You are on page 1of 6

International Journal of Trend in Scientific Research and Development (IJTSRD)

Volume 6 Issue 7, November-December 2022 Available Online: www.ijtsrd.com e-ISSN: 2456 – 6470

Apraxia of Speech and Grammatical Language Impairment in


Children with Autism: Procedural Deficit Hypothesis
Dr. Maria Grace Treasa, Ph.D (SLP)
Senior Pediatric Speech-Language Pathologist, Aster KIND, Aster Medcity, Kochi, Kerala, India

ABSTRACT How to cite this paper: Dr. Maria Grace


The presence and extent of motor speech and grammatical language Treasa "Apraxia of Speech and
impairments are very heterogeneous in children with autism spectrum Grammatical Language Impairment in
disorders (ASD). Childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) is a prevalent Children with Autism: Procedural
concomitant motor speech disorder in children with autism affecting Deficit Hypothesis" Published in
International
imitation skills and impairing speech acquisition due to motor Journal of Trend in
sequencing deficits. Children with CAS also exhibit procedural Scientific Research
learning impairments (Iuzzini-Seigel, 2021). and Development
Ullman (2006) hypothesized in the ‘Procedural Deficit Hypothesis’ (ijtsrd), ISSN:
that procedural memory deficit underlies grammatical language 2456-6470,
Volume-6 | Issue-7, IJTSRD52447
impairments in ASD. This causes difficulty in formulating sentences
December 2022,
on the basis of grammatical or word order sequencing rules. pp.977-982, URL:
Therefore, children with ASD prefer to retrieve and use pre-learnt www.ijtsrd.com/papers/ijtsrd52447.pdf
utterances stored as whole units in their semantic memory.
Literature review suggests disruption of a common FOXP2 Copyright © 2022 by author (s) and
(Forkhead Box P2) gene in both CAS and developmental language International Journal of Trend in
Scientific Research and Development
impairment (Morgan, Fisher, Scheffer & Hildebrand, 2016).
Journal. This is an
Evidence also suggests that the same gene FOXP2 underlies Open Access article
functioning of procedural memory system (Ullman et. al., 1997). distributed under the
From the review of literature it is hypothesized that procedural terms of the Creative Commons
learning deficit is the plausible cause of grammatical language Attribution License (CC BY 4.0)
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0)
impairment and apraxia of speech in ASD. The potential implication
of this review is that procedural memory training could enhance both KEYWORDS: Procedural Deficit
speech motor planning/programming and grammar in children with Hypothesis, Autism Spectrum
ASD. Disorder, Childhood Apraxia of
Speech
1. INTRODUCTION 1.2. Childhood Apraxia of Speech (CAS)
1.1. Communication, Language and Speech Sequencing is vital for both sentence formulation/
Communication is crucial to strengthen human grammar and motor speech production. Childhood
relationships and to realize the purpose of life. The apraxia of speech (CAS) is a neurological speech
process of communication involves sending and sound disorder of motor sequencing and learning
receiving both verbal and nonverbal messages. The complex movements (Crary & Anderson, 1991) in the
input message is understood through a network of absence of neuromuscular deficits. The core
brain connections for receptive language. impairment in planning or programming
Subsequently, response or output message is spatiotemporal parameters of movement results in
formulated at expressive language areas in the frontal errors in speech sound production. This affects child’s
lobe based on linguistic rules. Motor planning and speech intelligibility, word formation, inflection or
programming of the movements necessary for rhythm. Difficulty with intentional production of
production of this planned utterance occurs. Then, speech as in a speech imitation task, is commonly
command is given to the respiratory, phonatory, observed. Children with mild CAS may be able to
articulatory, and resonatory subsystems for speech repeat monosyllables like no/ baa/ moo, but may
production. Speech, the vocal expression of language, struggle repeating multisyllabic words like potato/
is finally executed by a controlled and precisely umbrella/ butterfly. This is because of the greater
coordinated sequential movement of tongue, jaw, lips number of articulatory movements and coordination
and soft palate. involved in production of multisyllabic utterances.

