You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/236211617

Parent-absent signalling of need and its consequences for sibling competition


in the barn swallow

Article  in  Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology · March 2013


DOI: 10.1007/s00265-013-1508-7

CITATIONS READS

10 66

4 authors:

Andrea Romano Giuseppe Boncoraglio


University of Milan 38 PUBLICATIONS   1,581 CITATIONS   
97 PUBLICATIONS   1,357 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Diego Rubolini Nicola Saino


University of Milan University of Milan
307 PUBLICATIONS   8,976 CITATIONS    333 PUBLICATIONS   15,961 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Sibling interactions in barn swallow nestlings View project

Behavioural and Spatial Ecology of a Colonial Migratory Falcon View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Andrea Romano on 19 September 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Parent-absent signalling of need and its
consequences for sibling competition in the
barn swallow

Andrea Romano, Giuseppe Boncoraglio,


Diego Rubolini & Nicola Saino

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

ISSN 0340-5443
Volume 67
Number 5

Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851-859


DOI 10.1007/s00265-013-1508-7

1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer-
Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. This e-offprint is
for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you wish
to self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.

1 23
Author's personal copy
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859
DOI 10.1007/s00265-013-1508-7

ORIGINAL PAPER

Parent-absent signalling of need and its consequences


for sibling competition in the barn swallow
Andrea Romano & Giuseppe Boncoraglio &
Diego Rubolini & Nicola Saino

Received: 21 September 2012 / Revised: 14 February 2013 / Accepted: 19 February 2013 / Published online: 14 March 2013
# Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Abstract In species with parental care, competition among item. By eavesdropping siblings' PAB displays, nestlings
siblings for access to limited parental resources is common. may optimally balance the costs of scrambling compe-
Sibling competition can be mediated by begging behaviour, tition, the direct fitness gains of being fed and the
a suite of different visual and acoustic displays by which indirect fitness costs of subtracting food to needy kin.
offspring solicit parental care. These are mostly addressed to However, large asymmetries in satiation between competitors
the parents upon food provisioning, but can also be may lead individual offspring to monopolize parental re-
performed in the absence of the attending parents. This so- sources, irrespective of PAB displays.
called parent-absent begging (PAB) may function as an
intrabrood communication signal and potentially affect Keywords False alarm . Parent-absent begging . Sibling
intrabrood competition dynamics for access to food. We competition . Sibling negotiation
investigated the role of PAB in moulding sibling interactions
and its effect on food intake among altricial barn swallow
(Hirundo rustica) nestlings, both under normal and experi-
Introduction
mentally reduced food intake. Frequency of PAB increased
after food deprivation. Nestlings that had performed PAB
Life history strategies that maximize individual fitness may
increased their begging intensity upon the subsequent pa-
differ among family members (Clutton-Brock 1991; Stearns
rental feeding visit, while siblings reduced their own beg-
1992). In species with parental care, the per capita share of
ging level, but only when they had not been food-deprived.
resources received by an individual offspring progressively
As a consequence, nestlings which had performed PAB
declines with increasing brood size (Mock and Parker 1997).
before parental arrival had larger chances of receiving food.
Whenever the overall food demand by the progeny exceeds
However, nestlings did not benefit from displaying PAB
the availability of parental resources, conflicts of interest and
when competing with food-deprived siblings. Our findings
competition among siblings over access to limited resources
show that PAB reliably reflects need of food, indicating that
are expected to occur.
a nestling will vigorously compete for the subsequent food
Sibling competition can be severely detrimental to part of
the progeny, either because of physical aggression or star-
Communicated by M. Leonard
vation of younger/weaker offspring, whose access to paren-
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article tal resources is restrained by more competitive siblings
(doi:10.1007/s00265-013-1508-7) contains supplementary material, (Wright and Leonard 2002). In many taxa, solicitation of
which is available to authorized users.
care provisioning and competitive interactions among the
A. Romano (*) : D. Rubolini : N. Saino offspring are mediated by begging displays (Kilner and
Dipartimento di Bioscienze, Università degli Studi di Milano,
Johnstone 1997; Wright and Leonard 2002). In birds, beg-
via Celoria 26,
20133 Milan, Italy ging may include diverse behavioural and morphological
e-mail: andrea.romano@unimi.it displays addressed by the young to their parents to solicit
provisioning of care (Kilner and Johnstone 1997; Wright
G. Boncoraglio
and Leonard 2002). The function of begging in the resolu-
Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge,
Downing Street, tion of sibling conflicts is still vividly debated (e.g. Godfray
CB2 3EJ Cambridge, UK 1991, 1995; Johnstone 1999; Mock et al. 2011): irrespective
Author's personal copy
852 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859

