You are on page 1of 9

Comparison of Clinical Symptoms in Autism

and Asperger’s Disorder


RICHARD EISENMAJER, MARGOT PRIOR, PH.D., SUSAN LEEKAM, PH.D., LORNA WING, M.D.,
JUDITH GOULD, PH.D., MICHAEL WELHAM, M.D., AND BEN ONG, PH.D.

ABSTRACT
Objective: To determine what clinical symptoms clinicians have been using to distinguish between Asperger’s disorder
(AsD) and autistic disorder (AD). Method: Parents of children and adolescents with high-functioning AD (n = 48) and
AsD ( n = 69) were given a structured interview based on DSM-///-Rand ICD-10 diagnostic criteria. Information regarding
early and current symptom presentation and family, developmental, and verbal mental age information were collected.
Logistic regression analyses were conducted to determine which variables best predicted clinician’s diagnosis. Results:
A number of clinical variables predicted diagnosis. Delayed language onset was the only variable of the family and
developmental variables that predicted diagnosis. The AsD group was also significantly higher than the AD group in
verbal mental age. Conclusion: Clinicians appear to be diagnosing AsD and AD on the basis of published research
and case study accounts. The findings question whether DSM-/V and ICD-10 criteria adequately describe the AsD
individual, particularly in the communication domain. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psycbiafry, 1996, 35(11):1523-1531.
Key Words: autism, Asperger’s disorder, diagnosis, communication.

The nosological integrity of autistic disorders listed Labels such as atypical autism, residual autism, PDD-
under the DSM umbrella term, “pervasive develop- not otherwise specified, and, more recently, Asperger’s
mental disorders” (PDD), has long been a contentious syndrome/disorder have all at one time been used to
area (Rutter, 1989; Schopler, 1985; Wing, 1986). describe a group of individuals who do not appear to
strictly fulfill autism criteria. The recent description
of Asperger’s disorder (AsD) in DSM-IV (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994) with criteria that are
Accepted April 1 6 1996.
nearly identical with those for autistic disorder (AD)
Mr. Eisenmajer is a psychologist and Ph.D. candidate and Dr. Ong is
Lecturer, School of Psychology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, would suggest that there is not currently any clear
Australia. Dr. Prior is Profissor, Department of Psychology, Royal Children i boundary between the two disorders. Since Wing’s
Hospital, Melbourne, Australia. Dr. Leekam is Lecturer, Department ofPsychol- (198 1) account of Asperger’s syndrome, clinicians have
ogy, University of Kent at Canterbuy, U.K Dr. Wing is a psychiatrist and
Dr. Gould is a clinicalpsychologist, The Centrefor Social and Communication been using this diagnostic label for certain individuals.
Disorders, Kent, U.K. Dr. Welham is a psychiatrist, Department of Child and The aim of this study was to determine what symptom
Adolescent Psychiany, Austin Hospital, Melbourne, Australia. characteristics clinicians have been using to distinguish
Presented in part by thefirst author to the 1995 NationalAutism Conference,
Brisbane, Australia. between AD and AsD.
This research wm supported in part by a grant to Dr. Leekam fiom the Asperger’s syndrome has recently been included in
University of Kent (1991-1993) and a travel grant fiom the Royal Society. the DSM-Was AsD under the section of PDD. The
Richard Eisenmajer was supported by the Apex Trustfor Autism (1993). The
authors thank Emma Hunnisen ( U K ) f o r help with coding, Sonia Jordan DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for AD and AsD were
(U.K ) with data colkrtion, Amanda Golding (Australia), the Autism and modeled from ICD-10 draft criteria (World Health
Asperger Syndrome Associations in Victoria and South Australia, and all the Organization, 1988) after field trials suggested that
parents and children who gave their time f i r this project. The authors also
thank the anonymous reviewersfor their helpfil comments on an earlier version ICD- 10 criteria had certain advantages over DSM-III
of this paper. and DSM-III-R criteria for autism (Volkmar et al.,
Reprint requests to Mr. Eisenmajer, School of Psychology, La Trobe University, 1994). The criteria for AsD in DSM-Ware the same
Bundoora, Victoria, Australia 3083.
0890-8567/96/3511- 1523$03.00/001996 by the American Academy as those for AD, with three exceptions. First, the
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. communication and imagination impairment criteria

J . AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 3 5 : 1 1 , NOVEMBER 1996 1523


E I S E N M A J E R E T AL.

