You are on page 1of 11

526269

research-article2014
JRA0010.1177/1470320314526269Journal of the Renin-Angiotensin-Aldosterone SystemWincewicz and Braszko

Original Article

Journal of the Renin-Angiotensin-

Angiotensin II AT1 receptor blockade Aldosterone System


2015, Vol. 16(3) 495­–505

by telmisartan reduces impairment of


© The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
spatial maze performance induced by DOI: 10.1177/1470320314526269
jra.sagepub.com

both acute and chronic stress

Dominik Wincewicz and Jan J Braszko

Abstract
Introduction: Despite recognition of stress as a causation of severe neuropsychological dysfunctions, no casual and
clinically effective anti-stress therapeutic strategy has yet been found. We have previously shown that blockade of initial
stress response by angiotensin receptor blockers alleviates the negative effect of prolonged stress on cognitive non-spatial
functions of rats. Here we aimed to find whether telmisartan reduces stress-related memory decline in spatial hippocampal-
dependent learning tasks conditioned upon differences in level of stress induced by aversive nature of memory tests.
Methods: Male Wistar rats were exposed to chronic restraint stress for three weeks and daily treated with either
vehicle or telmisartan (1 mg/kg). Afterwards rats were tested in three spatial learning and memory paradigms: Morris
water maze (MWM), radial arm maze (RAM), and Barnes maze (BM).
Results: Stressed animals demonstrated significantly impaired performance in all the tests, which was normalized in the
animals stressed and treated with telmisartan. Interestingly, despite the fact that MWM and RAM are more stressful,
which affects animal behavior, therefore considered less sensitive than BM, more significant effect of telmisartan was
found in MWM and RAM than BM.
Conclusions: AT1 angiotensin receptor blockade attenuates negative effect of both acute and chronic stress on spatial
memory.

Keywords
Angiotensin II, Barnes maze, Morris water maze, radial arm maze, stress, telmisartan

Date received: 5 September 2013; accepted: 26 January 2014

Abbreviations However, this is an example of adaptive behavior in


healthy individuals and deregulation of the underlying
ACE-I Angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibitor mechanism might result in cognitive decline.3 Chronic and
Ang II Angiotensin II acute stress modulates learning and memory processes,
ANOVA I One-way analysis of variance acquisition, consolidation, retention, and retrieval.
ARB Angiotensin II type 1 receptor blocker Prolonged experience of continuous physiological arousal
AT1 Angiotensin II type 1 receptor negatively affects cognitive performance.4 Acute stimulus
BM Barnes maze might enhance memory performance,5 but acute intense
CNS Central nervous system event in previously stressed subjects results in amplifica-
HPA Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis tion of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis deregu-
LTP Long-term potentiation lation, which might trigger memory loss.6
MWM Morris water maze Stress exceeding adaptation mechanisms is potentially
PPARγ Peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor γ harmful due to over activity of three major neuroendocrine
RAM Radial arm maze
SEM Standard error of mean
Department of Clinical Pharmacology, Medical University of Bialystok,
Introduction Poland

Corresponding author:
Stressful experiences are generally well remembered,1 Dominik Wincewicz, Department of Clinical Pharmacology, Medical
especially if the context of perceived stress matches the University of Bialystok, Waszyngtona 15a, Bialystok, 15-274, Poland.
context of the information being encoded.2 Email: d.wincewicz@gmail.com
496 Journal of the Renin-Angiotensin-Aldosterone System 16(3)

systems – the sympatho-adrenergic system (SAS), the subjected to repeated restraint stress procedure; (4) 30 rats
HPA axis, and the renin–angiotensin system (RAS).7 receiving telmisartan suspended in 0.5% methylcellulose
However, this effect might be reduced by suppression of subjected to repeated restraint stress procedure. The subjects
the brain RAS system, as we have previously shown using received either telmisartan or vehicle each day approxi-
highly selective angiotensin type one (AT1) receptor block- mately 30 min before the beginning of stressing procedure.
ade, which effectively prevented deleterious effects of Animal weight was measured each day prior to drug
chronic stress on retrieval of memory in rats.8,9 administration.
In this study, we attempted to evaluate the effect of AT1
receptor blockade using telmisartan in hippocampal-
Stress procedure
dependent memory tasks. Spatial and non-spatial cogni-
tion are two important aspects of cognitive function. Two groups of animals (30 rats each) were subjected to
Various types of behavioral tasks have been designed to chronic restraint stress, 2 h daily for 21 days.16,17 The
evaluate specific type of memory in rodents.10–13 Most of restraint was imposed during the light phase from 1:00
the spatial memory tasks use positive or negative rein- p.m. to 3:00 p.m. The restrainer was made of transparent
forces, such as food for radial arm maze (RAM) or water perforated plastic tube, 20 cm long, and 7 cm in diameter.
immersion for a Morris water maze (MWM) test, however A rat was eased into the restrainer, head first, and once in
those effects are minimized in Barnes maze (BM). the tube it was closed with a Plexiglass lid. The animals fit
Therefore, we decided to use all three paradigms to evalu- tightly into the restrainers and it was not possible for them
ate potential protective effect of telmisartan on memory to move or turn around. At the same time not stressed con-
loss due to chronic stress and learning abilities under trol rats were briefly handled and returned to their home
stressful conditions in a variety of intensification. cages. Randomly chosen animals subjected to stress were
Moreover, using mazes with diverse reinforces seems use- checked for gastric ulceration on the next day after ending
ful to incorporate results of animal studies with the human the behavioral tests. Rats were anaesthetized with the mix-
population. ture of ketamine (50 mg/kg) and xylazine (7.5 mg/kg)
injected intraperitoneally and sacrificed. Exposed gastric
mucosa was visually examined under the 5× magnification
Materials and methods lens for gastric ulceration. No visible signs of injury were
Animals found, that could potentially interfere with the results of
behavioral tests.
The experiments were conducted on male Wistar rats,
weighing approximately 150 g at the beginning. Animals
were housed five to a cage in a temperature (22°C) and Behavioral tests
humidity (50–60%) controlled room, on a 12 h: 12 h light/ Hippocampal-dependent cognitive performance of rats
dark cycle with light on from 6:00 a.m. Free access to was estimated the next day after ending stress and drug
standard laboratory food and tap water was provided. All administration procedures. Three different paradigms with
animals were handled daily for 2 min each until the day of three different levels of stress induced by the procedure
experiment. All the procedures were conducted between were used to assess its influence on working memory.
1:00 p.m. and 6:00 p.m. A 30 min adaptation period in the Moreover this work has benefited from evaluating spatial
experimental room preceded all the tests. To minimize the perception, spatial cognition, and spatially directed action
influence of hemodynamic changes, we did not study in both allocentric (MWM, BM) and egocentric (RAM)
hypertensive animals. All procedures involving animals representations. Each animal would participate in one
were approved by the local Ethics Commission for Animal behavioral paradigm only (40 animals assigned to MWM,
Experimentation. 40 participating in BM and 40 tested in RAM, from total
number of 120 rats).
Drugs
Morris water maze.  Forty rats (different from those used
The specific AT1 receptor antagonist – telmisartan – was for RAM and BM) randomly assigned to four groups as
suspended in 0.5% methylcellulose (vehicle) at concentra- described above were used. This task was adapted from
tion of 1 mg/ml. Telmisartan or its vehicle was dosed by oral the paradigm originally described by Morris.13 The water
gavage at 1 mg/kg body weight, considered to be a non- maze was a black circular pool (180 cm in diameter, 60
hypotensive dose in rats.14,15 One hundred and twenty male cm high), filled with water (26±1°C) made opaque with
Wistar rats were randomly assigned to four groups: (1) 30 a grain instant coffee, to the depth of 30 cm, placed in a
control rats – receiving 0.5% methylcellulose as a vehicle; room rich in consistently located spatial cues (door,
(2) 30 rats receiving telmisartan suspended in 0.5% methyl- shelves, illumination lights, posters). The pool was
cellulose; (3) 30 rats receiving 0.5% methylcellulose divided into four quadrants designated Northeast (NE),
Wincewicz and Braszko 497