@ IJTSRD | Unique Paper ID – IJTSRD52447 | Volume – 6 | Issue – 7 | November-December 2022 Page 977
International Journal of Trend in Scientific Research and Development @ www.ijtsrd.com eISSN: 2456-6470
1.3. Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) automatically, while it was acquired implicitly
Autism (or autism spectrum disorders, ASD) is a (without conscious recall) through repeated exposure
complex neurodevelopmental condition characterized to and practice of a task (Squire, 1986; Ewen &
by persistent social communication deficits and Mostofsky, 2013). Hence, it is called as implicit/non-
restricted/repetitive behaviors or interests. declarative memory. Furthermore, Ullman (2001,
Approximately 1/100 children are diagnosed with 2004) explained that procedural memory sustains
ASD around the world (Zeidan et al., 2022). Autism grammar/sentence formation because it’s a rule-
begins in early childhood and has a wide range of governed sequential combination of units.
symptoms and severity. Many children show signs of
On the contrary, lexical knowledge about words, facts
autism in early infancy while others go through a
and events are rapidly and consciously acquired/used
period of regression between 18 and 24 months of
using the semantic/explicit/declarative memory, as
age. They lose previously acquired language skills,
distinguished by Paradis (2004) and Ullman (2004).
eye contact or response to their name, and become
Evidence suggests that semantic memory is rooted in
socially withdrawn. On providing early intervention,
temporal lobe structures while the procedural memory
they acquire new vocabulary but thereafter may
is rooted in frontal/basal-ganglia structures and they
exhibit grammar or word order sequencing deficits.
play critical roles in language production (Ullman et
Apraxia of speech is a prevalent concomitant disorder
al., 1997). The discovery of the disassociation
in many children with autism contributing to
between declarative memory and procedural memory
significant delay in emergence of speech. The
developed principally from the study of amnestic
presence and extent of motor speech and grammatical
patients in the 1970s (Squire,1986).
impairments are very heterogeneous in children with
autism. 2.2. Procedural vs Semantic Memory
Impairments
2. Literature findings Ullman (2013) distinguished procedural and semantic
Although substantial research on the various types of
memory impairments in developmental and adult-
speech and language impairments is evident, only few
onset disorders (Table 1). Developmental disorders
researchers have attempted to identify the link
like ASD and specific language impairment have
between memory, speech, language and its clinical
impaired procedural memory and relatively intact
implications. Review of literature reveals that both
semantic memory. Semantic memory may play
motor sequencing (speech) and grammar (language)
compensatory roles in these disorders. On the
are sustained by the procedural memory system in the
contrary, procedural memory is intact and semantic
long-term memory store.
memory is impaired in adult cognitive
2.1. Procedural and Semantic Memory communication disorders like dementia and fluent
Procedural memory involves unconscious recollection aphasia.
of how to perform a task, and performing the task
Table 1: Procedural vs semantic memory impairment across various disorders
Procedural Semantic
Different Disorders
memory memory
Specific language impairment, Autism spectrum disorder, Tourette syndrome,
Impaired Intact
Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and non-fluent aphasia
Alzheimer’s disease, semantic Intact Impaired
dementia, fluent aphasia and
amnesia
2.3. Procedural Memory in Neurodevelopmental Disorders
A systematic review of literature on procedural memory across various neurodevelopmental disorders was done
and summarized in Table 2. Earlier research focused on exploring procedural memory impairments in autism
spectrum disorder (ASD) and specific language impairment (SLI). More recently procedural memory has been
studied in disorders like childhood apraxia of speech (CAS). Research also provides evidence for the
compensatory roles of declarative memory across neurodevelopmental disorders (Ullman & Pullman, 2015) like
SLI, dyslexia, ASD, Tourette syndrome, and obsessive-compulsive disorder.

@ IJTSRD | Unique Paper ID – IJTSRD52447 | Volume – 6 | Issue – 7 | November-December 2022 Page 978
International Journal of Trend in Scientific Research and Development @ www.ijtsrd.com eISSN: 2456-6470
Table 2: Procedural memory in various neurodevelopmental disorders
No Authors Year Disorder Finding
1 Toichi & Kamio 2002 ASD Superior “rote memory”
Procedural deficit causes grammar impairment;
2 Ullman & Pierpont 2005 SLI
lexical or declarative memory is relatively spared.
Slower procedural learning when language
Tomblin, Mainela-
3 2007 SLI impairment was defined in terms of grammar
Arnold & Zhang
impairments.
Language
4 Kemény & Lukács 2010 Impaired procedural learning
impairment
Faulty procedural memory causes deficits in
5 Romero-Munguía 2013 ASD
several cognitive skills
Developmental
6 Hedenius et. al 2013 Impaired implicit sequence learning
Dyslexia
Lum, Conti-Ramsden, Impaired procedural learning in serial reaction
7 2014 SLI
Morgan & Ullman time (SRT) task
Tourette Enhanced grammar processing and procedural
8 Takács et. al 2018
syndrome memory
Rochette, Soulières, Atypical relationship between frontal area and
9 2018 ASD
Berthiaume, & Godbout encoding of sensory-motor procedural memory
10 Bombonato et. al 2022 CAS Implicit learning deficit (using SRT task).

