You are on page 1of 11

European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

European Management Journal


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/emj

When the victim becomes vicious: Combined effects of pseudo


transformational leadership and epistemic motivation on contempt
and deviant behaviors
Fauzia Syed, Saima Naseer, Javaria Nawaz*, Syed Zulfiqar Ali Shah
Faculty of Management Sciences International Islamic University Sector H 10, Islamabad, Pakistan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Utilizing affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), the current study unravels how and when
Received 30 August 2019 pseudo-transformational leaders promote contempt, avoidance, and aggression. Specifically, we exam-
Received in revised form ined whether pseudo-transformational leadership enforces feelings of contempt, which, in turn, fosters
2 July 2020
interaction avoidance and covert aggression in followers. We hypothesized that contempt also has an
Accepted 16 July 2020
indirect effect that is contingent on levels of epistemic motivation of followers. By adopting a multi-wave
Available online 30 July 2020
research design, we gathered data from 277 employees of the service sector of Pakistan. The results
signified that followers of pseudo-transformational leaders feel contemptuous of them and respond with
Keywords:
Pseudo-transformational leadership
interaction avoidance and indirect aggression. Further, we found that the impact of pseudo-
Contempt transformational leadership on contempt was more pronounced in individuals with higher levels of
Indirect aggression epistemic motivation. We believe that this study opens up a new avenue in the pseudo-transformational
Interaction avoidance leadership literature by highlighting the mechanism and conditions under which such leaders are
Epistemic motivation deleterious for organizations.
© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction attention. Specifically, one such self-interested leadership named


pseudo-transformational leadership is a relatively unheeded area
Leadership phenomenon has been glamorized since its begin- (Lin, Huang, Chen, & Huang, 2017; Naber & Moffett, 2017; Schmid,
ning and focused primarily on the beneficial effects for followers Pircher Verdorfer, & Peus, 2018).
and organizations (Schilling, 2009). The new millennium has Bass and Steidlmeier (1999) asserted that there are two
brought critical challenges to the workplace and dark leadership opposing types of transformational leaders in an organization, i.e.
has emerged as one of the most prevalent employee concerns authentic transformational and pseudo-transformational leader-
(Krasikova, Green, & LeBreton, 2013; Schyns & Shillings, 2013). The ship. Authentic transformational leaders are presented as heroic
presence of “bad bosses” is thus a pertinent reality of the corporate figures with altruistic behaviors whose ultimate focus is always on
world since a bundle of corporate scandals is ascribed to organi- the greater good, resulting in employee well-being and organiza-
zational leaders’ dysfunctional behaviors and tendencies (Brown, tional success (Braun, Peus, Weisweiler, & Frey, 2013; Crane &
Trevino, & Harrison, 2005; Huang & Paterson, 2017). Hartwell, 2018; Frieder, Wang, & Oh, 2018). Pseudo-
Extant research ascertained the self-interest and egotistic mo- transformational leaders, however, are labeled as unethical and
tives of leaders as strong driving forces behind their deleterious inauthentic transformational leaders. Such leaders who bring
behaviors (Einarsen, Aasland, & Skogstad, 2007; Schyns & Schilling, enormous harm to followers and organizations are characterized
2013). However, how such leaders influence followers and bring by egotistic motives (House & Howell, 1992; Howell & Avolio, 1992).
harm to employees and organizations is not yet given due Bass and Steidlmeier (1999) took a behavioral approach in
conceptualizing pseudo-transformational leadership and defined
such leaders as the ones who fail to uphold ethical values and moral
* Corresponding author.
principles which are essential for transformational leaders. Pseudo-
E-mail addresses: syedfauzia@ymail.com, fauzia.syed@iiu.edu.pk (F. Syed), transformational leaders exhibit self-interested behaviors,
saimanaseersheikh@yahoo.com (S. Naseer), javaria.nawaz@yahoo.com (J. Nawaz), whereby they may exploit transformational behaviors to promote
zulfiqar.shah@iiu.edu.pk (S.Z.A. Shah).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.emj.2020.07.005
0263-2373/© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246 237

their hidden agendas by demanding unconditional loyalty and reasoning, emotions such as fear (Barling et al., 2008; Bass & Riggio,
complete submission from their followers (Barling, Christie, & 2006; Christie et al., 2011) have been studied in the context of
Turner, 2008; Bass & Steidlmeier, 1999; Conger & Kanungo, 1998). pseudo-transformational leadership, where individuals develop
In contrast to authentic transformational leaders, less attention fear from their leaders due to his/her self-serving behaviors. Also,
has been paid to pseudo-transformational leadership and its there is an outcry of recent calls for more research on the link of
impact on followers and organizations (Hughes & Harris, 2017; dark leadership with negative emotions and destructive outcomes
Naber & Moffett, 2017), thus we know very little how such leaders (Gooty, Connelly, Griffith, & Gupta, 2010; Lebel, 2017). Specifically,
harm employees and the organizations. Specifically, literature is researchers called for more research to unveil the impact of dark,
sparse that under what conditions and how employees respond to egotistic and manipulative leaders on follower’s discrete emotions
such leaders (Hoption, Barling, & Turner, 2013; Lin et al., 2017; and their distinct behavioral reactions (Schmid et al., 2018; Oh &
Schuh, Zhang, & Tian, 2013). Moreover, mainly leadership research Farh, 2017). Contempt has also been studied as an underlying
has focused on deviant behaviors such as leader directed aggres- mechanism between culturally (in)congruent leadership and fol-
sion, hostility, and retaliation, which are ultimately self-defeating lower outcomes (Sund & Lines, 2017). This stream of research
for followers’ selves (Decoster, Stouten, & Tripp, 2019) and less explicitly signifies the relevance and use of emotion of contempt
attention is given to deviant behaviors that are covert and con- between other leadership styles and outcomes. Based on these
cealed (Lian et al., 2014). Particularly in the context of self-serving recent calls, the current study thus unfolds state contempt, a
(Decoster et al., 2019) and inauthentic or pseudo- powerful, prevalent yet theoretically discounted discrete emotion
transformational leadership, the past research has mostly focused (Fischer & Sorolla, 2016) as an underlying mechanism to explain
on negative outcomes such as deviant behaviors (e.g. Bass & Riggio, employee reactions and behaviors under pseudo-transformational
2006; Barling et al., 2008; Christie, Barling, & Turner, 2011; Schuh leaders.
et al., 2013) and lower proactive behaviors (Eisenbeib & Boerner, Followers play a crucial role in success or failure of any leader-
2013; Lin et al., 2017). This stream of research depicts that fol- ship; thus, it is imperative to know how followers perceive and
lowers of pseudo-transformational leaders tend to develop the fear, assess their leaders’ actions (Avolio & Gardner, 2005; Clapp-Smith,
apparently comply with the orders of leaders, are less likely to Vogelgesang, & Avey, 2009; Van Gils, Van Quaquebeke, Van
indulge in explicit retaliation, and are more likely to adopt covert Knippenberg, Van Dijke, & De Cremer, 2015). Ignoring the dispo-
aggression and retaliation strategies. Hence, given the untapped sitional aspects of understanding pseudo-transformational leaders
nature of this research field, the primary motivation for this study (Christie et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2017; Nifadkar, Tsui, & Ashforth,
stems from the need to explore the processes and conditions 2012) and developing contempt will lead to an incomplete pic-
through which pseudo-transformational leadership is associated ture of the whole process. Employees due to differences in their
with deviance (avoidance and aggressive behaviors). While estab- personalities respond differently to environmental and organiza-
lishing the underlying mechanisms and boundary conditions tional provocations including leadership (Bies & Tripp, 2005). Much
associated with pseudo-transformational leadership and outcomes, of the literature on pseudo-transformational leadership has
we focus on two types of deviance: (a) interaction avoidance and focused on examining the leader’s dark-side personality traits
(b) indirect aggression. (House & Howell, 1992) and egoistic values (Price, 2003). However,
Interaction avoidance is one’s deliberate attempt to evade this research has ignored the role of the follower’s personality and
interaction with another person, which is an important stress characteristics that can play in evaluating and responding to
coping strategy (Lazarus & Folkman, 1991), whereas employee pseudo-transformational leadership. Hence, the current study
aggression is an important and costly phenomenon in organiza- identifies followers’ epistemic motivation e a tendency associated
tions (Fida et al., 2018; Hassard, Teoh, Visockaite, Dewe, & Cox, with deep information processing would help individuals to un-
2018). Covert aggression has long-term devastating effects on or- leash self-serving intentions behind apparent transformational
ganizations than direct aggression, yet empirical evidence linking leadership.
dark leadership and employee covert aggression is scarce (Lian In this inquiry, we mainly build on the underpinnings of affec-
et al., 2014). Past research has linked pseudo-transformational tive events theory (AET; Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). AET asserts
leadership to contextual performance and organizational devi- that arousal of affect or discrete emotions can be ascribed to certain
ance (Christie et al., 2011; Schuh et al., 2013), neglecting the role events at the workplace and these affective responses then trigger
that such leaders play in instigating indirect aggression. This sig- distinct behavioral and attitudinal reactions. Along with this, it
nifies a missing link of pseudo-transformational leadership with substantiates the role of individual dispositions and motivations in
covert aggression including indirect aggression and interaction arousal of affective responses and behavioral reactions (Weiss &
avoidance, which needs to be examined. Cropanzano, 1996). Our study setting provides an ideal context
Existing research claims that how dark leaders influence fol- for examining the unique mechanisms and boundary conditions of
lowers is not a simple process and is likely to flow through some pseudo-transformational leadership behaviors. An eastern cultural
mediational mechanism (Eissa, Chinchanachokchai, & Wyland, context of Pakistan, which scores high on power distance, collec-
2017). Dark leaders can trigger affective responses in employees, tivism, and uncertainty avoidance along with masculine and short-
which might influence their behaviors (Glaso, Skogstad, Notelaers term orientation (Hofstede, 1991), can add fruitful insights into the
& Einarsen, 2018; Lian et al., 2014). Although leadership and dynamics of dark leadership. Past research has already corrobo-
affect relationship is established, it is still surrounded by ambigu- rated that dark leadership is more prevalent in power distant,
ities and holds ample scope for further investigation (Ashkanasy, collectivist, masculine, and uncertainty avoidance cultures
Humpherey & Huy, 2017; Glaso et al., 2018; Oh & Farh, 2017). (Luthans, Peterson, & Ibrayeva, 1998). House (1995) asserted that
More specifically, existing studies have shown that the impact of the majority of research on leadership comes from Western cul-
pseudo-transformational leadership on outcomes is not simple and tures that are more individualistic and there is a pressing need to
passes through certain underlying mechanisms and boundary explicate the way in which leadership, specifically dark leadership,
conditions (Christie et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2017). Moreover, only a influences follower outcomes across a variety of samples and cul-
handful of studies have investigated the impact of pseudo- tures. By utilizing a sample of employees belonging to the eastern
transformational leadership on emotions and outcomes of fol- context of Pakistan, our study offers new prospects to examine
lowers (Lin et al., 2017; Schuh et al., 2013). In line with the same pseudo-transformational leader’s effects in an ideal cultural setting.
238 F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246

