You are on page 1of 5

Appetite 105 (2016) 591e595

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Appetite
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

The evolutionary psychology of hunger


Laith Al-Shawaf a, b, *
a
Department of Psychology, Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey
b
College of Life Sciences, Wissenschaftskolleg zu Berlin (Institute for Advanced Study), Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: An evolutionary psychological perspective suggests that emotions can be understood as coordinating
Received 24 February 2016 mechanisms whose job is to regulate various psychological and physiological programs in the service of
Received in revised form solving an adaptive problem. This paper suggests that it may also be fruitful to approach hunger from this
14 June 2016
coordinating mechanism perspective. To this end, I put forward an evolutionary task analysis of hunger,
Accepted 15 June 2016
Available online 18 June 2016
generating novel a priori hypotheses about the coordinating effects of hunger on psychological processes
such as perception, attention, categorization, and memory. This approach appears empirically fruitful in
that it yields a bounty of testable new hypotheses.
Keywords:
Evolutionary psychology
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Hunger
Eating
Coordinating mechanisms
Superordinate mechanisms

1. Theoretical background opportunity (Al-Shawaf et al., 2015).


This coordinating mechanism approach was presented as a way
1.1. Emotions as coordinating mechanisms of conceptualizing emotions (Cosmides & Tooby, 2000; Tooby &
Cosmides, 1990; 2008), but it may also be usefully applied to
An evolutionary psychological approach to the emotions sug- drives or motivational states. Hunger is usually regarded as a
gests that they are coordinating mechanisms e information- motive or drive as opposed to an emotion (e.g., Kleinginna, Jr. &
processing programs that evolved to regulate the activity of other Kleinginna, 1981; Lazarus, 1991; Oatley, Keltner, & Jenkins, 2006),
programs in the service of solving a specific adaptive problem (Al- but it may be possible to use this approach to shed light on hunger
Shawaf, Conroy-Beam, Asao, & Buss, 2015; Cosmides & Tooby, and to generate novel hypotheses about how it coordinates other
2000; Tooby & Cosmides, 1990; 2008). On this view, the emotions mechanisms in the body and mind.1 According to this view, hunger
coordinate the activity of a wide range of psychological and phys- may usefully be regarded as a mechanism that coordinates the
iological mechanisms, ranging from perception, attention, and activity of psychological processes in the service of solving the
memory to specialized inference mechanisms, physiology, and adaptive problem of acquiring food (Cosmides & Tooby, 2000).
behavior. In their presentation of the coordinating mechanism approach,
This approach suggests that each emotion evolved to coordinate Tooby and Cosmides (1990, 2008; Cosmides & Tooby, 2000) sug-
the operation of psychological and physiological mechanisms in gested a list of 14 programs regulated by the emotions. I present
solving a particular adaptive problem. For example, fear co- these again here because they serve as a foundation for our hy-
ordinates the activity of psychological and physiological programs pothesis generation method: (1) perceptual mechanisms, (2)
in the service of avoiding predators or escaping danger (Bracha, attention, (3) memory, (4) categorization, (5) motivational prior-
2004; Marks & Nesse, 1994), disgust regulates mechanisms in or- ities, (6) current goals, (7) information-gathering adaptations, (8)
der to avoid infection (Curtis, Aunger, & Rabie, 2004; Tybur, specialized inference mechanisms, (9) communication & expres-
Lieberman, Kurzban, & DeScioli, 2012), and sexual arousal orches- sion, (10) learning mechanisms, (11) reflexes, (12) energy level,
trates programs in preparation for an advantageous sexual

1
Indeed, the original authors of this idea suggest that, on their view, there may
* Department of Psychology, Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey. be no principled, non-arbitrary distinction between drive states and other emotion
E-mail address: laith@bilkent.edu.tr. programs (Cosmides & Tooby, 2000).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.06.021
0195-6663/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
592 L. Al-Shawaf / Appetite 105 (2016) 591e595

mood, and effort allocation, (13) physiology, and (14) behavior. Bryson, & Seth, 2007; and Redgrave, Prescott, & Gurney, 1999, for
The theoretical foundations of this approach, including a dis- related discussions about natural action selection mechanisms).