for AD are not listed for AsD. Second, it is claimed that It is of interest to ascertain how clinicians have put
individuals with AsD do not suffer from a “clinically into clinical practice the research on AsD. The aim of
significant general delay in language (e.g., single words this study is to identify the clinical features used by
by 2 years, communicative phrases by age 3 years)” clinicians to distinguish between a diagnosis of AD
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 77). Third, and AsD (prior to the publication of DSM-IV). W e
the child with AsD does not have a “clinically signifi- wish to clarify whether diagnosis was dependent on
cant delay in cognitive development or in the develop- the presence/absence of specific autism symptomatol-
ment of age-appropriate self-help skills, adaptive ogy, on developmental factors such as presence/absence
behavior (other than in social interaction), and curiosity of language delay, or on level of intelligence. Once
about the environment in childhood” (p. 77). These these factors are identified, it is possible to compare
criteria are in accordance with those ofAsperger (1944), these features with those described by Asperger (1944)
who believed that the main handicap was of a social and other researchers (e.g., Szatmari et al., 1989b;
nature and not due to intellectual or language delays. Wing, 1981). A further question is to what extent
Unlike Kanner, Asperger did not list essential criteria any identified distinguishing features corroborate those
for his patients. Among the many behaviors he referred proposed by DSM-K
to, he did note a tendency for pedantic speech patterns Two methods could be used to investigate clinicians’
and uncoordinated, “clumsy” gross motor movements. practice in the use of AD and AsD diagnoses. One
There is no mention of these features in the DSM- approach would be to interview a cross-section of
IV criteria, although the Course description of AsD clinicians in child and adolescent psychiatry and ask
recognizes that “motor delays or motor clumsiness may which criteria they use to make a distinction between
the two groups. A recent Australian survey of child
be noted in the pre-school years” (American Psychiatric
and adolescent health professionals found that no clear
Association, 1994, p. 76),and the Associated Features
consensus was available on which differential criteria
section states that “motor milestones may be delayed,
were used to distinguish AsD from AD (Brooks and
and motor clumsiness is often observed” (p. 76).
McGillivray, personal communication). Another ap-
Prior to the publication of DSM-II/: prominent
proach would be to assess individuals from each clini-
researchers in autism used Asperger’s descriptions to
cally diagnosed group using a structured interview to
identify such individuals in order to further investigate
find possible distinguishing clinical features between
and elaborate on areas such as cognitive skills and early
the two groups. By this process it may be possible to
developmental history (Gillberg, 1989; Szatmari et al.,
detect characteristics that clinicians do not readily
1989a; Tantam, 1988; Wing, 1981). Social impair- report through an interview process but that may be
ments, clumsiness, pedantic or unusual speech, and manifest in the diagnoses they give. This second method
circumscribed interests were noted as essential criteria. is used in this study.
Szatmari et al. (1989b) differed from other research
groups by not stating in their criteria whether cognitive
METHOD
and speech delays were admissible. Other researchers
believed that such delays could be present and still Sample
allow a diagnosis of AsD (e.g., Gillberg, 1989; Wing, Subjects were recruited from autism and Asperger’s syndrome
1981). Ghaziuddin et al. (1992) warned that different support groups in Melbourne and Adelaide, referrals to the child
conceptions of AsD between investigators could make and adolescent outpatient clinic of a major Melbourne metropolitan
hospital in Australia, and the Centre for Communication Disorders,
comparisons between studies difficult. Studies compar- Kent, England. Twenty-two clinicians including child psychiatrists,
ing AsD and AD groups on a range of factors (e.g., pediatricians, and psychologists were responsible for diagnosing
developmental indices, IQ) have been compromised the cases. Only children described as “high hnctioning” by a
clinician were recruited. All children had language skills, and overall
by methodological limitations, such as unclear sampling there was a nonsignificant difference between chronological age
procedures to exclude AsD subjects from an AD group (CA) (10.65 years; range 2.7 to 21.3 years) and verbal mental age
and failing to match groups on IQ, thereby allowing (10 years; range 2.5 to 33.7 years). This study makes no claim to
represent the more prevalent moderate-to-low-functioning children
mental retardation as a potential explanation for ob- with autistic behaviors, for whom the diagnosis of AsD is rarely
served differences (Szatmari, 1992). an issue.