Northwest (NW), Southeast (SE), and Southwest (SW). animals (were brought in the testing suite) and allowed to
An escape platform (9 cm in diameter) was placed in the enter the goal box through one of the holes in the circular
middle of one quadrant (NW), 1.5 cm below the water surface (120 cm diameter) of the maze. Once the animal
surface, equidistant from the sidewall and middle of the entered the hole, a black Plexiglass cover was positioned
pool. The platform provided the only escape from the over the hole to prevent escape. A 4-min habituation in the
water and was located in the same quadrant conducted goal box was given prior to the first training trial. Next,
through trials each day, but its location was changed the animal was placed for 30 s in a 20 cm diameter by 30
daily. Four different starting points for rats were equally cm high round nontransparent holding box that was posi-
spaced around the perimeter of the pool (N, E, S, and tioned in the center of the maze. The holding box was then
W). On each of the three training days, three start points removed, a timer begun, and the experimenter moved
were used once each in a pseudo-random sequence. A behind the curtain. An escape was counted when all four
trial began by placing the animal in the water facing the paws of the animal were in the goal box. Following suc-
wall of the pool at one of the starting points. If the ani- cessful location of the goal box, the animals were allowed
mal failed to escape on the platform within 60 s it was to stay there for 60 s. A maximum of 4 min was allowed
gently placed there by the experimenter and allowed to for each trial and if an animal did not locate in the goal
stay for 15 s. box during this 4 min period it was removed from the
The interval was 5 min. After each trial, the rats were maze and placed in the goal box for 60 s. During the 60 s
towel and fan dried and returned to their home cages. All period in the goal box, the maze was wiped with a paper
animals were trained one session of three trials daily for towel to remove any feces or urine prior to the beginning
three consecutive days. The latency to reach the original of the next trial. The test paradigm consisted of two trials
position of the platform, the number of crossings over its per day for 3 days. Before the start of the second trial, the
previous location during 1 min, and time spent by rat in the animal was returned to its home cage for 1 min and then
target quadrant and in the opposite quadrant and the border placed again in the holding box in the center of the maze
zone were measured manually. to start the second trial as described above. The maze was
then rotated a random number of holes (between 1 and 8
Eight-arm radial maze.  Forty rats (different from those used holes using a random numbers table) to prevent use of
for MWM and BM) randomly assigned to four groups as odor trails in solving the task. The hole under which was
described above were used. The apparatus was an elevated the escape tunnel was changed to assure its constant posi-
eight arm radial-maze which was made of 0.5 cm thick tion relative to extra-maze cues (shelves, posters, and
gray wooden planks. Each arm (68 × 10 cm2) extended laboratory bench) for 3 days. Following the rotation of
from an octagonal-shaped center hub (30 cm in diameter). the table, the procedure was repeated with the next ani-
A small cup (4.5 cm in diameter) was placed at the end of mal. Two measures were recorded on each trial. The first
each arm. The maze was elevated 80 cm above the floor was the latency to find the goal box. The second was the
and placed in a sound attenuated room with a masking number of errors committed by each animal. An error
noise of 70 dB above the human thresholds.18 The rats was defined as a head poke or exploration of any hole
were submitted to three habituation sessions (one per day). other than the hole above the goal box and including pre-
They were put on the central platform and allowed to servative investigations of the same hole. At the end of
explore the maze entries for 10min.At the end of habitua- each trial the maze was cleaned using a 70% ethanol
tion stage, they were deprived of food for 24 h before the solution.11
beginning of the training phase. Rats were given 12 ses-
sions (one per day). The training session lasted until the rat
Statistical analysis
made eight choices or 15 min elapsed. The cups at the end
of each arm contained arachnid peanuts. The experimenter Data were presented as means of ± standard error of mean
recorded order of the eight choices made by each rat. One (SEM). A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA II)
hour after the completion of the session, rats were given (treatment × days) with repeated measures followed by the
access to food for 10 min. The following data was consid- post hoc Bonferroni test for multiple comparisons was
ered: mean number of errors – an error was an entrance in used for latencies to reach platform in the MWM and mean
a previously visited arm; mean number of correct entries number of errors, latencies, and number of correct entries
– correct choices made in sequence before the first error in in RAM. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA I), fol-
each session; mean time spend by rats to visit eight maze lowed by post hoc Bonferroni test for chosen group com-
arms within each session. parisons, was applied for calculating performance of the
rats in MWM (savings ratios, border zone time, and swim-
Barnes maze. Forty rats (different from those used for ming speed), Barnes maze (escape latencies and number of
MWM and RAM) randomly assigned to four groups as errors), and body weight gain. Levels were deemed signifi-
described above were used. On the first day of testing, the cant at p < 0.05.
498 Journal of the Renin-Angiotensin-Aldosterone System 16(3)