cussion of a) the conceptual strengths of this view, b) why mech- Evolutionary psychological reasoning suggests that the solu-
anisms need coordination in the first place, and c) how to tions to these adaptive metaproblems should differ systematically
empirically test the validity of this approach, are all outlined in by sex, context, and individual difference variables. For example,
longer conceptual papers (Al-Shawaf et al., 2015; Cosmides & given men’s stronger predilection for short-term mating and casual
Tooby, 2000; Tooby & Cosmides, 1990; 2008). I therefore do not sex (Buss, 2003; Lippa, 2009; Symons, 1979), a short-term mating
repeat those points here. Instead, in this article I approach hunger opportunity with an attractive member of the opposite sex should
from this “coordinating mechanism” perspective, using a method suppress hunger much more powerfully among men than among
called evolutionary task analysis to generate an array of novel a women. By contrast, given the robust sex difference in disgust
priori hypotheses about hunger. (women are more easily disgusted than men; Al-Shawaf & Lewis,
2013; Al-Shawaf, Lewis, & Buss, 2014; Curtis et al., 2004; Tybur
1.2. Heuristic value and predictive power et al., 2012), the presentation of a pathogen threat should suppress
hunger more effectively among women than among men.
The coordinating mechanism approach offers a systematic hy- Ultimately, the outcome of tradeoffs between competing
pothesis generation method based on evolutionary task analysis mechanisms such as hunger and sexual arousal will depend on the
(Marr, 1982). It consists of several key questions: (1) what adaptive costs and benefits associated with allocating resources to one
problem, if any, did this mechanism evolve to solve?, (2) which adaptive problem over another. Ancestrally, the costs and benefits
subtasks must be solved in the solution of this adaptive problem?, of solving one adaptive problem over another would often have
(3) which information-processing programs are capable of solving differed systematically as a function of a) context, b) sex, and c)
these subtasks?, and (4) how should these programs be coordi- individual difference variables such as immune competence, mate
nated to deliver a well-designed solution to this adaptive problem? value, and ability to evade predators.
(Al-Shawaf et al., 2015). These systematic differences in costs and benefits between
This framework provides a systematic way to generate novel different contexts and individuals can be used to generate a priori
empirical hypotheses for each candidate emotion or motivational hypotheses about a) how hunger is expected to coordinate mech-
drive by asking which programs it is expected to coordinate and anisms in a context-specific and individual-specific manner, as well
how it is expected to do so. Of course, not every coordinating as b) how hunger is expected to trade off with competing drives or
mechanism is expected to regulate the full list of 14 programs listed emotions when an organism is faced with distinct adaptive prob-
above. lems at once. More broadly, this coordinating mechanism approach
In what follows, I apply this hypothesis-generation framework offers a way to generate context-specific hypotheses about how
to hunger. The purpose of this exercise is not to provide a hunger might regulate mechanisms in the body and mind.
comprehensive analysis of hunger or generate an exhaustive list of
hypotheses, but rather to illustrate the heuristic value and predic- 2.2. Perception
tive power of this approach and to suggest that there may be utility
in conceptualizing hunger as a coordinating mechanism. Hunger may influence perceptual mechanisms in order to solve
the adaptive problem of finding food. Early theorists recognized
2. Novel hypotheses this possibility: “What we must study … are the variations
perception itself undergoes when one is hungry, in love, in pain, or
2.1. Motivational priorities solving a problem” (Bruner & Goodman, 1947, p. 33).
These effects may operate through more than one perceptual
Hunger, a universal mechanism that evolved to solve a critical mode. For example, there is some evidence that hunger leads to
adaptive problem, is the first step in the food cycle e the sequence more positive evaluations of flavor, motivating further eating when
of behaviors that culminates in eating (Rozin & Todd, 2015). In or- an individual has yet to reach satiation (e.g., Cabanac, 1971). We
der to solve the problem of food acquisition, hunger likely reorders might hypothesize that the gustatory reward associated with
an individual’s motivational priorities, elevating the importance of eating should be directly dependent on hunger level, with greater
finding food and temporarily inhibiting other concerns such as pleasure accompanying greater hunger and diminishing hedonic
status enhancement, mating opportunities, and pathogen avoid- returns accompanying increased satiety. Tying gustatory pleasure
ance. The intensity of this motivational reordering should depend to metabolic need in this way would be a good design for a system
on the intensity of the hunger as well as the immediacy and that motivates eating.
