You are on page 1of 10

Stepparent-Stepchild Relationships in Stepmother and Stepfather Families: A

Multimethod Study
Author(s): W. Glenn Clingempeel, Eulalee Brand and Richard Ievoli
Source: Family Relations , Jul., 1984, Vol. 33, No. 3, Remarriage and Stepparenting (Jul.,
1984), pp. 465-473
Published by: National Council on Family Relations

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/584718

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/584718?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

National Council on Family Relations is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Family Relations

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Stepparent-Stepchild Relationships in
Stepmother and Stepfather Families:
A Multimethod Study*

W. GLENN CLINGEMPEEL, EULALEE BRAND, AND RICHARD IEVOLI**

The present study employed a multimethod-multimeasure-multisource assess-


ment of the quality of the stepparent-stepchild relationship in 16 stepmother and 16
stepfather families (with half of each type (n = 8) having a male and half having a
female, 9-12 year old target child). Data were collected in 31/2 hour home visits. Self-
reports from stepchildren and stepparents and ratings by biological parents all
revealed lower scores on Love and higher scores on Detachment dimensions for
stepparent-stepdaughter relationships. Behavioral measures revealed that girls
emitted less positive verbal and more negative problem-solving behavior toward their
stepparents than did boys. Stepparents did not differ in their responses to boys and
girls on the behavioral measures.

It is becoming increasingly common for Despite increasing numbers of stepfamilies,


American children to experience the divorce empirical research which focuses on step-
and remarriage of their biological parents. The parent-stepchild relationships and child devel-
divorce rate has undergone an unprecedented opment in this emerging family form is surpris-
surge in the United States with 50% of first ingly rare. While research on family responses
marriages now ending in divorce (Weed, 1980) to divorce has proliferated in recent years,
and approximately 60% of these divorces in- divorce has been treated as a terminal rather
volving minor children (Glick, 1980). The rate of than a transitional event (Furstenberg, 1979).
remarriage has almost matched the divorce Furthermore, the extant research on stepfam-
rate with 83% of divorced men and 80% of ilies is fraught with methodological problems
divorced women remarrying (Glick, 1980). (e.g., focusing on one member of the remar-
Moreover, divorces and remarriages are occur- riage family; relying primarily on retrospective
ring earlier in the life cycle and with a decreas- recall; collapsing across rather than differen-
ing intermarriage interval (Furstenberg, 1979). tiating structural types of stepfamilies; and
One consequence of these trends is that more failing to employ multiple-measures of family
children are now living in stepfamilies. By 1990, relationships).
it is estimated that the number of children liv- Several studies have focused on the cogni-
ing in a stepfamily may exceed 7 million or ap- tive and social development of children in step-
proximately 11% of all minor children (Glick, father families, where the child lives with a bio-
1980). logical mother and stepfather (e.g., Chapman,
1977; Oshman & Manosevitz, 1976; Santrock,
1972). Overall, these studies have found that
stepfathers attenuate, at least for boys, the
'This research was supported by Temple University Fac- adverse effects of divorce and single-parent
ulty Senate and Research Incentive Fund Grants.
**W. Gl'enn Clingempeel is an Assistant Professor of Psy-
custody. Chapman (1977) found that male col-
chology and Eulalee Brand is a fourth year doctoral student at lege students from stepfather families and
Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122. Richard levoli is a nuclear families scored higher on a Scholastic
clinical psychologist in private practice in Philadelphia, PA.
Aptitude Test and were more field independent
on the Embedded Figures Test than their
Key Concepts: multi-method study, stepmother families, counterparts in divorced, mother-custody
stepfather families, stepparent-stepchid relationships. families. Santrock (1972) compared 3rd and 6th
grade children and found that the entrance of a
(Family Relations, 1984, 33, 465-473.) stepfather had a positive effect on the