Our research makes four important contributions. First, the model of pseudo-transformational leadership that consisted of two
current study aims to answer the recent calls for more research on of the four transformational leadership behaviors as viewed by the
the potentially dark side of transformational leaders (Lin et al., followers, namely, idealized influence and inspirational motivation.
2017; Naber & Moffett, 2017); thus, it adds to the nascent litera- They assert that transformational leaders display an idealized in-
ture on pseudo-transformational leadership by studying employee fluence which indicates that such leaders place the group interests
interaction avoidance and indirect aggression as probable out- above their self-interests, whereas pseudo-transformational
comes of such leadership. Second, by treating contempt as an leaders promote their self-interests by controlling and subduing
intervening mechanism between pseudo-transformational leader- followers (Barling et al., 2008; Bass & Steidlmeier, 1999). Second,
ship and outcomes (Lin et al., 2017), current research adds to the the inspirational motivation dimension of transformational lead-
limited research on underlying mechanisms in the realm of ership signifies a leader’s capacity to enhance the follower’s un-
pseudo-transformational leadership (Lin et al., 2017), thus adding derstanding and commitment to collective goals and motivate
to the domain of dark leadership and outcomes relationship them to achieve such goals. However, pseudo-transformational
through the lens of affect/emotions such as contempt. Third, recent leaders are quite well versed in communicating their self-
studies highlight the need to examine different dispositional vari- interested beliefs, but they falsely use their inspiration to deceive
ables that may strengthen or weaken the relationship between followers through fake promises and invoke emotionality rather
negative leadership-affect/emotions relationships (Ferris, Yan, Lim, than logic for motivating followers (Bass & Steidlmeier, 1999;
Chen, & Fatimah, 2016). Therefore, the current study not only adds Conger & Kanungo, 1998). Pseudo-transformational leaders
to the literature on emotions (contempt) but also accentuates an formulate and communicate a vision that discounts the best in-
important and unexplored boundary condition of pseudo- terests of the followers and utilize their status and position to attain
transformational leadership, namely, epistemic motivation. personal gain (Dasborough & Ashkanasy, 2002).
Fourth, integration of AET to the literature of self-serving leader- Past research argued that, if leaders are perceived as belittling
ship, i.e. pseudo-transformational leadership is unique to this their followers’ interests and engage in self-focused behaviors, this
study, prior studies in this domain have not utilized the un- is likely to deteriorate followers’ motivation to engage in beneficial
derpinnings of AET as a theoretical framework to extend the liter- behaviors for organizations (Schuh et al., 2013). Thus, it is most
ature. Finally, instead of adopting a leader-centric approach as likely that interactions with such leaders will be taxing. As frequent
widely applied in transformational leadership literature (Hunter, interactions are not a part of one’s formal job requirements (Hu &
Bedell-Avers, & Mumford, 2007; Naber & Moffett, 2017), the cur- Shi, 2015), we expect that followers of pseudo-transformational
rent study proposed and tested the follower centric approach of leaders would be cautious in initiating interactions and also avoid
assessing leader’s integrity and intentions, which subsequently interactions. Additionally, recipient of mistreatment often retorts
influences their affective responses and behaviors. with deviance and offense (Hershcovis & Barling, 2010), but if the
source of aggression/mistreatment is powerful and possess control
2. Theory and hypotheses over resources, then expressing overt aggression could be detri-
mental (Aquino, Tripp, & Bies, 2006; Lian et al., 2014; Lin et al.,
2.1. Pseudo-transformational leadership and outcomes (interaction 2017). Thus, followers of pseudo-transformational leaders are
avoidance, indirect aggression) likely to indulge in fear and undue obedience to refrain themselves
from the damage caused by such self-interested leaders at the
Affective events theory (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996) is a theo- workplace (Barling et al., 2008; Christie et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2017).
retical framework that encompasses affective experiences with Therefore, such individuals do not indulge in direct aggression and
their causes and consequences, asserting the importance of emo- are more likely to balance the harm caused by leaders by with-
tions in shaping attitudes and behaviors. Central to AET is the idea drawing their performance and voluntary behaviors; and indulge in
that certain events at the workplace are perceived to be supporting/ covert aggression and deviance (Barling et al., 2008; Christie et al.,
suppressing a person’s advancement towards goals and elicit pos- 2011; Lin et al., 2017). Indirect aggression is a relevant concept in
itive/negative affective responses, which subsequently shape one’s this regard where the invader, instead of openly challenging the
attitudes and behaviors. These events can affect the general mood target, harms indirectly through social manipulation (Warren,
or elicit distinct emotions. Moreover, AET claims that events elicit Richardson, & McQuillin, 2011). This proposition is in line with
different intensities of emotional and affective responses in in- AET, which asserts that an event appraised negatively would result
dividuals due to their dispositions and individual differences. in dysfunctional behaviors. Therefore, when individuals appraise
Leadership has been frequently taken from the lens of “affective taxing and negative events, they would express indirect aggression
event” (Brief & Weiss, 2002; Dasborough, 2006). Dark/destructive and interaction avoidance towards their supervisors. Thus, it is
leadership has been linked to creating negative emotions and un- hypothesized that:
favorable outcomes in followers (Glaso et al., 2018; Schmid et al.,
Hypothesis 1. Pseudo-transformational leadership is positively
2018). Specifically, pseudo-transformational leadership has been
related to (a) interaction avoidance and (b) indirect aggression.
found to develop feelings of fear in followers and subsequently
decrease their performance-related outcomes and increase devi-
ance (Barling et al., 2008; Christie et al., 2011). If the leader is a 2.2. The mediating effects of contempt in pseudo-transformational
source of tension and victimization, then avoidance lies among leadership and outcomes
many of the strategies deployed to deal with such strenuous situ-
ations (Tepper, 2000; Zapf & Gross, 2001); it helps to counterbal- Utilizing AET, we propose that followers of pseudo-
ance stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1991) by buffering the discomfort transformational leaders would feel contemptuous of their leader,
surfaced after perplexing encounters with such leaders and pro- which would ultimately shape their behaviors. Past literature found
tects from further harm (Nifadkar et al., 2012). that dark leadership, i.e. destructive and exploitative (Schmid et al.,
Pseudo-transformational leaders are self-aggrandizing and 2018), supervisory abuse, and undermining (Mitchell, Vogel, &
egotistic, demanding undue submission from followers (Bass & Folger, 2015), elicit negative affect and discrete emotions such as
Steildemier, 1999); their followers fear and distrust them (Christie fear, anger, guilt, and shame in followers (Chintakananda &
et al., 2011). Barling et al. (2008) conceptualized and examined a Greguras, 2017, p. 16094; Syed, Naseer, & Khan, 2018), which
F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246 239

