not use it if the lis-
noted by Max et al., perception of second speech au-
tener heard the altered signal as well. The altered signal
ditory or visual signals, such as DAF, FAF, or visual
could be presented directly to the earpiece of the de-
speech feedback (visual images of a mouth presented
vice, or transmitted via a Bluetooth capable device (i.e.
to speakers), may decrease stuttering by activating the
Bluetooth headset/ear piece or car interface). Use of this
auditory cortex for efference model comparisons sooner
technology would dramatically increase fluency during
than would occur during speech initiation without feed-
telephone conversations, especially if the technology al-
back. By providing better feedback and improving com-
lowed for various combinations of effects.
parisons, it should also correct deficits in preparatory,
Additionally, to extrapolate the findings with the
forward, internal models. Also noted by the Max et al.
help of previous research, PWS could record and play
(2004), neighboring areas to the auditory cortex, such
back sustained (Dayalu et al. 2011) or intermittent
as the superior temporal sulcus (STS), or the medial
phoneme presentations (Kalinowski et al. 2000), and
portion of the superior temporal gryus (STGm), are
non-linguistically matched content (Guntupalli et al.
activated during perception of visual speech feedback.
2005) while they speak on the telephone to help reduce
It is possible that these sensorimotor integration areas
their stuttering frequency. However, it should be noted
of the STS, STGm and potentially other areas, often
that content which is more speech-like and linguistically
described in mirror neuron system literature (for a re-
matched reduces stuttering to greater extents. Similarly,
view of the mirror system see Oztop, Kawato & Arbib,
perception of vowels reduces stuttering to greater extents
2013; for information on the role of the STS and the MN
than voiceless continuants or non-speech signals. If soft-
during imitation see Molenberghs, Brander, Mattingley
ware or applications would offer these alternatives in a
& Cunnington, 2010), generate the efference model,
portable and Bluetooth pairable device, it would likely
or act as a multisensory/sensorimotor comparator be-
be very beneficial to PWS and potentially for people
tween feedforward and feedback motor control systems.
with other communication disorders.
In the current study, the COMBO-4 condition may
have further increased the effectiveness of the second
signals by presenting additional information for com-
Conclusions
parison and therefore decreased overreliance to a greater
extent. Reductions in stuttering under AAF may also Stuttering frequency was significantly reduced by 72%
be interpreted using the EXPLAN model (Howell and under monaural AAF presentation during scripted tele-
Au-Yeung 2002). The EXPLAN model suggests that phone conversations with naı̈ve listeners. In addition,
internal timekeepers govern motor planning and exe- COMBO-4 reduced stuttering to greater extents than
cution during speech using parallel processes for load COMBO-2. Levels of reduction in stuttering reported
monitoring mechanisms (Howell and Au-Yeung 2002). during this intense hierarchically feared communication
Using this theory, AAF acts as an external timekeeper situation indicate the robust effect of second speech sig-
reducing the load on the system by implementing local nals during speech productions for PWS. Furthermore,
of global rate changes. increased inhibition of stuttering during COMBO-4 as
146 Daniel Hudock and Joseph Kalinowski
compared with COMBO-2 supports the notion of a DAYALU, V., GUNTUPALLI, V., KALINOWSKI, J., STUART, A., SALTUK-
continuum of inhibition during the perception of sec- LAROGLU, T. and RASTATTER, M., 2011, Effect of continu-

ond speech signals. Future studies should examine stut- ous speech and non-speech signals on stuttering frequency
in adults who stutter. Logopedics, Phoniatrics, Vocology, 36,
tering during monaural and binaural presentations using 121–127.
the same then different alterations of AAF combinations GEORGIEVA, D., 1994, Speech situations increasing stuttering in 13–
compared with single signal presentations during vari- 16-year-old persons. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 19, 175–
ous hierarchically difficult speaking situations. 175.
GUNTUPALLI, V. K., KALINOWSKI, J., SALTUKLAROGLU, T. and NAN-
JUNDESWARAN, C., 2005, The effects of temporal modifica-
Limitations tion of second speech signals on stuttering inhibition at two
speech rates in adults. Neuroscience Letters, 385, 7–12.
Some limitations of the current study include the fact HARGRAVE, S., KALINOWSKI, J., STUART, A., ARMSON, J. and JONES,
that monaural headsets and having the speaker hold- K., 1994, Effect of frequency-altered feedback on stuttering
frequency at normal and fast speech rates. Journal of Speech
ing the receiver to the contralateral ear are not often and Hearing Research, 37, 1313–1319.
used during typical telephone conversations. Addition- HOWELL, P. and AU-YEUNG, J., 2002, The EXPLAN theory of flu-
ally, the use of scripts attempted to account for avoid- ency control and the diagnosis of stuttering 2005. In Current
ances, circumlocutions and substitutions while main- Issues in Linguistic Theory Series: Pathology and Therapy of
taining consistency between participants. Speakers do Speech Disorders (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), pp. 75–94.
