You are on page 1of 7

Commentary

Social Ecology of Children’s Vulnerability to Environmental Pollutants


Bernard Weiss1 and David C. Bellinger 2
1Department of Environmental Medicine, Environmental Health Sciences Center, and Center for Reproductive Epidemiology, University
of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, New York, USA; 2Children’s Hospital Boston, Harvard Medical School and
Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts, USA

the converse, which would represent a type II


BACKGROUND: The outcomes of exposure to neurotoxic chemicals early in life depend on the proper- error. Such a strategy presumes that the influ-
ties of both the chemical and the host’s environment. When our questions focus on the toxicant, the ence of the selected social ecologic factors on
environmental properties tend to be regarded as marginal and designated as covariates or confounders. the measured outcome is completely indepen-
Such approaches blur the reality of how the early environment establishes enduring biologic substrates. dent of the effects of the neurotoxicant. It
OBJECTIVES: In this commentary, we describe another perspective, based on decades of biopsycho- further assumes that the potency of the
logical research on animals, that shows how the early, even prenatal, environment creates perma- neurotoxicant is invariant regardless of the
nent changes in brain structure and chemistry and behavior. Aspects of the early social ecology within which it occurs. Such a
environment—encompassing enrichment, deprivation, and maternal and neonatal stress—all help
determine the functional responses later in life that derive from the biologic substrate imparted by
bold assumption is appealing in its simplicity
that environment. Their effects then become biologically embedded. Human data, particularly but almost certainly wrong. It steers us, in
those connected to economically disadvantaged populations, yield equivalent conclusions. fact, to a scientific impasse. All “main effects”
DISCUSSION: In this commentary, we argue that treating such environmental conditions as con-
are misnamed because such entities are not
founders is equivalent to defining genetic differences as confounders, a tactic that laboratory readily identifiable. Each is the result of the
research, such as that based on transgenic manipulations, clearly rejects. The implications extend factor of interest operating on the end point
from laboratory experiments that, implicitly, assume that the early environment can be standard- of interest in a particular ecologic context.
ized to risk assessments based on epidemiologic investigations. Pearce (1989) summarized the problem by
CONCLUSIONS: The biologic properties implanted by the early social environment should be observing that “every effect estimate . . .
regarded as crucial elements of the translation from laboratory research to human health and, in reflects numerous unknown or unmeasured
fact, should be incorporated into human health research. The methods for doing so are not clearly modifying factors.” A “main effect,” so to
defined and present many challenges to investigators. speak, merely describes an interaction that is
KEY WORDS: covariates, effect modification, environment deprivation, environmental enrichment, incompletely characterized.
risk assessment, stress. Environ Health Perspect 114:1479–1485 (2006). doi:10.1289/ehp.9101 Although the current approaches may be
available via http://dx.doi.org/ [Online 10 May 2006] convenient for segregating the role of a partic-
ular agent, they are less useful as a policy
guide. Neurotoxicant exposures are not dis-
In this commentary, we argue that the child development. Recognizing this princi- tributed randomly. They are chained to a
outcomes of exposure to neurotoxic chemicals ple, investigators attempting to determine multitude of other risk factors that resist parti-
early in life are shaped by the nature of a whether and how a particular agent induces tioning. The focus of arguments about the
child’s social environment, including that pre- developmental neurotoxicity strive to separate relationship of developmental lead exposure to
vailing before birth. Our guiding thesis is that its unique contribution from those of the indices such as IQ, for example, is the ability
toxicity is not simply an inherent property of larger, particularly social environment. Such of such studies to separate the contribution of
the toxicant but derives from an assortment of investigations tend to strip away those contri- lead itself from those of maternal IQ, family
jointly acting variables bound implacably into butions (covariates, confounders) statistically income and social class, marital status, prenatal
the individual. Neurotoxicology accepts through techniques such as multiple regres- care, maternal drug use, family caregiving,
genetic predispositions as intrinsic influences, sion. The major goal of such analyses is to and other aspects of the child’s total environ-
but aspects of the broader environment, espe- avoid misattributing to the neurotoxicant of ment. It is a puzzle reminiscent of the one
cially its social characteristics, tend to be either interest an adverse effect that is actually due posed by chemical mixtures, but, for even this
regarded, at best, as a marginal influence or to one of these other factors. In such an simpler puzzle, solutions to questions of addi-
dismissed as merely a nuisance, contributing a analysis, a model of the outcome of interest is tivity, antagonism, and synergism continue to
source of confounding bias. We contend that a first constructed in which consideration is elude us.
true evaluation of toxic potential and its neu- given only to the contributions of factors
robehavioral consequences is inseparable from other than the neurotoxicant. In essence, the
Address correspondence to B. Weiss, Department of
the ecologic setting in which they act and analyst tries to “explain” as much of the varia- Environmental Medicine, University of Rochester
which creates unique, enduring individual vul- tion in the outcome as possible by reference Medical Center, Room G-6820, 575 Elmwood Ave.,
nerabilities that warrant the same status as only to these other factors. This effort never Rochester, NY 14642 USA. Telephone: (585) 275-
genetic predispositions and are imprinted as achieves complete success, leaving some of the 1736. Fax: (585) 256-2591. E-mail: bernard_weiss@
forcefully. Although aspects of this perspective variation “unexplained.” urmc.rochester.edu
We thank F. Perera for encouragement in preparing
are well accepted among neurotoxicologists, it The second step is to determine whether
this commentary.
has not yet found wide application in study the neurotoxicant of interest accounts for a Preparation of this commentary was supported in
design and analysis. Therefore, we also suggest significant portion of this residual variance. part by grants 1R01ES013247 from the National
factors, which are primarily methodologic, that Although such a strategy will fulfill its princi- Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIEHS)
appear to be important impediments. pal aim of reducing the risk of type I error, it to B.W., Center grant P30 01247 from the NIEHS
carries the attendant risks of other types of to the University of Rochester, and 1T32MH073122
The Customary Approach to inferential errors. The strategy is rather con- from the National Institute of Mental Health to D.B.
Human Studies and by the John Merck Fund.
servative: It values avoidance of mistaking The authors declare they have no competing
Environmental chemicals comprise only one what is a true social ecologic effect inter- financial interests.
of many exogenous factors that can influence woven with a neurotoxicant more highly than Received 15 February 2006; accepted 10 May 2006.