3. Discussions 3.2. Procedural Memory Impairment and CAS


3.1. Procedural Deficit Hypothesis and ASD Children with CAS exhibit struggle in smoothly and
Walenski, Tager-Flusberg, and Ullman (2006) precisely sequencing speech sound into syllables,
subsequently hypothesized in the ‘Procedural Deficit syllables into words, and words into sentences with
Hypothesis’ that procedural memory deficit underlies appropriate prosody (Morgan, Fisher, Scheffer &
grammatical language impairments in children with Hildebrand, 2016). This is because of impaired
autism. This causes difficulty in formulating production, sequencing, timing, and stress of sounds,
sentences on the basis of grammatical or word order syllables and words (American Speech-Language-
sequencing rules. However, lexical knowledge, which Hearing Association, 2007). The core features of
depends on the declarative memory system, remains CAS include inconsistent speech errors, lengthened
relatively spared in children with ASD. Children with and disrupted co-articulatory transitions and
ASD compensate for procedural memory impairment inappropriate prosody. Children with CAS also
by learning these words as whole lexical units using exhibited procedural learning impairments (Iuzzini-
their intact semantic/declarative memory. Ullman and Seigel, 2021) as supported by studies examining the
Pullman (2015) explained this as the ‘See-Saw effect’ number of practice trials. Maas et al (2019) reported
in their ‘dual-systems’ model of language acquisition. that children with CAS showed greater benefit from
Procedural memory impairment and the ‘See-Saw the massed practice (many trials in a short period of
effect’ in children with ASD was also explored by the time) conditions. Likewise, Edeal and Gildersleeve-
present author (Treasa, 2021) in previous Neumann (2011) found greater improvement for
investigations. If a novel utterance has be learnt, training in high (100-150 trials/session) practice as
repeated teaching trials (discrete trial training) with compared to low (30-40 trials/session) practice
specific stimuli for each of the target utterance should amount.
be used for successful procedural learning. This was Iuzzini-Seigal (2021) used an adapted version of the
evident in a case study by Treasa and Chengappa classic serial reaction time task (Nissen & Bullemer,
(2014) who found that discrete trial training (which 1987) to test procedural learning of a five-step
incorporates principles of procedural learning) and visuospatial sequence. They found that procedural
incidental teaching was useful in emergence of learning impairment was more evident in CAS and
expressive grammatical morphology. In addition, those with poor grammar. Interestingly, their findings
comorbid impairments such as childhood apraxia of show that children with CAS ultimately demonstrated
speech (CAS) is very common in ASD. Receptive procedural learning although they had slower reaction
language can be intact while expressive language can times and required more trials to initially learn the
get confounded due to the presence of CAS. sequence. Iuzzini-Seigal (2021) reviewed various