Figure 1.  Effects of chronic stress (daily restraint for 2 h, 21


days), chronic telmisartan (1 mg/kg p.o., daily, 21 days), or both
in combination on mean ± SEM escape latency in the Morris
water maze over three days (three trials per day), n = 10. *p
< 0.05 stress + telmisartan vs. stress, **p < 0.01 control vs.
stress, ***p < 0.001 telmisartan vs. stress.
Figure 2.  Effects of chronic stress (daily restraint for 2 h,
21 days), chronic telmisartan (1 mg/kg p.o., daily, 21 days), or
Results both in combination on the task-solving strategy in the Morris
water maze. Bars represent means + SEM of the time spend in
Effects of chronic stress, chronic telmisartan the border zone from nine trials (three trials per day for three
treatment, or both in combination on rats’ days), n = 10. *p < 0.05 vs. control and telmisartan.
performance in the Morris water maze
Figure 1 presents mean ± SEM escape latency in the According to the interpretation suggested by Glenn and
MWM. The stressed subjects reached the platform signifi- Mumby,19 the smaller savings ratios the better retention of
cantly later than the control (p < 0.01), telmisartan (p < the platform position. ANOVA I of the calculated savings
0.001), and stress + telmisartan (p < 0.05) group of rats. ratios from MWM test yielded F3,36 = 4.595; p < 0.05,
Telmisartan administration not only abolished adverse showing statistically significant differences between
effects of stress on spatial working memory, but also stressed rats in comparison to control and telmisartan
improved performance of not stressed subjects. ANOVA II groups.
of the latencies to reach platform in the water maze Figure 4 presents mean ± SEM swimming speed, calcu-
revealed significant treatment effect (F3,36 = 7.226; p < lated as number of line crossings divided by escape latency.
0.001), but no significant treatments × days interaction The results suggest that all rats were performing the task
(F6,72 = 1.367; p > 0.05). Nevertheless, the fact that most of with equal swimming speed and this parameter did not
the rats effectively learned the task meant the day effect influence the outcome of their performance. ANOVA I of
was not significant (F2,6 = 1.818; p > 0.05). the results obtained in MWM test revealed no significant
Figure 2 presents mean ± SEM of the time spent in bor- (F3,36 = 3.265; p > 0.05) differences between the groups.
der zone in the MWM. ANOVA I of the time spent in the
border zone from nine trials yielded F3,36 = 4.663; p < 0.05, Effects of chronic stress, chronic telmisartan
indicating that stressed subjects spent significantly more treatment, or both in combination on rats’
time in the border zone than non-stressed rats. This effect
was abolished by telmisartan treatment of stressed rats.
performance in the radial arm maze
Figure 3 presents performance of rats in MWM Figure 5(a) presents the mean ± SEM numbers of errors,
expressed as mean ± SEM savings ratio, a proportion of cumulated in three four-day blocks. ANOVA II of these
second trial latency to total trial time for the session. results yielded F2,6 = 20.523; p < 0.001, showing significant
Wincewicz and Braszko 499

Figure 3.  Effects of chronic stress (daily restraint for 2 h, 21 Figure 4.  Effects of chronic stress (daily restraint for 2 h,
days), chronic telmisartan (1 mg/kg p.o., daily, 21 days), or both 21 days), chronic telmisartan (1 mg/kg p.o., daily, 21 days), or
in combination on working memory in the Morris water maze both in combination on swimming speed (line crossed/s) in the
expressed in mean savings ratios. Bars represent means + SEM, Morris water maze. Bars represent means + SEM, n = 10. No
n = 10. *p < 0.05 vs. control and telmisartan. statistically significant differences were found.