of considering begging as an honest signal allowing parents We investigated the effects of spontaneous (i.e. not ex-
to identify the neediest offspring in the brood (Godfray perimentally elicited) occurrences of postural PAB on sib-
1991, 1995) or as a means by which siblings gain access ling begging upon parental food provisioning and food
to parental resources via scrambling interactions (Macnair intake in altricial nestlings of the barn swallow (Hirundo
and Parker 1979; Parker and Macnair 1979), experimental rustica), an insectivorous passerine with biparental care. In
studies have consistently demonstrated that higher begging this species, parents feed a single offspring at each provi-
intensity secures individual offspring a larger share of re- sioning visit to the nest, and access to food cannot be
sources (reviewed in Kilner and Johnstone 1997; Mock et monopolized by individual nestlings by e.g. occupying ad-
al. 2011), which in turn increases their direct fitness. vantageous positions in the nest (personal observation), as
However, such benefits are expected to decrease with in- conversely occurs in cavity-nesting species (e.g. Cotton et
creasing individual condition (e.g. Godfray 1991). al. 1999). Sib–sib competition for access to food is mediated
Moreover, higher begging intensity may entail the offspring by the intensity of calling, gaping and posturing upon food
with larger costs of displaying (e.g. physiological costs: provisioning visits (Boncoraglio et al. 2009; Romano et al.
Moreno-Rueda and Redondo 2011; Boncoraglio et al. 2011, 2012), but nestlings also perform postural PAB be-
2012; but see Rodríguez-Gironés et al. 2001) and potentially tween feeding visits, mostly elicited by diverse external
increases the starvation risk of outcompeted siblings (e.g. stimuli such as movements by nestmates, vocal calls or
Cotton et al. 1999). Thus, offspring are selected to strike the adults flying nearby the nest (A. Romano, personal obser-
optimal balance between selfish exploitation of parental re- vation), with ca. 30 % of feeding visits preceded by a
sources and altruism towards kin according to variation in postural PAB event by at least one nestling in full broods
their own condition relative to that of the competing siblings (our unpublished data). Postural PAB in barn swallows is
(Romano et al. 2012). characterized by sudden gape opening and/or neck
Besides begging towards attending parents, offspring of stretching, similarly to parent-present begging. These pos-
several bird species perform begging also when parents are tural displays are almost always associated with intense
not present, the so-called ‘parent-absent begging’ (PAB). calling (AR and DR, personal observation), and vocal and
Being not immediately connected to parental food provi- postural displays are strictly correlated in the barn swallow
sioning, PAB has been regarded as a misdirected behaviour, (our unpublished data). Begging intensity increases with
possibly functioning to minimize costly misses of parental hunger, and parents preferentially feed the nestlings that
feeding visits by needy nestlings (Budden and Wright 2001; beg the most (Boncoraglio et al. 2008, 2009; Bonisoli-
Leonard and Horn 2001; Leonard et al. 2005; Dor et al. Alquati et al. 2011). Nestlings adjust their competitive be-
2007). ‘Misdirected’ PAB, which is widespread among pas- haviour according to the payoffs of increasing begging
serine species, is characterized by sudden bursts of postural, intensity to obtain additional food versus drawing back
gaping, and calling displays and is usually elicited by di- and favouring a needy sibling (Romano et al. 2012).
verse external stimuli, such as sounds or movements occur- We performed feeding trials whereby pairs of similar-
ring nearby the nest site, especially when nestlings are sized nestlings were first tested under a normal food provi-
hungry (Leonard and Horn 2001; Leonard et al. 2005; sioning regime. The same pairs of nestlings were then tested
Rivers 2009; Magrath et al. 2010). PAB events do not occur under different satiation conditions, by temporarily depriv-
between all parental visits, but only in a variable fraction of ing of food one of them before the start of the trial. The data
interfeeding intervals (e.g. Budden and Wright 2001; presented here were obtained from an experiment aimed at
Leonard and Horn 2001). In other species, such as testing whether nestlings modulated their parent-present
Strigiformes, PAB displays may be continuously performed competitive behaviour according to nestmates' hunger level,
by nestlings between parental feeding visits and do not seem whose results are extensively reported elsewhere (Romano
to be associated with external stimuli (e.g. Roulin et al. et al. 2012). In this companion study based on the same set
2000, 2009; Dreiss et al. 2010). Such PAB signals may of nestlings, we concentrate on the occurrence of PAB
function to negotiate access to parental resources at subse- displays and on their consequences on food intake.
quent provisioning events (Roulin et al. 2000, 2009; We first tested whether PAB reliably reflects offspring
Johnstone and Roulin 2003; Roulin 2004; Dreiss et al. need, as observed in other species (Roulin 2001; Bulmer et
2010). According to this ‘sibling negotiation hypothesis’ al. 2008; Rivers 2009). If this is the case, PAB rate and
(Roulin et al. 2000; Johnstone and Roulin 2003), offspring intensity should increase with increasing hunger level.
might signal to each other their individual state and willing- Secondly, if PAB reliably reflects contingent need, we ex-
ness to compete for the next indivisible food item and pect begging by individual nestlings upon parental visits to
reciprocally modulate competition effort accordingly, in or- be more intense if they had previously performed PAB
der to maximize individual inclusive fitness (Roulin et al. compared to their own begging levels when not performing
2000, 2009; Johnstone and Roulin 2003; Dreiss et al. 2010). PAB before parental arrival. Thirdly, we examined whether
Author's personal copy
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859 853

