You are on page 1of 10

Retirement Adjustment

A Review of Theoretical and Empirical Advancements

Mo Wang University of Maryland, College Park


Kène Henkens Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute and
Tilburg University
Hanna van Solinge Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic Institute

In this article, we review both theoretical and empirical nition of the fact that the impact of retirement may vary not
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

advancements in retirement adjustment research. After re- only across individuals but also within individuals over
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

viewing and integrating current theories about retirement time. Thus, empirical studies have started to probe and
adjustment, we propose a resource-based dynamic per- examine this heterogeneity in retirement adjustment (e.g.,
spective to apply to the understanding of retirement adjust- Pinquart & Schindler, 2007; van Solinge & Henkens, 2005,
ment. We then review empirical findings that are associ- 2008; Wang, 2007). Nevertheless, little effort has been
ated with the key research questions in this literature: (a) invested in integrating the different theoretical perspectives
What is the general impact of retirement on the individual? used by researchers to study this heterogeneity in retire-
and (b) What are the factors that influence retirement ment adjustment. Therefore, the first purpose of this article
adjustment quality? We also highlight important future is to review and integrate current theories about retirement
research directions that may be fruitful for psychologists to adjustment and establish a resource-based dynamic per-
pursue in this area. spective as a coherent theoretical framework to guide fur-
Keywords: retirement adjustment, older workers, resource ther research on retirement adjustment. In particular, we
perspective argue that retirement adjustment is a longitudinal process
during which retirees’ levels of adjustment (i.e., psycho-

R etirement from work is one of the major life course logical comfort regarding the retirement life) may fluctuate
transitions in late adult life. Associated with this as a function of individual resources and changes in these
transition, the question of how well individuals resources. Further, as in most other studies on this topic, we
adjust to retirement has been a focus of interest to research- assume that adjustment is reached when individuals are not
ers as well as to the popular media (Wang & Shultz, 2010). preoccupied with the retirement transition but are comfort-
Although retirement has received extensive study, the im- able with the changed circumstances of life in retirement
pact of retirement on the individual and the factors impor- (i.e., are able to integrate retirement into their lives; Good-
tant for a successful adjustment in retirement still remain man, Schlossberg, & Anderson, 2006; Schlossberg, 1981).
less clear than other aspects of retirement phenomena (e.g., The second purpose of this article is to review empir-
retirement decision making and retirement planning; ical findings in retirement adjustment research. To organize
Wang, 2007). It is important to study retirement adjustment these empirical findings, we make a distinction between
because it directly provides information about how to im- retirement adjustment as a process of getting used to the
prove the quality of postretirement life. In addition, study- changed circumstances of life and the outcomes of this
ing retirement adjustment provides an opportunity for re- process in terms of retirement adjustment quality. The
searchers to understand how people simultaneously adjust retirement adjustment process may be more or less chal-
to internal (i.e., physical and psychological aging) and lenging to retiring individuals, and this variability may
external (i.e., lifestyle and societal norms) challenges in translate into differences in retirement adjustment quality
their later life. Findings from this research have the poten- over time and across individuals. Consequently, the key
tial to yield information about other complex adjustment questions addressed in this review of empirical findings
processes people experience when facing significant inter- include the following: (a) What is the general impact of
nal and external changes.
Until the 1960s, studies on retirement adjustment typ-
This article was published Online First February 21, 2011.
ically viewed retirement as a “crisis” event that created a Mo Wang, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland,
challenge to personal well-being (van Solinge & Henkens, College Park; Kène Henkens, Netherlands Interdisciplinary Demographic
2008). Although researchers still view retirement as a chal- Institute, The Hague, The Netherlands, and Tilburg University, Tilburg,
lenging, life-changing event, it has been acknowledged in The Netherlands; Hanna van Solinge, Netherlands Interdisciplinary De-
mographic Institute, The Hague, The Netherlands.
recent studies that retirement may also have beneficial Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mo
effects (e.g., Mein, Martikainen, Hemingway, Stansfeld, & Wang, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park,
Marmot, 2003; Wang, 2007). There is also growing recog- MD 20742. E-mail: mwang@psyc.umd.edu

204 April 2011 ● American Psychologist


© 2011 American Psychological Association 0003-066X/11/$12.00
Vol. 66, No. 3, 204 –213 DOI: 10.1037/a0022414
that one is highly invested in a particular role (e.g., the
work role), feelings of self-worth tend to be associated with
the ability to carry out that role in an effective manner
(Ashforth, 2001). Therefore, being retired can be charac-
terized as a role transition (Riley & Riley, 1994), which
includes the processes of losing or weakening work roles
(e.g., the worker role, the organizational member role, and
the career role) and strengthening the family member role
and the community member role. Role theorists argue that
the role loss resulting from retirement transition can cause
people to feel anxious or depressed, which may lead to low
levels of well-being in retirement (Riley & Riley, 1994).
These feelings may be due to the functional importance of
work-related roles, which serve to maintain one’s positive
self-image (Feldman, 1994). Consequently, one’s life after
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