importance of other adaptive problems that the organism is facing. There is already evidence to suggest that food odors e but not
For example, when simultaneously presented with hunger and a other odors e are more pleasant when nutritionally depleted
threat or opportunity of great importance to fitness, hunger may (Duclaux, Feisthauer, & Cabanac, 1973). We also have evidence that
have a more muted effect on motivational reordering. In extreme hunger increases the perceived brightness of foods (Gilchrist &
cases, intense hunger may be suppressed in order to solve a more Nesberg, 1952), and improves perceptual encoding or accuracy of
pressing adaptive problem, such as urgent predator avoidance or food-relevant stimuli (Lazarus, 1998; Radel & Cle ment-Guillotin,
the consummation of a valuable mating opportunity. We can refer 2012), and that thirst may increase the perceptual or cognitive
to the simultaneous presentation of conflicting adaptive problems accessibility of drinking-related stimuli (Aarts, Dijksterhuis, & De
as an adaptive metaproblem. Vries, 2001), as well as making it easier for people to perceive
Solving an adaptive metaproblem requires a tradeoff between transparency (a quality of water) in ambiguous stimuli (Changizi &
the mechanisms responsible for solving the competing adaptive Hall, 2001). It would also be reasonable to expect that hunger and
problems. In some cases, one adaptive problem may dominate, as thirst make food and drink look more visually appealing as well
when hunger is suppressed during a near-death escape from a (but see Hoefling et al., 2009).
predator. In other cases, a compromise solution is possible, and an Relatedly, hunger may enhance olfactory acuity, aiding the
individual may simultaneously allocate a portion of her resources process of detecting and locating food when nutritionally depleted.
to two different adaptive problems (see Crabbe, 2007; Prescott, This modulation of olfactory sensitivity by metabolic status and
L. Al-Shawaf / Appetite 105 (2016) 591e595 593

feeding state has been demonstrated in nonhuman species such as safe or spoiled. And nested in the category of safe foods, hunger may
rodents, Drosophila, and C. elegans (Fadool, Tucker, & Pedarzani, further categorize items according to caloric density or nutritional
2011; Palouzier-Paulignan et al., 2012; Root, Ko, Jafari, & Wang, value. Higher priorities should be assigned to foods of greater value
2011). Testing this hypothesis in humans seems both feasible and and foods containing specific nutrients that an organism lacks (e.g.,
worthwhile. Rozin & Vollmecke, 1986; Rozin & Todd, 2015; Siep et al., 2009).
An intriguing further possibility is that hunger may improve Evolutionary thinking suggests that this categorization may be
detection of food-related sounds (e.g., sounds emitted by prey an- adaptively biased, such that intense hunger results in a more
imals, or the sound of group members eating nearby). These hy- lenient categorization of safe foods (e.g., see Cabanac, 1971). In the
potheses about olfactory and auditory perception can be tested in extreme, we might expect an organism on the brink of starvation to
the lab by experimentally manipulating hunger and then present- categorize as “safe” food sources that would otherwise be classified
ing audio recordings and odors of different kinds, testing partici- as spoiled, as long as the on-average cost of such overinclusive
pants’ accuracy and sensitivity in detecting food-relevant and food- categorization is less than the cost of forgoing viable food sources
irrelevant sounds and smells. and risking death by starvation.
