You are on page 1of 10

College Teaching

ISSN: 8756-7555 (Print) 1930-8299 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vcol20

How Does Literature Affect Empathy in Students?

Christine R. Junker & Stephen J. Jacquemin

To cite this article: Christine R. Junker & Stephen J. Jacquemin (2017) How Does Literature Affect
Empathy in Students?, College Teaching, 65:2, 79-87, DOI: 10.1080/87567555.2016.1255583

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/87567555.2016.1255583

Published online: 13 Jan 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1796

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 5 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=vcol20
COLLEGE TEACHING
2017, VOL. 65, NO. 2, 79–87
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87567555.2016.1255583

How Does Literature Affect Empathy in Students?


Christine R. Junker and Stephen J. Jacquemin
Wright State University—Lake Campus

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
Scholars have suggested that reading literature can foster empathy. However, learning empathy Literature pedagogy;
through literature in the classroom is understudied. The primary objective of this study was to diversity; empathy
assess whether affective and cognitive empathy, as demonstrated in student writing, relates to pedagogy; multivariate
textual attributes, the style of writing prompt, student writing ability, and whether it changes over analysis
time. Students in a college literature classroom were asked to assess texts according to a series of
attributes related to engagement and textual difficulty, followed by a series of analytical and
creative writing prompts. These responses were scored on a comparative scale according to metrics
of empathy and compared with textual attributes, strength of writing, and time using a general
linear model. Textual difficulty was identified as the greatest predictor of empathy (inverse
relationship) followed by assignment grade (positive relationship). These results indicate that
textual attributes, strength of writing ability, and style of writing response play a central role in
explaining empathetic responses in students. The furthest-reaching implications of this study may,
however, rest in the findings that empathy didn’t change over the short time period and that
textual accessibility may trump all other aspects in facilitating empathetic responses.

Introduction
well as unreliable or inconsistent means of measuring
The relationship between narrative, reading, and empa- empathy (Batt-Rawden 2013, 1175; Gerdes et al. 2010,
thy has become the subject of much debate and study 2337). Thus, prior to any study or assessment of empa-
over the past decade as research has begun to elicit thy, the meaning and methods to define empathy should
whether empathy can be taught or instilled at an individ- be outlined, since “empathy” can often be more of an
ual level or whether it is intrinsic and therefore difficult umbrella term used to describe a wide variety of traits,
to change. Research on teaching empathy within the behaviors, and/or reactions to a particular stimulus and
higher education classroom setting has been limited the assessment techniques can include anything which
mostly to professional fields, such as medicine or social describes a mental, emotional, or behavioral process in
work. Overall, scholars have become increasingly response to a stimulus (e.g. the Reading the Mind in the
invested in researching the most effective ways to both Eyes Test, personality scales, textual analysis, etc.; Kidd
teach and assess empathy, since research has shown that & Castano 2013, Djikic et.al. 2013, Mar et.al. 2009; Bal &
professional efficacy in a variety of disciplines (e.g. medi- Veltkamp 2013; Batson et. al. 2002; Oatley 1999). For the
cine, social work, etc.) increases with an individual’s abil- purposes of this research, and in keeping with much of
ity to empathize (Stepien and Baernstein 2006, 528; the research on empathy, we define the term “empathy”
Gerdes et al. 2010, 2336; Batt-Rawden et al. 2013, 1174) as a response that combines affective (for instance, the
with others (e.g. a patient, client, etc.). Despite this recent emotional reaction one may have toward or with a per-
emphasis on researching and teaching empathy, how- son or character) and cognitive dimensions (for example,
ever, little research has been done in higher education the mental processes of considering another’s perspective
classroom environments beyond professional schools, or imagining oneself in the situation of the person and/or
particularly those in non-professional general education character). Further, we use a combination of qualitative
areas. and quantitative textual analysis of both literature and
Much of the existing research to build upon to poten- written responses to assess empathy. Importantly, this
tially address this deficiency, however, faces similar chal- approach distinguishes empathy from an intrinsic
lenges: unclear or divergent definitions of empathy as trait - to echo Leslie Jamison’s words, “Empathy isn’t