further translate into unfavorable behaviors. Existing research Pseudo-transformational leadership and (a) interaction avoidance
provides some evidence of pseudo-transformational leadership in and (b) indirect aggression.
generating unfavorable outcomes through certain underlying
mechanisms (Lin et al., 2017). Moreover, past research considers 2.3. Moderating effects of epistemic motivation between pseudo-
follower emotions (such as fear; Barling et al., 2008; Christie et al., transformational leadership and contempt
2011) to be an important component in defining subordinate atti-
tudes and behaviors. This line of research argues that the emotions Epistemic motivation is defined as an individual’s inclination to
and attributions that followers make about the motives of their gain a thorough, systematic and rich understanding of the world
leaders shape their evaluations and labeling of the leadership around (Kruglanski, Piero, Mannetti, & De Grada, 2006). Research
(Dasborough & Ashkanasy, 2002). However, as pseudo- describes it as a function of context and circumstances as well as a
transformational leaders engage in impression management tac- trait-like tendency (De Dreu, 2003; Lerner & Tetlock, 1999). In-
tics by hiding their self-interests, this, in turn, will not blind fol- dividuals high in epistemic motivation seek and utilize all the
lowers to the leaders’ self-aggrandizing values (Barling et al., 2008). available information for sense-making (VanKleef, Homan,
Rather followers are more likely to account for such selfish motives Beersma, Knippenerg, Knippenberg & Damen, 2009), especially
because pseudo-transformational leaders usually exploit their fol- coping with unconventionality (Amit & Sagiv, 2013). These in-
lowers (Lin et al., 2017) evident through their inconsistent actions dividuals aspire for clarity that forces them to search for additional
(Christie et al., 2011). As pseudo-transformational leaders are information and pay heed to supplementary cues to make sense of
controlling and deceitful, followers are less likely to respect the their current situation (Greifeneder & Bless, 2007). Individuals who
leader’s motives or admire the leader’s style (Christie et al., 2011). possess high epistemic motivation do not like ambiguity and pro-
Past research already corroborates that the followers of pseudo- cess information in a more thoughtful and organized manner
transformational leaders depict negative attitudes, behaviors, and (Chaiken & Trope, 1999). Such individuals have an increased ten-
fear, which inhibit them to work in best interests of the firm dency to engage in systematic information processing and rely less
(Barling et al., 2008; Christie et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2017). on heuristics and stereotypes (Mayseless & Kruglanski, 1987).
Contempt, a “social emotion”, belongs to the triad of anger- Followers are central in making sense of leader’s intentions,
contempt-disgust; it is associated with feelings of disrespect, specifically, Dasborough and Ashkanasy (2002) argued that both
disdain, and scorn for others (Miller, 1997; Rozin, Lowery, Imada, & authentic and pseudo-transformational leaders depict same
Haidt, 1999). Those in hierarchical relationships are prone to feel behavioral manifestations with distinct underlying intentions,
contempt. Specifically, employees feel contemptuous when their which are arduous to be identified; therefore, only “astute” fol-
superiors exploit them, fail to fulfill their needs and achieve lowers could label leaders as legitimate/illegitimate, and hence, we
financial benefits at their cost (Melwani & Barsade, 2011; Pelzer, conjecture that epistemic motivation would help individuals to
2005). When top management uses their subordinates as a tool assess manipulative intentions behind transformational behaviors
for their gain and exploits them in pursuit of their self-interest then of a leader. Individuals high in epistemic motivation, to maintain a
contemptuous feelings are likely to occur in subordinates (Pelzer, structured view of everything, would deal with unconventional
2005). Pseudo-transformational leaders frequently exploit their transformational behaviors (Schuh et al., 2013) by decoding them,
followers and, under influence of their manipulative intentions, and in this attempt, they would make sense of leader’s nasty in-
prefer self-interest over collective good (Barling et al., 2008; tentions. Thus, individuals high in epistemic motivation don’t fall
Christie et al., 2011), so they are unable to work for organization and prey to their exploitation and identify their egotistic intents;
society at large and violate community norms. Pseudo- therefore, the arousal of contempt would be immediate and more
transformational leaders take credit for other’s tasks (Christie pronounced in such individuals.
et al., 2011), which would make employees doubt the compe- AET proposes the influence of personality traits in appraisal and
tence of such leaders and would instigate feelings of contempt in response of events (Eissa & Lester, 2017). In line with the reasoning
them. of AET, individuals with high epistemic motivation are likely to
Moreover, contempt being distinct in its action tendencies understand the fake intentions of pseudo-transformational leaders,
would further spur interaction avoidance and covert aggressive thus resulting in increased feelings of contempt in individuals. It is
behaviors. Action tendencies associated with contempt are exclu- also plausible that epistemic motivation will conditionally influ-
sionary in nature and target of contemptuous feelings is looked ence the strength of the relationship between pseudo-
down upon, socially excluded, belittled, and derogated (Fischer & transformational leadership and outcomes (interaction avoidance
Roseman, 2007; Roseman, 2001). Emotions can regulate our and indirect aggression) through contempt, thereby indicating a
approach or avoidance behaviors (Crawford & Cacioppo, 2002) and moderated mediation pattern between study variables (see Fig. 1).
contempt being an adherent of the exclusion-emotion family Thus it is hypothesized:
(Roseman, 2001) would result in avoidance behaviors. Employees
avoid interactions with their managers/superiors when they
initially have adverse interactions with them or are mistreated by
them (Nifadkar et al., 2012; Sund & Lines, 2017). Thus, as pseudo-
transformational leaders exploit abuse and mistreat their sub-
ordinates, the employees being the target of such leaders will
develop contemptuous feelings against them and thus it is
conceivable that interaction with such a leader would be avoided.
Moreover, although upfront agitation contempt fuels behaviors
that are less intense such as indirect aggressive behaviors (Rozin
et al., 1999), individuals would be more likely to engage in indi-
rect aggression against such leaders due to their contemptuous
feelings. Thus, it is hypothesized that: Fig. 1. Depicts a moderated mediation model whereby contempt mediates the pseudo-
transformational leadership and outcomes (interaction avoidance and indirect
Hypothesis 2. Contempt will mediate the relationship between
aggression) relationship at different levels of epistemic motivation.
240 F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246

