You are on page 1of 15

The Spanish Journal of Psychology (2020), 23, e5, 1–13.

© Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicó logos de Madrid 2020
doi:10.1017/SJP.2020.5

“Why are the Corrupt, Corrupt?”: The Multilevel


Analytical Model of Corruption

João Gabriel Modesto12 and Ronaldo Pilati3


1
Centro Universitário de Brasília (Brazil)
2
Universidade Estadual de Goiás
(Brazil) 3 Universidade de Brasília
(Brazil)

Abstract. Corruption is a global problem. Despite the importance of this theme, a shortage of theoretical models in both
psychology and related areas that favor its understanding and investigation is noted. Due to this scarcity of theoretical
models, in addition to the need to systematize studies on the topic, this theoretical article aims to describe the Analytical
Model of Corruption (AMC) as an interdisciplinary and multilevel proposal aimed at corruption analysis. To achieve this
goal, the concept of corruption was analyzed using related phenomena as reference. Similarities and differences in
corruption have been identified with dishonest behavior and unethical behavior. Subsequently, theoretical models on
corruption identified in the literature were presented, and their main characteristics and limitations were pointed out.
After describing the models, the AMC was presented and its advantages over the previous models were discussed. Finally,
it was
concluded that the AMC could be configured as a theoretical model that guides interdisciplinary studies on corruption,
allowing for a more complete analysis compared to previous theoretical models identified in the literature.

Received 16 March 2019; Revised 17 March 2020; Accepted 19 March 2020

Keywords: corruption, dishonest behavior, multilevel model, unethical behavior

Corruption is a global problem and its occurrence has objective, the first step was to define the concept
been identified in various countries during various of corruption based on correlated phenomena. The
periods (Tanzi, 1998). Nevertheless, despite the impor- improved definition of the phenomenon was used for
tance of this subject, very few theoretical models exist a more precise proposition of the AMC. Models relating
that help understand corruption (Judge et al., 2011). to corruption found in human and social sciences are
From a psychological standpoint, even though this also described. Finally, the potential of AMC is indi-
subject has much to contribute, proposals for systema- cated, in comparison to other models, as a theoretical
tization are few or non-existent. From a broader per- perspective that, although based on psychological sci-
spective, when taking other scientific disciplines into ence, may contribute with interdisciplinary research on
account, and even though systematization exists on a corruption.
larger scale as opposed to psychology, no models may
yet be found the seek to integrate the various factors
The Concept of Corruption
that can assist in understanding corruption. Psychology,
through its premise of multiple determinants of behav- For the proposal of the AMC as a theoretical model
ior, provides various relevant contributions on this sub- for corruption, it is necessary to identify the concep-
ject, such as the multilevel approach (Pettigrew, 2018), tual limits and similarities of corruption with corre-
when the interaction of contextual factors with individ- lated constructs. In this work, this conceptual analysis
ual and group variables are fundamental to understand of corruption was developed through a comparison
human behavior. between unethical and dishonest behavior due to con-
With this scenario as a backdrop, the lack of integra- ceptual overlap.
tive models and the pressing need for work that sys- (Un)ethical behavior may be defined as the conduct of
tematizes corruption, dishonesty and behavioral ethics, an individual that is subject to the moral norms of a
the objective of this theoretical study was to propose certain group (Treviñ o et al., 2006). Based on revisions
the Analytical Model of Corruption (AMC). To achieve and meta-analysis (Kish-Gephart et al., 2010; Treviñ o
this et al., 2014), it can be seen that the construct has been

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to João


How to cite this article:
Gabriel Modesto. Centro Universitário de Brasília, Faculdade de
Modesto, J. G., & Pilati, R. (2020). “Why are the corrupt,
Psicologia. 70790–075. Brasília, DF, Brazil. E-mail:
corrupt?”: The multilevel analytical model of corruption. The
jg.modesto@gmail.com
The study was supported by the Grant 308268/2016-4 of the Spanish Journal of Psychology, 23. e5. Doi:10.1017/SJP.2020.5
National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq)
for the second author.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
2 J. G. Modesto & R. Pilati