CONTACT Stephen J. Jacquemin stephen.jacquemin@wright.edu Wright State University - Lake Campus, 7600 Lake Campus Drive, 230J Dwyer, Celina,
OH 45822, USA.
© 2017 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
80 C. R. JUNKER AND S. J. JACQUEMIN

just something that happens to us—a meteor shower of difficulty results in too much “defamiliarization” and
synapses firing across the brain—it’s also a choice we inhibits, not increases, empathy (2007, 88). Similarly,
make: to pay attention, to extend ourselves” (2014, Kin- Bal and Veltkamp found that readers who are not
dle Location 430)—as well as provides a clear and repeat- “emotionally transported” while they’re reading experi-
able assessment approach. Thus, under this model, ence decreased levels of empathy (2013, 5). Though Bal
empathy is something that can be learned and practiced and Veltkamp’s study doesn’t directly address difficulty,
as well as assessed using written artifacts (Stepien and it is probable that difficulty would interfere with textual
Baernstein 2006, 528; Batt-Rawden et al. 2013, 1174; absorption, if for no other reason than the fact that tex-
1172). tual difficulty pulls the reader out of the story through
The objective of this study was to test whether levels the reminder that one is reading, not experiencing.
of cognitive and affective empathy as evidenced in stu- Kidd and Castano, on the other hand, found that liter-
dent writing relates to textual attributes of literature ary, as opposed to popular, texts led to a larger increase
assigned in class, a particular style or genre of writing in empathy (2013), perhaps because literary texts offer
response, an individual student’s writing ability, and more ambiguity and complexity, and thus engage read-
whether it can change over a semester with practice. ers in the kind of interpretative work that fosters a
While this research deals with specific texts from a gen- greater degree of empathy (Koopman and Hakemulder
eral literature course, given the broad range of circum- 2015, 88).
stances in which literature could be utilized to In addition to the variety of textual attributes or kind
potentially affect an individual’s empathetic responses, of text being read, research has also begun to elicit
makes these results potentially applicable over a wide whether or not the manner in which one responds to a
range of subject areas. Furthermore, this study may allow text relates to variation in empathy. For example, Koop-
researchers to understand why or why not a certain situ- man and Hakemulder argue that fictional narratives
ation (in this case, textual) may elicit a particular empa- prompt reflection, which they define as “thoughts and
thetic response. The implications of this could provide a insights on oneself, often in relation to others, and/or
framework for gauging what sorts of general stimuli society” and that this reflection may encourage “empathy
affect students and professionals. to emerge for the fullest extent” (2015, 101). Though
There is a precedent for linking the reading of litera- their research does not specifically test the effects of
ture with affective and cognitive empathy that has been reflective writing, it follows that writing reflectively about
supported by numerous experimental studies. For literary texts could encourage the emergence of empathy,
example, variation in empathy has been shown to provided that the writing prompted readers to link the
strongly depend on certain textual attributes (e.g. non- texts to themselves and to interrogate the relationship
fiction vs. fiction, narrative point of view), including between themselves and the characters and situations in
those attributes that specifically deal with the reader’s the texts they’re reading. In addition, Oatley argues for
perception of the text (e.g. getting pulled into a story, the importance of “role-taking” while reading in order to
feeling clear or unclear as to narrative meaning, etc.). facilitate empathy (1999, 445), which suggests that writ-
Generally, reading fiction seems to generate more ing in which students are asked to take on the perspec-
empathy than reading nonfiction texts, even if those tive of the character may correspond with an increase in
nonfiction texts are narrative in style (Kidd and Castano empathy. Writing, on the other hand, that does not
2013, 380; Bal and Veltkamp 2013, 5; Koopman and involve role-taking and/or personal reflection may not
Hakemulder 2015, 80; Djikic, Oatley, and Moldoveanu have an effect, or may negatively impact, reader/student
2009, 40). Another crucial factor that influences reader empathy.
empathy is the reader’s ability to identify with charac- Finally, whether empathy, in relation to reading,
ters (Keen 2007, 68), so textual attributes that facilitate changes over the course of time, short or long term, is
identification, such as the use of first-person narration, relevant to understanding the potential lasting influences
are likely to play a role in fostering empathy (Oatley of reading and what can drive these patterns. In the short
1999, 445). More specifically, Keen discovered that term, Djikic, Oatley, and Moldoveanu (2013, 40) as well
readers are more likely to identify with characters who as Kidd and Castano (2013), found that cognitive empa-
are experiencing negative affective situations and that thy increases after reading short stories or excerpts.
watching characters experience hardship makes readers These studies, however, measured empathy and reading
especially likely to be able to identify across categories within a lab setting. Longer term studies, such as Mar,
of difference (2007, 72). However, there is less agree- Oatley, and Peterson’s examination of empathy, reading
ment about the role of textual “difficulty” and empathy fiction, and personality traits, track how a lifetime of
as Keen, for example, contends that too much literary reading affects overall empathy levels, independent of
COLLEGE TEACHING 81