Extreme hunger should also suppress disgust to potentially
2.3. Attention and problem solving pathogenic foods (Hoefling et al., 2009). As individuals approach
starvation, we should see a muted disgust response to foods that
Hunger may influence attention, too, narrowing an organism’s would otherwise trigger revulsion. If this response is an adaptation
attentional span and sharpening its focus to food and food-relevant against starvation, it should be highly specific to food, and not other
stimuli (Channon & Hayward, 1990; Mogg, Bradley, Hyare, & Lee, disgusting items. Evolutionary considerations therefore suggest
1998; Stockburger, Schma €lzle, Flaisch, Bublatzky, & Schupp, that hunger should have no effect on disgust toward open sores,
2009). To deliver a well-designed solution to this adaptive prob- bodily effluvia, or other sources of pathogens and parasites. This
lem, hunger must down-regulate attention to other stimuli that appears to be exactly the pattern of results we see (Hoefling et al.,
would normally capture an organism’s interest. Severe hunger may 2009).
even suppress attention to fitness-relevant stimuli such as patho- Importantly, severe food shortages were likely more common
gens, attractive mates, or valuable alliance-building opportunities among our hunter-gatherer ancestors than they are in today’s
(but see Ainsworth & Maner, 2014). industrialized nations, and hunger probably played a prominent
It is also possible to generate very specific a priori predictions role in regulating psychology and behavior during human evolu-
about the effects of context and individual differences. For example, tion. It is easy to underestimate the significance of this psycho-
hunger’s ability to shift attention away from pathogens and toward physiological state in affluent modern-day societies, but hunger
food should be attenuated in people who are immunocompro- remains a pervasive aspect of hundreds of millions of people’s lives
mised. Hunger’s ability to suppress attention to other stimuli of in developing nations and in modern hunter-gatherer societies
adaptive significance should also depend on the magnitude of the (Gat, 2000; Gurven, Hill, Kaplan, Hurtado, & Lyles, 2000; Sanchez &
fitness threat or opportunity posed by those other stimuli. For Swaminathan, 2005; Serageldin, 2002; Speth & Spielmann, 1983),
example, hunger should be more effective at shifting a person’s and was likely a recurrent condition during the evolution of our
attention away from an unattractive mate or a moderately species.
dangerous predator compared to a highly attractive mate or an In addition to affecting the way we categorize foods and path-
extremely dangerous predator. ogens, hunger may also affect the way we categorize other humans.
It may also be difficult for hungry individuals to sustain atten- For example, hunger may lead us to categorize others as either
tion on food-irrelevant tasks. For example, hungry people may find having or not having surplus food that they might be willing to
it hard to engage in future planning, problem solving, and extended share. Similarly, hungry individuals may temporarily categorize
conversations. There should be a striking exception for food- others in terms of whether they are likely to be useful in helping
acquisition problems, which we might predict will elicit sustained with food acquisition (e.g., as a hunting or foraging partner).
interest and attention from hungry individuals. I would therefore The key point is that we may expect hunger to impose con-
hypothesize that hunger has opposite effects on different kinds of ceptual frameworks and categorization schemes relevant to the
problem solving: it leads to a decrement in people’s problem- problem of securing food. We might therefore expect hunger to
solving abilities in most domains, yet simultaneously enhances cause people to adaptively categorize objects, other people, and
their ability to solve food-acquisition problems. events in ways that facilitate the acquisition of food.