July1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 465

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
cognitive development of boys, but not girls. in stepmothers trying harder than stepfathers
Oshman and Manosevitz (1976) obtained higher to establish close relationships with step-
psychosocial adjustment scores on the Ego children and make the stepfamily resemble a
Identity Scale for male college students from nuclear family-a strategy which may disturb
stepfather families and nuclear families than stepchildren who fear replacement of their
from single-parent families. biological mother.
While empirical studies have examined child Although Cherlin's Incomplete Institution
outcomes in stepfather families, methodologi- hypothesis underscores the greater difficulty
cally adequate studies of stepparent-stepchild of the stepmother role, the Santrock et al.
relationships are virtually nonexistent. One ex- (1982), finding that boys displayed more
ception, Santrock, Warshak, Lindbergh & warmth toward their stepfathers than girls sug-
Meadows (1982) compared parent-child and gests the quality of the stepparent-stepchild
stepparent-stepchild interactions (using relationship may depend upon the sex of step-
trained observer evaluations of videotaped in- child. Moreover, studies of divorced and
teraction tasks) in stepfather, nuclear, and di- nuclear families have generally found that op-
vorced mother-custody families. They found posite sex, biological parent-child relation-
that boys displayed more warmth than girls ships are more problematic (Margolin & Patter-
toward their stepfathers. Furthermore, girls, son, 1975; Santrock & Warshak, 1979). How-
but not boys, were rated as more anxious in ever, no multimethod studies have examined
stepfather families than in nuclear families. stepparent-stepchild relationships in step-
No methodologically adequate studies have mother and stepfather families. Nor have any
examined stepparent-stepchild relationships methodologically adequate studies focused on
and child development in stepmother families, whether stepmothers have more difficult rela-
where the child lives with a biological father tionships with children of both sexes or
and stepmother. Since custody dispositions whether relationship quality varies with the sex
are awarded solely to the mother in 90% of of stepchild or the sex of stepparent-sex of
divorce cases (Sanders & Spanier, 1979), step- stepchild interaction. The current research at-
mother families are relatively rare. However, tempts to fill this gap in the literature.
there is evidence that more men are now seek- The present study employs a multimethod-
ing custody of children (Bodenheimer, 1977), multimeasure-multisource assessment of the
and consequently, the percentage of step- stepparent-stepchild relationship in step-
mother families may be increasing. While there mother and stepfather families. The "target"
is some evidence that stepfathers establish children in this research are between 9 and 12
closer relationships with their stepchildren years of age. This age range was chosen for
than stepmothers (Benson, 1968; Bowerman & two reasons. First, there is some evidence that
Irish, 1962; Duberman, 1973, 1975), these preadolescent, school age children are espe-
studies are replete with methodological prob- cially vulnerable to the stresses associated
lems (e.g., reliance on single-method ap- with remarriage of the residential parent
proaches and retrospective recall). (Wallerstein & Kelley, 1980). Nine- to 12-year-
An extrapolation from Cherlin's (1978) "in- olds may be more likely than younger children
complete institution" hypothesis would sug- to have established a close relationship with
gest that stepmothers may have more difficulty the nonresidential parent, but are less likely
relating to stepchildren than stepfathers. Ac- than adolescents to have established an ex-
cording to Cherlin, stepfamilies are more un- trafamilial support system. Second, this age
stable (with a higher rate of divorce) than nu- group, in comparison with infants and pre-
clear families because they are confronted schoolers, has been understudied in the devel-
with ambiguous social roles for which society opmental literature.
offers no institutional guidelines (e.g., there Stepfather families rather than nuclear fami-
are no clear role prescriptions to delineate lies were selected as the comparison group for
childrearing obligations of stepparent). In com- three reasons. First, the research question ad-
parison with the role of stepfather, cultural and dressed is not whether stepfamilies differ from
social biases probably have inculcated the nuclear families. Instead, the focus is on
stepmother role with fewer institutional guide- whether stepmother families with putatively
lines and greater difficulties (Visher & Visher, fewer societal role prescriptions and support
1978, 1979). Since stepmother families are systems will have lower quality stepparent-
relatively rare, stepmothers have fewer of their stepchild relationships. Second, the
own kind to use as social support and as a prevalence and heterogeneity of stepfamilies
basis for comparison in creating their role. In justify more fine-grained studies which com-
addition, the stepmother role may have less so- pare structural types and examine the
cietal acceptance since potential mother sub- "effects" of structural variations. Stepfamilies
stitutes are more alien than father substitutes have become the most common "alternative
to cultural norms which have established the lifestyle" in the United States (Spanier &
mother as "superior" parent (Weiss, 1979). Fur- Furstenberg, in press). Furthermore, stepfam-
thermore, these same social biases may result ilies are heterogeneous family forms and vary

466 FAMILY RELATIONS July1984

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
on a number of structural dimensions (e.g., sex and marital status criteria. However, only 900
of residential stepparent, presence or absence couples (53%) had addresses listed in current
of biological children of residential stepparent) telephone books and thus received letters of
which may differentially impact upon in- inquiry. Thirty-one percent (275 couples) re-
trafamilial relationships (cf. Clingempeel, turned RIFs and 29% of this group (81 couples)
1981). Third, the use of a nuclear family com- also met the children and custody arrangement
parison group would result in mismatches on criteria. Of the 81 couples who met both sets of
structural variables naturally associated with criteria, 69 were stepfather families and 12
family type. For example, matching step- were stepmother families.
families and nuclear families on age of target In an attempt to recruit additional step-
child necessarily unmatches these groups on mother families, advertisements were placed in
length of time target child has lived in the cur- local newspapers. Ten stepmother families
rent family situation and the total time in a two who met all criteria responded to these ads.
parent family (target children in stepfamilies Thus, 22 stepmother families (12 from Marriage
have experienced a period of single parent- License Records and 10 from newspaper ads)
hood). were recruited. However, 6 families refused to
The primary goal of this study is to describe participate, and thus the final sample con-
the characteristics of stepparent-stepchild re- sisted of 16 stepmother families.
lationships in stepmother and stepfather fam- From the pool of 69 stepfather families,
ilies. Independent perceptions of positive and those that best matched the 16 stepmother
negative dimensions of the stepparent-step- families (half with boys and half with girls as
child relationship were obtained from parents, target children) on age and sex of target child,
stepparents, and stepchildren. In addition, pro- number of custodial children, and length of
portions of stepparent and stepchild positive custodial parent's remarriage were asked to
and negative communication behaviors were participate. The final sample of 16 stepfather
derived from home-based videotapes of discus- families were obtained after 22 stepfather fam-
sion tasks. ilies were contacted and 6 had refused to par-
ticipate.
Thus, the present study includes two struc-
Method
tural types of stepfamilies:

Subject Recruitment
(1) Stepfather families, where a woman is a
Sixteen stepmother and 16 stepfather custodial parent of at least one child (age
families were recruited from the Marriage 9-12) and is in a second marriage with a
License Records of Philadelphia and Norris- man who was either never married, or is
town, Pennsylvania, and from newspaper ad- also in a second marriage, but does not
vertisements. Half of each structural type have children from the prior marriage.
(n = 8) had a 9 to 12 year old boy as target child (2) Stepmother families, in which a man is a
and half had a 9 to 12 year old girl. Prospective custodial parent of at least one child
subjects were sent an introductory letter (ages 9-12) and is in a second marriage
describing the project, and a Remarriage Infor- with a woman who was either never mar-
mation Form (RIF) inquiring about the pres- ried, or is also in a second marriage, but
ence or absence of children, their age and sex, does not have children from the previous
and present custody arrangements. Couples marriage.
were requested to mail back the RIF irrespec-
tive of their decision to participate. Description of Subjects

Stepparent couples who met the following Means and standard deviations for step-
criteria were contacted by telephone and asked father and stepmother families with boy and
to participate: (a) one or both spouses were girl target children were obtained on the follow-
currently in a second marriage and the pre- ing characteristics: (a) stepparent age (step-
vious marriage was dissolved by divorce; (b) father-boy, M = 34.4 years, SD = 5.1 years;
both spouses were under 45 years of age, had a stepfather-girl, M = 30.1 years, SD = 4.5
minimum of a high school education, and a years; stepmother-boy, M = 30.9 years,
total annual income of at least $15,000; (c) the SD = 6.1 years; stepmother-girl, M = 29.4
present marriage was 6-24 months old; (d) only years, SD = 5.9 years); (b) stepparent formal
one spouse had custody of children from the education (stepfather-boy M = 14.6 years,
prior marriage, of which one child was 9 to 12 SD = 3.5 years; stepfather-girl, M = 14.9
years old; and (e) there were no children from years, SD = 3.6 years; stepmother-boy,
the current marriage. All families were paid $15 M = 15.9 years, SD = 2.9 years; stepmother-
for their participation and were promised a girl, M = 14.8 years, SD = 2.8 years); (c) length
summary of the major findings of this re- of parents' previous marriage (stepfather-boy,
search. M = 92 months, SD = 42.5 months; step-
Summing across two Marriage License father-girl, M = 97.6 months, SD = 49.4
Bureaus, 1700 couples met the demographic months; stepmother-boy, M = 112.3 months,