personality, and conclude that the more a person reads, Table 1. Texts with mean student scores (standard error in paren-
the more empathetic that person is likely to be, even theses) arranged by specific empathy metrics.
when traits like gender, age, language fluency, and per- Cultural Multiple
Text Openness Frameworks Perspectives Affective
sonality traits are controlled for (2009, 420-1). This
empirical research is consistent with the philosophical American Salvage 3.1 (0.18) 2.5 (0.22) 2.8 (0.25) 2.9 (0.23)
writings of cultural theorists who make strong claims for Beloved 2.7 (0.24) 2.4 (0.24) 2.5 (0.24) 2.4 (0.20)
Billy Lynn 2.5 (0.28) 2.9 (0.32) 2.6 (0.31) 3.0 (0.14)
the value of reading novels. For example, Martha Nuss- Constellation 2.9 (0.24) 2.5 (0.22) 2.8 (0.24) 2.5 (0.22)
baum (1995, 92) argues that reading realist novels instills Curious Incident 2.6 (0.20) 2.3 (0.14) 2.5 (0.21) 2.7 (0.20)
Glass Castle 2.5 (0.34) 2.5 (0.34) 2.5 (0.31) 2.4 (0.25)
“habits of mind that lead toward social equality” largely Henrietta Lacks 3.1 (0.15) 2.9 (0.20) 3.0 (0.17) 2.8 (0.18)
because novels teach us to see humans, including indi- Part-Time Indian 3.0 (0.17) 3.0 (0.20) 3.0 (0.15) 2.9 (0.20)
Kindred 3.5 (0.17) 3.6 (0.22) 3.6 (0.16) 2.9 (0.16)
viduals very different from ourselves, as distinct beings Olive Kitteridge 2.7 (0.23) 2.7 (0.20) 2.8 (0.22) 2.9 (0.19)
with their own desires, identities, and complex life histo- Paradise 2.7 (0.21) 2.5 (0.22) 2.3 (0.30) 2.4 (0.27)
Poisonwood Bible 3.0 (0.21) 2.6 (0.27) 3.0 (0.26) 2.8 (0.20)
ries. Taken together, these studies make a compelling
case for the idea that reading literature increases habits Note. Full text and author information is included in the methods section.
of mind consistent with empathy, both short term and
long term. However, classroom scale research over the frameworks that affected characters’ behaviors, affective
course of an entire semester has not been undertaken. responses to the texts, and literary concepts. A subset of
This study seeks to fill in this gap by assessing a series the 12 total texts used also included a creative writing
of four related questions and hypotheses relating to prompt (two in each semester), for which students were
empathy, reading, and writing in the higher-education asked to rewrite a scene from one of the novels from a
classroom: (1) Is there a relationship between empathy different narrative perspective. All writing assignments
and text? (2) Does the type of writing assignment that stu- were given an overall grade, independent of empathy,
dents complete in response to a text coincide with their that addressed literary analysis and content, paper struc-
level of empathy? (3) Is empathy related to writing ability? ture, formatting, and writing style. Following this, writ-
and (4) Does empathy increase over time in a classroom ing assignments were further assessed for empathy using
setting? a 4 point scale (table 1), independent of assignment
grade, according to the following measures of cognitive
and affective empathy: “openness,” (student’s ability to
Methods
read about diverse cultural experiences and people with-
To assess potential relationships between empathy and out leaping to premature judgments), “cultural frame-
text, texts were chosen in this study for their representa- works” (student’s ability to identify and understand the
tion of diverse perspectives, characters, and cultural complexity and implications of a character’s cultural
frameworks (table 1). Study participants were chosen as background and frameworks), “multiple perspectives”
part of two sophomore-level English courses. To increase (the student’s imaginative ability to view a situation from
the number of texts utilized in the study, texts varied a multiplicity of viewpoints), and “affective” (the stu-
between classes; however, all participants were engaged dent’s identification of emotional nuance and expression
in identical course structure (e.g. assignments, schedule, of shared emotional experiences or traits) (table 1). To
etc.). A total of six unique texts were used in each semes- establish textual attributes pertaining to each text and
ter (including: Bonnie Jo Campbell’s American Salvage, overall difficulty, students rated each text on a 5-point
Toni Morrison’s Beloved, Ben Fountain’s Billy Lynn’s scale according to 13 textual attributes that addressed
Long Halftime Walk, Anthony Marra’s A Constellation how engrossed students were in the story, whether stu-
of Vital Phenomena, Mark Haddon’s The Curious Inci- dents found the narrative confusing or easy to follow,
dent of the Dog in the Night-Time, Jeanette Walls’ The and how convincing each narrative was relative to story
Glass Castle, Rebecca Skloot’s The Immortal Life of Hen- and overall theme (table 2) as well as how difficult overall
rietta Lacks, Sherman Alexie’s The Absolutely True Diary they found the text (Buselle and Bilandzic 2009). All use
of a Part-Time Indian, Octavia Butler’s Kindred, Eliza- of student artifacts was undertaken in accordance with
beth Strout’s Olive Kitteridge, Toni Morrison’s Paradise, approved protocols outlined through the university IRB
and Barbara Kingsolver’s The Poisonwood Bible. After committee (#5391 Service Learning and Multicultural
reading and discussing each text, students completed Competency in the Literature Classroom).
standard analytical writing assignments using compara- To assess potential linkages between student empathy
ble prompts across texts. Each analytical prompt was and textual attributes, a principal components analysis
specifically designed to build students’ skills at discerning (PCA) was used to condense variation in the student
and analyzing: multiple perspectives, cultural scored textual attribute matrix to facilitate direct
82
C. R. JUNKER AND S. J. JACQUEMIN