retirement might be viewed as less satisfying than one’s life


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

during the years when one was employed. On the other


hand, retirees with other role involvements or those who
retire from unpleasant jobs may be less troubled by, and
even pleased with, the loss of those work roles. For exam-
ple, for individuals who find their jobs stressful or burden-
Mo Wang some, retiring could be a relief from ongoing strains and
conflicts. Also, for individuals who would like to engage
more fully in the roles of family member and community
member, retirement is an opportunity for them to free up
retirement on the individual (i.e., how well is the individual time to enjoy the rewards and responsibilities tied to those
getting used to the retirement life)? and (b) What are the roles.
factors that influence retirement adjustment quality? We Continuity Theory
aim to use this review to summarize the up-to-date knowl-
edge regarding these research questions. When reviewing Continuity theory emphasizes the consistency of life pat-
the empirical findings, we also tie them to the proposed terns over time (i.e., the accommodation of life changes
resource-based dynamic perspective when feasible and il- without the experience of stressful disruption; Atchley,
lustrate the utility of applying this perspective to reconcil- 1999). According to Atchley (1999), there is considerable
ing inconsistent theoretical predictions and improving fu- continuity in identity and self-concept over the retirement
ture studies. transition, and this continuity contributes to the individu-
At the end of this review, we highlight important al’s retirement adjustment. Rather than focusing on retire-
future research directions that may be fruitful for psychol- ment as a disruptive role loss, continuity theorists view it as
ogists to pursue in this area. These future directions address an opportunity to maintain social relationship and lifestyle
both methodological issues and empirical gaps in the liter- patterns. Thus, this theory predicts that there should not be
ature. We hope the consideration of these future directions a significant drop in well-being when people transition
will enrich the empirical findings about retirement adjust- from work into retirement life unless severe difficulty in
ment and provide additional knowledge for refining the maintaining those general patterns is experienced.
theoretical framework. Stage Theory
Theoretical Review and Integration The literature on employee retirement has recognized that
for Conceptualizing Retirement the workforce exit process often happens gradually and that
Adjustment people may have multiple job exits and entries using var-
ious forms of bridge employment before they fully retire
The major theories that have been applied to the study (e.g., Wang, Adams, Beehr, & Shultz, 2009). As such,
of retirement adjustment include role theory, continuity retirement adjustment constitutes a long-term process dur-
theory, stage theory, the life course perspective, and the ing which variation in retirees’ well-being may be ob-
resource perspective. Below, we first review each of these served. This is indeed the premise put forth by stage theory
specific theories. We then integrate them and conceptualize (e.g., Atchley, 1976; Gall, Evans, & Howard, 1997). Ac-
retirement adjustment as a resource-based dynamic pro- cording to stage theory, early in the transition to retirement,
cess. retirees experience a honeymoon stage, in which they may
feel more energetic and satisfied as they pursue new activ-
Role Theory
ities and roles. Later on, retirees may experience a disen-
Role theory emphasizes the importance of the role exit and chantment stage, in which they realize that they have fewer
the role transition in retirement adjustment. To the extent resources and/or had unrealistic expectations about retire-

April 2011 ● American Psychologist 205


characteristics, and career trajectories; van Solinge & Hen-
kens, 2008; Wang, Zhan, Liu, & Shultz, 2008). The general
premise is that if an individual has cultivated a flexible
style in dealing with previous life transitions, is less so-
cially integrated with their work, and has the attributes that
help smooth and accomplish the transition, he or she will
be more likely to prepare well for the adjustment and to
achieve better outcomes of the adjustment (Wang & Shultz,
2010).
The life course perspective also emphasizes that the
experiences in one life sphere (e.g., work life) influence
and are influenced by experiences in other life spheres
(e.g., marital life). According to this concept of interde-
pendent life spheres, nonwork life spheres are important for
retirement adjustment because they provide retirees with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

alternative salient identities after retirement and offer op-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

portunities for retirees to engage in meaningful and desir-


able activities after retirement. Supporting this argument is
consistent evidence that individuals who are married and
strongly identify with their family roles have more positive
Kène experiences in retirement (e.g., Calasanti, 1996). Van
Henkens Solinge and Henkens (2005) have also shown that adjust-
ment problems faced by one partner affect the quality of the
other partner’s retirement experience.