nestlings respond to PAB of their siblings by reducing period of food deprivation (food-deprived nestling). The
begging intensity during the subsequent feeding event, other focal nestling (control nestling) was handled shortly
which is expected if nestlings modulate begging efforts in and left in the nest with all its siblings (the mean brood size
response to their siblings' contingent need (Romano et al. of nests included in the study was 4.2 nestlings; range 3–6)
2012). As a consequence, nestlings should benefit from for normal food provisioning. Food deprivation was
performing PAB by increasing their food share at the very intended to simulate a short period of starvation, as it may
next provisioning visit. Finally, in line with the above rea- naturally occur in case of bad weather. The food-deprived
soning, we might expect experimentally food-deprived nes- nestling was then returned to the nest for the 90-min AFD
tlings to be less sensitive to PAB by normally fed siblings trial together with the control one, while the non-focal
(‘control’ nestlings hereafter), i.e. they may not decrease nestlings were temporarily removed.
begging intensity at parental feeding visits when their con- Nestlings were videotaped with a Sony DCR-SR72E
trol nestmates perform PAB before parental arrival. camera positioned at 2–4 m from the nest. For the purpose
of the present study, we recorded all the events of PAB,
defined as any occurrence of postural begging in the ab-
Material and methods sence of parents on the nest, irrespective of the stimulus that
originated it. We excluded all instances of postural begging
Experimental procedures and behavioural observations performed while the parent was leaving the nest and all
cases of PAB performed when the parents were either
This study was carried out between June and August 2011 in perched nearby the nest or flying around it. Postural begging
two barn swallow colonies (=farms) located near Milano intensity (maximum score per bout) of both focal nestlings
(Northern Italy). Nests were visited every second day to record at the first feeding visit by parents following each PAB by
breeding events. At 7–11 days of age (day 0=day of hatching either nestling, as well as at feeding events not preceded by
of the first egg), we ringed all nestlings and measured their any PAB, was evaluated using a four-level scale of postural
body mass (nearest 0.1 g). We selected as ‘focal nestlings’ to be scores (0=no begging, 1=gape open, 2=gape open and
included in feeding trials the two nestmates that had the neck stretched, 3=vigorous begging with fully stretched
smallest difference in body mass. When three or more neck and tarsi; see Kilner 2002 for additional details; see
nestmates had the same difference in body mass, the choice also Romano et al. 2012). The same scores were assigned to
was randomized. Hence, we chose nestlings with similar PAB events to calculate postural PAB intensity. In the barn
size-mediated competitive ability (see Bonisoli-Alquati et al. swallow, postural and vocal begging are strictly related (our
2011). Trials were started on the day of measurement, unpublished data): we are thus confident that postural beg-
when nestlings were 7 (n=1 nest), 8 (n=5), 9 (n=2), ging score should reliably reflect the overall intensity of
10 (n=8) or 11 (n=6)days old. begging displays. We limited our analyses to the first feed-
We videotaped the same pair of nestlings during two ing event following a PAB because preliminary analyses
feeding trials. Prior to the first trial, both nestlings had been revealed that PAB did not predict nestling behaviour and
fed under a normal provisioning regime (hereafter, ‘before food intake during subsequent feeding visits (see Electronic
food deprivation’ trial, BFD); prior to the second trial (here- supplementary material (ESM) for details). Begging inten-
after, ‘after food deprivation’ trial, AFD), one randomly sity in feeding events not preceded by PAB of either nestling
chosen nestling was food-deprived (see below). AFD trials was used as a measure of the ‘baseline begging intensity’.
were always performed the day after BFD trials. Since food We then recorded which nestling received the unit of food
deprivation leads to a significant decrease in nestling body from parents during each feeding event.
mass (see Romano et al. 2012) and possibly disrupts size- To avoid the dissipation of the effect of food deprivation
mediated competitive hierarchies established in the brood, on begging intensity, for the AFD trial, the analyses of PAB
we chose to perform the BFD trial before the AFD trial in intensity, begging intensity at parental arrival and food
order to minimize any potential cascading effect of food intake were restricted to the initial third of all feeding events
deprivation on behaviour of nestlings during the BFD trial. of the trial, including on average 8.40±2.55 SD (range, 5–
Trials started between 10:00 and 11:00 a.m. and were 14) feedings (feeding rate in the BFD trial was 15.12±5.77
conducted as follows: focal nestlings were temporarily SD feedings per hour). We used data for the first third of
marked with small white spots on their head and left in the feeding events because hunger, and thus begging behaviour,
nest for 90 min to compete for food. Non-focal nestlings is expected to depend more strictly on the amount of food
were removed from the nest and kept in a safe place at received rather than on time elapsed since the start of the
ambient temperature. After the trial, all nestlings were trial (see also Romano et al. 2012). However, all results
returned to the nest. The next day, one randomly chosen were qualitatively similar when we selected data for the first
focal nestling was removed from the nest for a 120-min 30 min of the AFD trial (details not shown for brevity).
Author's personal copy
854 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859