decrease of the errors made by all rats across 12 days. No maze arms. Significant treatments × days interaction was
significant treatments × days interaction was found (F6,72 = found (F6,72 = 4.380; p < 0.001). ANOVA I of the times
0.576; p > 0.05). ANOVA I of the total number of errors spent by rats visiting all arms over 12 days yielded F3,36 =
made by the rats over 12 days yielded F2,6 = 7.496, p < 0.01 3.726; p < 0.05, showing significant differences among
showing significant treatment effect. These results demon- stressed and not stressed subjects.
strate that impairment of working memory displayed by
chronically stressed rats that is partially diminished by Effects of chronic stress, chronic telmisartan
telmisartan.
treatment, or both in combination on rats’
Figure 5(b) presents mean number ± SEM of correct
and sequential entries to arms of the radial maze, prior to performance in the Barnes maze
first error. ANOVA II of the daily numbers of the correct Figure 6 presents mean ± SEM number of errors made by
entries made by each rat over 12 days yielded F2,6 = rats in the Barnes maze. ANOVA I of the results reviled
18.791; p < 0.001, showing significant time effect, i.e. significant difference between stressed and control sub-
increase of the correct entries made by all rats across 12 jects (F3,36 = 2.867; p < 0.05) in selected pair comparison.
days. No significant treatments × days interaction was Telmisartan treatment of stressed rats reduced this effect
found (F6,72 = 0.664; p > 0.05). ANOVA I of the total num- but further post hoc comparisons made with Bonferroni
ber of the correct entries made by each rat over 12 days revealed that the difference is not statically significant.
yielded F3,36 = 5.592; p < 0.01 control vs stress; p < 0.05 Figure 7 presents mean ± SEM of escape latency in the
telmisartan vs stress. Barnes maze. Exploration time was extended in the
Figure 5(c) presents the mean ± SEM of the time spent stressed subjects and this effect was lowered in telmisartan
by rats visiting eight arms. ANOVA II of the times used by treated group, but ANOVA I of the results from all the tri-
all rats yielded F6,72 = 4,38; p < 0.001, pointing to the sig- als and post hoc comparisons made with Bonferroni
nificant increase of the rats performance in time of visiting revealed that the differences are not statically significant.
500 Journal of the Renin-Angiotensin-Aldosterone System 16(3)

Figure 5.  Points represent means ± SEM from 4 days each of the values obtained from 10 animals. (a) Mean number of errors,
**p < 0.01 control and telmisartan vs. stress; (b) mean number of correct sequential entries before the first error, **p < 0.01 control
vs. stress, *p < 0.05 telmisartan vs. stress; (c) mean time of eight arms exploration, *p < 0.05 control and telmisartan vs. stress.

Effects of stress and telmisartan on body


weight gain
As shown in Table 1, all of the rats gained weight during
the course of the study, but growth rate differed among the
groups. Restraint induced a significant weight loss (p <
0.001) that was even greater in the rats receiving telmisar-
tan. The effect became apparent during the first week of
restraint. Moreover, animals that were not subjected to
stress procedure and received telmisartan also gained sig-
nificantly (p < 0.005) less weight than control group; how-
ever, this effect appeared after two weeks of the study.

Discussion
Despite rapid development of technology and biochemical
methods, behavioral tests are still recognized as very sen-
sitive tools in testing animal cognition, which might reveal
minor hippocampal-induced impairment before, or even in
the absence of, neuron death.20 Although the Morris water
maze, the radial arm maze and the Barnes maze are well
established means of testing spatial memory,11,13,21,22 nei-
ther might be considered optimal for studying the behavio-
Figure 6.  Effects of chronic stress (daily restraint for 2 h, ral effects of stress. The MWM is itself highly stressful to
21 days), chronic telmisartan (1 mg/kg p.o., daily, 21 days), or
the animals.23 The RAM relies on a food reward to moti-
both in combination on working memory in the Barnes maze.
Bars represent mean + SEM number of errors from six trials vate the animals, and glucocorticoids have profound
(two trials per day for three days), n = 10. *p < 0.05 stress vs. effects on hunger and satiety.24 While strong aversive stim-
control. uli (i.e. water or food deprivation) are likely to produce
Wincewicz and Braszko 501

stress in the animal that influences the performance in the The general processes used for visuospatial navigation in
task, weak aversive stimulation might result in lack of rats also contribute considerably to human day-to-day cog-
motivation to escape from the circular platform in the nitive processes.29 Therefore, to broaden utility of this
Barnes maze.25 Performance of rodents with hippocampal study all three paradigms were used (MWM, RAM, and
damage in all the mazes mentioned above is impaired, BM) as it might become relevant to neurodegenerative and
which strongly supports hypothesis of the spatial nature of neuropsychiatric illnesses where cognition is impaired.30–32
all the tasks.26,27 All the paradigms are challenging tasks As a continuation of our previous studies,9 and screen-
for rodents and employ a variety of sophisticated mne- ing for compounds with potential cognitive enhancing
monic processes.28 These processes comprise the acquisi- effects, as well as delineating deleterious effects of stress
tion and spatial localization of relevant visual cues that are on cognition, we proceeded with development studies of
subsequently processed, consolidated, retained, and then telmisartan. The results of the present study clearly dem-
retrieved in order to successfully navigate using allocen- onstrate that pre-treatment with telmisartan significantly
tric (MWM, BM) or egocentric (RAM) representations. reduces stress-related cognitive impairment in rats.
Parameters of cognitive performance (latencies, errors and
correct choices, savings ratios) in MWM, RAM, and BM
were significantly deteriorated in chronically stressed sub-
jects. This effect was reduced in stressed subjects that
received telmisartan. Interestingly, the most profound
effect was reviled in the MWM, which is the most stressful
paradigm therefore, capitalizes upon aversive motivation.
Moreover, the MWM allows for the assessment of spatial
memory without potential experimental manipulation
incorporating food reward. Rats exhibit a robust increase
in corticosterone on the first day of water maze testing that
is stable and only slightly diminished throughout days of
testing.33 No such data are available for the RAM and BM.
According to inverted-U-shaped fashion, suggesting that
an optimal level of corticosterone will enhance memory
and higher levels may impair it,26 telmisartan treatment
presumably diminish excessive release of corticosterone in
stressed subjects, suppressing over-activation of the HPA
axis and enhanced corticotrophin-releasing hormone, as
has been recently described by Braszko et al.8 Noteworthy,
time latencies in the MWM seems to efface among the
stressed and non-stressed subjects on the second and the
third day of testing. It is possible that control rats sooner
than stressed rats switch from spatial to non-spatial strate-
gies with overtraining.4 The least profound effect of pro-
tective outcome of telmisartan treatment in stress induced
cognitive decline was reviled in the Barnes maze, the least
Figure 7.  Effects of chronic stress (daily restraint for 2 h, stressful of all three mazes. Number of errors made by
21 days), chronic telmisartan (1 mg/kg p.o., daily, 21 days), or
both in combination on working memory in the Barnes maze.
stressed subjects in the BM was significantly higher and
Bars represent mean + SEM time of escape latency from six telmisartan treatment reduced this effect, but no statisti-
trials (two trials per day for three days), n = 10. No statistically cally significant differences in the escape latency were
significant differences were found. found. However, unitary measures such as latency and

Table 1.  Effects of stress and telmisartan (1 mg/kg) on body weight gain. Values are means ± SEM of body weight (g). *p < 0.05,
**p < 0.005, ***p < 0.001 in comparison to control group, n = 10 rats in each group.