Hypothesis 3. Epistemic motivation will moderate the positive universities in Pakistan. Past studies conducted in Pakistan in
relationship between pseudo-transformational leadership and similar fields have used English questionnaires (e.g. Abbas, Raja,
contempt, such that this relationship will be stronger for high levels Darr, & Bouckenooghe, 2014; Naseer, Raja, Syed, Donia, & Darr,
of epistemic motivation as compared to low. 2016) and reported no major language-related issues. Further, we
had to brief respondents about the process of filling out the ques-
Hypothesis 4. Epistemic motivation will moderate the indirect
tionnaire because of the time-lagged nature of the data. For all
relationship between pseudo-transformational leadership and (a)
these reasons, we decided to keep all the measures in English
interaction avoidance and (b) indirect aggression through
instead of translating and back-translating surveys into Urdu.
contempt, such that this relationship will be stronger for high levels
Pseudo-transformational leadership, epistemic motivation, and
of epistemic motivation as compared to low.
interaction avoidance were measured on a seven-point Likert scale
ranging from “1 ¼ strongly disagree” to “7 ¼ strongly agree”;
3. Methods whereas contempt and indirect aggression were measured on a
seven-point Likert scale “1 ¼ Never” to “7 ¼ Everyday”.
3.1. Sample and data collection Pseudo-transformational leadership. Past literature on
pseudo-transformational leadership has measured this concept
The current study employed a three-wave research design, either using experimental techniques (Christie et al., 2011; Kazmi,
which allowed temporal segregation of measurement of the inde- 2017) or using a reverse coded measure of full transformational
pendent variable (Pseudo-transformational Leadership: T1) and leadership scale (20 items multifactor leadership questionnaire
moderator variable (Epistemic Motivation: T1), mediator variable (MLQ) scale; Bass & Avolio, 2000). We delved into a deeper ex-
(Contempt: T2) and outcome variables (Interaction avoidance, In- amination of the measurement of pseudo-transformational lead-
direct aggression: T3). The time-lag between each measurement ership scale and found that most of the studies have combined two
point was 2e3 weeks minimum. All the measures were self- dimensions of transformational leadership behaviors, i.e. idealized
reported. Adopting such design helps in addressing potential is- influence and inspirational motivation (reversed) to constitute
sues arising from solely using self-reported and single-source data pseudo-transformational leadership behaviors (Barling et al., 2008;
collection methods. Lin et al., 2017). The basic contention of these authors is that
A field survey was designed and disseminated to employees pseudo-transformational leadership can be distinguished from
working in 14 service sector firms located in Rawalpindi and authentic transformational leadership based on these two com-
Islamabad (Pakistan), using personal and professional contacts. The ponents rather than all of the components (Barling et al., 2008).
sample firms included four banks, five universities, three telecom Hence, to more closely align our conceptual and methodological
firms, one financial service providing agency, and one approach with previous research, we combine and reverse only
manufacturing firm. Respondents of the current study were mostly these two components (i.e. 8 items of idealized influence and 4
the entry and middle managers working in the above-mentioned items of inspirational motivation) out of the full transformational
firms. A cover letter explaining the objectives of the study and as- leadership scale (MLQ; Bass & Avolio, 2000) to capture a 12-item
suring them of the strictest confidentiality was attached to the pseudo-transformational leadership scale. Sample items include
questionnaire. The letter also stated that participation was volun- “My leader goes beyond self-interest” and “My leader emphasizes
tary, and it included the contact information of the principal the importance of having a collective sense of mission”. The
investigator in case participants had questions about the study or Cronbach alpha coefficient of this scale is 0.94.
wanted to share their feedback. Data were collected at three points Epistemic Motivation. Epistemic motivation was measured by
of time, thus each respondent was asked to provide a primary key utilizing 12-items need for structure scale (Neuberg & Newsom,
at time 1 and mention the same key for time 2 and time 3 re- 1993). Previous research has highlighted a plethora of studies
sponses, thus keeping track of responses from the same that have validated this scale’s capacity to differentiate among
respondent. people possessing varying chronic levels of information processing
At T1, we distributed 550 questionnaires containing measures of motivation, thus establishing it as a parsimonious, well-validated,
pseudo-transformational leadership and epistemic motivation; 430 and reliable scale for tapping epistemic motivation (Moskowitz,
of these were completed, yielding a response rate of 78%. After 1993; Thompson, Naccarato, Parker, & Moskowitz, 2001). The
three weeks of T1, we requested the participants to complete psychometric properties of the scale have been tested and validated
another questionnaire round to measure contempt (i.e. mediator). in previous work settings across several countries and samples
A total of 310 useable surveys were completed at T2, yielding a (Moskowitz, 1993; Rietzschel, De Dreu, & Nijstad, 2007; Van Kleef
response rate of 72%. Finally, three weeks after T2, we again con- et al., 2009). The items were coded such that high values depic-
tacted the same participants and asked them to fill out a third ted higher epistemic motivation. Sample items include: “I enjoy
survey measuring the study’s outcomes (T3). We received 277 having a clear and structured mode of life” and “It upsets me to go
completed surveys at a time 3 yielding a response rate of 89%. After into a situation without knowing what I can expect from it”. Reli-
removing all missing data, we achieved an overall effective ability and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) revealed negative
response rate of 50%. loadings for 4 reverse coded items, thus those items were dropped
Participants in our final sample had an average age of 29.02 and Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of scale with 8 items was 0.80.
years (SD ¼ 6.07), consisted of males (54.9%), belonged mostly to Contempt. A 3-item scale developed by Zevon and Tellegen
management/operations department (23.1%) and mostly held (1982) was used to tap the feeling of contempt; respondents
master’s degree (43.2%), had spent 4.06 years (SD ¼ 4.72) with the rated the frequency of their contemptuous feelings. Sample items
present company and had total work experience of 5.70 years include “I feel disdainful”, “I feel contemptuous”, and “I feel
(SD ¼ 5.10). scornful”. The Cronbach alpha coefficient of this scale is 0.88.
Interaction Avoidance. Social avoidance and distress scale,
3.2. Measures which is a 28-item measure originally developed by Watson and
Friend (1969), was used to measure social avoidance at work.
The survey was administered in English, as it is the official Later Hu and Shi (2015) based on the above scale developed their
language and medium of instruction at all high schools and own 3-item scale in which they used and modified the interaction
F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246 241

avoidance items by replacing “him/her” with the word “leader”. We interaction avoidance (r ¼ .30, p < .01). Similarly, contempt is
used the 3-item modified version of interaction avoidance scale positively correlated with epistemic motivation (r ¼ .18, p < .01),
adopted from Hu and Shi (2015), as it highlights the specific source indirect aggression (r¼ .44, p < .01), and interaction avoidance
(i.e. the leader) that the employee is avoiding at work rather than (r¼.32, p < .01).
just peers or any other person. For the present study, the interaction We tested proposed associations, i.e. direct, mediation, moder-
avoidance 3-item measure was self -reported by the focal respon- ation, and moderated mediation effects using Preacher and Hayes’
dent at time 3. The sample items include: “I try to stay away from (2004) bootstrapping macro. Detailed results of direct and media-
interactions with my leader at work”, “I never initiate interactions tion effects are depicted in Table 3 (a & b). Hypothesis 1 (a & b)
with my leader”, and “I often intentionally avoid opportunities to argued a positive association between pseudo-transformational
interact with my leader”. This scale has a Cronbach alpha coefficient leadership and interaction avoidance and indirect aggression,
of 0.91 in this study. respectively. The results fully support Hypothesis 1 (a & b), i.e.
Indirect Aggression. We extracted 12 items from the direct/in- pseudo-transformational leadership is positively and significantly
direct aggression scale (DIAS) developed by Bjo €
€rkqvist, Osterman, associated with interaction avoidance (B ¼ 0.25, t ¼ 2.95, p < .005)
and Kaukiainen (1992) to tap indirect aggression. The DIAS is a and indirect aggression (B ¼ 0.20, t ¼ 2.72, p < .01).
24-item scale consisting of three dimensions, i.e. direct physical Hypothesis 2 (a & b) contended an indirect effect of pseudo-
aggression (7 items), direct verbal aggression (5 items), and indirect transformational leadership on interaction avoidance and indirect
aggression (12 items). As the objective of our study is to consider aggression through contempt. The results fully supported
how followers indirectly show aggression, we utilized only the 12 Hypothesis 2 (a & b), i.e. pseudo-transformational leadership had a
items that tap indirect aggression from the DIAS. Participants rated significant indirect effect on interaction avoidance (indirect
the degree to which they indulged in indirect aggressive behaviors. effect ¼ .11, p < .01) and indirect aggression (indirect effect ¼ 0.16,
Sample items include “Ignores the other one” and “Plans secretly to p < .001). Furthermore, non-zero values in 95% bootstrapped con-
bother the other one”. This scale has a Cronbach alpha coefficient of fidence interval for interaction avoidance (0.02, 0.22) and indirect
.94 in the current study. aggression (0.08, 0.27) also supported Hypothesis 2 (a & b).
We ran hierarchical moderated regression analysis in SPSS using
4. Control variables PROCESS macro (Preacher & Hayes, 2004) to test Hypothesis 3 and
plotted the interactions at ±1 SD. Hypothesis 3 argued that the
One-way ANOVA revealed significant variations in our study’s positive association between pseudo-transformational leadership
dependent variables with education, organization, and organiza- and contempt would be strengthened at higher levels of epistemic
tion type. We created three dummy-coded variables, i.e. two for motivation. Table 4 represents the results of the moderated
organization and one for organization type having multiple cate- regression analysis. Hypothesis 3 received full support as the
gories. Finally, education (0 ¼ bachelor’s and below, 1 ¼ master’s interaction term of PTL x EM is significant for contempt (B ¼ 0.21,
and above) had only two categories, and we could directly control SE ¼ 0.06, p < .001), and bootstrap results further verified that
it. pseudo-transformational leadership’s effects on contempt varied at
different levels of the moderator (see Table 4). On the basis of
significant moderation effects, we formulated the interaction plot
4.1. Confirmatory factor analysis
for mean ± 1 SD. Fig. 2 illustrates the moderating role of epistemic
motivation in the relationship between pseudo-transformational
Confirmatory factor analysis was executed to establish the
leadership and contempt. As depicted in Fig. 2, the relationship
discriminant validity of the variables measured at the same time
between pseudo-transformational and contempt is significant and
and from the same source. Following Anderson and Gerbing’s
stronger at higher levels of epistemic motivation (B ¼ 0.79,
(1988) approach, a series of one-on-one CFA was run, where all
p < .001), whereas the pseudo-transformational and contempt
variables tapped at the same time from the same source were
relationship though significant became weaker at low levels of
compared by pairing a two-factor model with a single-factor
epistemic motivation (B ¼ 0.32, p < .01), thus lending support to
model. Additionally, a 5-factor model containing all the measured
Hypothesis 3.
variables was also tested against a 1-factor alternative model (see
We further tested the conditional indirect effect of pseudo-
Table 1).
transformational leadership on interaction avoidance and indirect
aggression through contempt and investigated moderated media-
5. Results
tion patterns between study variables at three levels of epistemic
motivation, i.e. ±1 SD and mean. The results show that the indirect
Table 2 represents descriptive statistics (means and standard
effect of pseudo-transformational leadership, through contempt,
deviations), bivariate correlations, and Cronbach’s alpha reliability
on interaction avoidance (indirect effect ¼ 0.15, p < .001) and in-
of the main variables. Pseudo-transformational leadership is posi-
direct aggression (indirect effect ¼ 0.22, p < .001), is stronger in case
tively correlated with epistemic motivation (r ¼ .17, p < .01),
of high epistemic motivation (þ1 SD) as compared to low epistemic
contempt (r ¼.50, p < .01), indirect aggression (r ¼ .36, p < .01), and