All the measurements on video recordings were Variation in begging intensity at feeding events following
performed blindly with respect to the experimental treat- own or sibling's PAB (analysis 3) was analysed in a multi-
ment. We recorded 22 pairs of nestlings that performed a nomial mixed model (though in the ‘Results’ we show mean
total of 96 or 153 PAB in BFD and AFD trials, respectively. score values for ease of interpretation) including feeding
In nestling pairs, PAB constituted ca. 11.0 % of total beg- trial and experimental group as fixed effects and a three-
ging events (excluding begging scores at parental arrival=0) level factor (PAB category) indicating if the feeding event
and may occur in short, rapid sequences (25 % of PAB), was: (1) preceded by own PAB, (2) preceded by a sibling's
consisting of two to seven consecutive distinct events of PAB or (3) not preceded by any PAB (baseline begging
PAB. There was no statistically significant difference in the intensity). Two- and three-way interactions between fixed
mean number of consecutive PAB (including single PAB effects were also included in the model.
events) between food-deprived (1.43±0.75 SD) and control All the above analyses were repeated including age of
nestlings (1.25±0.70 SD; see ESM for details of statistical nestlings as a covariate (see Budden and Wright 2001),
tests). Moreover, there was no statistically significant differ- but its effect was always far from significance (p values
ence in mean begging intensity or in the probability of always >0.43) and was therefore removed from the models
obtaining a feeding after a single or multiple PAB (see (details not shown for brevity).
ESM for details). For simplicity, in all the analyses of In addition, we tested the effects of PAB on the proba-
begging intensity at parental arrival and probability of bility that a nestling did not beg at parental visits (analysis 4)
obtaining a feeding, we therefore considered multiple PAB by running a binomial mixed model including experimental
as single PAB displays. Moreover the few begging events at group, a two-level factor indicating whether a feeding event
parental arrival preceded by simultaneous PAB by both was preceded (coded as 1) or not (coded as 0) by a PAB
nestlings (12 and 5 cases in the BFD and AFD trial, respec- event by the sibling (i.e. excluding own PAB events), and
tively) were excluded from the analyses of begging intensity their interaction as fixed effects.
at parental arrival and probability of obtaining a feeding. Finally, we investigated whether the probability of
Finally if both nestlings performed one or more obtaining a feeding after own PAB varied according to
nonsimultaneous PAB during a same interfeeding interval, experimental group, feeding trial and their interaction in
we considered only the PAB event that was closer to paren- binomial mixed models (analysis 5). We note that this
tal arrival. These procedures resulted in a total of 39 (BFD) analysis was carried out only for the nestlings that
or 31 (AFD) feeding events preceded by a PAB of one of the performed PAB, as the probability of the other nestling
two nestlings (i.e. ca. 10 % of total feeding events, the obtaining food was simply [1 - (the probability of obtaining
remainder being either occurrences of simultaneous PAB a food item after an own PAB)].
or feeding events not preceded by PAB; our unpublished In Poisson and multinomial mixed models, degrees of
data) that were entered in the analyses of begging intensity freedom were conservatively set equal to the number of
upon parental feeding visits and food intake. pairs included in each analysis. Due to the unbalanced
sample sizes for each level of the random effect and the
Statistical analyses uncertainties in estimating degrees of freedom in non-
Gaussian mixed models, tests of fixed effects of binomial
We relied on repeated-measures mixed models including models were performed with z scores. Mixed models were
pair (nest) and nestling identity as random intercept effects, run with the SAS 9.1 GLIMMIX procedure (Poisson and
using data from both feeding trials. However, to investigate multinomial models; SAS Institute 2006) and the R 2.8.1
the effect of PAB on nestling behaviour and outcome of lmer procedure (binomial models; Bates et al. 2008;
sibling competition under normal food provisioning regime, Development Core Team 2008).
some models were run on data from the BFD trial only (see
‘Results’). In these models, we only included nest identity
as a random intercept effect. Results
Variation in the total number of PAB events per nestling
during the 90-min feeding trials was analysed in Poisson Effects of food deprivation on PAB frequency and intensity
models where feeding trial (BFD or AFD), a dichotomous
variable (‘experimental group’ hereafter) indicating if nes- Analysis 1: effects on the number of PAB events
tlings belonged to the food deprivation or control group, and
their interaction were all included as fixed effects (analysis 1). Across both feeding trials, the number of PAB events was
Variation in PAB intensity according to feeding trial, experi- affected by food deprivation, as shown by the effect of
mental group and their interaction was analysed in a multino- experimental group × feeding trial interaction (F1, 22 =
mial mixed model (analysis 2). 11.18, p=0.003). As expected, in the BFD trial, the nestlings
Author's personal copy
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859 855

that were subsequently assigned to either treatment events, nestlings begged at highest intensity if they had
performed a similar number of PAB events, while in the previously performed PAB and at lowest intensity if their
AFD trial, the mean number of PAB events was higher for siblings had performed PAB; baseline begging intensity was
the food-deprived nestling, either compared to its control intermediate (Fig. 2).
nestmate or to itself BFD (Fig. 1a). Conversely, the number In a model including data from both trials, there was a
of PAB by control nestlings did not change between the statistically significant three-way interaction effect among
BFD and the AFD trials (Fig. 1a). experimental group, feeding trial and PAB category on
begging intensity (F2, 22 =5.03, p=0.016). To better interpret
Analysis 2: effects on PAB intensity this effect, we ran separate mixed models for each feeding
trial and explored the effects of the experimental group ×
There was no statistically significant difference in PAB PAB category interaction on begging intensity (Table 1). As
intensity according to experimental group, feeding trial or expected, because of random composition of experimental
their interaction (F1, 22 <2.70, p>0.11 in all cases), though groups, in the BFD trial, the interaction effect was non-
the pattern was coherent with previous analyses (Fig. 1b). significant, whereas it was statistically significant in the
AFD trial (Table 1, Fig. 2). In the AFD trial, control nes-
Effects of PAB and food deprivation on begging intensity tlings behaved similarly to the BFD trial (Fig. 2). However,
at parental visits and food intake begging intensity of food-deprived nestlings did not differ
after their own compared to their sibling's PAB (Fig. 2).
Analysis 3: effects on begging intensity Moreover, in feeding events not preceded by any PAB,
baseline begging intensity of food-deprived nestlings was
In the BFD trial, begging intensity varied according to PAB significantly lower compared with feeding events following
category (F2, 22 =6.02, p=0.008). Specifically, at feeding own or sibling's PAB (Fig. 2).