Groups Day 1 Day 7 Day 14 Day 21


Control 150.4 ± 1.41 181.6 ± 3.21 225.5 ± 4.20 271.5 ± 5.25
Telmisartan 151 ± 2.28 179.4 ± 2.60 211.5 ± 4.03* 252.6 ± 3.48**
Stress 152.7 ± 2.37 169.2 ± 3.08* 188.8 ± 2.91*** 222 ± 3.61***
Stress + telmisartan 155 ± 1.27 167.4 ± 2.81** 184.4 ± 3.54*** 213 ± 3.81***
502 Journal of the Renin-Angiotensin-Aldosterone System 16(3)

path length are more susceptible to non-mnemonic influ- also a partial agonist of the peroxisome proliferator–
ences and are not the best indicators of cognitive perfor- activated receptor-gamma (PPAR-γ), a well-known target
mance in learning tasks.34 Moreover, the BM induces a of insulin sensitizing drugs used to treat type 2 diabetes.
modest increase in corticosterone,35 which might be insuf- Only telmisartan, and to a lesser extent candesartan, are
ficient for motivation of animals to perform in the task.36 the only commercially available ARBs producing PPAR-γ
In our study, adaptation procedure was not effective activation, when tested at concentrations typically achieved
enough, to exert animals for more readily exploration of in plasma with conventional oral dosing.42,43 Telmisartan
the holes on the surface of circular platform of the BM. influences the expression of PPAR target genes involved in
Rats often hesitated to enter the escape hole and started to carbohydrate and lipid metabolism and reduction of glu-
inspect other holes, what clearly had a direct effect on cose, insulin, and triglyceride levels,44 but the significance
latency and distance measures. Control rats needed more of PPAR-γ activation in this study goes beyond its meta-
than 60 s to find the escape hole in the BM, which is rela- bolic effects. These nuclear receptors are considered to be
tively long when compared to the MWM performance attractive targets for the treatment of cognitive impairment
(<45 s) in which the diameter of the apparatus is much due to their ability to suppress microglial-mediated inflam-
longer. Execution of the task in the RAM was impaired in matory responses,45 promote tissue repair,46 and induce the
the stressed subjects and expressed by mean number of degradation of the Aβ peptides in the brain by activating
errors. Interestingly mean number of correct entries was genes responsible for reverse cholesterol transport.47
slightly lower in rats receiving telmisartan, as those rats Nevertheless, PPAR-γ are expressed in the fetal brain as a
chose more often not to enter arms sequentially, but with- factor involved in cellular development, its expression is
out committing more errors than controls. Despite the fact rather limited in the healthy mature brain.48 Moreover, the
that RAM is generally used to measure spatial memory, use of the PPAR-γ activators (i.e. rosiglitazone, pioglita-
using this paradigm has notable limits. For example, even zone) for CNS-targeted disease treatments is compromised
blind rats can learn to enter arms in one direction (i.e. con- due to their poor blood brain barrier (BBB) penetrance.49
stantly turn left), which might be a failure in long-term Telmisartan, however, effectively penetrates BBB in a
testing procedure, when hippocampal mediated processes dose- and time-dependent manner, to inhibit centrally
become independent from the hippocampus over weeks.37 mediated effects of Ang II for over 4 hours following
Another limitation for using RAM in stressed subjects, is peripheral administration of a dose as low as 1 mg/kg.50
the fact that chronic stress may influence motivation with However, it is essential that a non-hypotensive dose of 1
clearly interferes with the results of the test. The effects of mg/kg does not result in a significant reduction in blood
stress and telmisartan treatment tested in the RAM were pressure and does not affect cerebral blood flow.51
slightly less profound than in MWM, but more expressed Although blood pressure (BP), even in normotensive rats,
in comparison to the BM. The data presented here, indi- as a continuous, not a static, variable might be altered due
cating the differences among three paradigms, are to restraint, which clearly is a factor for development and
assumed to be caused by the negative correlation between progression of cognitive decline, the negative effects of
learning and level of corticosterone release. However, cer- stress are simultaneously mediated through angiotensin II
tain causative direction cannot be drawn and needs further and glucocorticoids, affecting both circadian BP rhythm
investigation. and cognitive performance. Therefore, stress induced
Repeated restraint is used in this study as a model of alterations of BP cannot be considered as an independent
moderate psychological stress, which is a complex trigger of memory loss. Stress induces an increase in BBB
response that also affects metabolism, therefore commonly permeability,52 and disruptions of BBB are casually asso-
considered as an animal model of anorexia nervosa.38 ciated with cognitive decline.53 Interestingly, treatment
Repeated stressors in rats generally seem to reduce food with an ARB ameliorates cognitive impairment in type-2
intake and body weight.39 Stress-induced effects on metab- diabetic subjects,54 which is caused most likely due to
olism and energy balance have been demonstrated as a decrease in BBB permeability.55 Moreover, recent studies
result of repeated daily restraint and as an acute response suggest that salt-dependent hypertension and activation of
in the hours immediately after a single restraint.40 In our the brain RAS contributes to BBB malfunction and cogni-
study weight loss induced by repeated restraint appeared tive decline, which is reversed with an ARB treatment
within the first week of stress sessions and became more independent of blood pressure changes, by preventing
pronounced after each exposure to stress, which indirectly BBB permeability.56 These data indicates importance of
indicates the effectiveness of the restraint procedure. the brain RAS in the pathogenesis of impaired cognitive
Significant attenuation of weight gain was also a result of performance. The role of the Ang II is well established in a
telmisartan administration, which is not very surprising complexity of sympathoadrenal response.57 In rats, high
since reduced adipogenesis and weight gain have already density of AT1 receptors is present in brain regions that are
been reported as the effects of this ARB in rodents.41 In involved in the regulation of sympathetic tone, such as the
addition to being a blocker of AT1 receptor, telmisartan is endothelial cells and neurons of the circumventricular
Wincewicz and Braszko 503