Environmental Health Perspectives • VOLUME 114 | NUMBER 10 | October 2006 1479


Weiss and Bellinger

From another vantage point, these presum- animal experimenters often assume that typi- substantial magnitude by 18 months of age
ably external factors represent not simply con- cal laboratory conditions, because they seem (see Sameroff 1993).
founders, but what in epidemiology are called free of the many complexities that plague Postnatal stress. Another pertinent litera-
effect modifiers. That is, “[e]ffect modification human research, offer an environment in ture focuses on environmental effects on
. . . is present when the magnitude of the asso- which the “pure” effects of a developmental endocrine function, particularly the stress hor-
ciation between an exposure and an outcome neurotoxicant can be observed. The methods mones (glucocorticoids), the hypothalamic–
varies across strata of some other factor” sections of such reports typically describe the pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, and adult
(Bellinger 2000). For example, the magnitude housing and rearing conditions without com- responses to stress. Until the experimental data
of the adverse effect of lead exposure on a ment—for example, by noting that the ani- emerged, responses to stress were deemed to be
measure such as the Mental Development mals were maintained in “standard” cages or “innate” and not modifiable by experience. In
Index of the Bayley Scales of Infant Develop- fed “standard” rodent diets. For another area rodent species, critical elements of brain devel-
ment depends partly on the child’s social class of developmental neuroscience, such a lack of opment take place during the first 2 weeks or
(usually defined by parents’ occupational and detail might be viewed with astonishment. so of life: neuronal proliferation, dendritic
educational levels). Unlike children from This area is devoted to research on the neuro- branching, synaptogenesis, and the wiring of
“lower-class” backgrounds, an adverse effect behavioral consequences of environmental neural circuitry. During this period, environ-
could not be demonstrated in children from enrichment and deprivation. In the following mental conditions play a major role in such
“upper-class” backgrounds unless cord blood section, we briefly summarize some key studies processes. The most influential is the nature of
levels at birth exceeded 10 µg/dL. Winneke on the effects of environmental enrichment, maternal care because the infant rodent, an
and Kraemer (1984) reported a similar finding. postnatal stress, and prenatal stress. This litera- altricial species, is totally dependent on the
Analogous relationships were traced in assess- ture, as observed by Laughlin (2000), provides dam for survival. When maternal care is modi-
ments of how polychlorinated biphenyls valuable lessons for neurotoxicology. fied by experimental manipulations, the effects
(PCBs) affect neurobehavioral development, Environmental enrichment. The 1960s on infant brain development and behavior
again demonstrating that nature is much more provided the earliest experimental evidence occur across a broad range of measures and are
complex than our usual assumptions. In a that modifying the environments of young detectable during adulthood.
study conducted in the Netherlands, prenatal rats, especially by adding complexity or Stress in this context is not unambigu-
exposure had adverse effects only among chil- enrichment, could alter the neurochemical ously defined because different degrees of
dren with “less optimal” parental and home and morphologic characteristics of their what are considered stressful manipulations
characteristics (Vreugdenhil et al. 2002). In a brains as well as their behavior (e.g., Bennett may be qualitatively dissimilar in effect. Mild
New York City (USA) cohort, Rauh et al. et al. 1964; Diamond et al. 1972, 1974). The stress during the first week of life, imposed by
(2004) observed a significant interaction enriched environment consisted of a large maneuvers such as brief periods, perhaps
between prenatal residential exposure to envi- cage with 10–12 animals and novel stimulus 3–10 min in length, of handling the infant
ronmental tobacco smoke (ETS) and socio- objects such as ladders, wheels, and blocks. (e.g., stroking it) after withdrawal from the
economic disadvantage. The greatest cognitive The standard environment consisted of a con- nest, is able to diminish the behavioral and
deficits on the Bayley Scales emerged among ventional laboratory cage containing three hormonal responses to stress (of a different
children who both were exposed to ETS and rats. The rats raised in the enriched environ- kind) during adulthood. Adrenal activity in
had families that suffered material hardships, ment showed increased cortical thickness, response to stress conditions such as confine-
operationally defined as “unmet basic needs in increased sizes of neuronal cell bodies and ment is reduced, and more activity occurs in
the areas of food, housing, and clothing” synaptic contact areas, increased numbers and the open field situation. Such stress also
(Rauh et al. 2004). extent and branching of dendrites, and more results in improved performance in a radial
An important implication of these exam- synapses per neuron than did the rats raised maze, improved shuttle box avoidance acqui-
ples for risk assessment is that the “main in the standard environment (Diamond sition, improved water maze performance,
effects” of lead or PCBs or ETS, estimated by 2001; Rosenzweig and Bennett 1996). Later, reduced evidence of anxiety on an elevated
multiple regression, are misleading characteri- it was shown that the effects of enriched and plus-maze, and diminished hippocampal neu-
zations of their respective associations with impoverished experience could be induced at ronal loss and cognitive decline with age.
neurodevelopment. In effect, such measures almost any part of the life span, even with Many experimenters have noted that handled
represent weighted averages of the associa- relatively short periods of exposure. The pups elicit more licking and grooming from
tions, summing across strata in which the investigators found, for example, that they the dams and that the dams show a higher
association with neurodevelopment is stronger could obtain similar effects by assigning incidence of the arched-back nursing posi-
and strata in which it is weaker. Exposure 50-day-old rats to enrichment for 30 days. tion. Increased maternal attention seems to be
standards that rest on such estimates will be Also, 2 hr/day in the differential environments one of the sources of the positive outcomes.
more protective for the subgroup of children for 30–54 days produced cerebral effects simi- In contrast, more severe types of stress dur-
whose social ecology has rendered them rela- lar to those induced by 24-hr exposure. More ing infancy produce distinctly different out-
tively insensitive to the adverse effects of recent research has confirmed these findings comes than does mild stress. For example,
neurotoxicant exposure, but less, perhaps even (Foster and Dumas 2001; Rampon et al. Matthews and Robbins (2003) removed litters
insufficiently, protective for the subgroup of 2000a, 2000b) and extended them to end of rat pups from their nest cages for 6 hr on 10
children whose social ecology has rendered points such as brain growth factors (Ickes et al. occasions between postnatal day (PND)5 and
them more sensitive. 2000), neurotransmitter function (Naka et al. PND20. Later in life, the pups who experi-
2005), motor coordination, spatial discrimina- enced maternal separation showed markedly
The Early Environment tions, and other behavioral indices (Pham reduced behavioral responses to both primary
as a Variable et al. 1999; Schrijver et al. 2002). and conditioned reward stimuli, elevated loco-
Much like the epidemiologic studies referred Substantial supporting evidence exists for motor activity in anticipation of presentation of
to above that attempt to hold constant, by the same phenomenon in humans. For exam- a daily food ration, a Pavlovian conditioned
means of statistical control, variables other ple, studies of children raised in impoverished response, and an attenuated response to the
than the specific exposure under investigation, environments revealed cognitive deficits of activity enhancement effects of d-amphetamine.