Table 1
Confirmatory factor analysis results.

Measurement Models c2 Df c2/Df CFI GFI IFI NFI RMSEA

PTL-EM (2 factor) 410.56 148 2.77 .92 .89 .92 .88 .08
PTL-EM (1 factor) 1017.27 157 6.48 .74 .68 .75 .71 .14
IAg-IAv (2 factor) 222.97 73 3.05 .95 .90 .96 .93 .08
IAg-IAv (1 factor) 704.48 74 9.52 .81 .75 .81 .79 .18
PTL-EM-Con-IAg-IAv (5 factor) 1336.63 614 2.18 .91 .82 .91 .85 .06
PTL-EM-Con-IAg-IAv (1 factor) 4394.78 652 6.74 .51 .46 .51 .47 .14

Note: N ¼ 277; PTL¼ Pseudo-Transformational leadership; EM¼ Epistemic motivation; Con¼ Contempt; IAg¼ Indirect aggression; IAv¼ Interaction Avoidance.
242 F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246

Table 2
Means, standard deviations, correlations, and reliabilities for key variables in the study.

Mean S. D 1 2 3 4 5

1 . Pseudo-Trans Leadership (Time 1) 3.09 1.34 (0.94)


2 . Epistemic Motivation (Time 1) 4.50 1.15 .17** (0.80)
3 . Contempt (Time 2) 3.64 1.70 .50** .18** (0.88)
4 . Indirect Aggression (Time 3) 3.49 1.55 .36** .10 .44** (0.94)
5 . Interactional Avoidance (Time 3) 3.70 1.75 .30** .22** .32** .44** (0.91)

Note: N ¼ 277; Pseudo-Trans Leadership ¼ Pseudo-Transformational leadership; Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities are given in parentheses.*Correlation is significant at 0.05 level
(2-tailed); ** Correlation is significant at 0.01 level.

Table 3
Mediated regression analysis results.

Variable R R2 B SE T P

.56 .31 .000


1 Direct effects of PTL on Contempt .58 .07 8.75 .000
.40 .16 .000
2 Direct effects of PTL on interaction avoidance .25 .09 2.95 .003
3 Direct effects of Contempt on Interaction Avoidance .19 .07 2.73 .006
Bootstrap Results for Indirect Effects
Indirect Effect M SE LL CI 95% UL CI 95%
.11 .05 .02 .22

Variable R R2 B SE T P

.56 .31 .000


1 Direct effects of PTL on Contempt .58 .07 8.75 .000
.50 .25 .000
2 Direct effects of PTL on Indirect Aggression .20 .07 2.72 .006
3 Direct effects of Contempt on Indirect Aggression .27 .06 4.71 .000
Bootstrap Results for Indirect Effects
Indirect Effect M SE LL CI 95% UL CI 95%
.16 .05 .08 .27

Note: N ¼ 277; PTL¼ Pseudo-Transformational Leadership; Control variables: organization, organization type, education; Bootstrap Sample Size ¼ 5000. LL ¼ Lower Limit, CI ¼
Confidence Interval, UL ¼ Upper Limit. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Table 4
Hierarchical moderated regression analysis.

Predictors Contempt

R R2 Estimate SE LLCI ULCI

Step 1 .59*** .35***


Constant 4.13*** .24 3.66 4.60
Pseudo-transformational leadership .56*** .07 .43 .69
Epistemic Motivation .17* .08 .02 .32
Step 2 DR2 .03***
PTL x EM .21*** .06 .09 .32

Conditional Direct Effects of X on Y at Values of Moderator (Epistemic Motivation) (Slope Test Results)
Moderator Contempt
Epistemic Motivation Effect Boot SE LLCI ULCI
1 SD (1.15) .32** .10 .13 .51
Mean (0.00) .56*** .07 .43 .69
þ1 SD (1.15) .79*** .09 .61 .97

Note: N ¼ 277; PTL¼ Pseudo-Transformational Leadership; Control variables: organization, organization type, education; Bootstrap Sample Size ¼ 5000. LL ¼ Lower Limit, CI ¼
Confidence Interval, UL ¼ Upper Limit. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

motivation (1 SD). See Table 5 for detailed results. In further 6. Discussion
support of Hypothesis 4, the bootstrapped 95% CIs contained non-
zero values for interaction avoidance (0.03, 0.30) and indirect Transformational leadership has although remained one of the
aggression (0.11, 0.36). In line with the suggestions of Hayes (2015), most popular theoretical styles in the leadership domain, its dark
we also examined the index of moderated mediation to confirm the side, i.e. pseudo-transformational leadership has been relatively
linear relationship between moderator and indirect effects. The ignored (Hoption et al., 2013). The prime motivation of this study
results of the index of moderated mediation were significant for stemmed from the inadequacy of research examining the influence
both interaction avoidance (index ¼ 0.04, SE ¼ 0.02, CI.95 ¼ 0.00, of such leaders on followers and the mechanisms through which
0.09) and indirect aggression (index ¼ 0.06, SE ¼ 0.02, CI.95 ¼ 0.02, this influence flows (Lin et al., 2017; Naber & Moffett, 2017; Schuh
0.11), thus further lending support to Hypothesis 4. et al., 2013). This study contributes to the literature on the dark side
of leadership by examining an emotion-based model that explains
how and why followers under a pseudo-transformational leader
F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246 243

Fig. 2. Interaction effects of pseudo-transformational leadership and epistemic motivation on contempt.

Table 5
Moderated mediation analysis.