organs outside the BBB (e.g. SFO, OVLT), cerebrovascu- Declaration of Conflicting Interests
lar endothelial cells of the BBB, and neuronal AT1 recep- The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
tors selectively localized in many brain areas located to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
inside the BBB (e.g., PVN, SCh, and MnPO).58 Significant
numbers of AT1 receptors are located in higher stress- Funding
regulatory structures: hippocampus, septum and amyg-
dala. AT1 receptors are also expressed in parvocellular The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article:
corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF) forming neurons
This work was supported by the Medical University of Bialystok.
controlling CRF release during stress.59 These brain struc-
tures are important therapeutic targets for ARBs, espe-
cially those lipophilic like telmisartan that readily crosses References
BBB to inhibit the brain RAS,50 as a treatment of stress 1. Kim JJ and Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, syn-
related CNS disorders. It has been reported that sartans aptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;
normalize cerebrovascular remodeling and autoregulation 3: 453–462.
in hypertension, prevents and reverses cerebrovascular 2. Joels M, Pu Z, Wiegert O, et al. Learning under stress: how
inflammation, reduces vulnerability to brain ischemia, and does it work? Trends Cogn Sci 2006; 10: 152–158.
3. McEwen BS. Protection and damage from acute and chronic
protects from stroke,60 but explanation of many positive
stress: allostasis and allostatic overload and relevance to the
effects of ARBs would go beyond AT1 receptor blockade. pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Ann N Y Acad Sci
Besides central functions of Ang II, other naturally occur- 2004; 1032: 1–7.
ring neuroactive peptides of the RAS are also involved in 4. Parkad CR, Campbella AM and Diamond DM. Chronic psy-
memory processing.61 The effects of angiotensin IV (Ang chosocial stress impairs learning and memory and increases
IV) and des-Phe(6)Ang IV include improvement of several sensitivity to yohimbine in adult rats. Biol Psychol 2001; 50:
memory aspects (i.e. recall of appetitively and aversively 994–1004.
motivated behaviors and learning of spatial tasks). 5. Smeets T, Giesbrecht T, Jelicic M, et al. Context-
Moreover, angiotensin metabolites modulate central func- dependent enhancement of declarative memory perfor-
tions by increasing levels of some of the other brain pep- mance following acute psychosocial stress. Biol Psychol
tides, including oxytocin, which is associated with 2007; 76: 116–123.
6. Roth MK, Bingham B, Shah A, et al. Effects of chronic plus
anxiolytic effects.62
acute prolonged stress on measures of coping style, anxi-
As a consequence of AT1 blockade, ARBs increase not ety, and evoked HPA-axis reactivity. Neuropharmacology
only Ang II levels, but also levels of Ang metabolites. 2012; 63: 1118–1126.
Interestingly, some data suggest that angiotensin neuro- 7. Saavedra JM and Benicky J. Brain and peripheral angioten-
peptide release, occurring in mild stressed subjects, sin II play a major role in stress. Stress 2007; 10: 185–193.
results in memory enhancement that could be attenuated 8. Braszko JJ, Wincewicz D and Jakubow P. Candesartan pre-
by both ACE-inhibitors and ARBs.63 ACE-inhibitors vents impairment of recall caused by chronic stress in rats.
decreases the formation of Ang II, Ang III, and Ang IV Psychopharmacology 2013; 225: 421–428.
from Ang I, while ARBs selectively block AT1 receptor, 9. Wincewicz D and Braszko JJ. Telmisartan attenuates
thus diminishing Ang II-mediated biological actions cognitive impairment caused by chronic stress in rats.
without affecting the actions of other angiotensin neuro- Pharmacological Reports; accepted.
10. Ader R, Weijnen JAWM and Moleman P. Retention of pas-
peptides, suggesting the key role of Ang II not only in
sive avoidance response as function of the intensity and
stress-induced memory impairment but also enhance- duration of electric shock. Psychon Sci 1972; 26: 125–129.
ment. This indicates flexibility and complexity of brain 11. Barnes CA. Memory deficits associated with senescence: a
RAS system demanding further evaluation conditioned neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. J Comp
upon different levels of stress. Physiol Psychol 1979; 93: 74–104.
Nevertheless, accumulating preclinical studies and 12. Ennaceur A and Meliani K. A new one-trial test for neuro-
clinical reports call attention on the therapeutic and neuro- biological studies of memory in rats. Spatial vs. non-spatial
protective effects of ARBs,64,65 including stress attenuating working memory. Behav Brain Res 1992; 51: 83–92.
properties of telmisartan,14,66–68 although at this point nar- 13. Morris M. Developments of a water-maze procedure for
rowing the group of ARBs, excluding those that are hydro- studying spatial learning in the rat. J Neurosci Methods
philic (i.e. losartan, eprosartan),69,70 to indicate the most 1984; 11: 47–60.
14. Washida K, Ihara M, Nishio K, et al. Non-hypotensive dose
potent of withdrawing negative effects of stress, seems
of telmisartan attenuates cognitive impairment partially due
unreasonable. to peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-gamma acti-
In conclusion, data presented in this study indicate that vation in mice with chronic cerebral hypoperfusion. Stroke
AT1 angiotensin receptor blockade with telmisartan attenu- 2010; 41: 1798–1806.
ates negative effect of both, acute and chronic stress on 15. Wienen W, Hauel N, Van Meel JC, et al. Pharmacological
memory, which is a novelty in the pursuit of effective ther- characterization of the novel nonpeptide angiotensin II receptor
apeutic approach to stress induced cognitive decline. antagonist, BIBR 277. Br J Pharmacol 2003; 110: 245–252.
504 Journal of the Renin-Angiotensin-Aldosterone System 16(3)