1524 J . A M . A C A D . C H I L D A D O L E S C . P S Y C H I A T R Y , 3 5 : 1 1 , N O V E M B E R 1996
C O M P A R I S O N OF A U T I S M A N D ASPERGER’S D I S O R D E R

Forty-eight subjects with AD (mean CA 10.5 years, SD 4.5; 39 child. When available, clinical diagnostic reports describing the
male, 9 female) and 69 subjects with AsD (mean CA 10.7 years, child’s earlier behaviors were used to confirm parents’ reports. A
SD 3.6; 61 male, 8 female) were recruited, making a total of 117 recent study by Yirmiya et al. (1994) found that parents of autistic
subjects. The groups did not differ in mean CA or ratio of males children, similar in age to those in the present study, were highly
to females. reliable reporters of early clinical information.
Developmental and historical information, including the follow-
Measures ing, was also recorded: ante-, peri-, and postnatal complications;
physical health in the first 2 years of life; motor milestones; early
Families were seen either in the various research clinics or at
language information; the child’s age when parents first recognized
home. T o obtain information concerning ICD- 10 (World Health
the existence of a problem: age at diagnosis: presence of other
Organization, 1993) and DSM-Iff-R(American Psychiatric Associa-
tion, 1987) diagnostic criteria for PDD, a checklist of items to be diagnosed medical conditions (apart from AD and AsD); family
used in an interview with parents was devised. This gave a range history of AD or AsD-like conditions; sibling psychopathology:
of specific examples of the types of behavior that were expressed and use of medication.
in more general terms in the two international classification systems. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (Dunn and Dunn,
1981) and the British Picture Vocabulary Scale (Dunn et at., 1982)
The version used in this study was based on an earlier checklist
devised by Wing (in Rapin, 1996) plus some items from the were used as measures of verbal mental age (raw scores converted
to standard score) in Australia and Britain, respectively.
Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders
(known as DISCO). This is an extensive interview schedule, not
yet published, that is being developed by Wing and Gould (personal Analysis
communication). The checklist contains 89 questions encompassing Logistic regression analyses were performed to determine the
the social, communication, and behavioral domains. These items clinical variables that distinguish between AD- and AsD-diagnosed
cover DSM-ffZ-R, ICD-10 research criteria, Kanner’s syndrome, groups. Stepwise logistic regressions were performed to select the
Asperger‘s syndrome, and Wing and Gould’s triad of social impair- best predictors within each domain of clinical symptoms: social,
ments. For the purpose of this article, only DSM-Iff-Rand ICD- communication/imagination, and stereotyped/repetitive behaviors
10 criteria for AD were used in the analyses, as it was considered variables. Variables selected in these three subsets were then used
that these were the two systems that would have been most likely in a standard logistic regression to evaluate their utility in predicting
to be used by clinicians in the period preceding recruitment (up membership of AsD and AD groups. The same method of variable
to 1994). All interviewers were experienced in working with the selection was used for “early” data as with the “current” data.
checklist and with families and children with autism-related Separate analyses were performed for DSM-Iff-R and ICD-10
conditions. criteria (Table 1).
Each child received two scores per item. Parents were asked T o determine whether any developmental history variables were
whether the itendbehavior had occurred earlier in the child’s life more predictive of AD or A D , variables were dichotomized (into
(an appropriate age would be given, usually between infancy and presence/absence of symptoms) and a stepwise logistic regression
6 years), denoting an “early” response, and whether the behavior analysis was performed. Multivariate tests of significance (Ho-
was still present, denoting a “current” response. A positive response telling’s 72) were used to compare groups on CA, verbal mental
to an item was recorded only if it was indicative of the child’s age standard scores (VMASS), and age at diagnosis.
usual behavior. Absence of the behavior and occasional or “one-
off” instances of behavior would score a negative response.
The suitability of an “early” item would depend on the behavior.
For example, the item “Concern for personal modesty” is usually RESULTS
observed in normal children 6 years of age or older. Parents would
be asked to recall an example of their child’s either being modest Diagnostic Variables
(e.g., worrying about visitors entering the house while he or she
was undressed) or not (e.g., leaving the school toilet without pulling The results show that the AsD-diagnosed subjects
up his or her pants). Other items were of interest between the were more likely to engage in more prosocial behaviors
ages of 1 and 3 years. For example, the item “Reciprocation in than the AD-diagnosed group. Important predictors
simple games” concerned whether the very young child would
engage with a parent in simple “peek-a-boo” type games. Although of diagnosis across time were desire for friendship and
some children in the sample would develop more interest in these an ability to engage with another person who had the
types of games after they reached 7 or 8 years, the absence of the same circumscribed interest. The AsD children were
behavior when it was developmentally appropriate was significant
and was scored accordingly (i.e., positive). Other items were devel- also more likely to have less severe eye contact avoidance
opmentally appropriate only for older children. For example, an when young and more willingness and ability at playing
“early” response for the item “Embarrassing remarks in public” with others as they got older.
would apply only to speaking children older than 9 years of age.
In the communication/imagination domain, the
No score would be given for items that could not be performed
because the subject was either too young or old. AsD-diagnosed subjects were less likely to use echolalic
A negative score was recorded when a parent could not recall speech when young compared with the AD subjects.
an example of a behavior. Although earlier childhood behaviors Their speech content was more likely to be repetitive
were more difficult for parents to remember than current behaviors,
most parents could recall vivid examples of their child’s behavior without appropriate turn-taking and contain idiosyn-
and how it compared with that of another sibling or a friend’s cratic words or phrases. Their tone of voice would also

J . AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 35:11, N O V E M B E R 1996 1525


EISENMAJER ET AL.