July 1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 467

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
SD = 52.9 months; stepmother-girl, M = 95 age and educational level of parents/step-
months, SD = 33.3 months); (d) length of time parents, annual income), marital history (e.g.,
between parents' final physical separation and length of previous marriage, intermarriage in-
remarriage (stepfather-boy, M = 56.8 months, terval, time spent dating current spouse, and
SD = 18.4 months; stepfather-girl, M = 58.6 current marriage), and family size/structure
months, SD = 30.5 months; stepmother-boy, characteristics (e.g., number, age, and sex of
M = 44 months, SD = 26.1 months; step- custodial children). In addition, the parent and
mother-girl, M = 40.1 months, SD = 24.4 stepparent collaborated in recording the total
months); and (e) length of current marriage number of days containing a visit from the
(stepfather-boy, M = 8.5 months, SD = 1.7 nonresidential parent for each of the last six
months; stepfather-girl, M = 9.5 months, months.' Test-retest reliabilities on the total
SD = 2.5 months; stepmother-boy, M = 14.2 frequency of visits in 6 months were obtained
months, SD = 2.6 months; stepmother-girl, between 1 and 2 months after the home visit on
M = 14.6 months, SD = 8.9 months). A series 15 randomly selected families. The Pearson
of 2 x 2 ANOVAS (with an .05 alpha level ad- correlation of time 1 and time 2 frequencies
justed for experimentwise error rates) revealed was .71.
that the four groups did not differ significantly
Dependent Measures
on any of these social-demographic or marital
status variables. Child Report of Stepparent Behavior Inven-
The total annual income of stepparent tory. A modified version of Schaefer's (1965)
couples (including gains or losses from child original 192 item, 18 scale inventory was
support payments) ranged from $20,000 to reworded to focus on the child's relationship
$29,999 for 12 families (stepfather = 8; step- with the stepparent rather than the parent. This
mother = 4), $30,000 to $39,999 for 8 families measure requested that the child indicate for
(stepfather = 5; stepmother = 3) and over 64 specific stepparent behaviors (e.g., s/he
$40,000 for 12 families (stepfather = 3; step- makes me feel better after talking over my wor-
mother = 9). A 2 x 3 cross-tabulation with type ries with him/her), whether each item is "like,"
of stepfamily by income group yielded a non- "somewhat like," or "not like" his/her step-
significant chi-square revealing similar parent. Four scales from the Love-Hostility di-
distributions of stepmother and stepfather mension including Acceptance, Positive In-
families across the three income groups. volvement, Rejection, and Hostile Detachment
Seven stepmothers and seven stepfathers were used because these scales have yielded
were previously divorced, and nine step- high internal consistency reliabilities
mothers and nine stepfathers were previously (Schaefer, 1965 and personal communication,
single. A series of t-tests performed separately 1982) and because they were considered most
for each family type on the dependent relevant to qualitative aspects of the
measures revealed no differences as a function stepparent-stepchild relationship. In addition,
of prior marital status. The subgroups were a Principal Component Analysis with a varimax
thus combined for the major analyses. rotation revealed high loadings on Factor 1 for
In stepmother families, six couples had these scales (Acceptance = .88, Positive In-
custody of one child, eight couples had volvement = .95, Rejection = -.57, Hostile
custody of two children, and two couples had Detachment = -.60). Two scores were derived
custody of three children. In stepfather from the four scales: (1) Child Love, which in-
families, six couples had one, seven couples cluded the 16 item Acceptance and 16 item
had two, and three couples had custody of Positive Involvement scales; and (2) Child
three children. A 2 x 2 ANOVA revealed a signi- Detachment, which included the 16 item Rejec-
ficant difference on one potentially important tion and 16 item Hostile Detachment scales.
variable. Children in stepmother families saw Parent and Stepparent Report of Child
their nonresidential biological parents signifi- Behavior toward the Stepparent Inventory. The
cantly more than children in stepfather 40 item, short version of the Parent Report of
families, F (1, 28) = 4.52, p < .05. Moreover, Child Behavior Inventory (Schaefer, Edgarton &
boys in both stepmother and stepfather Finkelstein, 1979) was reworded to focus on
families saw their nonresidential parents more the stepparent's (rather than the biological
than girls, although this difference only ap- parent's) relationship with the target child. Two
proached significance, F (1, 28) = 3.22,
p = .08.
'The parent and stepparent recorded on a chart the number
Measurement of Social Demographic. of days containing a visit for each of the last 6 months. In
Characteristics and Visitation Patterns recording monthly frequencies, subjects started with the most
recent month and moved backward to the most distant month.
Social Demographic and Marital History Subjects were asked to consider both the regular visitation
Questionnaire. This questionnaire, con- schedule and any special events (e.g., birthdays, holidays,
sickness of nonresidential parent). After the number of days
structed specifically for this research, included
containing a visit were recorded for each of the 6 months, par-
40 questions with fixed alternative response ticipants were asked to sum the monthly frequencies and
categories and focused on demographic (e.g., record a grand total.