Independent Variable Mediator Dependent Variable Moderator EM Indirect Effect SE 95% Bootstrap CI [LLCI, ULCI]

Pseudo-transformational Leadership. Contempt Interaction Avoidance High .15*** .07 [.03, .30]
Medium .10** .05 [.02, .21]
Low .06** .03 [.01, .15]
Indirect Aggression High .22*** .06 [.11, .36]
Medium .15*** .04 [.08, .25]
Low .09** .04 [.03, .18]

Note: n ¼ 277. Control variables: organization, organization type, education; EM ¼ Epistemic Motivation.
Bootstrap Sample Size ¼ 5000. LL ¼ Lower Limit, CI ¼ Confidence Interval, UL ¼ Upper Limit. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

engage in avoidance and aggressive behaviors. Besides, this study followers under pseudo leaders, but they did not study the ensuing
also incorporated an individual’s trait information processing ten- behaviors of followers. This study extends this line of reasoning and
dency in the form of epistemic motivation between pseudo- contends that contempt can be a unique emotional response to self-
transformational leadership and contempt relationship. Our study serving leaders, i.e. pseudo-transformational leaders, which then
highlights the mechanisms and the boundary conditions under translates into follower’s avoidance and indirect aggression.
which a leader’s nasty intentions in the disguise of so-called Our research was conducted in Pakistan, which provides
transformational behaviors promote negative consequences for important contextual implications for the study’s findings. As
followers. We found full support for our hypothesized Pakistan ranks high on power distance, where there are power
relationships. asymmetries, along with a collectivist orientation where there is a
Overall, the results suggest that a leader’s exploitative behaviors prevalence of harmony and collective good (Hofstede, 2001; 2011,
and interpersonal transgressions serve as a negative event that chap. 8), followers in such cultures are more likely to aggress
activates contempt as a negative emotion, which further triggers indirectly and avoid such dark leaders. Moreover, Pakistan rates
avoidance and aggressive behaviors. Extant research says that high on uncertainty avoidance, where people feel uncomfortable in
pseudo-transformational leaders aim at exhibiting excessive con- ambiguous situations and are more likely to exhibit higher stress
trol, and demand compliance and obedience from their followers and emotionality (Hofstede, 1991). Hence, such cultures would
(Barling et al., 2008; Christie et al., 2011), but our study contends especially be more instrumental in revealing the negative effects of
that followers rather respond with interaction avoidance and in- pseudo-transformational leadership in the form of showing
direct aggression. This study is an important addition to the contempt towards the leader and subsequent negative behaviors.
existing literature on leadership and affect. Categorically, Schmid Similarly, as Pakistan ranks high on masculinity and short-term
et al. (2018) verified that self-serving leaders (e.g. exploitative orientation (Hofstede, 2001), where there is an admiration for the
leaders) elicit negative affect in followers but suggested the need strong reliance on past and present, such cultures not only make
for researchers to identify and study other discrete emotions and dark leadership more probable but also tend to unveil the negative
consequent behaviors in response to dark leadership styles. mechanisms and outcomes in such contexts more evident.
Moreover, Christie et al. (2011) identified arousal of fear in Apart from the theoretical strengths, this study utilized a time-
244 F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246

lagged research design with three waves of data collection, would be exciting to know what type of followers are more prone
enhancing the methodological superiority of this study. By to engage in contempt under a pseudo-transformational leader. For
temporally segregating the measurement of predictor, mediator, example, it might be fruitful to investigate negative affectivity or
and criterion variable, we reduced the chances of common method dark triad personality as a moderator in the relationship between
bias arising from a single source and single time design. Despite the pseudo-transformational leadership and contempt.
theoretical and methodological strengths, this study holds some The findings of this study present several practical implications
limitations that cannot be overlooked. Even though time-lagged for managers working in organizations. First, our study highlights
research design is widely used in studies investigating the deleterious impact of pseudo-transformational leaders in
moderated-mediation patterns, it cannot be termed purely longi- creating contempt, which further promotes avoidance and indirect
tudinal. As pure longitudinal research designs are suitable for aggression. Second, managers in organizations should ensure that
making consistent causal inferences (Campbell & Fiske, 1959; Cook, organizations do not hire, select, train, or support pseudo-
Campbell, & Shadish, 2002), future studies should replicate this transformational leaders, as such fake leaders incite negative
study with all the variables tapped from the same respondent at all emotions and passive forms of aggression in followers. Third, the
periods. Second, based on past research, we measured pseudo- organization’s performance evaluation processes must highlight
transformational leadership through combining and reversing and uncover a leader’s ethical or unethical tendencies, thus keeping
two dimensions of the MLQ, i.e. idealized influence and inspira- pseudo-transformational leaders under strict monitoring and
tional motivation (e.g. Barling et al., 2008; Lin et al., 2017). However, control. Fourth, organizations must arrange workshops and
this scale might have restricted us to uncover the true essence of training for managers, which convey them the importance of
pseudo-transformational leadership. For example, this measure has following moral values and ethical code of conduct. Organizations
been critiqued since it defines leadership by outcomes and not need to provide a reliable and authentic platform, where em-
behaviors (Van Knippenberg & Sitkin, 2013). Second, it would seem ployees can safely raise their concerns regarding their leader’s
that this measure would indicate the absence of transformational conduct and dealt with timely. Lastly, this study highlighted that
leadership rather than pseudo-transformational leadership. followers who possess epistemic motivation can thwart the self-
Therefore, future researchers need to develop a scale independent serving efforts of leaders. Hence, managers need to make efforts
of MLQ to capture the essence of this construct beyond the current to hire employees with increased epistemic motivation traits, as
operationalization of pseudo-transformational leadership. such individuals do not fall prey to leader’s manipulation and
For instance, researchers while developing a new measure must exploitative tendencies.
account for the intricacies of a pseudo-transformational leader’s
self-interested behaviors, which are currently not possible through
7. Conclusion
a reverse measure of MLQ (Christie et al., 2011). Also, future re-
searchers while constructing a new measure, must realize that
Our research examined a unique angle into the processes and
pseudo-transformational leaders encompass actions, whereby
conditions under which pseudo-transformational leaders can
these leaders not only are unable to aid followers to think for
instigate detrimental consequences in followers. By studying fol-
themselves but openly discourage independent thinking through
lower’s discrete emotions and ensuing passive behavioral re-
promoting obedience and subduing (Barling et al., 2008; Bass &
sponses to pseudo-transformational leaders, we extend the
Steidlmeier, 1999). This new scale should include items that
literature on the dark side of leadership in a newer cultural setting
directly assess pseudo-transformational leader’s tendency to drive
of Pakistan. Our study opens up a new avenue in the pseudo-
influence based on glorifying themselves, which is presently lack-
transformational literature by highlighting why and when victims
ing in the MLQ scale. Such measure should also reflect items that
of such a leader may strike back through indirect and passive forms
clearly identify pseudo-transformational leader’s ability to propa-
of aggression. We encourage researchers not only to study the
gate their vision through manipulation, deceit, and false promises.
detrimental effects of such leaders but also to highlight other in-
Hence, a new measure of pseudo-transformational behaviors
dividual and contextual factors that might either support or sup-
should more explicitly operationalize and capture the above be-
press such leaders in a cross-cultural perspective.
haviors, which make it not only conceptually but also empirically
distinct from authentic transformational leadership behaviors.
Lastly, although an eastern culture provides an ideal setting for Appendix
examining the mechanisms and conditions of pseudo-
transformational leadership behaviors, it limits the generaliz- Measurements
ability of the findings. Future studies should replicate our research
model’s findings in a Western cultural context and conduct more Pseudo Transformational Leadership (Bass & Avolio, 2000).
cross-cultural comparisons while studying the effects of pseudo-
transformational leadership behaviors. 1. My leader instills pride in others for being associated with
Pseudo-transformational leadership is deemed to be an area me.
ripe for scholarly attention and we encourage future researchers to 2. My leader goes beyond self-interest.
study the effects of pseudo-transformational leaders on followers 3. My leader has my respect.
within organizations. It might be interesting for future researchers 4. My leader displays a sense of power and confidence.
to analyze direct aggressive behaviors such as organizational 5. My leader talks of values.
retaliatory behaviors, revenge and turnover intentions as detri- 6. My leader models ethical standards.
mental consequences of working under a pseudo-transformational 7. My leader considers the moral/ethical consequences of
leader. While this study introduced contempt as an intervening decisions.
mechanism, it might be plausible to examine other negative emo- 8. My leader emphasizes the collective mission.
tions such as anger and disgust, which might explain how pseudo- 9. My leader expresses confidence.
transformational leadership can promote aggression and avoidance 10. My leader talks enthusiastically.
behaviors. Similarly, individual dispositions can play a vital role in 11. My leader talks optimistically.
pseudo-transformational leader and emotions relationship and it 12. My leader arouses awareness about important issues.
F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246 245

Epistemic Motivation (Neuberg & Newsom, 1993). 315e338.