Table 2. Texts with mean student scores (standard error in parentheses) arranged by textual attributes. (attributes adapted from “measuring narrative engagement” Busselle and Bilandzic
2009).
Forgot Attn on Mind Easily followed Hard time Found story Hard time
Lost track Text problems surroundings Into world Not pulled Wanted to wandered actions recognizing logical and Found story making sense
Text of time dragged while reading instead of novel of story into story know ending while reading of story thread of story convincing unclear of story

American Salvage 2.4 (0.27) 3.2 (0.29) 2.8 (0.33) 3.2 (0.29) 2.8 (0.25) 3.4 (0.27) 3.2 (0.29) 3.1 (0.31) 3.3 (0.26) 2.8 (0.29) 2.9 (0.23) 3.2 (0.36) 3.1 (0.28)
Beloved 2.4 (0.37) 3.4 (0.27) 2.3 (0.25) 3.4 (0.24) 2.4 (0.29) 3.6 (0.22) 2.8 (0.28) 3.6 (0.22) 2.4 (0.22) 3.3 (0.25) 2.9 (0.18) 3.9 (0.14) 3.8 (0.25)
Billy Lynn 3.0 (0.27) 2.9 (0.29) 2.6 (0.20) 3.0 (0.30) 3.0 (0.30) 2.9 (0.32) 3.7 (0.27) 2.9 (0.29) 3.4 (0.24) 2.5 (0.21) 3.7 (0.20) 3.2 (0.30) 2.9 (0.21)
Constellation 2.7 (0.35) 3.3 (0.30) 2.8 (0.33) 3.3 (0.35) 2.9 (0.35) 3.2 (0.38) 3.4 (0.36) 3.3 (0.32) 3.1 (0.27) 3.3 (0.28) 3.5 (0.27) 3.9 (0.19) 3.8 (0.24)
Curious Incident 3.5 (0.21) 2.8 (0.35) 2.8 (.23) 2.6 (0.28) 3.4 (0.20) 3.0 (0.30) 3.8 (0.33) 3.1 (0.25) 3.4 (0.24) 2.4 (0.15) 3.6 (0.15) 3.2 (0.30) 3.2 (0.23)
Glass Castle 3.8 (0.26) 2.4 (0.24) 3.5 (0.31) 2.5 (0.25) 3.5 (0.21) 1.9 (0.21) 4.5 (0.16) 2.4 (0.28) 3.9 (0.21) 2.2 (0.26) 4.0 (0.27) 2.5 (0.28) 2.3 (0.24)
Henrietta Lacks 2.2 (0.21) 3.8 (0.25) 2.2 (0.26) 3.6 (0.23) 2.1 (0.25) 3.8 (0.27) 2.9 (0.31) 3.8 (0.19) 3.3 (0.25) 2.7 (0.29) 3.7 (0.21) 2.5 (0.25) 2.9 (0.30)
Part-Time Indian 4.3 (0.17) 1.7 (0.13) 3.7 (0.24) 2.1 (0.15) 4.2 (0.18) 1.8 (0.18) 4.3 (0.17) 2.2 (0.18) 4.7 (0.10) 1.6 (0.18) 4.1 (0.19) 2.0 (0.20) 1.8 (0.20)
Kindred 3.3 (0.20) 2.8 (0.30) 3.1 (0.24) 2.7 (0.23) 3.6 (0.26) 2.5 (0.23) 4.0 (0.20) 2.8 (0.25) 4.0 (0.26) 2.1 (0.23) 3.7 (0.24) 2.6 (0.25) 2.4 (0.27)
Olive Kitteridge 2.4 (0.25) 3.4 (0.26) 2.4 (0.24) 3.2 (0.29) 2.3 (0.21) 3.3 (0.27) 2.9 (0.28) 3.6 (0.22) 2.9 (0.27) 3.2 (0.25) 3.4 (0.24) 3.3 (0.24) 3.3 (0.25)
Paradise 3.6 (0.45) 3.0 (0.26) 2.7 (0.26) 2.9 (0.28) 3.1 (0.35) 2.3 (0.34) 4.2 (0.25) 3.0 (0.33) 3.1 (0.46) 2.5 (0.31) 3.2 (0.25) 3.7 (0.34) 3.4 (0.34)
Poisonwood Bible 3.4 (0.27) 3.1 (0.31) 2.8 (0.29) 3.3 (0.34) 3.0 (0.33) 3.0 (0.30) 3.9 (0.28) 3.0 (0.37) 3.2 (0.39) 2.6 (0.34) 3.6 (0.27) 3.4 (0.31) 3.4 (0.27)
COLLEGE TEACHING 83