1480 VOLUME 114 | NUMBER 10 | October 2006 • Environmental Health Perspectives


Social ecology and children’s development

Low-dose cocaine self-administration was atten- and emotional development of the child. Later the environments of the lead-exposed rats,
uated whereas high-dose self-administration studies have shown that stressful circumstances but brain chemistry was altered as well. Lead-
was enhanced. The authors interpreted these and events such as marital strife, loss of a associated deficits in n-methyl D -aspartate
findings as evidence of reduced central dopa- spouse, and especially poverty are also associ- receptor subunit 1 and brain-derived neuro-
minergic function. Others have also observed ated with developmental delays or attention trophic factor gene expression in the hippo-
altered neurotransmitter function in other sys- deficits in childhood and lifelong adverse campus were markedly attenuated in the
tems—for example, diminished GABA A health and neurobehavioral effects. Evans enriched rats compared with the isolated rats.
(gamma-aminobutyric acid) receptor binding (2004) argued that even these circumstances These two studies only begin to suggest the
during adulthood (e.g., Fries et al. 2004). Most fall far short of encompassing the multiple dis- power of the early postweaning environment
critically, these widespread neurobehavioral advantages faced by children living in poverty. to modify the consequences of exposure to
alterations endure and are not attenuated with He observed that “[a] limitation of psychologi- developmental neurotoxicants. A much wider
age. Fish et al. (2004) observed that variations cal research on poverty is the absence of an array of functional end points, beyond the
in maternal care in rats induce variations in ecological perspective” that extends beyond the crudeness of the water maze, deserves investiga-
gene expression in offspring brains that, in family setting. tion. Methods that can trace and quantify the
essence, result in differing phenotypes (Weaver Toxic interactions (lead, ethanol) with course of complex learning, such as schedule-
et al. 2004; Zhang et al. 2004). Cameron et al. early environments. As environmental health controlled operant behavior, offer perhaps the
(2005), reviewing the outcomes of poverty researchers, our primary interest in this litera- most promise, especially because they can pro-
during early development, write that such ture arises from its neurotoxicologic implica- vide homologous situations in animals and
effects on gene expression “represent, in part, tions. To what degree do early environmental humans (e.g., Chelonis et al. 2004; Paule et al.
the process by which variations in socio- conditions determine the consequences of 1999).
economic status (SES) are ‘biologically embed- neurotoxic exposures? Are their effects suffi- Wallace et al. (2003) examined this issue
ded.’” Seen from this perspective, the social ciently profound that ignoring them distorts from a different standpoint. By exposing rats
ecology prevailing during early development, our conclusions? prenatally to a known developmental neuro-
like genetic endowment, creates a unique Lead is the environmental neurotoxicant toxicant, methylazoxymethanol acetate
biologic heritage. whose entanglement with the social environ- (MAM), they blunted the increase in visual
Prenatal stress. Postnatal stress is directed at ment, as measured by SES, is clearest (Bellinger cortical thickness produced by being raised in
the neonates. Another segment of the literature and Matthews 1998). Two studies have now an enriched environment. This effect on
has explored relationships between stressful shown that environmental enrichment can experience-dependent neural plasticity
manipulations during pregnancy and their attenuate its developmental neurotoxicity. occurred at much lower exposure levels than
effects on the offspring. Operationally, stress Schneider et al. (2001) exposed rats, beginning did the overtly toxic effects of MAM seen
manipulations have taken the form of daily on PND25, to ordinary tap water or water under standard conditions, suggesting that
restraint periods (e.g., confinement in a plastic containing 0.2% lead acetate. The rats had impairments in neuronal plasticity may be a
tube), cold water immersion, inescapable elec- been assigned to two different environmental more sensitive index of neurotoxicant expo-
tric shock to the tail, saline injections, and conditions: single-cage housing (isolation) or sure than are the toxicant’s direct effects. One
unpredictable noise. The literature provides group housing (eight per group) containing a implication is that positive interventions in
compelling evidence that stress during gestation variety of stimulus objects. Lead-exposed ani- disadvantaged communities, such as expanded
exerts widespread effects on neurobehavioral mals raised in isolation tended to show educational resources, may be counteracted by
development that extend far beyond the period impaired water maze performance compared levels of pollution much smaller than would
of infancy. Neurochemical effects include with the other rats in which enrichment gener- be detected by conventional assessments.
altered monoamine turnover (e.g., reduced ally overcame the lead and isolation effects. Three recent reports (Cory-Slechta et al.
dopamine turnover in striatum and nucleus The blood lead values differed between the 2004; Virgolini et al. 2005, 2006) illustrate
accumbens) and increased dopamine turnover groups—26 µg/dL for the enriched group and how prenatal stress can modify the response to
in prefrontal cortex (Newport et al. 2002). 34 µg/dL for the isolated group—a surprising lead exposure. They were based on the knowl-
Morphologic changes include reduced cell pro- outcome, but differences in drinking water edge that low SES is itself a risk factor for
liferation in dentate gyrus and decreased intake were not measured and could have been adverse health consequences, including neuro-
neurogenesis in hippocampus evoked by a spa- determined by the housing situation. behavioral function, that in many ways parallel
tial learning task. Behavioral changes include Guilarte et al. (2003) adopted the approach, lead neurotoxicity. Proceeding from the thesis
altered locomotor activity patterns and more relevant to humans, of treating female that many of the effects of low SES arise from
enhanced sensitization to the locomotor effects rats from before breeding until weaning. Lead stress responses attributable to the HPA axis
of amphetamine. And, as with postnatal stress, acetate (1,500 ppm) was added to their diets and glucocorticoids, the investigators exposed
prenatal stress enhances the response to stress- during this period. At weaning, male litter- female rats to lead via drinking water 2 months
ful conditions in the offspring. For example, mates were assigned either to isolation housing before scheduled breeding. Exposure continued
such offspring tend to spend less time in the (one rat per cage) or to an enriched condition through lactation until weaning at PND21.
open arms of the elevated-plus maze and (eight rats per cage containing a variety of Maternal stress procedures, carried out on ges-
exhibit retarded learning of both active and stimulus objects). Water maze performance tational days 16 and 17, consisted of placing
passive avoidance. Sexual differentiation of the during adulthood served as a measure of spatial the dams in restraint tubes three times on each
brain and copulatory behavior are also altered learning. Isolation combined with develop- of those days. Measurements in offspring of
by prenatal stress; male offspring may be femi- mental lead exposure produced the poorest schedule-controlled operant behavior perfor-
nized (Gerardin et al. 2005). performance. Enrichment improved perfor- mance, neurotransmitter function, and corti-
Early studies in humans of the effects of mance in lead-exposed rats to levels seen in the costerone levels revealed a series of complex
maternal stress focused on outcomes such as control and enriched-control groups. In addi- interactive effects of stress and lead and marked
pregnancy complications, delivery complica- tion, this study suggested some possible bio- sex differences. The authors’ conclusions pro-
tions, and birth weight. Subsequently, psy- logic bases for the behavioral differences. Not vide a template for the next stage of environ-
chologists initiated research on the cognitive only was performance enhanced by enriching mental health research: “Greater attention to