ment. As time passes, retirees enter a reorientation stage, The Resource Perspective
during which they reevaluate their life status, accept limi-
tations, and focus on further adjustment to retirement. Although the life course perspective provides a general
Eventually, retirees enter the stability stage, settling into a framework for analyzing retirement adjustment and points
predictable daily life pattern until death or disability ter- to the utility of studying a broad range of variables, it offers
minates their retirement. So far, stage theory has received few concrete hypotheses about how these variables impact
little direct support from empirical studies. The strength of retirement adjustment (Wang, 2007). Consequently, re-
this theory (i.e., making specific predictions regarding the searchers often rely on a resource perspective to predict
dynamic nature of retirement adjustment) may also be its how variables may be associated with retirement adjust-
weakness because of the stringent pattern it predicts. Fur- ment. According to Hobfoll (2002), resources can be
ther, the specific time course for each stage is also unclear. broadly defined as the total capability an individual has to
fulfill his or her centrally valued needs. Reviewing differ-
The Life Course Perspective ent types of resources studied in previous retirement re-
The life course principle of “human agency within struc- search, Wang (2007) suggested that this total capability
ture” implies that individuals have plans, make choices, may include the individual’s physical resources (e.g., mus-
and undertake actions within the opportunities and con- cle strength; McArdle, Vasilaki, & Jackson, 2002), cogni-
straints of their social worlds, which are shaped by personal tive resources (e.g., processing speed and working mem-
history and social circumstances (Settersten, 2003). The ory; Park, 2000), motivational resources (e.g., self-efficacy;
life course perspective thus emphasizes that life transitions Bandura, 1997), financial resources (e.g., salary and pen-
are contextually embedded, which implies that the experi- sion; Hobfoll, 2002), social resources (e.g., social network
ences of retirement adjustment are contingent on the spe- and social support; S. Kim & Feldman, 2000), and emo-
cific circumstances under which the adjustment occurs. For tional resources (e.g., mood and affectivity; Wang, Liao,
example, a growing body of literature has revealed that Zhan, & Shi, in press). The central premise of this resource
perceived control over the departure from the workforce is perspective for studying retirement adjustment is that the
crucial to retirement adjustment (e.g., Henkens, van ease of adjustment is the direct result of the individual’s
Solinge, & Gallo, 2008; van Solinge & Henkens, 2005). access to resources. Specifically, when people have more
These circumstances may also include individual attributes resources to fulfill the needs they value in retirement, they
(e.g., demographic characteristics, finance and health sta- will experience less difficulty in adjusting to retirement. On
tus, and transition related abilities and skills; J. E. Kim & the other hand, decreases in retirees’ resources will have
Moen, 2002), individual history factors (e.g., how people adverse effects on their retirement adjustment. Therefore,
dealt with previous transitions, their work and leisure hab- according to this perspective, to understand retirement ad-
its, and their previous workforce participation patterns; justment, researchers should focus on examining variables
Orel, Ford, & Brock, 2004), and current and past job- that have a direct impact on retirees’ different types of
related experience (e.g., former job attitudes, former job resources.

206 April 2011 ● American Psychologist


ence a positive change in adjustment level. Thus, unlike
role theory, continuity theory, and stage theory, which we
reviewed earlier, this theoretical framework has the flexi-
bility to accommodate a variety of longitudinal patterns for
retirement adjustment. This flexibility significantly en-
riches the theoretical approach to understanding individual
differences in the longitudinal process of retirement adjust-
ment. This framework also relaxes the sequential assump-
tion regarding the development stages in retirement adjust-
ment (e.g., as specified in stage theory). Therefore,
recognizing the dynamic nature of retirement adjustment,
we contend that time courses are less important in describ-
ing the retirement adjustment process; retirees’ well-being
could fluctuate up and down at any given time point,
following the variation in their total resources.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Moreover, this resource-based dynamic perspective


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

can also be applied to identifying factors that may influence


retirement adjustment quality. Specifically, this framework
offers a large variety of antecedents that could influence
various retirees’ resources in the adjustment process, in-
Hanna cluding variables from the macro level, the organizational
van Solinge level, the job level, the household level, and the individual
level, as illustrated in Figure 1. In addition, the central
constructs in the life course perspective, such as the char-
acteristics of the retirement transition, the contexts of re-
Retirement Adjustment as a Resource-Based
tirement adjustment, and the interdependent life spheres,
Dynamic Process
can all be viewed as pointing to variables that may influ-
After this brief review of the current theories about retire- ence retirees’ resources at different time points and in
ment adjustment, it is important to note that each of them different aspects of the adjustment process. Adopting the
can provide insight only into a specific aspect of retirement resource-based dynamic perspective may lead to a more
adjustment. As such, a more integrated theoretical frame- comprehensive and fruitful examination of different factors
work is needed to account for the complex nature of re- that influence retirement adjustment. It should be noted that
tirement adjustment and to guide the future research in this the utility of the resource-based dynamic perspective is
field. To address this gap, we propose to apply a resource- consistent with the “balance of resources to deficits” ap-
based dynamic perspective to further the theoretical devel- proach that is often adopted in understanding human
opment of retirement adjustment. Specifically, we argue adaptation to transitions (e.g., Goodman, Schlossberg, &
that retirement adjustment is a longitudinal process during Anderson, 2006; Schlossberg, 1981). This approach em-
which retirees’ levels of adjustment may fluctuate as a phasizes that adaptation to a transition depends on the ratio
function of individual resources and changes in these re- of resources to deficits in terms of the transition itself, the
sources. Therefore, instead of focusing on the absolute pre–post environment, and the individual’s sense of com-
good or bad impact that retirement has on retirees’ well- petency, well-being, and health (Schlossberg, 1981).
being, we focus on the underlying mechanism through Finally, combining both the dynamic perspective and
which retirement has this impact. As illustrated in Figure 1, the resource perspective to conceptualize retirement adjust-
by incorporating the resource perspective, variation in the ment also provides new opportunities for researchers to
level of adjustment along the retirement adjustment process understand other characteristics of the adjustment process.
can be viewed as a result of resource changes. In other For example, although role theory, continuity theory, and
words, if over time (e.g., t4 to t5 in Figure 1) a retiree’s total stage theory provide specific predictions regarding the
resource does not change significantly (e.g., because the downward and/or upward trends in retirees’ well-being
retiree successfully maintains prior lifestyles and activi- change over time, they may not be as informative in terms
ties), he or she may not experience a significant change in of understanding the turning points (e.g., t2, t3, and t4 in
adjustment level. Alternatively, if over time (e.g., t2 to t3 in Figure 1) that connect two different trends in the longitu-
Figure 1) a retiree’s total resource significantly decreases dinal adjustment process. However, if one adopts the re-
(e.g., through losing a major income source), he or she may source perspective, it is not difficult to hypothesize that
experience a negative change in adjustment level. Further, certain individual differences (e.g., openness to change,
if over time (e.g., t1 to t2 or t3 to t4 in Figure 1) an goal orientation in retirement, and need for structure),
individual’s retirement enables him or her to invest signif- which may impact retirees’ motivational resources, and
icantly more resources (e.g., through gaining cognitive certain environmental factors (e.g., family support, com-
resources that were previously occupied by a stressful job) munity cohesiveness, and unemployment rate in the local
in fulfilling centrally valued needs, he or she may experi- labor market), which may impact retirees’ financial and