comparisons with individual empathy metrics. PCA is an the texts, a creative writing assignment that asked the stu-
unconstrained ordination technique that indirectly dents to take on the perspective of someone in the story
extracts gradients between variables in a given dataset. while they navigated a particular scene in the novel.
The PCA was run in the software PC-ORD using default PCA of the textual attributes explained 71% of the
analysis settings (version 6.19, MjM Software, USA). variation in responses along 2 significant axes describing
Axes were interpreted following the broken stick model each text. The first axis (eigenvalue 7.5) explained 58%
(Jackson 1993). To test for relatedness with empathy, the of the variation in textual attributes and primarily sepa-
resulting PCA axes were correlated (using Pearson’s rated texts on the basis of a given reader’s feelings per-
method) with each of the four measured empathy met- taining to getting absorbed into the text (positive loading
rics. To assess whether empathy scores changed with values) compared with negatively loading perceptions
time, writing ability, or textual difficulty, the four empa- that the text was tedious or uninteresting (figure 1). The
thy metrics were regressed against time, textual difficulty, second axis (eigenvalue 1.7) explained 13% of the varia-
and assignment grade using a linear mixed-effects model tion in textual attributes, separating texts on perceptions
fitted with a repeated-measures approach to account for of how easy it was to follow events and narrative (posi-
non-independence between multiple data points for each tive loading values) compared with negatively loading
student (i.e. multiple writing artifacts submitted by the perceptions that it was difficult to understand the text’s
same student over a given semester). Lastly, a paired t- narrative or order of events (figure 1). Post hoc correla-
test was used to compare relative scores between each of tions with individual empathy metrics identified signifi-
the four empathy metrics between analytical and creative cant positive relationships along PCA1, indicating that
writing style prompts by students. All models were higher levels of openness (r D 0.16; p D 0.05), multiple
implemented in the R statistical environment (R Core perspective awareness (r D 0.17; p D 0.03), cultural
Team 2016) using the stats package. framework awareness (r D 0.20; p D 0.01), and affective
mental attitude (r D 0.19; p D 0.01) coincided with
absorption into the textual world and feelings of disasso-
Results
ciation from reality (PCA1). No significant correlations
The study included 252 responses from 42 students covering between individual empathy metrics were identified
writing prompts from 12 novels over 2 semesters. Student along PCA2. Similar directionality and strength of indi-
artifacts collected included surveys of each text that assessed vidual empathy metrics with the first axis suggested a
their emotional transportation while reading, a single ana- degree of multicollinearity might have been occurring,
lytical writing assignment paired with each text that assessed which was tested for and confirmed post hoc by calculat-
their ability to critically think about each text, and for four of ing variance inflation statistics for the individual

Figure 1. PCA scatterplot of axes 1 and 2 with strongest loading variables labeled on axes and significant correlations with empathy
metrics indicated.
84 C. R. JUNKER AND S. J. JACQUEMIN