HOWELL, P., EL-YANIV, N. and POWELL, D., 1987, Factors affecting
not typically use prepared scripts while making tele- fluency in stutterers when speaking under altered auditory
phone calls. feedback. In H. Peters and W. Hulstijn (eds), Speech Motor
Dynamics in Stuttering (New York, NY: Springer), ch. 28.
HUDOCK, D., DAYALU, V. N., SALTUKLAROGLU, T., STUART, A.,
Acknowledgements ZHANG, J. and KALINOWSKI, J., 2011, Stuttering inhibition via
Declaration of interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. visual feedback at normal and fast speech rates. International
The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 46, 169–
paper. 178.
KALINOWSKI, J. and STUART, A., 1996, Stuttering amelioration at
various auditory feedback delays and speech rates. European
References Journal of Disordered Communication, 31, 259–269.
KALINOWSKI, J., ARMSON, J., ROLAND-MIESZKOWSKI, M., STUART,
ANTIPOVA, E. A., PURDY, S. C., BLAKELEY, M. and WILLIAMS, S., A. and GRACCO, V. L., 1993, Effects of alterations in auditory
2008, Effects of altered auditory feedback (AAF) on stuttering feedback and speech rate on stuttering frequency. Language
frequency during monologue speech production. Journal of and Speech, 36, 1–16.
Fluency Disorders, 33, 274–290. KALINOWSKI, J., DAYALU, V. N., STUART, A., RASTATTER, M. P.
ARMSON, J., FOOTE, S., WITT, C., KALINOWSKI, J. and STUART, A., and RAMI, M. K., 2000, Stutter-free and stutter-filled speech
1997, Effect of frequency altered feedback and audience size signals and their role in stuttering amelioration for English
on stuttering. European Journal of Disordered Communication, speaking adults. Neuroscience Letters, 293, 115–118.
32, 359–366. KEPPEL, G. and WICKENS, T. D., 2004, Design and Analysis: A Re-
ARMSON, J. and KEIFTE, M., 2008, The effect of SpeechEeasy on stut- searcher’s Handbook, 4th edn (Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
tering frequency, speech rate, and speech naturalness. Journal Education).
of Fluency Disorders, 33, 120–134. LADOUCEUR, R., COTE, C., LEBLOND, G. and BOUCHARD, L.,
ARMSON, J., KEIFTE, M., MASON, J. and DE CROOS, D., 2006, The 1982, Evaluation of regulated-breathing method and aware-
effect of SpeechEasy on stuttering frequency in laboratory ness training in the treatment of stuttering. Journal of Speech
conditions. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 31, 137–152. and Hearing Disorders, 47, 422–426.
ARMSON, J. and STUART, A., 1998, Effect of extended exposure to LEITH, W. R. and TIMMONS, J. L., 1983, The stutterer’s reaction
frequency-altered feedback on stuttering during reading and to the telephone as a speaking situation. Journal of Fluency
monologue. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, Disorders, 8, 233–243.
41, 479–490. MACLEOD, J., KALINOWSKI, J., STUART, A. and ARMSON, J., 1995,
BLOODSTEIN, O. and BERNSTEIN-RATNER, N., 2007, A Handbook Effect of single and combined altered auditory feedback on
on Stuttering, 6th edn (New York, NY: Thomson-Delmar). stuttering frequency at two speech rates. Journal of Commu-
BRAY, M. and JAMES, S., 2009, An evaluation of a telephone assistive nication Disorders, 28, 217–228.
device (TAD) for people who stutter. International Journal of MAX, L., GUENTHER, F. H., GRACCO, V. L., GOSH, S. S. and WAL-
Speech–Language Pathology, 11, 54–60. LACE, M. E., 2004, Internal models and feedback-biased mo-
CHAMBERS, N., 2009, Impact of the telephone assistive device (TAD) tor control as sources of dysfluency: a theoretical model of
on stuttering severity while speaking on the telephone. South stuttering. Contemporary Issues in Communication Science and
African Journal of Communication Disorders, 56, 23–34. Disorders, 31, 105–122.
CRAIG, A., HANCOCK, K., CHANG, E., MCCREADY, C., SHEPLEY, A., MOLENBERGHS, P., BRANDER, C., MATTINGLEY, J. B. and CUNNING-
MCCAUL, A. and REILLY, K., 1996, A controlled clinical trial TON, R., 2010, The role of the superior temporal sulcus
for stuttering in persons aged 9 to 14 years. Journal of Speech, and the mirror neuron system in imitation. Human Brain
Language and Hearing Research, 39, 808–826. Mapping, 31, 1316–1326.