b
6.0 a Analysis 4: effects of sibling's PAB on the probability
that a nestling did not beg
5.0
Number of PAB events

4.0
a
In the BFD trial, the probability that a nestling did not beg at
a
a parental arrival (begging score =0) increased at feeding
3.0
visits preceded by a sibling's PAB compared to feeding
2.0 visits not preceded by any sibling's PAB (1.047±0.454 SE,
z=2.30, p=0.021), while the other model terms were non-
1.0
significant (both p>0.75). This analysis could be performed
0.0 only on data for the BFD trial because models run either on
data from both trials (with feeding trial as a fixed factor) or
38
2.0
b on data from the AFD trial only did not converge, both
42 including or excluding random effects (details not shown).
1.8 54 18

Analysis 5: effects on food intake


PAB intensity

1.6

In the BFD trial, after performing PAB, both nestlings had a


1.4
nearly 0.8 probability of obtaining the first food item
1.2
brought by parents (Fig. 3). This value is significantly larger
than the expected value of 0.5 (each nestling should have an
1.0 equal probability of getting food if PAB does not affect food
BFD AFD intake; null binomial mixed model: intercept estimate=
Feeding trial 1.322±0.398 SE, z=3.32, p<0.001; Fig. 3).
Fig. 1 Mean (+SE) number of PAB events in 90-min feeding trials (a)
In addition, the experimental group × feeding trial inter-
and mean (+SE) intensity of PAB (b) in 22 pairs of food-deprived action effect on the probability of obtaining the first feeding
(white bars) and control (black bars) siblings before (BFD) or after after a PAB was marginally non-significant (z = 1.87,
(AFD) a period of food deprivation. In a, different letters indicate p=0.061). However, when trials were analysed separately,
statistically significant differences (p<0.05) between groups at post
hoc tests, while in b, numbers above bars denote the number of PAB
the probability of being fed after own PAB was significantly
events (for the AFD trial referring to the first third of feeding events larger for food-deprived nestlings than for controls in the
only, see ‘Material and methods’) AFD trial, but not in the BFD one (Table 1; Fig. 3), likely
Author's personal copy
856 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859

21 10 10
39
3.0 a a a Own PAB
a No PAB
Sibling PAB
2.5 927 153
39 b
b

Begging intensity
2.0 c 156
b
1.5 21
c
1.0

0.5

0.0
Food-deprived nestling Control nestling
BFD trial AFD trial

Fig. 2 Mean (+SE) begging intensity in feeding events occurring after control nestlings separately. Numbers above bars denote the number of
own (‘own PAB’) or sibling (‘sibling PAB’) event of PAB or when no feeding visits for which we scored begging intensity. Letters indicate
PAB had occurred, reflecting baseline begging intensity (‘no PAB’). statistically significant differences (p<0.05) as obtained by planned
Mean values are reported separately for before (left panel, BFD) or comparison tests and refer to comparisons among PAB categories
after food deprivation (right panel, AFD) trials (see Table 1 for statis- (BFD trial), or among PAB categories for food-deprived and control
tical analyses). Values for AFD trials are shown for food-deprived and nestlings separately (AFD trial)

because of the higher competitive efforts of hungry nes- to feeding visits not preceded by PAB was estimated at 0.28
tlings. Interestingly, after own PAB, in the AFD trial, the for both nestlings, while in the AFD trial, it was 0.22 for the
control nestling was not more likely to obtain food than its food-deprived nestling and only 0.07 for the control
food deprived nestmate that not performed PAB (Fig. 3). nestling.
When comparing feeding events occurring after own
PAB with those not preceded by any PAB, we found that
the probability of obtaining a unit of food after own PAB Discussion
was larger than in feeding events not preceded by any PAB
(see ESM Table S1). This was the case for both nestlings in We found that barn swallow nestlings increased the frequen-
both trials (see the non-significant three-way interaction in cy of PAB displays when food-deprived. Moreover, during
Table S1), suggesting that even in the AFD trial, the control parental feeding events, nestlings that had performed PAB
nestling may benefit from performing PAB. However, the begged more intensely relative to their baseline begging
latter result should be taken with caution because of the levels, whereas their siblings reduced their own begging
small sample of feeding events preceded by a PAB for these intensity. As a consequence, the chances of being fed were
nestlings (see Fig. 3). Indeed, the increase in the probability larger when a nestling had performed PAB before parental
of obtaining food in a feeding visit after own PAB compared arrival. This pattern emerged clearly in the BFD trial, when