systematically applied in psychology, especially in the


many cases, an individual may just not be aware that
field of organizational psychology, by which Rest’s
certain behavior is dishonest (Darley, 2005). To under-
(1986) four-component model has been widely used
stand dishonesty it is therefore necessary to evaluate
by researchers interested in the ethical decision-
the relationship between intrinsic, extrinsic rewards,
making process (Lehnert et al., 2015; Treviñ o et al.,
and possible biases in judgment and decision-making
2006). Cur- rent psychological research on unethical
of a dishonest decision.
behavior seeks to identify, from an interactionist
In light of the above, dishonest behavior may thus be
perspective, individ- ual and contextual factors to
considered as the action (not necessarily conscious) of
understand the phenome- non (Lehnert et al., 2015).
an individual that, when breaking a rule, may generate
On the other hand, dishonest behavior consists of, as
a reward (directly or indirectly) for that individual, as
does unethical behavior, the violation of norms, allow-
well as generate loss (directly or indirectly) for
ing for dishonesty to be seen as a form of expression of
someone else (e.g.: a person, organization, the State),
unethical behavior. In support of this, after a meta-
in addition
analysis of unethical behavior, the authors chose the
to losses to oneself (intrinsic or extrinsic). This definition
term “dishonesty” to be one of the keywords to inves-
merges typical elements of economic cost-benefit the-
tigate the construct as they considered dishonest behav-
ories (i.e., extrinsic rewards and gains) as well as psy-
ior to be a specific type of unethical behavior (Kish-
chological theories (i.e., the absence of a conscious
Gephart et al., 2010). Although understood to be an process, intrinsic rewards) (Mazar & Ariely, 2006, for
expression of unethical behavior, dishonest behavior
a comparison between psychological theories and eco-
has some characteristics that differentiate it from other
nomic theories).
unethical actions. In general, to understand dishonesty,
Corruption, like unethical and dishonest actions, is
it is necessary to consider an author, the act, the victim
associated with the violation of rules and norms (based
and the expected results (Scott & Jehn, 1999).
on a legalistic definition of the phenomenon Peters &
It is possible to identify the contributions of various
Welch, 1978). It is also assumed, as in dishonesty, that
fields towards understanding dishonest behavior,
there is a possibility of obtaining rewards. However,
examples of which are economy and psychology
corruption has one characteristic that differentiates
(Mazar & Ariely, 2006). In cost-benefit models, typical
it from other behaviors: Abuse of power. Generally,
in the economy, it is assumed that human beings are
corruption is defined as the abuse of entrusted power
rational and deliberative. As such, dishonesty would
for private gain (Transparency International, 2018).
occur based on a comparison between possible risks
Although this definition is widely used, it has been
and extrinsic rewards. If the benefits outweigh the
criticized. The concept of “entrusted power” does not
risks, it is more likely the dishonest behavior will take include, for example, those who have risen to power
place (Becker, 1968).
without the express confidence of all or of a majority to
On the other hand, psychological theory, aside from
legitimate that power, as is the example of dictatorships.
extrinsic rewards, include factors that are linked to
Additionally, the idea that the gain would be
intrinsic rewards. For example, there is neuroscientific
“private” allows us to infer that corruption is restricted
evidence indicating that actions based on socially
to individual gains (private) and does not take into
accepted norms, such as cooperation, stimulate areas
account wider dimensions such as benefits to groups,
of the brain that are linked to pleasure, in a way that is
organizations, political parties or even countries. As a
similar to the stimulation that occurs due to extrinsic
result of these and other limitations, based on the adap-
reward (Rilling et al., 2002). As such, an honest action,
tation of the original Transparency International defini-
by itself, may generate (intrinsic) rewards for the indi-
tion, corruption can be defined as the misuse of power
vidual. This finding has contributed towards studies
to obtain illegal gains (Andersson & Heywood, 2009).
that seek to understand the rationalization strategies This definition overcomes the limitations of the
that favor the legitimation of dishonest actions and
previous definition and has a more generic nature,
reduce intrinsic discomfort (Mazar et al., 2008), as well
covering var- ious behaviors that can be understood
as the development of interventions aimed at reducing as corrupt. The
dishonest actions (Ayal et al., 2015). causes and consequences of corruption have been stud-
Additionally, psychological theory posits that the ied mainly at the macro level, where negative effects on
process of involvement in dishonest actions may not various sectors of society have been verified, as is the
be overall conscious, seeing that there are various example of education and health (Tanzi, 1998).
cognitive biases limiting rationality (Kahneman & Conceptually, the relationship between unethical
Tversky, 1979). For example, there is evidence that loss behavior, dishonest behavior and corrupt behavior
aversion, a cognitive bias, may favor dishonest behav- can be seen in Figure 1.
ior (Schindler & Pfattheicher, 2017). Furthermore, in As can be seen in Figure 1, unethical behavior encom-
passes dishonest behavior where, in turn, corrupt
behavior is included. The common ground among all