TABLE 1
Early and Current DSM-III-R and ICD-10 Variables That Best Predicted Asperger’s Disorder (AD)
Subjects as Diagnosed by Clinicians
Diagnostic Systems
Criterion DSM-III-R DSM-III-R ICD-10 ICD- 10
Domains a Clinical Variables Current Early Current Early

A Some avoidant eye contact (not severe) Yes


A Desire for friendship (though has poor grasp Yes Yes Yes Yes
of concept of friendship)
A More willing/able to join others in play Yes -b -b
activities
A Engages with one other who has same circum- Yes Yes -b -b
scribed interest
B One-sided, repetitive conversations Yes
B No evidence of immediate or delayed echolalia Yes Yes
B Idiosyncratic use of words or phrases or signs Yes
B Long-winded, pedantic speech Yes Yes -b -b
B Tone of voice Yes Yes
C Asks repetitive questionsltalks on repetitive Yes Yes Yes Yes
themes
C Collecting facts on specific subjects Yes Yes
% Correct prediction of diagnosis of AsD 74.4 77.8 69.2 78.6
(x2[61= 37, (xz[7] = 58.9, (x’[3] = 20.3, (x2[61= 46.8,
p < .01) p < .01) p < .01) p < .01)
Goodness-of-fit statistic Z z = 105.0 Z 2 = 105.8 Z 2 = 116.4 Z z = 113.8

aCriterion domains: (A) corresponds to social, (B) communication, and (C) stereotyped/repetitive behaviors.
’Variables that are not mentioned in the diagnostic system.

be unusual (e.g., flat, monotonous). The AsD children a stepwise logistic regression analysis using all develop-
were also more likely to engage in long-winded ped- mental indices, no one or set of variables could predict
antic speech patterns from a young age to current diagnosis. Most of the sample had some disturbance
presentation. or deviance in communication, with only 7.5% of the
In the stereotyped/repetitive behaviors domain, AsD total sample (2.4% of AD and 10.8% of AsD, not
children were likely to have always asked repetitive significant) reported by parents as communicating nor-
questions, talked repetitively on one theme, and col- mally. Significantly more AsD children had attention-
lected facts on specific subjects. deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as a comorbid
(To ensure that variables with more than 5% missing condition ( ~ ’ [ l =] 5.2, p < .02), and children with
data did not influence the results, analyses were rerun AD were more likely to have experienced a delay in
without five “current” and seven “early” variables. walking (x2[1] = 5.6, p < .02) (Table 2).
There was no change in selected variables.) Table 2 shows that the groups were not different
on relative incidence of other diagnosed problems in
Developmental Variables siblings; use of medication; pregnancy, birth, and in-
All subjects, regardless of “clinical” diagnosis, ful- fancy complications; presence of AsD-like conditions
filled criteria for DSM-ZZZ-R and ICD-10 AD. ICD- in the father and family; and sitting and crawling
10 (and now DSM-ZV) stipulates that AsD children do milestones.
not experience a language delay or have communication
CA, VMASS, and Age at Diagnosis
difficulties as listed for AD. In this study, significantly
more of the AD group (73.8%) compared with the The multivariate Hotelling’s T2 conducted between
AsD group (43.1%) were reported to have experienced AD and AsD groups on CA and VMASS, and age
a delay in language onset ( ~ ’ [ l=] 9.74, p < .01). In at diagnosis variables, was statistically significant

1526 J . A M . A C A D . C H I L D A D O L E S C . P S Y C H I A T R Y , 3 5 : 1 1 , N O V E M B E R 1996
C O M P A R I S O N OF A U T I S M A N D ASPERGER’S D I S O R D E R