16. Magarinos AM, Verdugo JMG and McEwen BS. Chronic 33. Aguilar-Valles A, Sanchez E, de Gortari P, et al. Analysis
stress alters synaptic terminal structure in hippocampus. of the stress response in rats trained in the water-maze: dif-
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997; 94: 14002–14008. ferential expression of corticotropin-releasing hormone,
17. Walesiuk A, Trofimuik E and Braszko JJ. Ginkgo biloba CRH-R1, glucocorticoid receptors and brain-derived neuro-
extract diminishes stress-induced memory deficits in rats. trophic factor in limbic regions. Neuroendocrinology 2005;
Pharmacol Rep 2005; 57: 176–187. 82: 306–319.
18. Ennaceur A and Meliani K. Effects of physostigmine and 34. McDonald MP and Overmier JB. Present imperfect: a criti-
scopolamine on rats’ performances in object-recognition cal review of animal models of the mnemonic impairments
and radial-maze tests. Psychopharmacology 1992; 109: in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1998; 22:
321–330. 99–120.
19. Glenn MJ and Mumby DG. Place memory is intact in rats 35. Okuda S, Roozendaal B and McGaugh JL. Glucocorticoid
with perirhinal cortex lesions. Behav Neurosci 1998; 112: effects on object recognition memory require training-
1353–1365. associated emotional arousal. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
20. Bodnoff SR, Humphreys AG, Lehman JC, et al. Enduring 2004; 101: 853–858.
effects of chronic corticosterone treatment on spatial learn- 36. Koopmans G, Blokland A, van Nieuwenhuijzen P, et al.
ing, synaptic plasticity, and hippocampal neuropathology in Assessment of spatial learning abilities of mice in a new
young and mid-aged rats. J Neurosci 1995; 15: 61–69. circular maze. Physiol Behav 2003; 79: 683–693.
21. Cole MR and Chappell-Stephenson R. Exploring the limits 37. Eichenbaum H. A cortical-hippocampal system for declara-
of spatial memory in rats, using very large mazes. Learn tive memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 2000; 1: 41–50.
Behav 2003; 31: 349–368. 38. Krahn DD, Gosnell BA and Majchrzak M. The anorectic
22. Olton DS and Samuelson RJ. Remembrance of places past: effects of CRH and restraint stress decrease with repeated
spatial memory in rats. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Processes exposures. Biol Psychiatry 1990; 27: 1094–1102.
1976; 2: 97–116. 39. Marti O, Marti J and Armario A. Effects of chronic stress on
23. Harrison FE, Hosseini AH and McDonald MP. Endogenous food intake in rats: influence of stressor intensity and dura-
anxiety and stress responses in water maze and Barnes maze tion of daily exposure. Physiol Behav 1994; 55: 747–753.
spatial memory tasks. Behav Brain Res 2009; 198: 247–251. 40. Shimizu N, Oomura Y and Kai Y. Stress-induced anorexia
24. Castonguay TW. Glucocorticoids as modulators in the con- in rats mediated by serotonergic mechanisms in the hypo-
trol of feeding. Brain Res Bull 1991; 27: 423–428. thalamus. Physiol Behav 1989; 46: 835–841.
25. Pompl PN, Mullan MJ, Bjugstad K, et al. Adaptation of 41. He H, Yang D, Ma L, et al. Telmisartan prevents weight gain
the circular platform spatial memory task for mice: use in and obesity through activation of peroxisome proliferator-
detecting cognitive impairment in the APP(SW) transgenic activated receptor-delta-dependent pathways. Hypertension
mouse model for Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci Methods 2010; 55: 869–879
1999; 87: 87–95. 42. Benson SC, Pershadsingh HA, Ho CI, et al. Identification
26. Olton DS, Walker JA and Gage HF. Hippocampal con- of telmisartan as a unique angiotensin II receptor antago-
nections and spatial discrimination. Brain Res 1978; 139: nist with selective PPARgamma-modulating activity.
295–308. Hypertension 2004; 43: 993–1002.
27. Whishaw IQ, Cassel JC and Jarrad LE. Rats with fimbria- 43. Erbe DV, Gartrell K, Zhang Y-L, et al. Molecular activa-
fornix lesions display a place response in a swimming pool: tion of PPARγ by angiotensin II type 1-receptor antagonists.
a dissociation between getting there and knowing where. J Vasc Pharm 2006; 45: 154–162
Neurosci 1995; 15: 5779 –5788. 44. Schupp M, Janke J, Clasen R, et al. Angiotensin type 1
28. Rudy JW, Stadler-Morris S and Albert P. Ontogeny of spa- receptor blockers induce peroxisome proliferator-activated
tial navigation behaviors in the rat: dissociation of “proxi- receptor-gamma activity. Circulation 2004; 109: 2054–
mal”- and “distal”-cue-based behaviors. Behav Neurosci 2057.
1987; 101: 62–73. 45. Mandrekar-Colucci S and Landreth GE. Microglia and