Table 1 Models of begging intensity (multinomial mixed models) and of indicating if nestlings belonged to the food-deprivation or control group.
probability of being fed after own parent-absent begging event (binomial ‘PAB category’ is a three-level factor indicating whether begging inten-
mixed models), in before (BFD) or after (AFD) food deprivation trials sity was recorded in feeding events occurring after own PAB, a sibling's
(see ‘Results’). ‘Experimental group’ is a dichotomous variable PAB or when no PAB had occurred (see ‘Material and methods’)

BFD trial AFD trial

F/z df p F/z df p

Begging intensity
Experimental group 0.28 1, 22 0.60 17.64 1, 22 <0.001
PAB category 5.35 2, 22 0.013 6.09 2, 22 0.008
Experimental group × PAB category 1.33 2, 22 0.29 6.02 2, 22 0.008

Probability of obtaining food


Experimental group 0.36 – 0.74 2.25 – 0.025
Author's personal copy
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859 857

1.0 21
22 to decide whether escalating begging intensity during the
17
next feeding events to outcompete nestmates or refraining
Probability of obtaining food

0.8 from competition against a more vigorous competitor


(Roulin et al. 2000; Johnstone and Roulin 2003). When
10 exposed to a nestmate's PAB, a nestling may thus give up
0.6
from escalating competition because the indirect fitness
benefits of securing access to food to a sibling in poor
0.4
condition and the direct fitness advantage of saving energy
for begging displays can outweigh the cost of missing a
0.2 feeding event (Roulin et al. 2000; Johnstone and Roulin
BFD AFD
2003). We might thus envisage PAB among the mechanisms
Feeding trial
enforcing the moderation of selfish behaviour in the study
Fig. 3 Probability (+binomial SE) of obtaining the first feeding after species, which was documented in previous analyses of
own event of PAB for food-deprived (white bars) and control (black food allocation by parents when nestlings differ in hunger
bars) before (BFD) and after (AFD) a period of food deprivation (see level (Romano et al. 2012), body size, reflecting hatching
Table 1 for statistical analyses). By design, in the BFD trial, both the
control and food-deprived nestling were normally fed (see ‘Material asynchrony (Bonisoli-Alquati et al. 2011), sex (Boncoraglio
and methods’). Note that if PAB has no effect on the probability of et al. 2008) or kin (Boncoraglio et al. 2009). Therefore, our
obtaining food at the subsequent feeding visit, the expected probability findings, as well as those presented in Romano et al. (2012)
that each nestling obtains food after performing PAB is 0.5 (broken line) concerning modulation of parent-present begging in relation
to relative hunger of competing nestlings, have the impor-
nestlings were tested under the same hunger level. In the tant implication that the allocation of food in barn swallow
AFD trial, the response of nestlings to sibling's PAB, as well broods is not entirely under parental control (Godfray 1991,
as the probability of obtaining food, varied according to 1995): rather, offspring may significantly impact on parental
hunger level, as the food-deprived nestlings did not reduce feeding decisions via intrabrood signalling dynamics. In the
begging intensity at parental arrival after a PAB by their context of the present study, a further step would be to
control nestmates. assess the role of PAB in sibling interactions occurring in
Our results indicate that PAB reflects contingent need of natural, untreated barn swallow broods, consisting of three
food, similarly to parent-present begging (e.g. Boncoraglio to six nestlings of variable size and competitive ability.
et al. 2009; Romano et al. 2012). By performing PAB, Proximately, PAB displays may be a consequence of
hungry nestlings are therefore signalling their hunger, indi- hunger-induced low sensitivity in correctly detecting paren-
cating that they will vigorously compete for food at the tal feeding visits (sensu Leonard and Horn 2001; Leonard et
subsequent food provisioning event. As a result, less needy al. 2005), which may serve to minimize the chances of
nestlings reduced their begging levels at parental arrival, missing food provisioning (Dor et al. 2007), especially
making their siblings more successful at receiving food. It when the marginal benefit of acquiring an additional unit
could be argued that reduction in the intensity of begging of food is high (e.g. after food deprivation). Indeed, previ-
performed by a nestling upon a parental visit following PAB ous studies of passerines showed that PAB is generally
by its nestmate is caused by larger concomitant begging elicited by several stimuli (see e.g. Leonard and Horn
intensity by the nestling that performed PAB rather than 2001) and is apparently not directed towards nestmates.
by a response to PAB itself. This interpretation is unlikely, Nevertheless, our findings suggest that natural selection
however, because the probability that a nestling did not beg may have promoted the ability of nestlings at reciprocally
significantly increased after a PAB by its sibling, suggesting eavesdropping such ‘false alarms’ to identify highly com-
that the observed reduction of begging intensity after a petitive siblings in order to modulate their begging behav-
sibling's PAB mainly reflects a response to behavioural iour accordingly and reduce intensity of scramble
displays during interfeeding intervals. competition. Therefore, PAB displays in barn swallow nes-
Furthermore, we emphasize that, in the AFD trial, the con- tlings may have the dual adaptive function of minimizing
trol nestling maintained a high level of begging intensity during the chances of missing feedings (Dor et al. 2007) and
feeding visits preceded by its own PAB, as occurs in the BFD reducing the costs of scrambling when facing hungry, highly
trial (Fig. 2). This was the case despite the fact that its nestmate competitive siblings that are more likely to obtain the sub-
was food-deprived and thus much more needy, and indicates sequent parental food item.
that the begging intensity after own PAB does not depend on Besides testing if PAB is used by siblings to reciprocally
the competitive environment experienced by the nestling. adjust competitive efforts under a normal feeding regime, we
On the whole, our results suggest that PAB in the barn also evaluated whether its expression was context dependent,
swallow may be used by nestlings as a reliable information i.e. whether experimentally food-deprived nestlings behaved
Author's personal copy
858 Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859