Environmental Health Perspectives • VOLUME 114 | NUMBER 10 | October 2006 1481


Weiss and Bellinger

the problems arising from interactions of risk Skepticism about conventional approaches beforehand about their disparities would not
factors must certainly be considered, given that is warranted in another way. The terms “effect choose to combine them into a single popula-
such a scenario, in contrast to individual chemi- modification,” “covariate,” and “confounder” tion to study the effects of an environmental
cal exposures, constitutes the environmental imply external sources. This is not an accurate agent to which both are exposed. Most studies,
reality” (Cory-Slechta 2006). depiction. The reality is that there are no exter- however, do not seek to classify specific sub-
nal sources. What often are labeled as effect groups or to strive for homogeneity. Figure 2
Implications for Toxicologic modifiers or covariates are bound inextricably, depicts why heterogeneous samples can be mis-
Studies in the individual, with the measures they are leading. Figure 2A illustrates a situation in
The animal data highlight the principle that presumed to influence from the outside. which the sample population includes a sub-
even relatively modest differences in the qual- Many, perhaps most, effect modifiers actually population (30%) inherently sensitive to the
ity and properties of the early environment define different populations. exposure of interest. In the absence of such a
exert a powerful influence on later neuro- Consider the situation depicted in Figure 1 challenge, the sample population appears
behavioral function. Their effects range from (Weiss 2000), which compares the effects of homogeneous. In response to the challenge,
vulnerability to stress in later life to cognitive shifts in mean IQ in two different communi- the mean of the sensitive subpopulation is
performance, to emotional predispositions, to ties, one that would be ranked as advantaged shifted by 1 SD (Figure 2B). The entire sam-
enduring changes in neurotransmitter func- and one as disadvantaged. “Advantaged” and ple, however, yields a distribution like that of
tion and brain morphology. Even as super- “disadvantaged” are terms that embody differ- Figure 2C, which exhibits, at most, only a
ficially simple a variable as cage size can exert ent suites of risks and different ecologic set- slight shift in the mean. That slight, super-
significant effects. Wurbel (2001) argues that tings. Poverty in its many dimensions accounts ficially negligible shift would be misleading
the usual housing environment for laboratory for a large proportion of such risks. because it ignores the reality of a major change,
rodents is so at variance with their natural In many surveys, the differences in mean equivalent to 1.0 SD, in the sensitive subpopu-
habitats that we typically study subjects, even IQ scores of such populations approximate 15 lation. Figure 2D provides one example of how
before we undertake experimental procedures, points (e.g., Sameroff et al. 1987) or about a random sample of 15 subjects chosen from
whose neurobehavioral capacities have been 1 SD on intelligence tests such as the Wechsler the total population might reflect exposure to a
distorted. Given the accelerated investments in Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. toxic challenge if the sensitive subjects
mouse genetics and behavior, experimenters Assume, for modeling purposes, initial IQ responded with a shift of 1.0 SD.
need to be aware that the early environment, distributions with respective means of 100 Figure 3 underscores the difficulties posed
such as housing condition, can eliminate or and 85, both with SDs of 15. As an impact by such a confounding of subpopulations. It
even reverse behavioral differences ascribed to index, calculate the number of scores < 70. shows how large a sample would have to be
genetics (e.g., Crabbe 1999). Conventional education standards tend to enrolled to be able to detect displacements of
These implications have been overlooked in assume that a score < 70 indicates the need a specified value if the subpopulation had not
toxicology, not just in areas bearing on nervous- for remedial measures and, for some school been identified in advance. In terms of
system function but also in areas superficially systems, signifies a classification as “retarded.” Figure 1, if the disadvantaged population had
remote from them. One example was provided With population sizes of 100,000 each, as been enrolled in a larger study in which it
by Strange et al. (2000) in their review of work shown in Figure 1, a loss of 1 IQ point (1%) comprised 30% of the total sample, its
on tumor growth and housing conditions. In in the advantaged population will increase the response to a developmental toxicant would
their studies, they divided mice into two groups number of individuals < 70 from 2,280 to have been smothered in the analysis.
at weaning (PND21). One was housed individ- 2,660. In the disadvantaged population, the Encompassing ecology. Although the likeli-
ually; the other was assigned to group housing. loss assigns 17,530 rather than 15,870 individ- hood that the social ecology of neurotoxicant
At 2–4 months of age, the mice were injected uals to the < 70 category. Although the propor- exposure affects its expression is not an entirely
with mammary tumor cells and divided into tional shift is greater in the advantaged novel concept, several factors appear to be
additional groups. These manipulations pro- population (16.7%) than in the disadvantaged
25,000
duced marked differences in tumor growth population (10.5%), the number of individuals Mean of 100
among the various groups. The conventional added to the developmentally disabled category Mean of 85
No. < IQ 70 per 100,000