Table 3. Results of general linear model indicating relationship engaged in a more creative writing style (i.e. rewriting a
between average empathy score and time, textual difficulty, portion of the text) tend to exhibit higher degrees of
grade, and term interactions.
empathy than students analyzing a particular theme or
Variable Estimate SE df t P textual event.
Time ¡0.18 0.09 193 ¡1.91 0.06
Text difficulty ¡0.45 0.22 193 ¡1.99 0.05
Grade 0.14 0.03 193 4.7 <0.001 Discussion
Time x Text difficulty 0.04 0.03 193 1.58 0.12
The findings of this study build upon previous research
on the relationship between reading and empathy and
empathy variables (>2.5; Lin 2011) and observing inter- represent an important contribution to understanding
correlations between variables (all above r D 0.65). Given what particular facets of reading can influence empathy,
the similarity of these variables in direction, theoretical ultimately helping to guide pedagogical methods being
meaning, and measurement unit scale, a single compos- used inside the higher education classroom. This study
ite of all four metrics was calculated and rerun with simi- shows that the relationship between reading, writing,
lar positive results along PCA1 (r D 0.19; p D 0.01) and and empathy is nuanced; that careful attention must be
PCA2 (r D 0.03; p D 0.69). paid to selecting texts, the style of writing or response
Initial results of the linear model indicated strong pos- prompts, students’ writing ability; and that, moreover,
itive covariation between individual empathy metrics as reading alone may not be enough to substantively change
well as significant covariation between textual difficulty student’s levels of empathy over the course of a 15-week
and time (e.g. harder texts assigned later in the semester). semester. Our findings also suggest that key objectives in
To reduce model complexity and account for issues with many literature classes may conflict with the objective of
multicollinearity (e.g. highly correlated empathy metrics, building empathy, so when courses are designed, these
testing using variance inflation statistic), a composite conflicting objectives will have to be brought into bal-
empathy score of the four empathy metrics was used to ance. Finally, it’s essential to recognize the assumptions
configure the final model, which tested for a relationship about empathy, as well as some of the potential risks of
with time, textual difficulty, and writing ability while emphasizing empathy within the classroom.
accounting for potential interactions between time and Perhaps the most important factor in a classroom
textual difficulty and repeated replication of students. that aims to increase empathy is the selection of texts
The final linear model elicited strong significant relation- to read because cognitive and affective empathy were
ships between empathy and textual difficulty (negative) strongly affected by students’ perception of the diffi-
and writing ability (positive) with a weaker and techni- culty of the text. Initially, it seemed likely that stu-
cally non-significant relationship (p D 0.056) with time dents would be most empathetic while reading texts
(table 3). These results indicate that students that exhibit that had characters that closely reflected themselves,
higher empathy scores also tend to exhibit better writing since research indicates that we are more likely to
abilities but that empathy scores also tend to decrease empathize with people we perceive to be close to us,
notably as textual difficulty increases and weakly as the literally and figuratively (Dolby 2012, 68). Thus, it
semester progresses. seemed probable that the students who attend the
Related to whether writing prompt style can influence rural Midwestern university, where this study was
empathy, the paired t-test identified individual empathy conducted, would have been more likely to identify
metric scores for analytical writing assignments to be sig- with the characters living in rural Kalamazoo, Michi-
nificantly lower (average of 14%) than their creative gan in Bonnie Jo Campbell’s American Salvage than
counterparts (table 4). This suggests that students with Junior in Sherman Alexie’s The Absolutely True
Diary of a Part-Time Indian. It was expected that
empathy would be challenged by characters from dif-
ferent cultural, contextual, and geographical back-
Table 4. Results of paired t test indicating differences between
writing prompt styles across texts grounds. The results of the study showed that this
was not the case and that, instead, textual attributes
Mean Mean
analytical creative Effect were far more important to the question of whether
Variable score score df t size P or not students demonstrated empathy in their writ-
Openness 3.01 3.61 74 ¡6.2 15% <0.001 ing. For example, when students responded positively
Cultural 2.92 3.35 74 ¡3.51 11% <0.001 to questions like, “I could easily follow the action and
Perspectives 3.07 3.59 74 ¡4.97 13% <0.001
Affective 2.8 3.44 74 ¡6.66 16% <0.001
events of the book,” their empathy scores went up.
When students rated a text as “hard to follow” or
COLLEGE TEACHING 85