(F[3,96] = 12.5, p < .01). Follow-up univariate F fail to list any of these criteria for AsD children, apart
tests showed that the AsD group had a significantly from insisting that there is a normal onset of language.
higher VMASS than the AD group (F[1,98] = 13.9, The results suggest that clinicians may support this
p < .01). The AsD group were diagnosed significantly notion, since a significantly greater proportion of chil-
later than the AD group (F[1,98] = 15.6, p < .01). dren with the AsD diagnosis were described as having
a normal onset of language compared with the AD
group. Of note, however, was the finding that almost
DISCUSSION half (43%) of the AsD group reportedly had a delayed
This study does not make any claims about the onset of language. From these results it is clear that
diagnostic validity of AsD and the differences from clinicians have not been using a delay in onset of
AD. Rather, it is a study about the extent to which language as an exclusion criterion for AsD in many
clinicians can usefully apply existing criteria to higher- cases. This criterion was included in ICD-10 in accord-
functioning PDD children. The results suggest that ance with Asperger’s (1944) original view that in AsD
few clinical differences exist between high-functioning children there is no delay in language. However, con-
AD and AsD groups as categorized by clinicians. Al- temporary accounts of AsD agree that language delays
though all of the subjects fulfilled criteria for DSM- are possible (Gillberg, 1989; Tantam, 1988; Wing,
ZZZ-R and ICD-10 (and by default DSM-ZV) versions 198 1). Recent research findings have found age-depen-
of AD, the results suggest that a few key characteristics dent results using early language delay as a differentiat-
may influence clinicians’ use of the label AsD rather ing variable between AsD and AD. Szatmari et al.
than AD. AsD individuals were more likely to look (1995) compared language-delayed with normal-lan-
for social interaction, even from an early age. AsD guage-onset 4- to 6-year-old children with PDD and
children sought friendships more than the AD children, found that delayed children showed more autistic symp-
though both groups equally experienced difficulties in tomatology, fewer adaptive skills, and lower receptive
attaining and keeping friends. Parents in both groups language abilities. A similar study by Eisenmajer (1996)
were unable to report any friendship that shared the replicated the finding that language delay predicted
depth, quality, and range of experiences of normal autistic symptomatology for young PDD children;
children. It is possible that the notion of autistic however, this was not the case when the children
children desiring friendships is incompatible with the approached preadolescence (average age 11 years). He
more commonly reported descriptions of them as “liv- concluded that early language delay is not a suitable
ing in their own world” and aloof; hence clinicians differentiating variable for PDD subtypes.
prefer to use AsD rather than AD. In a similar vein, Other results question the decision to use language
children with the AsD diagnosis were more likely to play onset as an exclusion criterion and not to document
with a person with whom they shared a circumscribed communication impairment criteria in ICD- 10 and
interest (e.g., chess, computer games, train-spotting) DSM-ZVfor AsD. The diagnosis of AsD is defined
than were children with the AD diagnosis. The ability only in terms of the social impairments and repetitive
to interact meaningfully or play with another person routines and stereotyped behaviors as for AD. However,
is not commonly identified in autism. In clinicians’ this study found that all of the subjects experienced
judgment, a diagnosis of AsD may allow for such some type of communication problem and would meet
interactions, limited as they may be. The DSM-ZV criteria for AD. This is problematic, as DSM-ZVstipu-
Course description for AD alludes to an “increased lates that AsD is not to be used if the criteria for AD
interest in social functioning as the child reaches school are met. If this were the case, none of these subjects
age” (p. 69). However, the results from this study would be diagnosed with AsD. The results also show
would suggest that interest in social functioning is that normal language onset does not necessarily pre-
more characteristic of the AsD individual. No such clude later communication disturbances, as were evi-
description is offered for AsD children in DSM-ZK denced in more than half of the AsD group. The
It is the communication variables that are of most parents of both groups reported some sort of deviancy
interest, especially since both ICD- 10 and DSM-IV in their children’s communication, such as absence of

J . AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 3 5 : 1 1 , N O V E M B E R 1996 1527


EISENMAJER ET AL.