We can test these predicted effects of hunger on attention and
problem solving using problem-solving tasks that vary in hunger- 2.5. Memory
relevance, dot-probe tasks, fMRI studies, and eye-tracking
methods (e.g., Mogg et al., 1998; Nijs, Muris, Euser, & Franken, An intriguing possibility is that hunger might impact memory
2010; Siep et al., 2009). As with many of the mechanisms dis- mechanisms in the service of acquiring food. For example, hunger
cussed in this paper, the extent of hunger’s regulatory effects on may increase the accessibility of memories specifying locations in
attention should depend on the intensity of the hunger. which food was successfully obtained in the past. Indirect support
for this hypothesis comes from fMRI evidence that hunger prefer-
2.4. Categorization and conceptual frameworks entially enhances recognition memory for food over nonfood
(Morris & Dolan, 2001), and from evidence that people in a field
Just as fear automatically imposes conceptual frameworks that setting show preferential recall for the locations of calorie-dense
cause us to view our surroundings in terms of safety and danger food items over those of calorie-sparse ones (New, Krasnow,
(Tooby & Cosmides, 1990), hunger may automatically impose Truxaw, & Gaulin, 2007; Krasnow et al., 2011). Additional evi-
conceptual frameworks and categorization schemes relevant to the dence that hunger might impact memory comes from research
adaptive problem of finding food. For example, at the most global showing superior word sequence recall for words directly following
level, hunger may lead organisms to categorize items as either food food-related words, but only among hungry individuals (see Lerner,
or not food, with different implications for approach behavior. Singer, & Triandis, 1958). During times of nutritional scarcity,
Within the category of food, items may be further categorized as hunger may trigger especially strong encoding of information that
594 L. Al-Shawaf / Appetite 105 (2016) 591e595

can be used to resolve current or future hunger. Such a mechanism baseline activity in an orderly and predictable sequence, with more
might encode locations where food is available, useful methods of urgent and more impactful changes taking precedence. For
acquiring or preserving food, or individuals who offer food during example, the refocusing of attention on rivals or predators and the
one’s time of metabolic need. Intriguing indirect evidence for this reordering of motivational priorities may be more pressing than the
sort of hunger-mediated memory comes from studies showing that negation of hunger’s effects on memory. If this hypothesis is cor-
Drosophila flies “must be hungry to form and retrieve appetitive rect, we should expect to see broadened attentional focus and
memory” (Plaçais & Preat, 2013, p.440). renewed pathogen disgust before observing the reemergence of
We might also speculate that hunger could affect an individual’s memories of other duties and responsibilities. The broader point is
memory for group members. For example, hunger may temporarily that it is reasonable to expect the dissipation of hunger to sys-
increase the accessibility of memories indexing who typically tematically deactivate mechanisms in a predictable sequence ac-
shares food and who is stingy with it, who was recently observed to cording to the urgency or importance of the changes.
have surplus food, and who owes one an outstanding favor that At present, these suggestions are still speculative. But they
might be repaid in the currency of food. These hypotheses are highlight the fact that an evolutionary psychological approach
preliminary, but they represent an initial foray into the ways that points to a new conceptualization of hunger, novel hypotheses
hunger might regulate cognitive processes in the service of food about both hunger and eating, and new ideas about how eating
acquisition. And they are testable. may terminate the coordinating effects of hunger in a systematic
and predictable manner. Whatever its theoretical advantages, this
perspective is empirically fruitful in the sense that it provides a
2.6. Eating, the deactivation of hunger, and mechanism “offlining”
method for systematically generating a priori hypotheses about
hunger, as well as other drives and emotions.
Eating marks the solution of the adaptive problem that hunger
This approach also provides a logical way to generate hypoth-
was designed to solve. Eating should therefore be a critical stopping
eses about the “stopping points” of a drive or an emotion. When-
point, terminating the coordinating effects of hunger on other
ever a discrete event marks the solution to an adaptive problem
mechanisms (assuming that one eats enough to sate general hun-
that a motivational drive evolved to solve, that event can be ex-
ger plus any nutrient-specific hungers). This suggests the hypoth-
pected to cause the drive to dissipate. Consequently, the event in
esis that eating triggers a relatively abrupt deactivation of hunger’s
question should dissolve the coordinating effects of the drive,
coordinating effects on other mechanisms. This should manifest
resulting in a pronounced pre-event to post-event shift in a variety
itself in large pre-eating to post-eating shifts in people’s perception,
of mechanisms ranging from memory to attention to motivational
attention, categorization, memory, and motivational priorities.
priorities. We have hypothesized that in the case of sexual arousal,
For example, this hypothesis suggests that after sating them-
this “dissolving” event is orgasm (for males more strongly than
selves, people’s motivational priorities are once again reordered,
females; Al-Shawaf et al., 2015). In the case of hunger, this “dis-
attention is broadened beyond food-related stimuli, memories of
solving” event is eating. This leads to the testable hypothesis that
other duties and responsibilities resurface, categorization is no
eating until sated will cause the relatively rapid dissolution of
longer dominated by food, and previously suppressed adaptive
hunger’s effects on attention, perception, memory, and other psy-
problems such as mating and pathogen avoidance come to the fore
chological processes described above.