boring, their empathetic skills correspondingly went students’ demonstration of empathy. Students writing in
down. If a reader is struggling to understand the a creative mode scored substantially higher than those
novel at a basic level, focusing all her energy on figur- writing in an analytical mode. This result is likely
ing out “what’s happening,” there’s little analytical because the writing prompts for the creative assignments
power left over for emotional and cognitive complexi- required students to take on the role of other characters,
ties. This finding confirms what many professors which necessitated an understanding of multiple per-
(particularly in literature based fields) know about spectives and the cultural frameworks of the character in
teaching “difficult” texts, which is that as the difficulty question. Students were instructed to write a scene from
of the text goes up, so does student resistance and a different perspective, with a few limitations: they could
that, moreover, experimental novels tend to evoke not change any of the plot elements, and they had to
more resistance than traditional, realist novels (Pipino present the character in a way that demonstrated an
2005, 183). What is intriguing, potentially, about this understanding of the novel. That is to say, they couldn’t
result, is that diversity does not seem to have as big change the novel in plot, tone, or character as they imagi-
of an impact as has been suggested by other research. natively interacted with the text. Previous research has
At the same time, the relationship between textual dif- linked creativity and empathy (Yaniv 2011, 56), in large
ficulty and empathy has important implications for liter- part because of the role reversal that inherently occurs
ature professors because it brings two objectives into when writing fiction. When students wrote about these
conflict. While we may want to use our classes to characters creatively, they demonstrated a stronger cog-
increase an understanding of cultural diversity and nitive and affective understanding of characters and their
empathy, we also want our students to become better behavior. This may be because students enjoyed the crea-
readers who can tackle complicated texts. If we want stu- tive assignments more than the analytical assignments,
dent empathy to increase, accessibility may need to be a but even more significantly, writing creatively may lend
deciding factor when considering what texts to assign. If itself more to empathy than writing analytically. In gen-
we want student’s reading and literary analysis skills to eral, analytical writing asks students to use a good deal of
improve, though, it is essential that students are pre- judgment in order to break up ideas, characters, and
sented with texts with an increasing level of complexity. themes into smaller pieces; creative writing, on the other
Consideration of diversity makes this even more compli- hand, asks students to view texts more intuitively and
cated. Students found Toni Morrison’s novels, Paradise holistically, which, in turn, could engender empathy.
and Beloved, challenging, especially in terms of under- Once again, this finding suggests that it may be challeng-
standing events and following the thread of the story. ing to simultaneously cultivate empathy and help stu-
That difficulty is textual, but some students might attri- dents build the analytical skills necessary for literary
bute their difficulty understanding the novel to cultural criticism papers, which are often a standard genre in the
differences rather than textual challenges. Moreover, it is discipline of literature. At the very least, to optimize an
hard to discern what texts students will find accessible increase in empathy, writing prompts and assignments
and which ones they will find difficult, and there are would have to be varied enough that students had the
multiple kinds of textual difficulty. It seems likely that opportunity to write in modes other than traditional lit-
certain kinds of textual difficulty—unlikable narrators, erary analysis.
complicated imagery or language, unconventional chro- Overall writing scores did positively affect empathy
nological organization—would affect students’ empa- scores. Students who earned higher grades also scored
thetic responses differently, but further research needs to higher on the empathy measures. A number of factors
be done in this area to determine what kinds of textual could contribute to this relationship. First, reading texts
difficulty most impacts empathy scores. It also seems to carefully and perceptively, which is required before one
be the case that if the wrong texts are chosen, reading can write well about texts, often depends the ability to
about difference could actually have the opposite of the interpret character’s subtle reactions and motivations.
intended affect—students could become less empathetic The skills of Theory of Mind and empathy, in other
and more, instead of less, resistant to diversity and differ- words, are closely related to the skills of literary analysis,
ence. Moreover, this study suggests that it’s not what particularly in relation to character analysis (Zunshine
content is being presented; rather it is how the content is 2006, Kindle Location 228). This sets up a difficult prob-
presented and then experienced by the reader that may lem for researchers because it makes it almost impossible
determine whether or not empathy increases or to untangle empathy from reading and writing skills, and
decreases. it is possible that students’ empathy skills increased more
The type of writing assignment is also crucial, since quickly than their writing skills but that they could not
the genre of writing also had a significant impact on demonstrate this increase in writing. It also suggests that
86 C. R. JUNKER AND S. J. JACQUEMIN