20,000
cancer bioassay procedure takes no account of is much larger in the disadvantaged population
housing conditions, and standard cancer studies (1,660) than in the advantaged population 15,000
begin to expose animals at 8 weeks of age. (380). The discrepancies enlarge with greater
Effect modifiers as risk factors. How should IQ losses, which could result from higher 10,000
our practices in risk assessment and toxicology neurotoxicant exposures. This represents
incorporate what we have learned about the another aspect of the argument, also made by 5,000

profound ways in which the ecologic setting Bellinger (2000), that it is fruitless to search
0
prevailing early in life exerts effects on later for a single point estimate for a “true” relation- 0 1 2 3
neurobehavioral function? Perhaps we should ship between exposure and an end point such IQ point loss
discard the tenet, guiding much contemporary as IQ because the distributions of factors that
Figure 1. Advantaged communities typically show
research, that we need to simplify the world to affect the relationship will almost certainly dif- about a 15-point or 15% higher mean IQ score com-
produce an unequivocal result. This view, with fer in many respects between two populations. pared with disadvantaged communities. If both
its emphasis on independent main effects, does Insofar as each individual’s ecology is, at some populations, as a result of neurotoxic exposure,
not acknowledge that most adverse effects level of detail, unique, the point estimate can suffer an equivalent decrease in mean IQ, the
reflect the joint contributions of multiple be expected to differ for each individual. The effect will be greater in the disadvantaged commu-
sources. Even SES, as noted above, is far from group point estimate therefore represents the nity, as gauged by the number of IQ scores < 70,
although both populations suffer. Despite recogni-
a unitary dimension; the label itself is an central tendency of the distribution of the esti- tion that the IQ distribution is a continuous vari-
umbrella covering a multitude of variables mates of group members. able, a score < 70 is often taken as evidence of
(Bellinger 2000, 2001) that endure as biologic The model in Figure 1 defined two differ- retardation and, in many school districts, requires
substrates (Fish et al. 2004). ent populations. Most investigators who knew remedial education (Weiss 2000).

1482 VOLUME 114 | NUMBER 10 | October 2006 • Environmental Health Perspectives


Social ecology and children’s development

impediments to fully addressing it in studies. analytic methods is much more common in influence on child neurodevelopment of
Addressing these obstacles can be viewed as a the social than in toxicologic sciences. Fourth, neighborhood or community characteristics is
research agenda. First, investigating effect our thesis that the “imprinting” of social-eco- assumed to be mediated entirely by such fac-
modification requires that the factors most logic factors has its roots even in the prenatal tors. But these more immediate factors are
likely to modify neurotoxicity within a specific environment implies that it is necessary to entrenched in a wider social and cultural set-
setting are known, incorporated into the study implement the costly and time-consuming lon- ting integrated with the more traditional dis-
design, and measured well. Second, it is neces- gitudinal study designs that permit the system- ease models that focus on biologic response as
sary that the joint distributions of the exposure atic, prospective collection of high-quality data a function solely of host characteristics (Diez
of interest and the potential modifiers provide from that period. Roux 2004). The concepts and methods
an adequate opportunity to assess a potential Perhaps most important, to address the needed to assess such factors have been devel-
interaction. In general, assessments of effect dependence of neurotoxicant effects on the oped to a much greater extent by investigators
modification require larger study cohorts than social-ecologic environment in which exposure in social epidemiology and in the broader
assessments of main effects. Because of the large occurs will require strategies for measuring eco- social sciences. Collaborations with such inves-
number of potential effect modifiers, and the logic variables that are more robust and specific tigators will become increasingly important.
constraints that will be imposed by incorporat- than those currently applied. (Of course, one Recent work in social epidemiology illus-
ing them into the design, the choice of which can argue that this would be necessary even if trates how the traditional approaches are
factors to focus on will necessarily be limited the primary goal is to improve assessment of unlikely to leave us with adequate models. For
and will inevitably require trade-offs. Third, the confounding bias attributable to aspects of the example, Rauh et al. (2003) found that even
multilevel modeling methods that are required social ecology.) The social-environmental fac- when individual-level risk factors for low read-
to integrate individual-level, neighborhood- tors measured in neurotoxicity studies, whether ing scores were controlled in the analysis (e.g.,
level, and community-level variables have not subsequently modeled as potential confounders males, low birth weight, low maternal educa-
yet been widely applied in neurotoxicologic or effect modifiers, have generally consisted of tion), community factors such as poverty indi-
research. Expertise in the application of such individual- or family-level characteristics. Any cators and percentage of immigrants remained
significant predictors. Moreover, a cross-level
0.40
A B effect emerged: A high percentage of immi-
0.40 grants in the community conferred a greater
μNR
0.30 positive benefit on boys’ reading scores than on
μp
girls’ reading scores. This social-ecologic per-
0.30 0.20 spective must be incorporated in neurotoxicity
studies if we are to implement “truly integra-
Proportion
Proportion