29. Astur RS, Taylor LB, Mamelak AN, et al. Humans with hip- inflammation in Alzheimer’s disease. CNS Neurol Disord
pocampus damage display severe spatial memory impair- Drug Targets 2010; 9: 156–167.
ments in a virtual Morris water task. Behav Brain Res 2002; 46. Hong C and Tontonoz P. Coordination of inflammation
132: 77–84. and metabolism by PPAR and LXR nuclear receptors. Curr
30. Francis PT, Palmer AM, Snape M, et al. The cholinergic Opin Genet Dev 2008; 18: 461–467.
hypothesis of Alzheimer’s disease: a review of progress. J 47. Jiang Q, Lee CY, Mandrekar S, et al. ApoE promotes the
Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999; 66: 137–147. proteolytic degradation of Abeta. Neuron 2008; 58: 681–
31. Nazeri A, Mallar Chakravarty M, Felsky D, et al. Alterations 693.
of superficial white matter in schizophrenia and relation- 48. Wada K, Nakajima A, Katayama K, et al. Peroxisome pro-
ship to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology liferator-activated receptor γ-mediated regulation of neu-
Epub ahead of print 16 April 2013. DOI: 10.1038/ ral stem cell proliferation and differentiation. J Biol Chem
npp.2013.93. 2006; 281: 12673–12681.
32. Perry EK, Curtis M, Dick DJ, et al. Cholinergic correlates of 49. Hemauer SJ, Patrikeeva SL, Nanovskaya TN, et al. Role of
cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: comparisons human placental apical membrane transporters in the efflux
with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry of glyburide, rosiglitazone, and metformin. Am J Obstet
1985; 48: 413–421. Gynecol 2010; 202: 383.e1–7.
Wincewicz and Braszko 505

50. Gohlke P, Weiss S, Jansen A, et al. AT1 receptor antago- 61. Braszko JJ. Participation of D 1–4 dopamine receptors
nist telmisartan administered peripherally inhibits central in the pro-cognitive effects of angiotensin IV and des-
responses to angiotensin II in conscious rats. J Pharmacol Phe 6 angiotensin IV. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010; 34:
Exp Ther 2001; 298: 62–70. 343–350.
51. Washida K, Ihara M, Nishio K, et al. Non-hypotensive dose 62. Bali A and Jaggi AS. Angiotensin as stress mediator: role of
of telmisartan attenuates cognitive impairment partially due its receptor and interrelationships among other stress media-
to peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-gamma acti- tors and receptors. Pharmacol Res 2013; 76: 49–57.
vation in mice with chronic cerebral hypoperfusion. Stroke 63. Bali A, Singh N and Jaggi AS. Investigations into mild elec-
2010; 41: 1798–1806. tric foot shock stress-induced cognitive enhancement: pos-
52. Skultétyová I, Tokarev D and Jezová D. Stress-induced sible role of angiotensin neuropeptides. J Renin Angiotensin
increase in blood-brain barrier permeability in control and Aldosterone Syst 2013; 14: 197–203.
monosodium glutamate-treated rats. Brain Res Bull 1998; 64. Yasar S, Xia J, Yao W, et al. Antihypertensive drugs

45: 175–8. decrease risk of Alzheimer disease: ginkgo evaluation of
53. Popescu BO, Toescu EC, Popescu LM, et al. Blood–brain memory study. Neurology 2013; 81: 896–903.
barrier alterations in ageing and dementia. J Neurol Sci 65. Kume K, Hanyu H, Sakurai H, et al. Effects of telmisartan
2009; 283: 99–106. on cognition and regional cerebral blood flow in hyperten-
54. Tsukuda K, Mogi M, Li JM, et al. Amelioration of cognitive sive patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Geriatr Gerontol Int
impairment in the type-2 diabetic mouse by the angiotensin 2012; 12: 207–214.
II type-1 receptor blocker candesartan. Hypertension 2007; 66. Singh B, Sharma B, Jaggi AS, et al. Attenuating effect of
50: 1099–1105. lisinopril and telmisartan in intracerebroventricular strep-
55. Awad AS. Role of AT1 receptors in permeability of the tozotocin induced experimental dementia of Alzheimer’s
blood-brain barrier in diabetic hypertensive rats. Vasc disease type: possible involvement of PPAR-γ agonistic
Pharmacol 2006; 45: 141–147. property. J Renin Angiotensin Aldosterone Syst 2013; 14:
56. Pelisch N, Hosomi N, Ueno M, et al. Blockade of AT1 124–136.
receptors protects the blood–brain barrier and improves 67. Kurinami H, Shimamura M, Sato N, et al. Do angioten-
cognition in Dahl salt-sensitive hypertensive rats. Am J sin receptor blockers protect against Alzheimer’s disease?
Hypertens 2011; 24: 362–368 Drugs Aging 2013; 30: 367–372.
57. Saavedra JM and Benicky J. Brain and peripheral angioten- 68. Min LJ, Mogi M, Shudou M, et al. Peroxisome prolifera-
sin II play a major role in stress. Stress 2007; 10: 185–193. tor-activated receptor-γ activation with angiotensin II type
58. Wang JM, Tan J and Leenen FH. Central nervous system 1 receptor blockade is pivotal for the prevention of blood–
blockade by peripheral administration of AT1 receptor brain barrier impairment and cognitive decline in type 2 dia-
blockers. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 2003; 41: 593–599. betic mice. Hypertension 2012; 59: 1079–1088.
59. Aguilera G, Young WS, Kiss A, et al. Direct regulation of 69. Zhuo J, Song K, Abdelrahman A, et al. Blockade by intra-
hypothalamic corticotropin-releasinghormone neurons by venous losartan of AT1 angiotensin II receptors in rat brain,
angiotensin II. Neuroendocrinology 1995; 61: 437–444. kidney and adrenals demonstrated by in vitro autoradiogra-
60. Saavedra JM, Sánchez-Lemus E and Benicky J. Blockade of phy. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 1994; 21: 557–567.
brain angiotensin II AT1 receptors ameliorates stress, anxi- 70. Muders F, Palkovits M, Bahner U, et al. Central inhibition
ety, brain inflammation and ischemia: therapeutic implica- of AT1 receptors by eprosartan–in vitro autoradiography in
tions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011; 36: 1–18. the brain. Pharmacol Res 2001; 43: 251–255.

You might also like