@ IJTSRD | Unique Paper ID – IJTSRD52447 | Volume – 6 | Issue – 7 | November-December 2022 Page 979
International Journal of Trend in Scientific Research and Development @ www.ijtsrd.com eISSN: 2456-6470
studies and stated that “while typically developing can enhance both speech motor
children are able to acquire speech sounds, grammar, planning/programming and grammar in children with
and motor patterns without being explicitly taught, autism.
children with CAS tend to require copious amounts of
5. References
intensive treatment to make even minimal gains”. [1] Bombonato, C. et al. (2022). Implicit learning
3.3. FOXP2: Linking CAS, language impairment in children with Childhood Apraxia of Speech,
and procedural memory Research in Developmental Disabilities, 122,
Review of the research literature suggests disruption 104170, ISSN 0891-4222,
of a common FOXP2 (Forkhead Box P2) gene in both https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2021.104170.
CAS and developmental language impairment. [2] Crary, M. A., & Anderson, P. (1991). Speech
Morgan, Fisher, Scheffer and Hildebrand (2016) and motor performance in developmental
posited that CAS is the core phenotype of all FOXP2- apraxia of speech. Technical session presented
related speech and language disorders, regardless of at the annual convention of the American-
the underlying genetic alteration. This corroborates Speech-Language-Hearing Association,
evidence from previous research by Laffin et al Atlanta, GA.
(2012) and Turner et al (2013). There can be co-
morbid FOXP2-related language disorder. Watkins et [3] Edeal, D. M., & Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. E.
al (2002) reported that these children have stronger (2011). The importance of production
nonverbal (performance) IQ compared to verbal IQ, frequency in therapy for childhood apraxia of
and their fine and gross motor skills are intact. The speech. American Journal of Speech-Language
phenotype of FOXP2-plus-related disorders tends to Pathology, 20(2), 95–110.
be more severe clinically and have features of oral https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360 (2011/09-
motor deficits, global developmental delay or autism 0005.
spectrum disorder (Morgan, Fisher, Scheffer & [4] Ewen, J., & Mostofsky, S. (2013). Procedural
Hildebrand, 2016). Memory. In: Volkmar, F.R. (eds) Encyclopedia
Review also indicates that the same gene FOXP2 of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Springer, New
underlies functioning of procedural memory system York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-
which is affected in children with grammatical 1698-3_1740.
impairment and CAS. Takahashi, Liu, Hirokawa and [5] Hedenius, M. et al. (2013). Impaired implicit
Takahashi (2003) reported in their study that striatum sequence learning in children with
is involved in procedural memory processing, and developmental dyslexia, Research in
mutation of FOXP2 results in neurological speech Developmental Disabilities, 34(11), 3924-3935,
and language disorders. Their suggestion is consistent ISSN 0891-4222,
with the findings of Ullman et al. (1997) which https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.014.
indicated that procedural memory-dependent mental
grammar is rooted in the basal ganglia and the frontal [6] Iuzzini-Seigel, J. (2021). Procedural Learning,
cortex. Grammar, and Motor Skills in Children With
Childhood Apraxia of Speech, Speech Sound
4. Conclusion Disorder, and Typically Developing Speech.
To summarize, procedural memory deficit is causing Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
sequencing difficulty in grammatical impairments Research, 64(4), 1081–1103.
(evident as word sequencing error) as well as https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-
childhood apraxia of speech (sound or syllable 00581.
sequencing error). In addition, procedural deficit has
been reported in children with autism spectrum [7] Kemény, F. & Lukács, Á. (2010). Impaired
disorders who commonly have co-morbid CAS and procedural learning in language impairment:
exhibit grammatical language impairment. Evidence Results from probabilistic categorization,
also suggests that the same FOXP2 gene which Journal of Clinical and Experimental
underlies functioning of procedural memory system is Neuropsychology, 32(3), 249-258,
found to be disrupted in both ASD and CAS. From https://doi.org/ 10.1080/1380339090297113.
the review of literature, it is hypothesized that [8] Laffin, J. J., Raca, G., Jackson, C. A., Strand,
procedural learning deficit is the plausible cause of E. A., Jakielski, K. J., & Shriberg, L. D. (2012).
grammatical language impairment and childhood Novel candidate genes and regions for
apraxia of speech in ASD. The potential implication childhood apraxia of speech identified by array
for future studies is that procedural memory training comparative genomic hybridization. Genetics in