468 FAMILY RELATIONS July 1984

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
forms were developed with one form obtaining female). Interviewers ranged in age from 24 to
parent ratings and the other stepparent self- 35 years and were Caucasian, doctoral
reports. Except for the minor rewordings, the students in clinical psychology.
items and response format were identical to Upon arriving at the homes of stepfamilies,
the original inventory. Parents and stepparents the interviewers spent about 30 minutes
were asked to indicate on a 5 point Likert Scale establishing rapport, answering questions
whether specific child behaviors (e.g., s/he about the study, setting up video equipment,
tells his/her stepparent about things that and obtaining signatures on a consent form.
bother him/her) were "not at all like" to "very Then the parent and stepparent met with one
much like" the target child's interactions with interviewer and the target child met with a sec-
his/her stepparent. ond interviewer (who was always the same sex
The 40 item form has 17 scales which cluster as the target child) in a separate room of the
into six factors. The 11 item Love factor (which house. The spouses collaborated on the Social
includes the confiding, demonstrates com- Demographic and Marital History Question-
petence, initiates shared activities, and active naire and then independently completed the
concern scales) and the 5 item Detachment stepparent and parent forms of the Parent
factor (which includes the lack of involvement Report of Child Behavior Inventory. The inter-
and resistance to involvement scales) were viewer of the child administered the Child
employed as dependent measures. Schaefer Report of Stepparent Behavior Inventory. For
(Personal communication, 1982) indicated that this measure, the interviewer read aloud each
internal consistency reliabilities for these two item and recorded the child's verbal responses
factors are: Love = .88; and Detach- directly on the questionnaire.
ment = .77. Thus, the parent and stepparent The stepparent and stepchild were also
forms yielded four measures of the quality of asked to engage in two discussion tasks that
the stepparent-stepchild relationship: Step- were videotaped. The order of administering
parent self-reports of Love and Detachment; the questionnaire measures and the video-
parent ratings of Love and Detachment. taped interaction tasks was counterbalanced
Behavioral Observations: Family Problem across the four groups to control for sequence
Solving System (FPSS). A Modified FPSS effects. The first discussion task served as a
(Forgatch & Wieder, 1981) consisting of 30 ver- "warm-up" (and was designed to facilitate
bal and 13 nonverbal codes was used in the habituation to the video equipment), had a 5
behavioral coding of videotaped stepparent- minute time limit, and required the stepparent
stepchild interactions obtained during a single and stepchild to plan an activity together. The
visit to the homes of stepfamilies. The 43 second task had a 10 minute time limit and re-
codes are subsumed under the following sum- quired the participants to discuss changes
mary categories: they would like to see in their family. The
(A) Verbal Behavior Categories "discuss changes" task generated the discus-
(1) Positive Problem Solving: accept sion that was coded using the Family Problem
responsibility, contingent, closure, Solving System. The "plan activity" task was
countersolution, hypothetical, promise, chosen as a warm-up and was always present-
and positive solution. ed first because it has previously been found to
(2) Negative Problem Solving:, deny, de- elicit less anxiety (Lewis, Beavers, Gossett &
mand, don't, morality lesson, and non- Phillips, 1976). The interviewer gave instruc-
cooperate. tions for one task at a time and always left the
(3) Positive Verbal: agree, approve, comply, room during the discussions and did not return
defend other, empathize, humor, and until the time limit had expired (or until par-
restate. ticipants signaled completion). Discussions
(4) Negative Verbal: blame, command, always took place in an isolated room of the
complain, criticize, disagree, disqualify, house in an effort to minimize reactivity and to
guilt trip, leading question, mind read, control for differences due to the presence of
threat, and why. other family members.
(B) Nonverbal Behavior Categories
Coding, Reliability, and Scoring of FPSS
(1) Positive Nonverbal: laugh, lean forward,
positive gesture, and touch positive. The 32 videotapes of stepparent-stepchild
(2) Mildly Aversive Nonverbal: distract, interactions were sent to the Oregon Social
head hang, not track, slouch, and voice Learning Center in Eugene, Oregon (where the
bad. FPSS was developed and much of the reliabili-
(3) Negative Nonverbal: destructive, ty and validity research conducted). At Oregon,
hostile gesture, touch negative, and highly trained coders blind to the hypotheses
wander. under investigation independently coded the
videotapes using the 30 verbal and 13 non-
Procedure
verbal codes. Verbal codes were recorded con-
The data were collected in a single 31/2-hour tinuously in 15 second intervals and sequen-
home visit by two interviewers (one male, one tially from left to right. Nonverbal codes were

July 1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 469

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
coded for occurrence every 15 seconds with- child positive verbal behaviors (stepchild self-
out regard for duration or frequency. report, r = .30, p < .05; parent rating, r = .41, p
Nine videotapes were coded by two coders < .01). Significant negative correlations were
as reliability checks. Intercoder reliabilities obtained between stepparent reports of Love
(agreements/agreements + disagreements) x and the proportion of stepchild negative pro-
100 ranged from 71% to 97% for nonverbal blem solving (r = -.30, p < .05), and between
codes (M = 83%) and 69% to 85% for verbal parent ratings of Love and the proportion of
codes (M = 75%). stepchild negative verbal behaviors, r = -.32, p
Frequencies of stepparent and stepchild < .05. The parent rating of Detachment cor-
behaviors in the four verbal summary related positively with the proportion of step-
categories (positive verbal, positive problem child negative verbal behaviors, r = .30, p <
solving, negative verbal, negative problem solv- .05. Stepchild positive problem solving and
ing) and the three nonverbal categories positive nonverbal behaviors did not correlate
(positive nonverbal, mildly aversive nonverbal, significantly with any of the questionnaire
negative nonverbal) were first adjusted for measures.
between-dyad variations in total discussion A correlation matrix of the questionnaire
time by calculating rates of behaviors per measures and the stepparent behavioral
minute for each category. Second, rates of the measures also revealed only five significant
four verbal categories were expressed as pro- correlations. The parent perceptions of Love in
portions of total verbal behaviors per minute, the stepparent-stepchild relationship cor-
and the rate of positive nonverbal behaviors related positively with the proportion of step-
was expressed as a proportion of total nonver- parent positive verbal (r = .43, p < .01) and
bal behaviors per minute. The conversion of positive nonverbal behaviors (r = .36, p < .05)
rates to proportions was designed to control and negatively with the proportion of step-
for potential differences in basal activity level parent negative problem solving behaviors,
of stepchildren (which could result in higher r = -.46, p < .01. The parent rating of Detach-
rates of both positive and negative behavior ment correlated negatively with the proportion
but not reflect differences in the quality of of stepparent positive verbal behavior
communications). (r = -.35, p < .05) and positively with step-
parent negative problem solving, r = .35, p <
Results
.05. No other correlations were significant.
Intercorrelation of Dependent Measures
Multivariate Analyses:
Six dependent measures were derived from
Questionnaire Measures
the Schaefer inventories including parent,
stepparent, and target child perceptions of the Two 2 x 2 MANOVAS were performed on
Love and Detachment dimensions of the step- questionnaire measures. One MANOVA was
parent-stepchild relationship. A correlation performed on the three positive measures of
matrix of these measures revealed that all 15 the stepparent-stepchild relationship (Step-
correlations were significant and in expected parent Love, Parent Love, Child Love) and a
directions. Positive correlations ranged from separate MANOVA was performed on the three
.35 to .58, and negative correlations ranged negative measures (Stepparent Detachment,
from -.37 to -.81. All correlations were signifi- Parent Detachment, Child Detachment). On
cant at p < .01 except for child detachment- both positive and negative dimensions the
parent detachment (r = .35, p < .05) and multivariate F's for the type of stepfamily main
parent love-child detachment (r = -.37, p < effect and the sex of target child-type of step-
.05). Positive correlations were obtained be- family interaction were not significant.
tween child, parent and stepparent reports of However, significant multivariate F's for the
the Love dimension. Reports of the Detach- sex of target child were obtained on the three
ment dimension from all three sources were positive measures (F (3, 26) = 3.46, p = .03)
also significantly correlated. Significant and on the three negative measures, F (3,
negative correlations were obtained in all three 26) = 4.50, p = .01. Boys were reported to
cases of within-person Love-Detachment cor- have significantly higher quality relationships
relations (e.g., child love with child detach- with both stepmothers and stepfathers.
ment) and all six cases of between-person Moreover, univariate F's revealed significant
Love-Detachment correlations (e.g., child love differences in favor of boys on all six depen-
with stepparent detachment). dent measures. When the target child was a
A correlation matrix of the parent, step- boy, parents, stepparents, and stepchildren
parent and stepchild questionnaire measures reported significantly higher scores on Love
and stepchild behavioral measures revealed and lower scores on Detachment. Table 1
only 5 significant correlations. Significant presents means and standard deviations on
positive correlations were obtained between the questionnaire dependent measures for
stepchild self-reports and parent ratings of boys
the and girls in stepmother and stepfather
Love dimension and the proportion of step- families.