0.10 tive, multilevel research strategies that consider


–σ +σ
0.20 the pathways to health operating at and
0 between the social, cultural, individual, and
0.20 biological levels” (Bachrach and Abeles 2004).
μR
0.10 The application of such strategies generates
0.10 complex, multilevel data sets, but only in this
way will our models begin to approach the
–σ +σ
0 0 true complexity of associations as they exist in
4 8 12 16 20 4 8 12 16 20 nature.
Score Score
0.40 3.0
C D Nonresponders 10 2102 ~550 265
Responders

1.9
μp 20 ~550 ~150 65
Percentage of responders

0.30

0.8
Response

Response

30 265 65 30
0.20

–0.3
40 ~150 ~37 16

0.10
–1.4
50 90 23 12

0 –2.5 100 23 7 4
4 8 12 16 20 Control Challenge
Score
1.0 2.0 3.0
Figure 2. Influence of a susceptible subpopulation in a sample exposed to a toxic challenge. Abbreviations: d
Response amplitude ( σ )
µ, mean; NR, nonresponders; p, whole group; R, responders. (A) A hypothetical distribution generated to
contain a sample composed of 30% responders (lower curve) and 70% nonresponders. (B) Distribution after Figure 3. Sample size required to demonstrate a
exposure to a presumed neurotoxic agents that shifts the scores of the responders, on average, by 1 SD. (C) statistically significant effect (p = 0.01) 90% of the
Because responders would not be identified beforehand in the usual study, the total distribution after chal- time for different magnitudes of response (effect
lenge would display, as shown here, a negligible shift in the mean. Such a small shift would generally be size) and different proportions contributed by sen-
taken as evidence of no effect, a conclusion that fails to assimilate the original hypothetical distribution. (D) sitive subpopulations. The ability to detect a small
A randomly chosen sample of 15 individuals from the total population after challenge was generated to overall change when only a small proportion of the
show how, in such a sample, there is a marked overlap with control conditions despite the presence of a total population consists of responders requires
substantial proportion of responders. extremely large sample sizes.