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
Corruption Analytical Model 3

Figure 1. Diagram of the relationship between unethical behavior, dishonest behavior and corrupt behavior.

of these is the violation of norms or rules. In the dia-


differentiation and convergence between concepts that
gram, corruption is stated as a more specific phenome-
seeks to overcome this conceptual challenge and con-
non, especially due to its characteristic of abuse of
tribute towards the proposition of the AMC.
power, which is dispensable from the definition of the
other constructs.
It is important to point out that, although only the Theoretical Models on Corruption
relationship between these three constructs is repre-
Despite the rising interest in the study of corruption
sented in the diagram, each one may include various since the 1990s, a multitude of studies on this subject
subtypes. Corrupt behavior, for example, despite being seem to exist without real corroboration between
more specific in the diagram, has different aspects and related fields and lacking the development of
forms of expression, as is the example of cronyism,
theoretical models to guide research. Among the 42
bribery and white-collar crime, among others that will studies used to
not be specifically considered in this work. compile a meta-analysis, 26 do not provide a theoretical
It should also be emphasized, with regards to the model to substantiate them (Judge et al., 2011). One of
conceptual proposal, that, although it is admitted that the theoretical models used, which was an exception in
the violation of norms and rules is the common element the meta-analysis, was the Corruption Institutional
to the three phenomena (unethical behavior, dishonest Choice Analytic Frame (CICAF) (Collier, 2002).
behavior and corrupt behavior), one should bear in CICAF is an interdisciplinary proposal to understand
mind that this concept is based on a broader corruption. It is assumed that corruption is a phenom-
perspective relating to the evaluated norms and rules. enon that takes place in institutions (or tangentially to
Situations exist where dishonest behavior and them) and that can be explained by assessing an indi-
corruption are jus- vidual decision restricted by various political, economic
tified within small groups or organizations by descrip- and cultural factors (Collier, 2002). CICAF is shown in
tive norms (norms about what people effectively do), Figure 2.
however, injunctive norms (what is known to be the As can be seen in Figure 2, CICAF proposes “two
correct thing to do) are still being violated on a social worlds” for the understanding of corruption. An “inter-
dimension. nal world”, referring to the micro-level of analysis, that
It is further emphasized that the dashed lines used in includes the individual decision-making process to act
the diagram indicate that, in spite of the conceptual in a corrupt manner. Analysis of this “world” is influ-
effort, those limits are not always clearly perceived ence by typical economic models (see Becker, 1968),
when the phenomena occur naturally. This, however, seeing that the author considers that decision-making
does not mean the researchers should not consider this is based on the deliberative assessment of the cost-
differentiation. The conceptual difference between each benefit expectations of the action. If the expectation
phenomenon contributes to better implementation and, of benefits is greater than the expectation of costs,
consequently, results in improved research. there is a greater probability the action will take place
It was found that the level of concern among (Collier, 2002).
researchers in differentiating corruption from related Nevertheless, according to the author, this individual
topics was low. The difficulty in defining and delimiting decision-making process is related to the “external
the constructs results in greater difficulty in implement- world”. The individual decision is influenced by polit-
ing them for the research and development of theoret- ical, economic, and sociocultural norms that exist in the
ical models. We have drawn up a proposal for the individual’s “external world”. In this “world”, aside

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
4 J. G. Modesto & R. Pilati

Figure 2. Corruption Institutional Choice Analytic Frame (adapted from Collier, 2002).