@ IJTSRD | Unique Paper ID – IJTSRD52447 | Volume – 6 | Issue – 7 | November-December 2022 Page 980
International Journal of Trend in Scientific Research and Development @ www.ijtsrd.com eISSN: 2456-6470
medicine: official journal of the American [17] Takahashi, K., Liu, F. C., Hirokawa, K., &
College of Medical Genetics, 14(11), 928–936. Takahashi, H. (2003). Expression of Foxp2, a
https://doi.org/10.1038/gim.2012.72. gene involved in speech and language, in the
developing and adult striatum. Journal of
[9] Lum, J.A.G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Morgan,
Neuroscience Research, 73(1), 61–72.
A.T., & Ullman, M. T. (2014). Procedural
https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.10638.
learning deficits in specific language
impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial [18] Toichi, M., & Kamio, Y. (2002). Long-term
reaction time task performance, Cortex, 51, 1- memory and levels-of-processing in autism.
10, ISSN 0010-9452, Neuropsychologia, 40(7), 964–969.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.011. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932 (01)00163-
4.
[10] Maas, E., Gildersleeve-Neumann, C., Jakielski,
K., Kovacs, N., Stoeckel, R., Vradelis, H., & [19] Tomblin, J. B., Mainela-Arnold, E., & Zhang,
Welsh, M. (2019). Bang for Your Buck: A X. (2007). Procedural Learning in Adolescents
Single-Case Experimental Design Study of With and Without Specific Language
Practice Amount and Distribution in Treatment Impairment, Language Learning and
for Childhood Apraxia of Speech. Journal of Development, 3(4), 269-293,
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, https://doi.org/10.1080/15475440701377477.
62(9), 3160–3182. [20] Treasa, M. G. (2021). Procedural Memory in
https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-18- Children with Autism: Double Dissociation?
0212. International Journal of Trend in Scientific
[11] Morgan, A., Fisher, S. E., Scheffer, I., & Research and Development, ISSN: 2456-6470,
Hildebrand, M. (2016). FOXP2-Related Speech 5 (4), 1327-1330, URL:
and Language Disorders. In M. P. Adam (Eds.) www.ijtsrd.com/papers/ijtsrd42557.pdf.
et. al., GeneReviews®. University of [21] Treasa, M. G., & Chengappa, S. K. (2014).
Washington, Seattle. Emergence of expressive grammatical
[12] Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). morphemes following Discrete Trial Training
Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence and Incidental Teaching: A Case Study.
from performance measures. Cognitive Language in India, 14(1).
Psychology, 19(1), 1–32. [22] Turner, S. J., Hildebrand, M. S., Block, S.,
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8. Damiano, J., Fahey, M., Reilly, S., Bahlo, M.,
[13] Paradis, M. (2004). A neurolinguistic theory of Scheffer, I. E., & Morgan, A. T. (2013). Small
bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. intragenic deletion in FOXP2 associated with
Rochette, A.-C., Soulières, I., Berthiaume, C. childhood apraxia of speech and dysarthria.
and Godbout, R. (2018), NREM sleep EEG American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A,
activity and procedural memory: A comparison 161A (9), 2321–2326.
between young neurotypical and autistic adults https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.36055.
without sleep complaints. Autism Research, 11, [23] Ullman, M. T. (2001). The
613-623. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1933. declarative/procedural model of lexicon and
[14] Romero-Munguía M. Á. (2013). Theory of grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic
Mind Deficit versus Faulty Procedural Memory Research, 30(1), 37–69.
in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Autism research https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005204207369.
and treatment, 128264. [24] Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of
https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/128264. memory circuits to language: The
[15] Squire, L. R. (1986). Mechanisms of memory. declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92,
Science, 232, 1612–1619. 231–270.
[16] Takács, Á. et al. (2018). Is procedural memory [25] Ullman, M. T. et al. (1997). A neural
enhanced in Tourette syndrome? Evidence from dissociation within language: Evidence that the
a sequence learning task, Cortex, 100, 84-94, mental dictionary is part of declarative
ISSN 0010-9452, memory, and that grammatical rules are
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.037. processed by the procedural system. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience 9(2): 266-276.

@ IJTSRD | Unique Paper ID – IJTSRD52447 | Volume – 6 | Issue – 7 | November-December 2022 Page 981
International Journal of Trend in Scientific Research and Development @ www.ijtsrd.com eISSN: 2456-6470
[26] Ullman, M. T (2013). The role of declarative [29] Walenski, M., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Ullman,
and procedural memory in disorders of M. T. (2006). Language in Autism. In S. O.
language. Linguistic Variation, 13, 133-154. Moldin & J. L. R. Rubenstein (Eds.),
Understanding autism: From basic
[27] Ullman, M.T. & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific
Language Impairment is not Specific to neuroscience to treatment (pp.175–203). CRC
Press /Routledge/ Taylor & Francis Group.
Language: the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis,
Cortex, 41(3), 399-433, ISSN 0010-9452, https://doi.org/10.1201/9781420004205.ch9.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452 (08)70276- [30] Zeidan, J., Fombonne, E., Scorah, J., Ibrahim,
4. A., Durkin, M. S., Saxena, S., Yusuf, A., Shih,
[28] Ullman, M. T., & Pullman, M. Y. (2015). A A., & Elsabbagh, M. (2022). Global prevalence
compensatory role for declarative memory in of autism: A systematic review update. Autism
Research, 15(5), 778–790.
neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 205-222. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2696.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.008

@ IJTSRD | Unique Paper ID – IJTSRD52447 | Volume – 6 | Issue – 7 | November-December 2022 Page 982

You might also like