April 2011 ● American Psychologist 207


Figure 1
Illustration of the Resource-Based Dynamic Perspective for Understanding the Retirement Adjustment Process

Potential Antecedents*
Physical Resources
Macro Level Cognitive Resources
Societal Norms Motivational Resources
Government Policies Financial Resources
Social Resources
Organizational Level
Emotional Resources
Organizational Climate
Human Resource Practices
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Level of Adjustment
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Job Level
Resource Resource Resource
Job Conditions
Decrease Increase Unchanged
Job Attachment

Household Level
Marital Quality
Caregiving Demands

Individual Level
Health Behaviors
Psychological Resilience

Time

t1 t2 t3 t4 t5

Retirement Adjustment Process



Note. The potential antecedents for resource changes listed here are illustrative rather than exhaustive.

social resources, may predict how fast the turning points The Impact of Retirement
will be reached for retirees who experience negative
change first but later experience positive change in their A large body of literature has considered the extent to
which retirees are psychologically comfortable with the
well-being. This is because these individual differences and
changed circumstances of life in retirement. Indicators of
environmental factors all facilitate retirees’ obtaining more
this psychological comfort have included happiness (Beck,
resources, which leads them to be more likely to switch 1982), emotional well-being (Richardson & Kilty, 1991),
from the downward trend to the upward trend. Therefore, in retirement satisfaction (Gall et al., 1997; Quick & Moen,
future studies, applying this resource-based dynamic per- 1998), life satisfaction (Calasanti, 1996), as well as mental
spective may further improve researchers’ understanding health and depression (e.g., Midanik, Soghikian, Ransom,
about the form and the nature of the retirement process. & Tekawa, 1995). These studies have in common that they
infer adjustment indirectly via outcome measures. Recent
Empirical Findings literature has suggested more direct measures of adjust-
ment, such as retirees’ own evaluation of the difficulties
The empirical studies in the field of retirement adjustment they had in adjusting to retirement and the amount of time
have mainly focused on seeking answers for the following adjustment took (e.g., van Solinge & Henkens, 2005,
two research questions: (a) What is the general impact of 2008).
retirement on the individual? and (b) What are the factors The literature has demonstrated a noticeable hetero-
that influence retirement adjustment quality? Therefore, we geneity of findings in terms of the impact of retirement.
organize our review by examining empirical findings that Some research has found that retirees, in comparison with
answer each of these two questions separately. workers, tend to report greater depression and loneliness,

208 April 2011 ● American Psychologist


lower life satisfaction and happiness, a less positive view seven years before and after retirement and found that
about retirement, and lower activity levels (e.g., J. E. Kim retirement was associated with a decrease in suboptimum
& Moen, 2002; Richardson & Kilty, 1991). In contrast, health. However, a longitudinal study of British civil ser-
other research has found that most individuals tend to look vants by Mein et al. (2003) found that retirement had no
forward to retirement (e.g., Dorfman, 1992) as well as to effect on physical health but was associated with an im-
report being satisfied with retirement (e.g., Calasanti, provement in mental health. Finally, using data from seven
1996). Finally, retirement has been shown to be a benign longitudinal waves of the Health and Retirement Study,
event with no apparent impact on an individual’s well- Dave, Rashad, and Spasojevic (2008) found that complete
being (e.g., Gall et al., 1997). Minimal differences in mea- retirement was associated with increases in illness condi-
sures of mental health, coping, and health behaviors were tions and difficulties in mobility and daily activities as well
reported between workers and retirees within a similar age as with a decline in mental health over an average postre-
range (e.g., Wu, Tang, & Yan, 2005). tirement period of six years.
To reconcile these inconsistent findings, Wang (2007) Adding uncertainty to these inconsistent findings is
hypothesized that multiple forms of retirement transition the fact that none of these studies explicitly examined the
and adjustment coexist in the retiree population. Using extent to which retirees were psychologically comfortable
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