470 FAMILY RELATIONS July 1984

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Table 1.
Means and Standard Deviations of Stepfather and Stepmother Families on Questionnaire Depen-
dent Measures

Stepmother Stepfather
Boys Girls Boys Girls
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Love Dimension
Child 87.63 7.00 70.88 22.93 83.13 8.20 72.00 11.86
Stepparent 42.63 5.73 34.50 12.68 42.13 7.70 33.75 5.57
Parent 41.75 6.25 34.63 9.27 43.00 6.30 37.00 7.84
Detachment Dimension
Child 38.88 4.16 51.50 16.89 44.13 7.30 53.13 15.80
Stepparent 7.00 2.98 11.88 4.91 8.00 3.12 11.50 3.51
Parent 6.38 1.69 10.63 3.54 7.38 3.33 9.25 3.11

Multivariate and Univariate Analyses: Discussion


Behavioral Measures

Prior to the major analyses, arc sine trans- The major finding of this research is that
formations were performed on all proportional stepparent-stepdaughter relationships in both
data; 2 x 2 (type of stepfamily by sex of target stepmother and stepfather families were more
child) MANOVAs were performed separately problematic than stepparent-stepson relation-
for stepparents and stepchildren on the two ships. Self-reports from stepchildren and step-
positive summary categories of verbal behavior parents and ratings by biological parents all re-
(positive verbal, positive problem solving) and vealed lower scores on Love and higher scores
the two negative summary categories of verbal on Detachment dimensions for stepparent-
behavior (negative verbal, negative problem stepdaughter relationships. Behavioral mea-
solving). In addition, 2 x 2 ANOVAs were per- sures derived from the structured interaction
formed on the proportion of positive nonverbal tasks revealed that girls emitted a lower pro-
behaviors emitted by stepparents and stepchil- portion of positive verbal and a higher propor-
dren. tion of negative problem solving behavior
For stepparents, multivariate F's for the type toward their stepparents than did boys. Step-
of stepfamily-sex of target child interaction parents, however, did not differ in their re-
and the type of stepfamily and sex of target sponses to boys and girls on any of the be-
child main effects revealed no significant dif- havioral measures.
ferences on the positive and negative verbal It is possible that this "sex of the child" ef-
behavior categories. Moreover, the univariate F fect is not unique to stepfamilies and would
revealed no differences on the proportion of have been obtained with a sample of biological
positive nonverbal behaviors emitted by step- parents and children. However, the extant
parents. research on divorced and nuclear families has
For stepchildren, no multivariate F's were yielded no evidence that girls have more diffi-
significant for the type of stepfamily-sex of cult or lower quality relationships with both
target child interaction or the type of stepfam- mothers and fathers. Instead, studies have
ily main effect. However, significant multi- generally found that opposite sex parent-child
variate F's for the sex of target child were ob- relationships are more difficult (Hetherington,
tained on both the positive summary category Cox, & Cox, 1978; Margolin & Patterson, 1975;
of verbal behavior, F (2, 27) = 3.44, p < .05, and Santrock & Warshak, 1979).
the negative summary category of verbal be- The finding that girls had more difficulty re-
havior, F (2, 27) = 3.59, p < .05. The univariate lating to stepfathers than boys is consistent
F's revealed that girls engaged in a significant- with the results of Santrock et al. (1982). Since
ly lower proportion of positive verbal behaviors the mother-son relationship after divorce is
(girls, M = .23, SD = .13; boys, M = .34, SD = often more problematic than the mother-
.13), F (1, 28) = 5.25, p < .05, and a significantly daughter (Hetherington, Cox, & Cox, 1978),
higher proportion of negative problem solving boys may welcome the same sex parent figure
behaviors (girls, M = .22, SD = .11; boys, M = while girls may fear a stepfather will disrupt
.15, SD = .08), F (1,28) = 4.07, p < .05. Girls the mother-daughter bond. Remarriage may
and boys did not differ on the proportion of also constitute a greater threat to a girl's rela-
positive problem solving and negative verbal tionship with her nonresidential father. In this
behaviors. The univariate F for proportion of study, nonresidential fathers visited boys more
positive nonverbal behaviors revealed no sig- than twice as often as girls, and this finding is
nificant differences for the two-way interaction consistent with other research (e.g., Hess &
or the two main effects. Camara, 1979).