TABLE 2
Developmental History Variables for AD and AsD Groups
AD AsD
Variable ( n = 48) ( n = 69) P
Mean verbal mental age (yr) (standard 78.2 95.2 < .01
score) SD = 25.77 SD = 19.78
Mean age (yr) when parent(s) first recog- 1.79 1.63 NS
nized existence of a problem with SD = 1.05 SD = 1.2
child n = 37' n = 57
History of language onset Normal = 26.2% Normal = 56.9% < .01
Delayed = 73.8% Delayed = 43.1%
History of language deviance Normal = 2.4% Normal = 10.8% NS
Deviant = 97.6% Deviant = 89.2%
Mean age at diagnosis (yr) 6.02 8.88 < .01
SD = 3.28 SD = 4.08
Other diagnosed problems No problems = 82.5% No problems = 67.2% NS
Yes problems = 17.5% Yes problems= 32.8%
n = 40' n = 64
Diagnosed ADHD ADHD yes = 2.5% ADHD yes = 17.2% < .02
NO ADHD = 97.5% NO ADHD = 82.8%
List of other diagnosed problems ADHD = 1 ADHD = 11
(in addition to autism) Tourette's syndrome = 1 Tourette's syndrome = 2
Cerebral palsy = 1 Cerebral palsy = 1
Schizophrenia = 0 Schizophrenia = 1
Semantic-pragmatic language Semantic-pragmatic language
disorder = 1 disorder = 0
Epilepsy = 1 Epilepsy = 5
Dyspraxia = 0 Dyspraxia = 1
Other = 2 Other = 0
Sibling diagnosed problems No problems = 82.1% No problems = 79.4% NS
Yes problems = 17.9% Yes problems = 20.6%
n = 39' n = 63

List of sibling diagnosed AD = 3 AD = 0


problems AsD = 0 h D = 2
Learning problems = 2 Learning problems = 3
Genetic disorder = 0 Genetic disorder = 1
Mental illness = 0 Mental illness = 2
Language disorder = 1 Language disorder = 1
ADHD = 0 ADHD = 4
OCD = 1 OCD = 0
Use of medication No medication = 82.1% No medication = 72.1% NS
Yes medication = 17.9% Yes medication = 26.9%
n = 39' n = 63
List of medication Dextroamphetamine = 0 Dextroamphetamine = 3
Methylphenidate = 0 Methylphenidate = 7
Sodium valproate = 2 Sodium valproate = 1
Albuterol = 4 Albuterol = 1
Other = 1 Other = 5
Difficulties during pregnancy No problems = 56.8% No problems = 66.1% NS
Yes problems = 43.2% Yes problems = 33.9%

-Continued

1528 J. AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 35:11, N O V E M B E R 1996


C O M P A R I S O N OF A U T I S M A N D ASPERGER’S D I S O R D E R

TABLE 2
Continued
~

AD AsD
Variable ( n = 48) (n = 69) P
Dificulties during birth No problems = 29.7% No problems = 42.4% NS
Yes problems = 70.3% Yes problems = 57.6%
n = 37’ n = 59
List of difficulties during birth Induced labor = 1 Induced labor = 5
Forceps delivery = 8 Forceps delivery = 18
Cesarean = 9 Cesarean = 4
Low oxygen = 3 Low oxygen = 2
Other = 5 Other = 5
Significant illness during first 2 yr of life No problems = 48.6% No problems = 35.6% NS
Yes problems = 51.4% Yes problems = 64.4%
Sitting milestone (delayed: >9 mo) Normal = 87.5% Normal = 82.8% NS
Delayed = 12.5% Delayed = 17.2%
Crawling milestone (delayed: > 12 mo) Normal = 65% Normal = 78.1% NS
Delayed = 35% Delayed = 22.2%
Walking milestone (delayed: >20 mo) Normal = 60% Normal = 81% < .02
Delayed = 40% Delayed = 19%
Reported family history of AD or AsD- No family history = 57.5% N o family history = 39.1% NS
like behavior Yes family history = 42.5% Yes family history = 60.9%
n = 40* n = 64
Reported AsD-like traits in father No father traits = 87.5% No father traits = 71.9% NS
Father traits = 12.5% Father traits = 28.1%
List of AsD-like traits in family (relative Father = 5 Father = 17 NS
to child) Mother = 1 Mother = 1
Grandfather (paternal) = 1 Grandfather (paternal) = 5
Grandfather (maternal) = 0 Grandfather (maternal) = 1
Paternal relative =7 Paternal relative = 9
Maternal relative = 3 Maternal relative = 6

Note: AD = autistic disorder; AsD = Asperger’s disorder; ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; OCD =
obsessive-compulsive disorder.
* Reduced sample sizes due to parents unable to offer age when they first recognized a problem in their child.