longitudinal data from two nationally representative sam- with their retirement life. Therefore, we simply still do not
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ples obtained by the U.S. Health and Retirement Study and know whether retirees who had higher levels of adjustment
the growth mixture modeling technique (Wang & Bodner, to retirement enjoyed better health in their postretirement
2007), he was able to consistently demonstrate that over an lives. For example, individuals who smoothly complete the
eight-year period in the retirement adjustment process, process of social and psychological detachment from work
about 70% of retirees experienced minimum psychological may feel healthier. This may be reinforced whenever their
well-being changes; about 25% of retirees experienced postretirement lifestyle includes opportunities for positive
negative changes in psychological well-being during the health behavior, such as exercise-related leisure activities.
initial transition stage but then showed improvements af- Adjustment quality may also indirectly affect health. Re-
terward; and about 5% of retirees experienced positive tirees’ lack of adjustment to their retirement life often
changes in psychological well-being. These findings sug- manifests as a risk factor for them to engage in maladaptive
gest that retirees do not follow a uniform pattern of retire- coping behaviors. For example, empirical studies found
ment adjustment. that lower levels of adjustment to retirement were associ-
Wang’s (2007) findings were further corroborated by ated with increased alcohol use (Perreira & Sloan, 2001)
Pinquart and Schindler (2007). They used a nationally and increased smoking (Henkens et al., 2008).
representative sample of German retirees from the German
Socioeconomic Panel Study and found that during retire- Factors That Influence Retirement Adjustment
ment transition and adjustment, about 75% of German Quality
retirees experienced trivial changes in life satisfaction; Much research has been conducted in recent decades to
about 9% of German retirees experienced a significant identify factors that influence retirement adjustment qual-
decrease in their life satisfaction during the initial transition ity. However, the theoretical basis of most of these studies
stage but continued on a stable or increasing life satisfac- remains rather implicit, with few hypotheses explicitly
tion trajectory thereafter; and about 15% of German retir- formulated (Wang & Shultz, 2010). Further, these studies
ees experienced significant increases in their life satis- usually focus on a relatively narrow set of explanatory
faction. factors, despite the fact that a wide variety of predictors of
Taken as a whole, both studies suggest that multiple retirement adjustment quality have been considered in the
longitudinal change patterns in retirees’ psychological literature. These predictors can be summarized in five
well-being exist during the retirement adjustment process broader categories: individual attributes, preretirement
and that these patterns correspond to different subpopula- job-related variables, family-related variables, retirement-
tions of retirees. As such, these findings support the mul- transition-related variables, and postretirement activities
tiple-pathway dynamic nature of retirement adjustment, (Wang & Shultz, 2010). Table 1 presents a summary of
illustrating that the same retirement decision may lead to these predictors’ effects on retirement adjustment quality,
different adjustment processes in retirement for different as well as citations of sample empirical studies in which
retirees (Wang & Shultz, 2010). These findings also di- these effects were identified.
rectly support the dynamic perspective we endorsed earlier, In reviewing these empirical findings regarding the
because they demonstrate systematic changes in retirees’ predictors of retirement adjustment quality, it is important
well-being over time. to note that most of them are directly associated with
A separate area of research focuses on the impact of different types of resources that retirees have during the
retirement on health, as well as on more distal outcomes, retirement adjustment. For example, retirees’ physical and
such as longevity and mortality. Similar to what we con- mental health, preretirement work stress and psychological
cluded with regard to studies on retirement and psycholog- and physical job demands, and postretirement leisure ac-
ical well-being, the empirical findings on this issue are also tivities are associated with their physical, emotional, and
mixed. For example, Westerlund et al. (2009) investigated cognitive resources. Retirees’ financial statuses, unemploy-
more than 14,000 members of a French occupational cohort ment before retirement, number of dependent children,

April 2011 ● American Psychologist 209


Table 1
Summary of Variables That Influence Retirement Adjustment Quality
Predictor category Variable Effect Sample empirical studies

Individual attributes Physical health ⫹ Dorfman, 1992; S. Kim & Feldman, 2000; van
Solinge & Henkens, 2008
Mental health ⫹ J. E. Kim & Moen, 2002; Wang, 2007
Financial status ⫹ Gall et al., 1997; Pinquart & Schindler, 2007;
Quick & Moen; 1998
Physical health decline ⫺ J. E. Kim & Moen, 2002; van Solinge &
Henkens, 2008; Wang, 2007
Preretirement job-related Work stress ⫹ Wang, 2007
variables Job demands ⫹ Quick & Moen, 1998; Wang, 2007
Job challenges ⫹ van Solinge & Henkens, 2008
Job dissatisfaction ⫹ Wang, 2007
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Unemployment before retirement ⫹ Pinquart & Schindler, 2007


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Work role identity ⫺ Quick & Moen, 1998; Reitzes & Mutran, 2004
Family-related variables Marital status (married vs. single/ ⫹ Pinquart & Schindler, 2007
widowed)
Spouse working status (working vs. ⫺ Wang, 2007
not)
Marital quality ⫹ Szinovacz & Davey, 2004; Wang, 2007
Number of dependent children ⫺ S. Kim & Feldman, 2000
Losing a partner during the transition ⫺ van Solinge & Henkens, 2008
Retirement transition- Voluntariness of the retirement ⫹ Reitzes & Mutran, 2004; van Solinge &
related variables Henkens, 2005, 2008
Retirement planning ⫹ Reitzes & Mutran, 2004; Wang, 2007
Retiring earlier than expected ⫺ Quick & Moen, 1998; Wang, 2007
Retiring for health care reasons ⫺ Quick & Moen, 1998
Retiring to do other things ⫹ Quick & Moen, 1998
Retiring to receive financial incentives ⫹ Quick & Moen, 1998
Postretirement activities Bridge employment ⫹ S. Kim & Feldman, 2000; Wang, 2007; Zhan,
Wang, Liu, & Shultz, 2009
Volunteer work ⫹ Dorfman & Douglas, 2005; S. Kim & Feldman,
2000
Leisure activities ⫹ Dorfman & Douglas, 2005; S. Kim & Feldman,
2000
Anxiety associated with social ⫺ van Solinge & Henkens, 2005, 2008
activities
Note. Plus sign (⫹) denotes positive effect on retirement adjustment quality, and minus sign (⫺) denotes negative effect on retirement adjustment quality.