again. For example, the sight and smell of more food can become
disgusting after one is sated, a stark reversal in hunger’s disgust-
3. Conclusions
suppression effects. We might therefore expect that eating, a
discrete event that ushers in a suite of psychophysiological changes
An evolutionary psychological approach suggests that the
(Liu, Gao, & Fox, 2000), annuls or reverses the effects of hunger and
emotions are coordinating mechanisms whose evolved function is
marks the endpoint of the motivational state.2 This hypothesis
to orchestrate the activity of other mechanisms in the service of
suggests that we should see stark differences in people’s attention,
solving an adaptive problem (Al-Shawaf et al., 2015; Cosmides &
perception, categorization, and memory before and after eating. An
Tooby, 2000; Tooby & Cosmides, 2008). For example, disgust co-
intriguing hint of this possibility comes from a memory study:
ordinates mechanisms in the service of avoiding parasitic infection
“Most notably, in the 20 min interval between pre-satiation and
(Curtis et al., 2004; Tybur et al., 2012), fear regulates programs to
post-satiation scans, a >20% memory advantage for food stimuli
escape from danger (Bracha, 2004; Marks & Nesse, 1994; Marks,
relative to non-food was abolished in all 10 subjects” (Morris &
1987), and romantic love coordinates mechanisms in the service
Dolan, p. 5307).
of forming a long-term mating bond (Buss, 2006).
Of course, the effects of eating cannot be instantaneous. It takes
This approach might also be usefully applied to states such as
time for hunger to dissipate (e.g., Liu, Gao, Liu, & Fox, 2000; de
hunger, pain, and sexual arousal. These states are not traditionally
Graaf, Blom, Smeets, Stafleu, & Hendriks, 2004), so the return to
regarded as emotions, but may nonetheless be fruitfully regarded
baseline triggered by a meal may be speedy but not immediate. This
as coordinating mechanisms that have disparate effects on
return to baseline activity may be more pressing for certain
perception, attention, memory, conceptual categorization, and
mechanisms than for others. Stated differently, different sequences
other psychological processes.
of mechanism “offlining” may have different impacts on fitness.
Empirically, this approach provides a powerful hypothesis-
During the evolution of human hunger mechanisms, designs that
generation method. Researchers can use evolutionary task analysis
coordinated this “offlining” in a systematic and beneficial manner
to systematically generate an array of novel hypotheses for any
would have outcompeted designs that turned programs off
coordinating mechanism e including hunger e by asking (1) what
randomly or in inappropriate sequence.
adaptive problem, if any, the mechanism might have evolved to
This suggests the hypothesis that eating reverts mechanisms to
solve, (2) what subtasks constitute the adaptive problem, (3) which
psychological mechanisms are capable of solving these subtasks,
2
and (4) how these mechanisms ought to be coordinated to produce
This is admittedly an oversimplification. Eating is not a discrete event, but
rather a continuous process whose duration can vary. The “dissolving” effect of
a well-designed solution to the adaptive problem. This systematic
eating may therefore itself be continuous, and should depend on whether the or- approach can be applied to any candidate emotion or drive, pro-
ganism has eaten enough to sate itself. ducing a priori hypotheses about the effects of the mechanism on
L. Al-Shawaf / Appetite 105 (2016) 591e595 595

psychological programs, the sequence of these effects, the context- definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and
Emotion, 5(4), 345e379.
sensitivity of these effects, and even the coordinated dissolution of
Krasnow, M. M., Truxaw, D., Gaulin, S. J., New, J., Ozono, H., Uono, S., et al. (2011).
these effects. Evolutionary task analysis thus offers both heuristic Cognitive adaptations for gathering-related navigation in humans. Evolution
value and predictive power, and may be used to great effect in the and Human Behavior, 32(1), 1e12.
study of emotions and motivational drives.More broadly, whatever Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lazarus, R. S. (1998). Fifty years of the research and theory of R.S. Lazarus: An analysis
its theoretical merits, this “coordinating mechanism” approach of historical and perennial issues. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
raises new questions, suggests new domains of inquiry, and ap- Associates.