Barling, J., Christie, A., & Turner, N. (2008). Pseudo-transformational leadership:
Towards the development and test of a model. Journal of Business Ethics, 81(4),
1. It upsets me to go into a situation without knowing what I 851e861.
can expect from it. Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (2000). Multifactor leadership questionnaire. Redwood City,
2. I’m not bothered by things that interrupt my daily routine. CA: Mind Garden.
Bass, B. M., & Riggio, R. E. (2006). Transformational leadership. Mahwah, NJ: Law-
(R) rence Erlbaum.
3. I enjoy having a clear and structured mode of life. Bass, B. M., & Steidlmeier, P. (1999). Ethics, character, and authentic trans-
4. I like to have a place for everything and everything in its formational leadership behavior. The Leadership Quarterly, 10(2), 181e217.
Bies, R. J., & Tripp, T. M. (2005). The study of revenge in the workplace: Conceptual,
place. ideological, and empirical issues. In S. Fox, & P. E. Spector (Eds.), Counterpro-
5. I enjoy being spontaneous. (R) ductive work behavior: Investigations of actors and targets (pp. 65e81). Wash-
6. I find that a well-ordered life with regular hours makes my ington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.

€rkqvist, K., Osterman,
Bjo K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). The development of direct and
life tedious. (R) indirect aggressive strategies in males and females. In K. Bjo €rkqvist, &
7. I don’t like situations that are uncertain. P. Niemel€ a (Eds.), Of mice and women: Aspects of female aggression (pp. 51e64).
8. I hate to change my plans at the last minute. San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.
Braun, S., Peus, C., Weisweiler, S., & Frey, D. (2013). Transformational leadership, job
9. I hate to be with people who are unpredictable. satisfaction, and team performance: A multilevel mediation model of trust. The
10. I find that a consistent routine enables me to enjoy life more. Leadership Quarterly, 24(1), 270e283.
11. I enjoy the exhilaration of being in unpredictable situations. Brief, A. P., & Weiss, H. M. (2002). Organizational behavior: Affect in the workplace.
Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 279e307.
(R) ~ o, L. K., & Harrison, D. A. (2005). Ethical leadership: A social
Brown, M. E., Trevin
12. I become uncomfortable when the rules in a situation are not learning perspective for construct development and testing. Organizational
clear. Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 97(2), 117e134.
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the
multi trait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 81e105.
Contempt (Zevon & Tellegen, 1982). Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology.
Guilford Press.
1. Scornful Chintakananda, K., & Greguras, G. (2017). Abusive supervision and power distance:
Exploring discrete emotions. In , Vol. 2017. Academy of management proceedings.
2. Contemptuous NY 10510: Briarcliff Manor. Academy of Management.
3. Disdainful Christie, A., Barling, J., & Turner, N. (2011). Pseudo-transformational leadership:
Model specification and outcomes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 41(12),
2943e2984.
Interaction Avoidance (Hu & Shi, 2015; Watson & Friend, 1969). Clapp-Smith, R., Vogelgesang, G. R., & Avey, J. B. (2009). Authentic leadership and
positive psychological capital: The mediating role of trust at the group level of
1. I try to stay away from interactions with my leader at work. analysis. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 15(3), 227e240.
Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1998). Charismatic leadership in organizations.
2. I never initiate interactions with my leader. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
3. I often intentionally avoid opportunities to interact with my Cook, T. D., Campbell, D. T., & Shadish, W. (2002). Experimental and quasi-
leader. experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Crane, B., & Hartwell, C. J. (2018). Developing employees’ mental complexity:
Transformational leadership as a catalyst in employee development. Human
€rkqvist et al., 1992).
Indirect Aggression (Bjo Resource Development Review, 17(3), 1e24.
Crawford, L. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2002). Learning where to look for danger: Inte-
1. Shut the other one out of the group. grating affective and spatial information. Psychological Science, 13(5), 449e453.
Dasborough, M. T. (2006). Cognitive asymmetry in employee emotional reactions to
2. Become friends with another as a kind of revenge? leadership behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly, 17(2), 163e178.
3. Ignored him/her? Dasborough, M. T., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2002). Emotion and attribution of inten-
4. Gossiped about the one I am angry with? tionality in leader-member relationships. The Leadership Quarterly, 13(5),
615e634.
5. Told bad or false stories about him/her? De Dreu, C. K. (2003). Time pressure and closing of the mind in negotiation.
6. Planned secretly to bother the other one? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91(2), 280e295.
7. Said bad things behind the other one’s back? Decoster, S., Stouten, J., & Tripp, T. M. (2019). When employees retaliate against self-
serving leaders: The influence of the ethical climate. Journal of Business Ethics,
8. Said to others “Let’s not be with him/her!"? 1e19.
9. Told the other one’s secrets to a third person? Einarsen, S., Aasland, M. S., & Skogstad, A. (2007). Destructive leadership behavior:
10. Wrote small notes where the other one is criticized? A definition and conceptual model. The Leadership Quarterly, 18(3), 207e216.
Eisenbeiß, S. A., & Boerner, S. (2013). A double-edged sword: Transformational
11. Criticized the other one’s hair or clothing? leadership and individual creativity. British Journal of Management, 24(1),
12. Tried to get others to dislike the person you are angry with? 54e68.
Eissa, G., Chinchanachokchai, S., & Wyland, R. (2017). The influence of supervisor
undermining on self-esteem, creativity, and overall job performance: A multi-
References ple mediation model. Organization Management Journal, 14(4), 185e197.
Eissa, G., & Lester, S. W. (2017). Supervisor role overload and frustration as ante-
Abbas, M., Raja, U., Darr, W., & Bouckenooghe, D. (2014). Combined effects of cedents of abusive supervision: The moderating role of supervisor personality.
perceived politics and psychological capital on job satisfaction, turnover in- Journal of Organizational Behavior, 38(3), 307e326.
tentions, and performance. Journal of Management, 40(7), 1813e1830. Ferris, D. L., Yan, M., Lim, V. K., Chen, Y., & Fatimah, S. (2016). An approach-avoidance
Amit, A., & Sagiv, L. (2013). The role of epistemic motivation in individuals’ response framework of workplace aggression. Academy of Management Journal, 59(5),
to decision complexity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 1777e1800.
121(1), 104e117. Fida, R., Tramontano, C., Paciello, M., Guglielmetti, C., Gilardi, S., Probst, T. M., et al.
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A (2018). ‘First, do No harm’: The role of negative emotions and moral disen-
review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103(3), gagement in understanding the relationship between workplace aggression
411e423. and misbehavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1e17.
Aquino, K., Tripp, T. M., & Bies, R. J. (2006). Getting even or moving on? Power, Fischer, A., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2016). Contempt: Derogating others while keeping
procedural justice, and types of offense as predictors of revenge, forgiveness, calm. Emotion Review, 8(4), 346e357.
reconciliation, and avoidance in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007). Beat them or ban them: The characteristics
91(3), 653e668. and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social
Ashkanasy, N. M., Humphrey, R. H., & Huy, Q. N. (2017). Integrating emotions and Psychology, 93(1), 103e115.
affect in theories of management. Academy of Management Review, 42(2), Frieder, R. E., Wang, G., & Oh, I. S. (2018). Linking job-relevant personality traits,
175e189. transformational leadership, and job performance via perceived meaningful-
Avolio, B. J., & Gardner, W. L. (2005). Authentic leadership development: Getting to ness at work: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Applied Psychology,
the root of positive forms of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 16(3), 103(3), 324e333.
246 F. Syed et al. / European Management Journal 39 (2021) 236e246