retirement planning, and bridge employment activities are Future Directions in Retirement
associated with their financial resources. Retirees’ marital Adjustment Research
status, spouses’ working status, marital quality, and post-
retirement volunteer work and bridge employment are as- Now that we have reviewed both theoretical and empirical
sociated with their social resources. Retirees’ work role advancements in the retirement adjustment literature, we
identity, preretirement job dissatisfaction, and retirement propose several new research directions, based on the re-
motivations are associated with their motivational re- source-based dynamic perspective, to help guide further
sources. Finally, involuntary retirement, losing one’s part- theoretical and empirical examinations on retirement ad-
ner during the retirement transition, and anxiety associated justment. First, although personality variables and disposi-
with the retirement transition are linked with retirees’ emo- tional traits have been shown to be important individual
tional resources. Therefore, the empirical findings in gen- resources that influence adjustment quality in other types of
eral are consistent with the resource-based dynamic per- life transition and adjustment processes (e.g., adjustment to
spective we proposed earlier. All in all, these findings unfamiliar work and cultural environments; Wang &
suggest that resource change in important life domains is Takeuchi, 2007), very few empirical studies have examined
associated with the variation in retirement adjustment them as predictors of retirement adjustment quality. One
quality. exception is the study by Reitzes and Mutran (2004) that

210 April 2011 ● American Psychologist


showed a positive relationship between retirees’ self es- macroeconomic trends, government policies related to retire-
teem and their retirement satisfaction. Another exception is ment (e.g., the Social Security and Medicare programs), and
a study by Gall et al. (1997) that showed a positive rela- the social norms for retirement. To the extent that these
tionship between retirees’ internal locus of control and their variables may directly or indirectly influence retirees’ re-
retirement adjustment quality. Given that personality vari- sources, they could have an important impact on retirees’
ables and dispositional traits are important for people in adjustment quality. For example, the only study (Pinquart &
conducting emotional appraisals, setting up motivational Schindler, 2007) that linked the regional labor force market to
priorities, and choosing coping strategies (Löckenhoff, Ter- retirees’ adjustment showed that retirees who retired in a
racciano, & Costa, 2009), more research should focus on region where the unemployment rate was higher were more
explicating how these variables may influence retirement likely to experience increased life satisfaction than their coun-
adjustment quality. terparts who retired in a region where the unemployment rate
Second, an evaluation of the literature on the impact was lower. This finding might have resulted because retire-
of retirement reveals that most studies adopting a resource- ment constitutes an escape from the job insecurity and stress
based perspective have concentrated only on a narrow set associated with labor markets in which jobs are difficult to
of resources, in particular on retirees’ health and wealth find, thus saving retirees’ emotional and cognitive resources.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Wang & Shultz, 2010). As a result, social resources and Sixth, in terms of research designs, the vast major-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the spouse’s resources have received less attention. A ity of previous studies in the field of employee retire-
broader view that recognizes the impact of the social net- ment research have relied on cross-sectional designs.
work may advance the existing literature on the prediction Although cross-sectional designs may be useful in es-
of retirement adjustment quality. In achieving this goal, the tablishing correlations between variables, it is difficult to
recent conceptualization of the role that “embeddedness” capture the dynamic nature of the retirement adjustment
plays in an individual’s career adjustment may be particu- process with them. Further, it is difficult to make sound
larly relevant. Embeddedness refers to a number of factors causal inferences on the basis of correlational findings.
contributing to how enmeshed the individual is in his or her Indeed, to test the resource-based dynamic perspective
job, organization, and community (Feldman, 2007). To the we are proposing here, one will need to directly examine
extent that retirees can draw resources from the social how changes in resources over the course of retirement
environment in which they are embedded their retirement adjustment may lead to corresponding changes in retir-
adjustment may be facilitated. ees’ well-being. Modeling retirement adjustment as
Third, although Quick and Moen (1998) have already well-being change trajectories, as Wang (2007) and Pin-
demonstrated that it might be fruitful to systematically quart and Schindler (2007) did, may help researchers
examine the impact of different retirement motivations on directly test this theoretical framework. Therefore, we
retirees’ adjustment quality, few studies have done so. As recommend that future research use more longitudinal
a result, despite the fact that motivation research has made designs to provide more information for understanding
significant theoretical progress in conceptualizing the ad- the causal processes.
justment and coping process as a resource-based self-reg- Another methodological issue in previous retirement
ulatory process (Wang et al., in press), this progress has not adjustment studies is that most studies rely on only a single
been applied to advance our understanding about the re- or limited number of indicators of retirement adjustment
tirement adjustment process. Related motivational theories, quality. However, different adjustment indicators may be
such as affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, sensitive to changes in different types of resources. For
1996) and social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1997), may be example, retirees’ psychological well-being may be more
able to tie the examinations of retirement motivations and sensitive to changes in emotional and cognitive resources,
postretirement activities together and inform the processes whereas their physical well-being may be more sensitive to
that underlie retirement adjustment. changes in physical resources. Therefore, to increase un-
Fourth, although many self-help strategies have been derstanding of the resource-based dynamic process in re-
proposed and described in popular media that aim to im- tirement adjustment, future studies should consider a more
prove adjustment to retirement, little research has been comprehensive view of the indicators of retirement adjust-
conducted to specifically evaluate the effectiveness of these ment.
different self-help strategies. Given that many of these
strategies have roots in established clinical and counseling
Conclusion
psychology practices, we are confident that they would be In summary, in this article we have reviewed both the-
effective in terms of addressing the stress and anxiety oretical and empirical research on the retirement adjust-
associated with the major life change embedded in the ment process. Integrating theories that have been applied
retirement transition. Nevertheless, whether these self-help to the study of retirement adjustment, we propose to
strategies may improve the adjustment to retirement in a conceptualize retirement adjustment as a resource-based
more general sense is still unclear. We urge future re- dynamic process in which retirees’ levels of adjustment
searchers to address this issue in a more systematic way. may fluctuate as a function of resource change. We also
Fifth, few studies have examined the effect of variables summarized the up-to-date knowledge about the general
related to the socioeconomic context on retirement adjust- pattern of retirement adjustment, as well as the predic-
ment. These variables may include labor market demands, tors and consequences of retirement adjustment quality.