pears to be empirically fruitful (Al-Shawaf et al., 2015; Cosmides & Lerner, R. G., Singer, I., & Triandis, H. C. (1958). Serial verbal learning under two
conditions of hunger motivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(6),
Tooby, 2000; Tooby & Cosmides, 2008). The next step will be to test 572e574.
some of the hypotheses in this paper and see how they stand up to Lippa, R. A. (2009). Sex differences in sex drive, sociosexuality, and height across 53
empirical investigation. nations: Testing evolutionary and social structural theories. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 38, 631e651.
Liu, Y., Gao, J., Liu, H., & Fox, P. (2000). The temporal response of the brain after
Acknowledgements eating revealed by functional MRI. Nature, 405, 1058e1062.
Marks, I. M. (1987). Fears, phobias, and rituals: Panic, anxiety, and their disorders. New
York: Oxford University Press.
The author wishes to thank Peter M. Todd and an anonymous Marks, I. M., & Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary analysis of
reviewer for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this pa- anxiety disorders. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(5), 247e261.
per, as well as Dan Conroy-Beam for our early discussions about Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation
and processing of visual information. San Francisco: Freeman.
adaptive metaproblems.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Hyare, H., & Lee, S. (1998). Selective attention to food-related
stimuli in hunger: Are attentional biases specific to emotional and psycho-
References pathological states, or are they also found in normal drive states? Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 36(2), 227e237.
Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Vries, P. (2001). On the psychology of drinking: Being Morris, J. S., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Involvement of human amygdala and orbitofrontal
thirsty and perceptually ready. British Journal of Psychology, 92(4), 631e642. cortex in hunger-enhanced memory for food stimuli. The Journal of Neurosci-
Ainsworth, S. E., & Maner, J. K. (2014). Hunger moderates the activation of psy- ence, 21(14), 5304e5310.
chological disease avoidance mechanisms. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, New, J., Krasnow, M. M., Truxaw, D., & Gaulin, S. J. (2007). Spatial adaptations for
8(4), 303e313. plant foraging: Women excel and calories count. Proceedings of the Royal Society
Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Human emotions: An of London B: Biological Sciences, 274(1626), 2679e2684.
evolutionary psychological perspective. Emotion Review, 1e14. http://dx.doi.org/ Nijs, I. M., Muris, P., Euser, A. S., & Franken, I. H. (2010). Differences in attention to
10.1177/1754073914565518. food and food intake between overweight/obese and normal-weight females
Al-Shawaf, L., & Lewis, D. M. G. (2013). Exposed intestines and contaminated cooks: under conditions of hunger and satiety. Appetite, 54(2), 243e254.
Sex, stress, & satiation predict disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Oatley, K., Keltner, D., & Jenkins, J. M. (2006). Understanding emotions (2nd ed.).
Differences, 54, 698e702. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Palouzier-Paulignan, B., Lacroix, M. C., Aime , P., Baly, C., Caillol, M., Congar, P., et al.
Al-Shawaf, L., Lewis, D. M., & Buss, D. M. (2014). Disgust and mating strategy.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3), 199e205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ (2012). Olfaction under metabolic influences. Chemical Senses, 37(9), 769e797.
j.evolhumbehav.2014.11.003. Plaçais, P. Y., & Preat, T. (2013). To favor survival under food shortage, the brain
Bracha, H. S. (2004). Freeze, flight, fight, fright, faint: Adaptationist perspectives on disables costly memory. Science, 339(6118), 440e442.
the acute stress response spectrum. CNS Spectrums, 9(9), 679e685. Prescott, T. J., Bryson, J. J., & Seth, A. K. (2007). Introduction. Modelling natural
Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as organizing factors in action selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B:
perception. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 42(1), 33e44. Biological Sciences, 362(1485), 1521e1529.