differently in response to own or sibling's PAB compared to modulation of PAB behaviour in response to experimentally
control and normally fed nestmates. The results were neat in broadcasted PAB signals, but did not analyse the variation of
showing that food-deprived nestlings did not respond to a begging behaviour at parental arrival nor the consequences in
sibling's PAB by reducing their begging intensity, as they terms of food intake (Bulmer et al. 2008). Remarkably, in the
conversely did when normally fed, but rather escalated com- barn swallow, the effect of PAB on the outcome of sibling
petitive efforts by begging at a significantly higher intensity competition, despite feeding events are not preceded by
than their baseline begging level (Fig. 2). Thus, PAB by prolonged and reciprocal ‘negotiation’ displays among nes-
control nestmates exacerbated competition efforts by food- tlings as occurs in barn owl broods, is compatible with that
deprived siblings when parents arrived at the nest with food, observed in the latter species.
again suggesting that PAB induces a strong behavioural re- In conclusion, we suggest that nestling barn swallows, by
sponse by the interacting siblings. Such competitive escalation eavesdropping PAB signals displayed by siblings, may strive
affected access to food in the AFD trial: after own PAB, to achieve an optimal balance between the direct fitness costs
control nestlings were not more likely to obtain food than of sibling competition, the direct fitness benefits of obtaining
their food-deprived nestmates not performing PAB (see food by parents and the indirect fitness costs of subtracting
Fig. 3). Thus, whenever marked competitive asymmetries in food to needy kin, while additionally reducing the chances of
need between otherwise similar siblings occur, the benefit of missing feeding events. However, we also experimentally
obtaining an additional food item is likely to be dispropor- demonstrated that strong asymmetries in individual need
tionately larger for the food-deprived than for the control may lead competing siblings, especially under food shortage,
nestling. Indeed, for the food-deprived nestling, the potential to attempt to monopolize parental resources.
cost of refraining from competition could be too large and
probably not compensated by the indirect fitness benefit of
Acknowledgments We thank all farm owners for granting access to
favouring the access to food by a less needy sibling. In such
their properties. We also thank the Associate Editor and two anony-
circumstances, PAB may have the counterproductive effects mous reviewers for constructive criticism on a previous version of the
of stimulating competition by the more needy nestmate, manuscript. AR was funded by a MIUR PhD grant. GB was funded by
suggesting that, despite being perceived and used by Marie Curie Intra-European Fellowship PIEF-GA-2009-252120.
nestmates to modulate their behaviour, it constitutes a
Ethical standards When removed from their nest, nestlings were
sort of ‘false alarm’. We can also safely rule out the kept in a safe and warm place. At each measurement session, each
possibility that the escalating competitive behaviour of chick was handled only for few minutes, and nests were never left
food-deprived nestlings after control nestmates PAB was without at least one nestling inside to avoid parental desertion. No
obvious negative consequences of handling nestlings were detected.
caused by a low-intensity PAB signal by control siblings,
During videotaping, we could not note any obvious effect derived from
which may not convey sufficient information about its need the presence of recording equipment on both parental and offspring
of obtaining the subsequent food item: we did not find any behaviour. The study was carried out under permission of the local
statistically significant difference in intensity of PAB signals authority (Regione Lombardia #M1.2011.0002141) responsible for
authorizing animal studies in the wild, and the study protocol was
between BFD and AFD trials for the control nestlings
approved by the Ethical Committee of the Department of Biology
(see Fig. 1b). (now Department of Biosciences), University of Milan, Italy.
Previous studies investigating the evolutionary functions
of PAB mostly focused on barn owl (Tyto alba) nestlings,
which ‘negotiate’ priority in scrambling for food before
arrival of parents at the nest via a large and complex reper- References
toire of within-brood communication signals displayed al-
most before any feeding visit (Roulin et al. 2000, 2009; Bates D, Maechler M, Dai B (2008) lme4: linear mixed-effects models
Roulin 2001, 2004; Dreiss et al. 2010). Such an intense using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-28. http://lme4.
r-forge.r-project.org/. Accessed 18 June 2012
negotiation among nestlings, resulting in a reciprocal modu- Boncoraglio G, Martinelli R, Saino N (2008) Sex-related asymmetry in
lation of competitive behaviour for access to food in re- competitive ability of sexually monomorphic barn swallow nes-
sponse to own or nestmates' PAB signals (Dreiss et al. tlings. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:729–738
2010), has the twofold advantage of reducing the costs of Boncoraglio G, Caprioli M, Saino N (2009) Fine-tuned modulation of
competitive behaviour according to kinship in barn swallow nes-
competition and increasing indirect fitness (Roulin et al. tlings. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2117–2123
2000; Johnstone and Roulin 2003). On the other hand, most Boncoraglio G, Caprioli M, Saino N (2012) Solicitation displays
studies of PAB in other bird species did not investigate reliably reflect oxidative damage in barn swallow nestlings.
whether PAB is involved in intrabrood communication Behav Ecol Sociobiol 66:539–546
Bonisoli-Alquati A, Boncoraglio G, Caprioli M, Saino N (2011) Birth
(Budden and Wright 2001; Leonard and Horn 2001; Rivers order, individual sex and sex of competitors determine the out-
et al. 2009; Leonard et al. 2005; Dor et al. 2007). A single come of conflict among siblings over parental care. Proc R Soc
study of spotless starlings (Sturnus unicolor) demonstrated Lond B 278:1273–1279
Author's personal copy
Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2013) 67:851–859 859