Environmental Health Perspectives • VOLUME 114 | NUMBER 10 | October 2006 1483


Weiss and Bellinger

Such issues and questions are not unique to Translations into Human Health REFERENCES
neurobehavioral epidemiology. Environmental The National Institute of Environmental
ecologists confront questions that in many ways Health Sciences (NIEHS) 2006 strategic plan Bachrach CA, Abeles RP. 2004. Social science and health
research: growth at the National Institutes of Health. Am J
are analogous. For example, in assessing metal (NIEHS 2006) was formulated to promote Public Health 94:22–28.
toxicity, they have to take account of how dif- “models for research that integrate patient- Bellinger DC. 2000. Effect modification in epidemiologic studies
ferent environmental conditions across the oriented or public health research with basic of low-level neurotoxicant exposures and health outcomes.
Neurotoxicol Teratol 22:133–140.
United States influence metal biogeochemistry mechanistic studies to address disease etiology, Bellinger DC. 2001. Future directions for neurobehavioral stud-
rather than setting one universal standard. They pathogenesis, susceptibility and progression.” ies of environmental neurotoxicants. Neurotoxicology
may, for example, define a set of exposure sce- Such an objective will prove elusive if basic 22:645–656.
Bellinger DC, Matthews JA. 1998. Social and economic dimen-
narios and conduct comprehensive analyses for and mechanistic as well as human research sions of environmental policy: lead poisoning as a case
each. For a specific location, their assessments fails to take into account the complexity of the study. Perspect Biol Med 41:307–326.
would include the chemical form of the metal human setting. Primarily, it must acknowl- Bennett EL, Krech D, Rosenzweig MR. 1964. Reliability and
regional specificity of cerebral effects of environmental
as it enters the environment, the environmental edge that variations in the early environment complexity and training. J Comp Physiol Psychol 57:440–441.
conditions affecting its form and distribution create biologically distinctive organisms. Caldwell B, Bradley R. 1984. Home Observation for Measure-
(e.g., soil geochemistry, rainfall), and the effects Consider how toxicology has exploited the ment of the Environment. Little Rock, AR:University of
Arkansas at Little Rock.
that particular forms will have on target organ- tools of contemporary molecular biology to Cameron NM, Champagne FA, Parent C, Fish EW, Ozaki-
isms. The parallels with health risk assessment create novel organisms such as mice with Kuroda K, Meaney MJ. 2005. The programming of individ-
are intriguing because each of these variables unique genetic properties. These genetically ual differences in defensive responses and reproductive
can be considered effect modifiers. modified mice are tested for how they differ strategies in the rat through variations in maternal care.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:843–865.
One perspective on the influence of the from ordinary mice in their response to toxic Chelonis JJ, Flake RA, Baldwin RL, Blake DJ, Paule MG. 2004.
communal environment is plotted in chemicals. Scientists carry out such a strategy Developmental aspects of timing behavior in children.
Figure 4. Herrnstein and Murray argued in because they know that toxicity is not a prop- Neurotoxicol Teratol 26:461–476.
Cory-Slechta D. 2006. Interactions of lead exposure and stress:
The Bell Curve (1994) that IQ, in essence, erty of the chemical alone but, like the infec- implications for cognitive dysfunction. In: Neurotoxicity and
determines one’s status in society. They calcu- tivity of a bacterium, a property shared jointly Developmental Disabilities (Davidson PW, Myers GJ,
lated the benefits to society of a 3% rise in IQ with the host. We deceive ourselves in the Weiss B, eds). San Diego:Elsevier Academic Press, 87–139.
Cory-Slechta DA, Virgolini MB, Thiruchelvam M, Weston DD,
in their population. If the effects are symmet- laboratory by assuming that testing animals Bauter MR. 2004. Maternal stress modulates the effects of
rical, as we have assumed in Figure 4, then a raised under conventional conditions yields a developmental lead exposure. Environ Health Perspect
3% lowering of IQ should produce an equiv- “pure” evaluation of toxic potential free of the 112:717–730.
Crabbe JC, Wahlsten D, Dudek BC. 1999. Genetics of mouse
alent loss to society. But, we maintain, isn’t complexities that obscure human research. behavior: interactions with laboratory environment.
the argument inverted? Don’t the outcomes Like investigators trying by statistical surgery to Science 284:1670–1672.
depicted in Figure 4 represent, instead, a con- isolate the effects of toxic chemicals on human Diamond MC. 2001. Response of the brain to enrichment. An
Acad Bras Cienc 73:211–220.
stellation of social-ecologic variables con- development from its environmental setting, Diamond MC, Lindner B, Johnson R, Bennett EL, Rosenzweig
tributing to or even determining the IQ loss? we cannot ignore the history of the host. The MR. 1975. Differences in occipital cortical synapses from
We certainly understood in 1994 the potency early environment, including that prevailing environmentally enriched, impoverished, and standard
of the social environment in shaping child before birth, is not simply an influential vari- colony rats. J Neurosci Res 1:109–119.
Diamond MC, Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL, Lindner B, Lyon L.
development. The Bell Curve argument is an able but a biologic determinant. It creates 1972. Effects of environmental enrichment and impoverish-
unintended verification on an ecologic scale. qualitative rather than quantitative variation. ment on rat cerebral cortex. J Neurobiol 3:47–64.
The core of our argument, then, is that Diez Roux AV. 2004. The study of group-level factors in epi-
demiology: rethinking variables, study designs, and
Poverty during the enduring biologic consequences of the analytical approaches. Epidemiol Rev 26:104–111.
first 3 years individual’s developmental ecology are insep- Evans GW. 2004. The environment of childhood poverty. Am
Bottom decile arable from how and to what extent he or she Psychol 59:77–92.
HOME scores Fish EW, Shahrokh D, Bagot R, Caldji C, Bredy T, Szyf M, et al.
will be affected by neurotoxicant exposure. If 2004. Epigenetic programming of stress responses through
Out of wedlock
births genes are not treated as confounders but as variations in maternal care. Ann NY Acad Sci 1036:167–180.

Low-weight
biologic givens, neither should the vulnerabil- Foster TC, Dumas TC. 2001. Mechanism for increased hippo-
campal synaptic strength following differential experience.
births ities or protective mechanisms conferred by J Neurophysiol 85:1377–1383.
Welfare social ecology. Sociomics, so to speak, might, Fries E, Moragues N, Caldji C, Hellhammer DH, Meaney MJ.
recipiency
in fact, be an apt description of such a tenet. 2004. Preliminary evidence of altered sensitivity to benzo-
diazepines as a function of maternal care in the rat. Ann
Children without
parents
The pioneering research of Cory-Slechta NY Acad Sci 1032:320–323.
(2006) and others is forcing us to confront the Gerardin DC, Pereira OC, Kempinas WG, Florio JC, Moreira EG,
High school
dropouts reality that the association between a defined Bernardi MM. 2005. Sexual behavior, neuroendocrine, and
chemical exposure index and a defined neu- neurochemical aspects in male rats exposed prenatally to
Males interviewed stress. Physiol Behav 84:97–104.
in jail rodevelopmental outcome depends on a vari- Guilarte TR, Toscano CD, McGlothan JL, Weaver SA. 2003.
Poverty rate ety of host characteristics imprinted, so to Environmental enrichment reverses cognitive and molecu-
speak, by the properties of the early environ- lar deficits induced by developmental lead exposure. Ann
Neurol 53:50–56.
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 ment. Current practices consolidate these Herrnstein RJ, Murray C. 1994. The Bell Curve. New York:Free
Percent increase properties into gross indices such as SES and, Press.
Figure 4. Estimated consequences of a 3% loss of as well, tend to ignore the broader community Ickes BR, Pham TM, Sanders LA, Albeck DS, Mohammed AH,
Granholm AC. 2000. Long-term environmental enrichment
IQ, based on calculations in Herrnstein and Murray setting. We know enough now to recognize leads to regional increases in neurotrophin levels in rat
(1994), showing percent changes in the selected that what we have labeled as covariates, con- brain. Exp Neurol 164:45–52
indices. HOME, Home Observation for Measurement founders, and effect modifiers possess a bio- Laughlin NK. 2000. Environmental factors: modulators of devel-
of the Environment (Caldwell and Bradley 1984).The opment. Papers from a symposium sponsored by the
logic reality that we have yet to resolve.
calculations are based on the National Longitudinal Neurobehavioral Teratology Society. Neurotoxicol Teratol
Survey of Youth, a data set provided by the U.S. Solutions to these dilemmas do not unequivo- 22:87–88.
Bureau of Labor Statistics (Herrnstein and Murray cally present themselves, but we hope to stir a Matthews K, Robbins TW. 2003. Early experience as a determi-
nant of adult behavioural responses to reward: the effects
1994). colloquy among our colleagues.