Glasø, L., Skogstad, A., Notelaers, G., & Einarsen, S. (2018). Leadership, affect and perceptions of transformational leadership behavior. Journal of Applied Social
outcomes: Symmetrical and asymmetrical relationships. The Leadership & Or- Psychology, 47(2), 99e112.
ganization Development Journal, 39(1), 51e65. Naseer, S., Raja, U., Syed, F., Donia, M. B., & Darr, W. (2016). Perils of being close to a
Gooty, J., Connelly, S., Griffith, J., & Gupta, A. (2010). Leadership, affect and emo- bad leader in a bad environment: Exploring the combined effects of despotic
tions: A state of the science review. The Leadership Quarterly, 21(6), 979e1004. leadership, leader-member exchange, and perceived organizational politics on
Greifeneder, R., & Bless, H. (2007). Relying on accessible content versus accessibility behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly, 27(1), 14e33.
experiences: The case of processing capacity. Social Cognition, 25(6), 853e881. Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: Individual dif-
Hassard, J., Teoh, K. R., Visockaite, G., Dewe, P., & Cox, T. (2018). The financial burden ferences in the desire for simpler structure. Journal of Personality and Social
of psychosocial workplace aggression: A systematic review of cost-of-illness Psychology, 65(1), 113e131.
studies. Work & Stress, 32(1), 6e32. Nifadkar, S., Tsui, A. S., & Ashforth, B. E. (2012). The way you make me feel and
Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate behave: Supervisor-triggered newcomer affect and approach-avoidance
Behavioral Research, 50(1), 1e22. behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 55(5), 1146e1168.
Hershcovis, M. S., & Barling, J. (2010). Comparing victim attributions and outcomes Oh, J. K., & Farh, C. I. (2017). An emotional process theory of how subordinates
for workplace aggression and sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, appraise, experience, and respond to abusive supervision over time. Academy of
95(5), 874e888. Management Review, 42(2), 207e232.
Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. London: Pelzer, P. (2005). Essai: Contempt and organization: Present in practicedignored by
McGraw-Hill. research? Organization Studies, 26(8), 1217e1227.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede model in context. effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &
Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1) (chapter 8). Computers, 36(4), 717e731.
Hoption, C., Barling, J., & Turner, N. (2013). “It’s not you, it’s me”: Transformational Price, T. L. (2003). The ethics of authentic transformational leadership. The Lead-
leadership and self-deprecating humor. The Leadership & Organization Devel- ership Quarterly, 14(1), 67e81.
opment Journal, 34(1), 4e19. Rietzschel, E. F., De Dreu, C. K., & Nijstad, B. A. (2007). Personal need for structure
House, R. J. (1995). Leadership in the twenty-first century. In A. Howard (Ed.), The and creative performance: The moderating influence of fear of invalidity. Per-
changing nature of work (pp. 411e450). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(6), 855e866.
House, R. J., & Howell, J. M. (1992). Personality and charismatic leadership. The Roseman, I. J. (2001). A model of appraisal in the emotion system. Appraisal pro-
Leadership Quarterly, 3(2), 81e108. cesses in emotion: Theory, methods, research, 68e91.
Howell, J. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1992). The ethics of charismatic leadership: Submission Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A
or liberation? Academy of Management Perspectives, 6(2), 43e54. mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three
Huang, L., & Paterson, T. A. (2017). Group ethical voice: Influence of ethical lead- moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social
ership and impact on ethical performance. Journal of Management, 43(4), Psychology, 76(4), 574e586.
1157e1184. Schilling, J. (2009). From ineffectiveness to destruction: A qualitative study on the
Hughes, P. J., & Harris, M. D. (2017). Organizational laundering: A case study of meaning of negative leadership. Leadership, 5(1), 102e128.
pseudo-transformational leadership. Organization Development Journal, 35(2), Schmid, E. A., Pircher Verdorfer, A., & Peus, C. V. (2018). Different shadesedifferent
59e77. effects? Consequences of different types of destructive leadership. Frontiers in
Hunter, S. T., Bedell-Avers, K. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2007). The typical leadership Psychology, 9, 1e16.
study: Assumptions, implications, and potential remedies. The Leadership Schuh, S. C., Zhang, X. A., & Tian, P. (2013). For the good or the bad? Interactive
Quarterly, 18(5), 435e446. effects of transformational leadership with moral and authoritarian leadership
Hu, X., & Shi, J. (2015). Employees’ surface acting in interactions with leaders and behaviors. Journal of Business Ethics, 116(3), 629e640.
peers. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36(8), 1132e1152. Schyns, B., & Schilling, J. (2013). How bad are the effects of bad leaders? A meta-
Kazmi, S. A. Z. (2017). Cross comparative analysis on the models of transformational analysis of destructive leadership and its outcomes. The Leadership Quarterly,
leadership and pseudo-transformational leadership. International Journal of 24(1), 138e158.
Strategic Decision Sciences, 8(3), 59e77. Sund, B., & Lines, R. (2017). When leadership leads to loathing: The effect of
Krasikova, D. V., Green, S. G., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Destructive leadership: A culturally (In)Congruent leadership on employee contempt and voluntary work
theoretical review, integration, and future research agenda. Journal of Man- behaviors. The Journal of Values-Based Leadership, 10(2). Article 11.
agement, 39(5), 1308e1338. Syed, F., Naseer, S., & Khan, A. K. (2018). Interplay of supervisor undermining &
Kruglanski, A. W., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & De Grada, E. (2006). Groups as epistemic rejection sensitivity on FNE & outcomes. In Academy of management proceedings
providers: Need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. Psychological (Vol. 2018, p. 14594). Briarcliff Manor NY 10510: Academy of Management.
Review, 113(1), 84e100. Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision. Academy of Management
Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1991). The concept of coping. In A. Monat, & R. S. Lazarus Journal, 43(2), 178e190.
(Eds.), Stress and coping: An anthology (pp. 189e206). New York: Columbia Thompson, M. M., Naccarato, M. E., Parker, K. C. E., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2001). The
University Press. personal need for structure and personal fear of invalidity measures: Historical
Lebel, R. D. (2017). Moving beyond fight and flight: A contingent model of how the perspectives, current applications, and future directions. In G. B. Moskowitz
emotional regulation of anger and fear sparks proactivity. Academy of Man- (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and
agement Review, 42(2), 190e206. future of social cognition (pp. 19e39). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Accounting for the effects of accountability. Van Gils, S., Van Quaquebeke, N., Van Knippenberg, D., Van Dijke, M., & De
Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 255e275. Cremer, D. (2015). Ethical leadership and follower organizational deviance: The
Lian, H., Brown, D. J., Ferris, D. L., Liang, L. H., Keeping, L. M., & Morrison, R. (2014). moderating role of follower moral attentiveness. The Leadership Quarterly, 26(2),
Abusive supervision and retaliation: A self-control framework. Academy of 190e203.
Management Journal, 57(1), 116e139. Van Kleef, G. A., Homan, A. C., Beersma, B., Van Knippenerg, D., Van Knippenberg, B.,
Lin, C. S., Huang, P. C., Chen, S. J., & Huang, L. C. (2017). Pseudo-transformational & Damen, F. (2009). Searing sentiment or cold calculation? The effects of leader
leadership is in the eyes of the subordinates. Journal of Business Ethics, 141(1), emotional displays on team performance depend on follower epistemic moti-
179e190. vation. Academy of Management Journal, 52, 562e580.
Luthans, F., Peterson, S. J., & Ibrayeva, E. (1998). The potential for the “dark side” of Van Knippenberg, D., & Sitkin, S. B. (2013). A critical assessment of charismaticd-
leadership in post-communist countries. Journal of World Business, 33(2), transformational leadership research: Back to the drawing board? The Academy
185e201. of Management Annals, 7(1), 1e60.
Mayseless, O., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1987). What makes you so sure? Effects of Warren, P., Richardson, D. S., & McQuillin, S. (2011). Distinguishing among nondirect
epistemic motivations on judgmental confidence. Organizational Behavior and forms of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 37(4), 291e301.
Human Decision Processes, 39(2), 162e183. Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of
Melwani, S., & Barsade, S. G. (2011). Held in contempt: The psychological, inter- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33(4), 448e457.
personal, and performance consequences of contempt in a work context. Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical dis-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(3), 503e520. cussion of the structure, causes, and consequences of affective experiences at
Miller, W. I. (1997). The anatomy of disgust. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University work. In B. M. Staw, & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Vol. 18. Research in organizational
Press. behavior: An annual series of analytical essays and critical reviews (pp. 1e74). US:
Mitchell, M. S., Vogel, R. M., & Folger, R. (2015). Third parties’ reactions to the Elsevier Science/JAI Press.
abusive supervision of coworkers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(4), Zapf, D., & Gross, C. (2001). Conflict escalation and coping with workplace bullying:
1040e1056. A replication and extension. European Journal of Work & Organizational Psy-
Moskowitz, G. B. (1993). Individual differences in social categorization: The influ- chology, 10(4), 497e522.
ence of personal need for structure on spontaneous trait inferences. Journal of Zevon, M. A., & Tellegen, A. (1982). The structure of mood change: An idiographic/
Personality and Social Psychology, 65(1), 132e142. nomothetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(1), 111e122.
Naber, A. M., & Moffett, R. G., III (2017). Follower moral reasoning influences

You might also like