April 2011 ● American Psychologist 211


Understanding these empirical findings is particularly model personality traits and the retirement transition: Longitudinal and
important for helping to improve individuals’ adaptation cross-sectional associations. Psychology and Aging, 24, 722–728. doi:
10.1037/a0015121
to retirement and their life quality in retirement. Finally, McArdle, A., Vasilaki, A., & Jackson, M. (2002). Exercise and skeletal
we discussed several future directions for retirement muscle aging: Cellular and molecular mechanisms. Aging Research
adjustment research based on the resource-based dy- Reviews, 1, 79 –93. doi:10.1016/S0047-6374(01)00368-2
namic perspective. It should be noted that the theoretical Mein, G., Martikainen, P., Hemingway, H., Stansfeld, S. A., & Marmot,
M. G. (2003). Is retirement good or bad for mental and physical health
and empirical advancements reviewed in this article are functioning? Whitehall II longitudinal study of civil servants. Journal
bounded by their specific socioeconomic and policy of Epidemiology and Community Health, 57, 46 – 49. doi:10.1136/
contexts. Changes have often occurred in these contexts jech.57.1.46
(e.g., Social Security reform, health care reform, and Midanik, L. T., Soghikian, K., Ransom, L. J., & Tekawa, I. S. (1995). The
early retirement incentive practices), which can alter the effect of retirement on mental health and health behaviors: The Kaiser
Permanente Retirement Study. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sci-
research questions investigators ask (Wang & Shultz, ences, 50B, S59 –S61.
2010). Researchers should be aware of the shift in these Orel, N. A., Ford, R. A., & Brock, C. (2004). Women’s financial planning
contexts when studying these phenomena and should for retirement: The impact of disruptive life events. Journal of Women
also be prepared to tackle problems that emerge in & Aging, 16, 39 –53. doi:10.1300/J074v16n03_04
Park, D. C. (2000). The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

practice. decline in cognitive function. In D. C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.),


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 3–21). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology


REFERENCES Press.
Perreira, K. M., & Sloan, F. A. (2001). Life events and alcohol consump-
Ashforth, B. (2001). Role transitions in organizational life: An identity- tion among mature adults: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Studies
based perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. on Alcohol, 62, 501–508.
Atchley, R. C. (1976). The sociology of retirement. New York, NY: Pinquart, M., & Schindler, I. (2007). Changes of life satisfaction in the
Wiley. transition to retirement: A latent-class approach. Psychology and Aging,
Atchley, R. C. (1999). Continuity theory, self, and social structure. In 22, 442– 455. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.442
C. D. Ryff & V. W. Marshall (Eds.), Families and retirement (pp. Quick, H. E., & Moen, P. (1998). Gender, employment, and retirement
145–158). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. quality: A life course approach to the differential experiences of men
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: and women. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 3, 44 – 64.
Freeman. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.3.1.44
Beck, S. H. (1982). Adjustment to and satisfaction with retirement. Reitzes, D. C., & Mutran, E. J. (2004). The transition into retirement:
Journal of Gerontology, 37, 616 – 624. doi:10.1093/geronj/37.5.616 Stages and factors that influence retirement adjustment. International
Calasanti, T. M. (1996). Gender and life satisfaction in retirement: An Journal of Aging and Human Development, 59, 63– 84. doi:10.2190/
assessment of the male model. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sci- NYPP-RFFP-5RFK-8EB8
ences, 51B, S18 –S29. doi:10.1093/geronb/51B.1.S18 Richardson, V., & Kilty, K. M. (1991). Adjustment to retirement: Conti-
Dave, D., Rashad, I., & Spasojevic, J. (2008). The effects of retirement on nuity vs. discontinuity. International Journal of Aging and Human
physical and mental health outcomes. Southern Economic Journal, 75, Development, 33, 151–169. doi:10.2190/6RPT-U8GN-VUCV-P0TU
497–523. Riley, M. W., & Riley, J. W., Jr. (1994). Structural lag: Past and future.
Dorfman, L. T. (1992). Academics and the transition to retirement. Edu- In M. W. Riley, R. L. Kahn, & A. Foner (Eds.), Age and structural lag:
cational Gerontology, 18, 343–363. doi:10.1080/0360127920180404 The mismatch between people’s lives and opportunities in work, family,
Dorfman, L. T., & Douglas, K. (2005). Leisure and the retired professor: and leisure (pp. 15–36). New York, NY: Wiley.
Occupation matters. Educational Gerontology, 31, 343–361. doi: Schlossberg, N. K. (1981). A model for analyzing human adaptation
10.1080/03601270590921645 to transition. The Counseling Psychologist, 9, 2–18. doi:10.1177/
Feldman, D. C. (1994). The decision to retire early: A review and 001100008100900202
conceptualization. Academy of Management Review, 19, 285–311. doi: Settersten, R. A. (2003). Invitation to the life course: The promise. In
10.2307/258706 R. A. Settersten Jr. (Ed.), Invitation to the life course: Toward new
Feldman, D. C. (2007). Career mobility and career stability among older understandings of later life (pp. 1–12). Amityville, NY: Baywood.
workers. In K. S. Shultz & G. A. Adams (Eds.), Aging and work in the Szinovacz, M. E., & Davey, A. (2004). Honeymoons and joint lunches:
21st century (pp. 179 –197). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Effects of retirement and spouse’s employment on depressive symp-
Gall, T. L., Evans, D. R., & Howard, J. (1997). The retirement adjustment toms. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 59B, P233–
process: Changes in the well-being of male retirees across time. Journal P245.
of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 52B, P110 –P117. van Solinge, H., & Henkens, K. (2005). Couples’ adjustment to retire-
Goodman, J., Schlossberg, N. K., & Anderson, M. L. (2006). Counseling ment: A multi-actor panel study. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sci-
adults in transition. New York, NY: Springer. ences, 60B, S11–S20.
Henkens, K., van Solinge, H., & Gallo, W. T. (2008). Effects of retirement van Solinge, H., & Henkens, K. (2008). Adjustment to and satisfaction
voluntariness on changes in smoking, drinking and physical activity with retirement: Two of a kind? Psychology and Aging, 23, 422– 434.
among Dutch older workers. European Journal of Public Health, 18, doi:10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.422
644 – 649. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckn095 Wang, M. (2007). Profiling retirees in the retirement transition and ad-
Hobfoll, S. E. (2002). Social and psychological resources and adap- justment process: Examining the longitudinal change patterns of retir-
tation. Review of General Psychology, 6, 307–324. doi:10.1037/ ees’ psychological well-being. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92,
1089-2680.6.4.307 455– 474. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.2.455
Kim, J. E., & Moen, P. (2002). Retirement transitions, gender, and Wang, M., Adams, G. A., Beehr, T. A., & Shultz, K. S. (2009). Career
psychological well-being: A life-course, ecological model. Journal of issues at the end of one’s career: Bridge employment and retirement. In
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, P212–P222. doi:10.1093/ S. G. Baugh & S. E. Sullivan (Eds.), Maintaining focus, energy, and
geronb/57.3.P212 options over the life span (pp. 135–162). Charlotte, NC: Information
Kim, S., & Feldman, D. C. (2000). Working in retirement: The anteced- Age.
ents of bridge employment and its consequences for quality of life in Wang, M., & Bodner, T. E. (2007). Growth mixture modeling: Iden-
retirement. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 1195–1210. doi: tifying and predicting unobserved subpopulations with longitudinal
10.2307/1556345 data. Organizational Research Methods, 10, 635– 656. doi:10.1177/
Löckenhoff, C. E., Terracciano, A., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2009). Five-factor 1094428106289397

212 April 2011 ● American Psychologist


Wang, M., Liao, H., Zhan, Y., & Shi, J. (in press). Daily customer theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of
mistreatment and employee sabotage against customers: Examining affective experiences at work. Research in Organizational Behavior,
emotion and resource perspectives. Academy of Management Journal. 18, 1–74.
Wang, M., & Shultz, K. S. (2010). Employee retirement: A review and Westerlund, H., Kivimäki, M., Singh-Manoux, A., Melchior, M., Ferrie,
recommendations for future investigation. Journal of Management, 36, J. E., Pentti, J., . . . Vahtera, J. (2009). Self-rated health before and after
172–206. doi:10.1177/0149206309347957 retirement in France (GAZEL): A cohort study. The Lancet, 374,
Wang, M., & Takeuchi, R. (2007). The role of goal orientation dur- 1889 –1896. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61570-1
ing expatriation: A cross-sectional and longitudinal investigation. Wu, A. M. S., Tang, C. S. K., & Yan, E. C. W. (2005). Post-retirement
Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1437–1445. doi:10.1037/ voluntary work and psychological functioning among older Chinese in
0021-9010.92.5.1437 Hong Kong. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 20, 27– 45. doi:
Wang, M., Zhan, Y., Liu, S., & Shultz, K. (2008). Antecedents of bridge 10.1007/s10823-005-3796-5
employment: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Applied Psychol- Zhan, Y., Wang, M., Liu, S., & Shultz, K. (2009). Bridge employment and
ogy, 93, 818 – 830. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.4.818 retirees’ health: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Occupational
Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A Health Psychology, 14, 374 –389. doi:10.1037/a0015285
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

April 2011 ● American Psychologist 213

You might also like