babbling, repetitive use of words, impaired prosody The results show that the early communication
(e.g., too loud, too soft, monotonous), pronoun rever- history may be important for clinicians making a
sal, poor response to instructionslsimple communica- diagnosis of AsD. AsD subjects were more likely to
tions, literal understanding of language, and problems have been one-sided in their conversation and to have
in initiation or ability to sustain a conversation. Never- used idiosyncratic wordslphraseslsigns, long-winded
theless clinicians still saw fit to diagnose AsD in children pedantic speech, and an unusual tone of voice. They
who had experienced a delay in a language or showed were also less likely to have been echolalic than the
communication disturbances but were less socially im- AD subjects. At current presentation only the long-
paired and thus more “AsD-like.” More research is winded, pedantic speech variable predicted the AsD
needed on communication development in AsD to group. This finding probably reflects the less impaired
determine whether future revisions of DSM and ICD communication skills that are found in the AsD child
need to include particular kinds of communication compared with the more language-delayed, echolalic
impairments as AsD criteria, or at least consider deleting AD child. There were relatively fewer reported differ-
the lack of language delay as an exclusion criterion. ences in communication as the groups got older. The

J . AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 3 5 : 1 1 , NOVEMBER 1996 1529


E I S E N M A J E R E T AL.

results suggest that with age, this able AD group O f significance is the finding that the two groups
developed “AsD-like” communication characteristics. were not different on many of the “classic” autism
This finding concurs with published clinical accounts impairments, such as in imagination, imitation, nonver-
(e.g., Kanner, 1973; Wing, 1981) where individuals bal communication, awareness of social rules, presence
may show “typical” AD characteristics in early child- of stereotyped movements (e.g., rocking, spinning,
hood, only to develop “AsD-like” features as they hand-flapping), routines and resistance to change, and
mature. literal understanding of language. The methodological
It is also proposed that clinicians are identifying a limitations of this study mean that the lack of differ-
less delayed or retarded group of children as having ences between the two groups could be the result of
AsD. The AsD group were less likely to have experi- either no differences between the two groups, clinicians
enced walking delays and at current presentation had misclassifying individuals, or unreliable or biased re-
a higher VMASS than the AD group. Results from porting of symptoms by parents. It is also possible that
intelligence testing of a subsample of the total group differences do exist in degree or severity of symptom;
in this study (Manjiviona, 1996) support the VMASS however, this issue was not able to be addressed in
finding, with the AsD group more able in the visuospa- this study. It is possible that AsD children display
tial domain and marginally more able in the verbal more subtle or less frequent demonstrations of these
area. The AsD group is less cognitively impaired than behaviors than do A D children. In this study each
the A D group. The results also suggest that the lower variable was measured in a presendabsent format and
verbal skills of the AD group at current presentation was probably too gross to detect more subtle differences.
may reflect the delays first experienced in language Future research may find differences between groups in
onset. symptom severity by using more fine-grained methods.
Another factor that points to a less severe impairment It appears that clinicians are identifying a group of
suffered by the AsD group compared with the AD autistic children who seem to desire friends and who
group is the increased likelihood for the AsD group make flawed attempts at social activities; who are less
to be diagnosed at a later age. This occurred despite likely to have experienced a delay in language onset
finding no difference in the age when parents first or echolalia, who use idiosyncratic words and pedantic
recognized a problem in their child’s functioning. Dif- speech patterns, and who engage in one-sided, repetitive
ferent explanations could account for this finding. conversations; and who have narrow, circumscribed
Diagnostic confusion on the part of the clinician may interests. Although this group fulfill criteria for AD,
have delayed a diagnosis if there was an absence of clinicians are using the label AsD. The AsD group is
early “classic” autistic signs (e.g., echolalia); if the less cognitively delayed than the AD group, and it is
individual’s problems went undetected until later in possible that the clinical differences reflect this factor.
childhood when significantly more social, communica- No suboptimal birth, genetic, or developmental factors
tive, and cognitive demands were encountered, such (apart from delay in language onset and walking) appear
as at school; or if parents were not sufficiently concerned to reflect differences in diagnosis. The clinical picture
in the early years to seek a diagnosis of an “odd” child. was also shown to change over time, where the young,
Also of clinical interest is the increased likelihood high-functioning AD child later came to resemble (in
of a diagnosis of A D H D as a comorbid condition with some respects) the child with an AsD diagnosis.
AsD. Although DSM-ZVstates that A D H D is not to These results suggest that clinicians have generally
be diagnosed in individuals with a PDD, in some cases accepted the descriptions outlined by Asperger himself
this had taken place. The DSM-IV description of and further elaborated by other authors such as Wing
the A D H D features of inattention, impulsivity, and (1981, 1991), Gillberg (1989), and Szatmari (1989b).
hyperactivity resembles the behavior of some AsD The above description of AsD is also very similar to
children; however, whether the two conditions actually Wing and Gould’s (1979) “active but odd” subcategory
do coexist or simply manifest in similar behaviors will of autistic children (the others being “aloof’ and “pas-
only be known in the future after the biological bases sive”). The “active but odd” children were noted to
of both disorders are found. make clumsy social approaches, to use repetitive and