1484 VOLUME 114 | NUMBER 10 | October 2006 • Environmental Health Perspectives


Social ecology and children’s development

of repeated maternal separation in the rat. Neurosci mice. Nat Neurosci 3:238–244. offspring subjected to combined maternal lead exposure
Biobehav Rev 27:45–55. Rauh VA, Parker FL, Garfinkel RS, Perry J, Andrews HF. 2003. and/or stress. Neurotoxicology 27:11–21.
Naka F, Narita N, Okado N, Narita M. 2005. Modification of Biological, social, and community influences on third- Virgolini MB, Chen K, Weston DD, Bauter MR, Cory-Slechta
AMPA receptor properties following environmental grade reading levels of minority head start children: a DA. 2005. Interactions of chronic lead exposure and inter-
enrichment. Brain Dev 27:275–278. multilevel approach. J Community Psychol 31:255–278. mittent stress: consequences for brain catecholamine
Newport DJ, Stowe ZN, Nemeroff CB. 2002. Parental depres- Rauh VA, Whyatt RM, Garfinkel R, Andrews H, Hoepner L, systems and associated behaviors and HPA axis function.
sion: animal models of an adverse life event. Am J Reyes A, et al. 2004. Developmental effects of exposure to Toxicol Sci 87:469–482.
Psychiatry 159:1265–1283. environmental tobacco smoke and material hardship Vreugdenhil HJ, Slijper FM, Mulder PG, Weisglas-Kuperus N.
NIEHS. 2006. New Frontiers in Environmental Sciences and among inner-city children. Neurotoxicol Teratol 26:373–385 2002. Effects of perinatal exposure to PCBs and dioxins on
Human Health: The 2006–2011 NIEHS Strategic Plan. Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. 1996. Psychobiology of plasticity: play behavior in Dutch children at school age. Environ
Researach Triangle Park, NC:National Institute of effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Health Perspect 110:A593–A598.
Environmental Health Sciences. Available: http://www. Behav Brain Res 78:57–65. Wallace CS, Reitzenstein J, Withers GS. 2003. Diminished
niehs.nih.gov/external/plan2006/ [accessed 25 April 2006]. Sameroff AJ, Seifer R, Baldwin A, Baldwin C. 1993. Stability of experience-dependent neuroanatomical plasticity: evi-
Paule MG, Chelonis JJ, Buffalo EA, Blake DJ, Casey PH. 1999. intelligence from preschool to adolescence: the influence dence for an improved biomarker of subtle neurotoxic
Operant test battery performance in children: correlation of social and family risk factors. Child Dev 64:80–97. damage to the developing rat brain. Environ Health
with IQ. Neurotoxicol Teratol 21:223–230. Sameroff AJ, Seifer R, Barocas R, Zax M, Greenspan S. 1987. Perspect 111:1294–1298.
Pearce N. 1989. Analytical implications of epidemiological con- Intelligence quotient scores of 4-year-old children: social- Weaver IC, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D’Alessio AC, Sharma S,
cepts of interaction. Int J Epidemiol 18:976–980. environmental risk factors. Pediatrics 79:343–350. Seckl JR, et al. 2004. Epigenetic programming by maternal
Pham TM, Ickes B, Albeck D, Soderstrom S, Granholm AC, Schneider JS, Lee MH, Anderson DW, Zuck L, Lidsky TI. 2001. behavior. Nat Neurosci 7:847–854.
Mohammed AH. 1999. Changes in brain nerve growth fac- Enriched environment during development is protective Weiss B. 2000. Vulnerability of children and the developing
tor levels and nerve growth factor receptors in rats against lead-induced neurotoxicity. Brain Res 896:48–55. brain to neurotoxic hazards. Environ Health Perspect
exposed to environmental enrichment for one year. Schrijver NC, Bahr NI, Weiss IC, Wurbel H. 2002. Dissociable 108(suppl 3):375–381.
Neuroscience 94:279–286. effects of isolation rearing and environmental enrichment Winneke G, Kraemer U. 1984. Neuropsychological effects of lead
Rampon C, Jiang CH, Dong H, Tang YP, Lockhart DJ, Schultz on exploration, spatial learning and HPA activity in adult in children: interactions with social background variables.
PG, et al. 2000a. Effects of environmental enrichment on rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 73:209–224. Neuropsychobiology 11:195–202.
gene expression in the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA Strange KS, Kerr LR, Andrews HN, Emerman JT, Weinberg J. Wurbel H. 2001. Ideal homes? Housing effects on rodent brain
97:12880–12884. 2000. Psychosocial stressors and mammary tumor growth: and behaviour. Trends Neurosci 24:207–211.
Rampon C, Tang YP, Goodhouse J, Shimizu E, Kyin M, Tsien JZ. an animal model. Neurotoxicol Teratol 22:89–102. Zhang TY, Parent C, Weaver I, Meaney MJ. 2004. Maternal pro-
2000b. Enrichment induces structural changes and recovery Virgolini MB, Bauter MR, Weston DD, Cory-Slechta DA. 2006. gramming of individual differences in defensive responses
from nonspatial memory deficits in CA1 NMDAR1-knockout Permanent alterations in stress responsivity in female in the rat. Ann NY Acad Sci 1032:85–103.

Environmental Health Perspectives • VOLUME 114 | NUMBER 10 | October 2006 1485

You might also like