July 1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 471

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
The finding that the stepmother-stepdaugh- Explanations for the findings of this study
ter relationship was more problematic was not are necessarily speculative. This research was
expected. Since previous research had found cross-sectional and correlational and has the
that boys exhibit more competent social be- concomitant limitations (e.g., third variable and
havior than girls in father custody, single directionality problems in interpreting results,
parent arrangements (Santrock & Warshak, nonequivalence of groups due to unknown co-
1979), it was assumed that boys would have variates, ignoring time-related changes in intra-
greater resentment for the potentially intruding familial relationships). In addition, small sam-
stepmother. ple sizes limit the generalizability of findings,
One explanation for this finding stems from different methods of recruiting stepmother and
prevailing biases in child custody decision- stepfather families may have engendered
making which seldom result in fathers obtain- selection biases, and the absence of a single
ing custody of children, and especially girls parent comparison group precluded an assess-
(Sanders & Spanier, 1979). Fathers may be ment of the "effects" of remarriage. Future
more likely to obtain custody of girls in situa- studies of stepmother and stepfather families
tions where the father-daughter relationship is should use longitudinal designs, mother
unusually close and the mother-daughter rela- custody and father custody comparison
tionship is unusually distant. Consequently, groups, and multiple methods and measures of
the remarriage of the father and the entry of a the target child's relationships with both
stepmother into the household may be per- biological parents and stepparent.
ceived by girls as a major threat to the father-
daughter relationship. Furthermore, the fre-
quency of visits from nonresidential mothers
revealed the same sex of child difference ob-
served in stepfather families. Girls were visited
less by their nonresidential mothers than boys. REFERENCES

Boys may feel more secure that the step-


Benson, L. (1968). Fatherhood. New York: Random House.
mother will augment, rather than supplant, the Bodenheimer, B. M. (1977). Progress under the Uniform Child
biological mother. Custody Jurisdiction Act and remaining problems: Punitive
The expectation that both boys and girls decrees, joint custody, and excessive modifications. Cali-
fornia Law Review, 65, 978-1014.
would have greater difficulty relating to step-
Bowerman, C. D., & Irish, D. P. (1962). Some relationships of
mothers was not supported by this research. stepchildren to their parents. Marriage and Family Living,
Extrapolating from Cherlin's incomplete insti- 24, 113-131.
Chapman, M. (1977). Father absence, stepfathers, and the cog-
tutional hypothesis, we had expected that
nitive performance of college students. Child Development,
stepmothers might confront greater ambigui- 48, 1155-1158.
ties regarding their stepparent role than step- Cherlin, A. (1978). Remarriage as an incomplete institution.
fathers and, as a result, stepmother-stepchild American Journal of Sociology, 84, 634-650.
Clingempeel, W. G. (1981). Quasi-kin relationships and marital
relationships might be plagued with greater quality in stepfather families. Journal of Personality and
difficulties. There are at least three explana- Social Psychology, 41, 890-901.
tions for the lack of support for this hypothe- Duberman, L. (1973). Step-kin relationships. Journal of Mar-
riage and the Family, 35, 283-292.
sis: (1) this sample of stepmothers did not
Duberman, L. (1975). The reconstituted family: A study of re-
experience greater role ambiguity; (2) step- married couples and their children. Chicago: Nelson-Hall
mothers experienced greater ambiguities but Publishers.

coped with them in relatively effective ways; Forgatch, M. S., & Wieder, G. (1981). Family problem solving
system. Unpublished Manuscript.
and (3) role ambiguity does not significantly in- Furstenberg, F. F. (1979). Recycling the family: Perspectives
fluence stepparent-stepchild relationships. for researching a neglected family form. Marriage and Fam-
However, these are all speculations. This re- ily Review, 2, 1-22.
Glick, P. C. (1980). Remarriage: Some recent changes and vari-
search did not include a direct measure of role
ations. Journal of Family Issues, 1, 455-478.
ambiguity, and thus no conclusions can be of- Hess, R. D., & Camara, K. A. (1979). Post-divorce relationships
fered regarding the impact of this factor on as mediating factors in the consequences of divorce for

stepparent-stepchild relationships. children. Journal of Social Issues, 35, 79-96.


Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. (1978). The aftermath
The finding that stepparent-stepdaughter of divorce. In J. H. Stevens, Jr., & M. Matthews (Eds.),
relationships were more problematic may be Mother-child, father-child relations (pp. 149-176).
unique to children in the 9-12 year age range. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of
Young Children.
Children of different ages vary in terms of the
Lewis, J., Beavers, W., Gossett, J., & Phillips, V. (1976). No
settings in which they are regularly immersed, single thread: Psychological health in family systems. New
the nature of developmental tasks they en- York: Brunner/Mazel.
Margolin, G., & Patterson, G. R. (1975). Differential conse-
counter, and their social and cognitive compe-
quences provided by mothers and fathers for their sons and
tencies. While age differences in response to daughters. Developmental Psychology, 11 (4) 537-538.
divorce have been documented (Wallerstein & Oshman, H. P., & Manosevitz, M. (1976). Father absence: Ef-
Kelly, 1980), empirical studies which examine fects of stepfathers upon psychosocial development in
males. Developmental Psychology, 12, 479-480.
how children of different ages cope with remar-
Sanders, R., & Spanier, G. B. (1979). Divorce, child custody and
riage of the residential parent are virtually non- child support. Current Population Reports, U.S. Bureau of
existent. the Census, Series P-23, No. 84.

472 FAMILY RELATIONS July 1984

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Santrock, J. W. (1972). Relation of type and onset of father
absence to cognitive development. Child Development, 43,
455-469.
Santrock, J. W., & Warshak, R. A. (1979). Father custody and
social development in boys and girls. The Journal of Social
Issues, 35, 112-125.
Santrock, J. W., Warshak, R., Linbergh, C., & Meadows, L.
(1982). Children's and parents' observed social behavior in
stepfather families. Child Development, 53, 472-480.
Schaefer, E. (1965). Parent Behavior Inventory. Bethesda, MD:
National Institutes of Health.
Schaefer, E., Edgarton, M., & Finkelstein, N. (1979). Relation-
ship Inventory for Families: Parent-child form. Chapel Hill,

You Child
NC: Carolina Institute for Research on Early Education of
the Handicapped, University of North Carolina.
Spanier, G. B., & Furstenberg, F. F. (in press). Remarriage and
reconstituted families. In M. B. Sussman and S. K. Stein-
metz (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family. New
York: Plenum.
Visher, E. B., & Visher, J. S. (1978). Major areas of difficulty for Practical Suggestionsfor
stepparent couples. International Journal of Family Coun-
seling, 6 (2), 70-80. Parents
Visher, E. B., & Visher, J. B. (1979). Stepfamilies: A guide to
working with stepparents and stepchildren. New York: Brun-
ner/Mazel.
Sonja Goldstein, LL.B.,
Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. (1980). Surviving the breakup: and Albert J. Solnit, M.D.
How children actually cope with divorce. New York: Basic
Books. Expert, practical advice from two renowned
Weed, J. A. National estimates of marriage, dissolution, and
survivorship. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 3. Washing-
authorities - a lawyer and a child psychiatrist
ton, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, DHHS Publica- on how parents can help their children deal
tions.
with the difficulties caused by family
Weiss, R. (1979). The adjudication of custody when parents
separate. In G. Levinger and 0. Moles (Eds.), Divorce.and dissolution. In their discussion of custodial
separation: Context, causes, and consequences (pp. arrangements, visitation, remarriage, and
324-336). New York: Basic Books.
stepparenting, Goldstein and Solnit
emphasize not only the options open to
parents but also the possible psychological
and emotional repercussions on the children
of decisions made by parents and other adults.
An invaluable resource for parents and
professionals alike.

"A very helpful and valuable book."


-Dr. Lee Salk

"Every caring, responsible, divorced parent


should become well-acquainted with the
fund of invaluable information and practical
suggestions to be found in this lucid,
compassionate book."
-Maggie Scarf

"An excellent book!" -Anna Freud

Please send me copies of


Divorce and Your Child at $12.9
My check for is enclosed. Add $1.50
for postage/handling (plus7.S% tax in CT).
Or charge MasterCard VISA
Account #

Expiration date

Signature
Name
Street Cit%_

State Zip

Yale University Press


Dept. 616J, 92A Yale Station, New Haven, Connecticut 06520

July 1984 FAMILY RELATIONS 473

This content downloaded from


157.193.240.141 on Thu, 06 Jan 2022 13:08:48 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like