1530 J . AM. ACAD. C H I L D ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 3 5 : 1 1 , N O V E M B E R 1996


C O M P A R I S O N O F A U T I S M A N D ASPERGER’S D I S O R D E R

one-sided communication, and to engage in developed, Ghaziuddin M , Tsai LY, Ghaziuddin N (1992), Brief report: a comparison
of the diagnostic criteria for Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Dis-
circumscribed interests. Follow-up into adolescence and ord 22:643-649
adulthood found roughly a fifth of the cohort had Gillberg C (1989), Asperger’s syndrome in 23 Swedish children. Dev Med
Child Neurol31:529-531
changed from an “aloof’ to a “passive” or “active but Kanner L (1973), Childhood Psychosis: Initial Studies and New Insights.
odd” style of interaction (Wing and Gould, 1979). New York: WinstonlWiley
Manjiviona J (1996), Neuropsychological profiles ofchildren with Asperger’s
Our results confirm this change in symptom profile syndrome and high functioning autism. PhD thesis, LaTrobe University,
over time and highlight the dilemma of affixing discrete Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
Rapin I (1996), Preschool children with inadequate communication: devel-
diagnostic categories to potentially “fluid” symptom opmental language disorders, autism or low IQ. Clinics in Developmental
states. It is argued that the nosological validity of AsD Medicine, No. 139. London: Mac Keith Press
Rutter M (1989), Annotation: child psychiatric disorders in ICD-10. J
and other PDD subtypes as discrete categories will Child Psycbol Psychiatry 30:499-5 13
occur only if there are differences in outcome, etiology, Schopler E (1985), Convergence of learning disability, higher-level autism,
or treatment; however, there is little evidence to date and Asperger’s syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 15:359
Szatmari P (1992), The validity of autistic spectrum disorders: a literature
to support such a notion (Szatmari, 1992). Wing review. J Autism Dev Disord 22:583-600
(1991) and others have argued for a broadening of Szatmari P, Archer L, Fisman S, Streiner DL, Wilson F (1995), Asperger’s
syndrome and autism: differences in behavior, cognition, and adaptive
the traditional conceptual boundaries of autism by functioning. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiaty 341662-1671
recognizing different manifestations of autistic behav- Szatmari P, Bartolucci G, Bremner R (1989a), Asperger’s syndrome and
autism: comparison of early history and outcome. Dev Med Child
iors on an “autistic spectrum.” We hope that future Neurol31:709-720
revisions of autism criteria more adequately reflect the Szatmari P, Bremner R, Nagy J (1989b), Asperger’s syndrome: a review
of clinical features. Can J Psychiatry 34:554-560
symptom profile of high-functioning autistic individu- T a n t a m D (1988), Asperger’s syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
als that is increasingly being recognized by clinicians. 29:245-255
Volkmar FR, Klin A, Siege1 B et al. (1994), Field trial for autistic disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 151:1361-1367
Wing L (1981), Asperger’s syndrome: a clinical account. Psychol Med
11:115-129
REFERENCES Wing L (1986), Clarification on Asperger’s syndrome. J Autism Dev
Disord 16:513-5 15
American Psychiatric Association (1987), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Wing L (199 1), The relationship between Asperger’s syndrome and Kanner’s
of Mental Disorders, 3rd edition-revised (DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: autism. In: Autism and Rrperger Syndrome, Frith U, ed. Cambridge,
American Psychiatric Association England: Cambridge University Press
American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Wing L, Gould J (1979), Severe impairments of social interaction and
of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American associated abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification. 1
Psychiatric Association Autism Dev Disord 9: 11-29
Asperger H (1944), Die “Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. Arch World Health Organization (1988), International Chsz3cation of Diseases
Psychiatr Neruenkr 117:76-136 and Disorders (ICD-10, Draft Version). Geneva: World Health
Dunn LM, Dunn LM (1981), Manualfor the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Organization
Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service World Health Organization (1993), The ICD-I0 Chszjication of Mental
Dunn LM, Dunn LM, Whetton C, Pintilie D (1982), British Picture and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva:
Vocabulary Scale. London: NFER-Nelson World Health Organization
Eisenmajer R (1996), Diagnosis and theory of mind relationships in Yirmiya N , Sigman M, Freeman BJ (1994), Comparison between diagnostic
autism spectrum disorders. P h D thesis, La Trobe University, Bundoora, instruments for identifying high-functioning children with autism. J
Victoria, Australia Autism Dev Disord 24281-291

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 3 5 : 1 1 , NOVEMBER 1996 1531

You might also like