Radel, R., & Cle ment-Guillotin, C. (2012). Evidence of motivational influences in
Buss, D. M. (2003). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York:
Basic Books. early visual perception: Hunger modulates conscious access. Psychological Sci-
Buss, D. M. (2006). The evolution of love. In R. J. Sternberg, & K. Weis (Eds.), The new ence, 23(3), 232e234.
psychology of love (pp. 65e86). Yale: University Press. Redgrave, P., Prescott, T. J., & Gurney, K. (1999). The basal ganglia: A vertebrate
Cabanac, M. (1971). Physiological role of pleasure. Science, 173(4002), 1103e1107. solution to the selection problem? Neuroscience, 89(4), 1009e1023.
Changizi, M. A., & Hall, W. G. (2001). Thirst modulates a perception. Perception, Root, C. M., Ko, K. I., Jafari, A., & Wang, J. W. (2011). Presynaptic facilitation by
30(12), 1489e1497. neuropeptide signaling mediates odor-driven food search. Cell, 145(1), 133e144.
Channon, S., & Hayward, A. (1990). The effect of short-term fasting on processing of Rozin, P., & Todd, P. M. (2015). The evolutionary psychology of food intake and
food cues in normal subjects. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9(4), choice. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd ed., pp.
447e452. 183e205). Wiley.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In Rozin, P., & Vollmecke, T. A. (1986). Food likes and dislikes. Annual Review of
M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. Nutrition, 6(1), 433e456.
91e115). New York: Guilford. Sanchez, P. A., & Swaminathan, M. S. (2005). Cutting world hunger in half. Science,
Crabbe, F. L. (2007). Compromise strategies for action selection. Philosophical 307(5708), 357e359.
Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362(1485), Serageldin, I. (2002). World poverty and hunger: The challenge for science. Science,
1559e1571. 296(5565), 54e58.
Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect Siep, N., Roefs, A., Roebroeck, A., Havermans, R., Bonte, M. L., & Jansen, A. (2009).
from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, 131e133. Hunger is the best spice: An fMRI study of the effects of attention, hunger and
Duclaux, R., Feisthauer, J., & Cabanac, M. (1973). Effects of a meal on the pleas- calorie content on food reward processing in the amygdala and orbitofrontal
antness of food and nonfood odors in man. Physiology & Behavior, 10(6), cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 198(1), 149e158.
1029e1033. Speth, J. D., & Spielmann, K. A. (1983). Energy source, protein metabolism, and
Fadool, D. A., Tucker, K., & Pedarzani, P. (2011). Mitral cells of the olfactory bulb hunter-gatherer subsistence strategies. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology,
perform metabolic sensing and are disrupted by obesity at the level of the Kv1. 2(1), 1e31.
Stockburger, J., Schma €lzle, R., Flaisch, T., Bublatzky, F., & Schupp, H. (2009). The
3 ion channel. PLoS One, 6(9), e24921.
Gat, A. (2000). The human motivational complex: Evolutionary theory and the impact of hunger on food cue processing: An event-related brain potential
causes of hunter-gatherer fighting. Part I. Primary somatic and reproductive study. Neuroimage, 47(4), 1819e1829.
causes. Anthropological Quarterly, 20e34. Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York: Oxford University
Gilchrist, J. C., & Nesberg, L. S. (1952). Need and perceptual change in need-related Press.
objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44(6), 369e376. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adapta-
de Graaf, C., Blom, W. A., Smeets, P. A., Stafleu, A., & Hendriks, H. F. (2004). Bio- tions and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociohiology, 11,
markers of satiation and satiety. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 79(6), 375e424.
946e961. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and
Gurven, M., Hill, K., Kaplan, H., Hurtado, A., & Lyles, R. (2000). Food transfers among their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-
Hiwi foragers of Venezuela: Tests of reciprocity. Human Ecology, 28(2), 171e218. Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 114e137). New
Hoefling, A., Likowski, K. U., Deutsch, R., H€ afner, M., Seibt, B., Mühlberger, A., et al. York: Guilford.
(2009). When hunger finds no fault with moldy corn: Food deprivation reduces Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2012). Disgust: Evolved
food-related disgust. Emotion, 9(1), 50e58. function and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 65e84.
Kleinginna, P. R., Jr., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of emotion

You might also like