Budden AE, Wright J (2001) Falling on deaf ears: the adaptive signif- Mock DW, Parker GA (1997) The evolution of sibling rivalry. Oxford
icance of begging in the absence of a parent. Behav Ecol University Press, New York
Sociobiol 49:474–481 Moreno-Rueda G, Redondo T (2011) Begging at high level simulta-
Bulmer E, Celis P, Gil D (2008) Parent-absent begging: evidence for neously impairs growth and immune response in southern shrike
sibling honesty and cooperation in the spotless starling (Sturnus (Lanius meridionalis) nestlings. J Evol Biol 24:1091–1098
unicolor). Behav Ecol 19:279–284 Parker GA, Macnair MR (1979) Models of parent-offspring conflict.
Clutton-Brock TH (1991) The evolution of parental care. Princeton IV. Suppression: evolutionary retaliation by the parent. Anim
University Press, Princeton Behav 27:1210–1235
Cotton PA, Wright J, Kacelnik A (1999) Chick begging strategies in Development Core Team R (2008) R: a language and environment for
relation to brood hierarchies and hatching asynchrony. Am Nat statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing,
153:412–420 Wien
Dor R, Kedar H, Winkler DW, Lotem A (2007) Begging in the absence Rivers JW (2009) Parent-absent begging in the brown-headed cowbird
of parents: a “quick on the trigger” strategy to minimize costly (Molothrus ater): the role of short-term need and nestmate size.
misses. Behav Ecol 18:97–102 Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:707–717
Dreiss A, Lahlah N, Roulin A (2010) How siblings adjust sib–sib Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Zúñiga JM, Redondo T (2001) Effects of
communication and begging signals to each other. Anim Behav begging on growth rates of nestling chicks. Behav Ecol 12:269–
80:1049–1055 274
Godfray HCJ (1991) Signalling of need by offspring to their parents. Romano A, Caprioli M, Boncoraglio G, Saino N, Rubolini D (2012)
Nature 352:328–330 With a little help from my kin: barn swallow nestlings modulate
Godfray HCJ (1995) Signaling of need between parents and young: solicitations of parental care according to nestmates' need. J Evol
parent offspring conflict and sibling rivalry. Am Nat 146:1–24 Biol 25:1703–1710
Johnstone RA (1999) Signalling of need, sibling competition and the Romano A, Rubolini D, Caprioli M, Boncoraglio G, Ambrosini R,
cost of honesty. P Natl Acad Sci USA 96:12644–12649 Saino N (2011) Sex-related effects of an immune challenge on
Johnstone RA, Roulin A (2003) Sibling negotiation. Behav Ecol growth and begging behavior of barn swallow nestlings. PLoS
14:780–786 One 6:e22805
Kilner RM (2002) Sex differences in canary (Serinus canaria) provi- Roulin A (2001) Food supply differentially affects sibling negotiation
sioning rules. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 52:400–407 and competition in the barn owl (Tyto alba). Behav Ecol Sociobiol
Kilner RM, Johnstone R (1997) Begging the question: are offspring 49:514–519
solicitation behaviours signals of need? Trends Ecol Evol 12:11– Roulin A (2004) Effects of hatching asynchrony on sibling negotiation,
15 begging, jostling for position and within-brood food allocation in
Leonard ML, Horn AG (2001) Begging in the absence of parents by the barn owl Tyto alba. Evol Ecol Res 6:1083–1098
nestling tree swallows. Behav Ecol 12:501–505 Roulin A, Dreiss A, Fioravanti C, Bize P (2009) Vocal sib–sib in-
Leonard ML, Horn AG, Mukhida A (2005) False alarms and begging teractions: how siblings adjust signalling level to each other.
in nestling birds. Anim Behav 69:701–708 Anim Behav 77:717–725
Magrath RD, Haff TM, Horn AG, Leonard ML (2010) Calling in the Roulin A, Kölliker M, Richner H (2000) Barn owl (Tyto alba) siblings
face of danger: predation risk and acoustic communication by vocally negotiate resources. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:459–463
parent birds and their offspring. Adv Stud Behav 41:187–253 SAS Institute (2006) The GLIMMIX procedure. SAS, Cary
Macnair MR, Parker GA (1979) Models of parent-offspring conflict. Stearns SC (1992) The evolution of life-histories. Oxford University
III. Intra-brood conflict. Anim Behav 27:1202–1209 Press, Oxford
Mock DW, Dugas MB, Strickler SA (2011) Honest begging: Wright J, Leonard ML (2002) The evolution of begging. Kluwer,
expanding from signal of need. Behav Ecol 22:909–917 Dordrecht

View publication stats

You might also like