by improving students’ skills at Theory of Mind and Though perceived textual difficulty, existing writing
empathy, we might also be improving their literary anal- abilities, and genre of writing assignment impacted over-
ysis skills, thus linking two important objectives in many all empathy score, the fact that empathy scores did not
literature classes focused on diversity. change significantly over the course of a semester sug-
One of the most confounding results of this study was gests that other components need to be added if the
that students’ empathy did not change over time; stu- course has the objective of increasing students’ overall
dents left the class no more empathetic than when they empathy. Service-learning, especially with a focus on
began the semester. Though readers in a lab setting show narrative-based projects, would be a natural fit in this
a significant shift over a relatively short amount of time, type of course because it would give students the oppor-
the way empathy was defined and measured in most of tunity to reinforce and practice the narratively oriented
these studies is substantially different from the way it empathy skills they’re learning in the classroom. Our
was defined and measured in this project. Similarly, future research will assess the impact of incorporating
empathy in professional fields, like medicine, social narrative-oriented service-learning projects into an
work, law, and education, indicates a particular set of introductory literature course.
behaviors and skills that are less nuanced, trainable, and
more quantifiable. It is measured by the participants’
ability to identify emotions when looking at photos of
someone’s face or through a series of questionnaires References
designed to elicit one’s own perceptions of oneself in Bal, P. M., & M. Veltkamp. 2013. "How Does Fiction Reading
relation to others. This study viewed empathy as a mind- Influence Empathy? An Experimental Investigation on the
set and a way of seeing the world. Accordingly, it was Role of Emotional Transportation." Plos One 8 (1): DOI:
measured through a holistic analysis of student writing. 10.1371/journal.pone.0055341
Batt-Rawden, S. A., M. S. Chisolm, B. Anton, & T. E. Flick-
Modifying behavior, especially when one is strongly inger. 2013. "Teaching Empathy to Medical Students: An
motivated to do so, or being able to identify emotions, is Updated, Systematic Review." Academic Medicine 88 (8):
easier to change than shifting one’s mindset or approach 1171–7.
to the world. Fifteen weeks may simply not be a signifi- Batson, D. C., J. Change, R. Orr, & J. Rowland. 2002.
cant enough time to see substantial shifts in students’ “Empathy, Attitudes, and Action Can Feeling for a
Member of a Stigmatized Group Motivate One to Help
mindset and empathy skills.
the Group?” Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin
While empathy is, in and of itself, a trait worth develop- 28 (12): 1656–66.
ing, within the context of the literature courses that were Buselle, R. & H. Bilandzic. 2009. "Measuring Narrative Engage-
part of this study, empathy was part of a larger goal of ment." Media Psychology 12 (4): 321–47.
increasing an awareness of diversity. One of the purposes of Djikic, M., K. Oatley, & M. C. Moldoveanu. 2013. "Reading
this project was to use empathy as a way to encourage stu- Other Minds: Effects of Literature on Empathy." Scientific
Study Of Literature 3 (1): 28–47.
dents to be more open-minded and less judgmental, particu- Dolby, N. 2012. Rethinking Multicultural Education for the
larly when related to issues of difference and diversity. The Next Generation: The New Empathy and Social Justice. New
idea that empathy, which tends to focus on individuals, York: Routledge.
often at the expense of fully accounting for the systemic Gerdes, K. E., E. A. Segal, & C. A. Lietz. 2010. "Conceptualising
social, political, cultural, and economic forces, will trans- and Measuring Empathy." British Journal Of Social Work
40 (7): 2326–43.
form into real-world behavior or social change, may be
Jackson, D. A. 1993. "Stopping Rules In Principal Compo-
wishful thinking, however (Kulbaga 2008, 510, 517). After nents-Analysis—A Comparison of Heuristic and Statistical
all, one of theories that explains why readers of fiction show Approaches." Ecology 74 (8): 2204–14.
stronger increases in empathy than readers of nonfiction is Jamison, L. 2014. The Empathy Exams. Minneapolis: Graywolf
that fiction provides readers with a sense of safety because Press.
they can feel freely for negative situations without any Keen, S. 2007. Empathy and the Novel. Oxford: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
accompanying sense of obligation to act in a concrete man- Kidd, D. C., & E. Castano. 2013. "Reading Literary Fiction
ner (Keen 2007, 3; Bal and Veltkamp 2013, 2-3). Though it’s Improves Theory of Mind." Science 342 (6156): 377–80.
possible that once empathy has been instilled, readers may Koopman, E. M., & F. Hakemulder. 2015. “Effects of Literature
react differently when confronted with real-world situations, on Empathy and Self-Reflection: A Theoretical-Empirical
this is far from a given. Therefore, if a commitment to social Framework.” Journal of Literary Theory 9 (1): 79-111.
Kulbaga, T. A. 2008. "Pleasurable Pedagogies: 'Reading Lolita
change is part of the course objectives, reading literature to
in Tehran’ and the Rhetoric of Empathy." College English 70
generate empathy would likely need to be paired with sub- (5): 506–21.
stantial other course activities and materials to be effective Lin, D. 2011. “VIF Regression: A Fast Regression Algorithm for
in meeting those goals. Large Data”. R package version 1.0
COLLEGE TEACHING 87

Mar, R. A., K. Oatley, & J. B. Peterson. 2009. "Exploring the Link R Core Team. 2016. “A Language and Environment for Statis-
between Reading Fiction and Empathy: Ruling Out Individual tical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing,
Differences and Examining Outcomes." Communications- Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/
European Journal Of Communication Research 34 (4): 407–28. Stepien, K. A. & A. Baernstein. 2006. "Educating for Empathy. A
Nussbaum, M. C. 1995. Poetic Justice: The Literary Imagination Review." Journal Of General Internal Medicine 21 (5): 524–30.
and Public Life. Boston: Beacon Press. Yaniv, D. 2011. "Revisiting Morenian Psychodramatic Encoun-
Oatley, K. 1999. “Meeting of Minds: Dialogue, Sympa- ter in Light of Contemporary Neuroscience: Relationship
thy, and Identification in Reading Fiction.” Poetics between Empathy and Creativity." The Arts In Psychother-
26: 439–54. apy 38: 52–8.
Pipino, M. F. 2005. "Resistance and the Pedagogy of Ethnic Lit- Zunshine, L. 2006. Why We Read Fiction: Theory of Mind and
erature." MELUS 30 (2): 175–90. the Novel. Columbus: The Ohio State University Press.

You might also like