DAYALU, V. N., 2004, Inhibition of stuttering from second speech sig- OZTOP, E., KAWATO, M. and ARBIB, M. A., 2013, Mirror neurons:
nals. Unpublished doctoral thesis, East Carolina University, function, mechanisms and models. Neuroscience Letters, 540,
Greenville, NC. 43–55.
Stuttering inhibition during scripted telephone conversations 147
POLLARD, R., ELLIS, J. B., FINAN, D. and RAMIG, P. R., 2009, Ef- (2) Museum: What is the special exhibition currently
fects of the SpeechEasy on objective and perceived aspects of going on? How much is general admission and
stuttering: a 6-month, phase I clinical trial in naturalistic en- admission to the special exhibition? What are your
vironments. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research,
52, 516–533. weekend hours? Do you have audio guides?
SALTUKLAROGLU, T. and KALINOWSKI, J., 2006, The inhibition of (3) Dry cleaner: How much do you charge to dry
stuttering via the presentation of natural speech and sinusoidal clean a man’s sport jacket? If I drop it off in the
speech analogs. Neuroscience Letters, 404, 196–201. morning, may I pick it up the same day? Are you
STAGER, S., LUDLOW, C., GORDON, C., COTELINGAM, M. and open on Sundays?
RAPOPORT, J., 1995, Fluency changes in persons who stut-
ter following a double blind trial of clomipramine and de- (4) Office supply: Do you sell external hard-drives?
sipramine. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, Where are you located? What time do you close
38, 516–525. today? Thank you for your time.
STUART, A., KALINOWSKI, J., ARMSON, J., STENSTROM, R. and (5) Florist: Can you tell me how much your long-
JONES, K., 1996, Fluency effect of frequency alterations of stem roses are per dozen? How much are a dozen
plus/minus one-half and one-quarter octave shifts in audi-
tory feedback of people who stutter. Journal of Speech and carnations? Do you take American Express?
Hearing Research, 39, 396–401. (6) Hotel: What are your weekend rates for a double
STUART, A., KALINOWSKI, J. and RASTATTER, M. P., 1997, Effect of room? Do you have a fitness center? Do you have
monaural and binaural altered auditory feedback on stuttering a swimming pool?
frequency. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, (7) Hardware store: Do you have a set of metric socket
3806–3809.
STUART, A., KALINOWSKI, J., SALTUKLAROGLU, T. and GUNTUPALLI, wrenches? What time do you close today?
V. K., 2006, Investigations of the impact of altered audi- (8) Conference centre: Do you have a conference
tory feedback in-the-ear devices on the speech of people who room to accommodate a party of ten? Do you
stutter: one-year follow-up. Disability and Rehabilitation, 28, cater? Do you have a lounge in your facility?
757–765. (9) Hair salon: How much do you charge for a man’s
UNGER, J., GLUCK, C. W. and CHOLEWA, J., 2012, Immediate effects
of AAF devices on the characteristics of stuttering: a clinical shampoo, cut, and blow dry? Do I have to make
analysis. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 37, 122–134. an appointment?
VAN BORSEL, J., REUNES, G. and VAN DEN BERGH, N., 2003, De- (10) Department store: What are your hours of opera-
layed auditory feedback in the treatment of stuttering: clients tion? Do you have any specials going on? Where
as consumers. International Journal of Language and Commu- is your store located, I’m from out of town and
nication Disorders, 38, 119–129.
VIERA, A. J. and GARRETT, J. M., 2005, Understanding interobserver coming from Greenville?
agreement: the kappa statistic. Family Medicine, 37, 360– (11) Restaurant: Do you have a Sunday brunch? What
363. time do you open on weekdays? Do you have a
ZIMMERMAN, S., KALINOWSKI, J., STUART, A. and RASTATTER, M., children’s menu?
1997, Effect of altered auditory feedback on people who stut- (12) Copy centre: Do you copy homemade DVD
ter during scripted telephone conversations. Journal of Speech,
Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1130–1134. movies? What is your charge per DVD copy? What
are your weekend hours?
Appendix A: Telephone Scripts (13) Bookstore: I was wondering if you have the book
outliers by Malcolm Gladwell in stock? Is that hard
Similar to Zimmerman et al. (1997), each partici- or soft cover? How much is it?
pant initiated the conversation with a ‘Hello’ and ended (14) Sporting goods: Do you carry rollerblades? And
the exchange with a ‘Thank you for your time.’ do you also carry snowboards? Are you open on
Sunday nights?
(1) Video store: Can you tell me if you have the movie (15) Bakery: Do you make birthday cakes? How many
The Kings Speech? What are your membership re- days’ notice would you require? Do you take
quirements? American Express?

You might also like