from the corrupt behavior predictors, is where the main


“internal world”, also discussed in CICAF, the authors
economic, political and social corruption consequences
point to the existence of a “meso world”. Despite the
can be found. If the corrupt actions were legitimated
inclusion of this dimension, according to Dimant and
instead of punished (Collier, 2002), these conse- Schulte (2016), the proposal should be seen as an
quences, in turn, result in the creation of new norms, “inner- to-outer-world approach” (p. 58). It should be
thus contributing towards the emergence of a “culture noted that, although terminology more closely related
of corruption”. to the
Although CICAF allows for some progress to be typical debate regarding levels of analysis is used, the
made by proposing an interdisciplinary perspective, authors also seem to analyze behavior from an internal-
its approach is open to criticism. In the “internal world”, external dichotomy perspective. Despite this criticism,
the author presents a decision-making model based on the proposal favors the understanding of corrupt
the cost-benefit models that are typical in economics. behavior in an interdisciplinary and, to some extent,
Various studies focusing on the decision-making pro- multilevel manner. Figure 3 shows the proposed
cess in finance (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), as well perspective.
as research into dishonesty (Mazar & Ariely, 2006), According to Dimant and Schulte (2016), the “internal
all point to that approach as being restrictive, seeing world”, aside from taking into account rational
that there are various biases that interfere with the decision-making processes, also considers a “behavioral
decision- making process that surpass a deliberate perspective” that, according to the theoretical frame-
choice between cost and benefit expectations. Another work of behavioral economics, favors the analysis of
element that may less deliberate corruption processes. On this subject,
be criticized is the “inner” and “outer” dichotomy as a we can see progress when compared to CICAF, insofar
basis to explain behavior. There is a tradition in that less conscious processes are included in the pro-
psychol- ogy literature that seeks to understand posal. It can, therefore, be construed that corrupt
behavior based on different levels of analysis (Doise, actions are not as completely rational and deliberate as
1980), taking into account biological, cultural and pre- sumed in the traditional economics “homo
interpersonal factors, econom- icus” that supports CICAF.
overcoming a dichotomy between the individual and On the other hand, in the “meso world”, Dimant and
society. Furthermore, the restrictive “internal world” Schulte (2016) point to the importance of evaluating
and “external world” proposal does not take into criminal and social factors, such as social norms, values,
account, for example, the influence of groups on behav- culture and education. The authors argue that the
ior, which has been systematically studied under the “meso
scope of social psychology. Another limitation of the world” analyses social interaction, however, the descrip-
model is that the concept of abuse of power, essential to tion provided involves, to a greater extent, the variables
the concept of corruption, is not included in the under- commonly referred to in social psychology as macro
standing of the phenomenon. variables (an example of which is culture). Although
Aside from CICAF, Dimant and Schulte (2016) also social interaction is mentioned, the role played by
propose an interdisciplinary perspective to analyze groups is not highlighted in this dimension proposed
corruption. Notwithstanding the “external world” and by the authors. As such, the “meso world” is thus
considered

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
Corruption Analytical Model 5

Figure 3. Interdisciplinary Perspective of Corruption (adapted from Dimant & Schulte, 2016).

to be just another extension of the “external world” concern in indicating that the notion of abuse of power
rather than an analysis of social interaction and group is essential to the understanding of corruption.
processes. Regarding the “external world”, extrinsic
Seeing that corruption is a procedural and systemic
factors that may influence corruption are considered. phenomenon, by which its understanding cannot be
Dimant and Schulte (2016) focus on history, geography, restricted to an “internal world” and “external world”
economics,
dichotomy, Ashforth et al. (2008) propose that corrup-
politics and legal aspects as factors to be considered in
tion be investigated, at organizational level, according
this dimension. For example, the effects of bureaucratic
structures, the role played by political and legal institu- to five views: Micro view, macro view, wide view, long
tions, and even the repercussions of colonization on view, and deep view.
corruption are analyzed. This dimension, conceptually, The micro view refers to an analysis of individual
processes, such as demographic variables and dis-
is related to the “external world” of CICAF.
positional factors that explain corrupt behavior. The
Although more complete than CICAF, and showing
authors, contrary to CICAF, highlight the existence of
significant improvement such as the consideration of
biases in individual decision-making as an important
less rational and deliberate processes, the proposal
element towards the understanding of corruption. Ash-
undertaken by Dimant and Schulte (2016) has limita-
forth et al. (2008) further argue that an exclusively
tions. In spite of the term “meso world”, its description
intraindividual analysis would favor the simplistic
does not seem to focus on group processes, appearing
more to be an extension of the “external world” rather understanding that removing “the bad apples” would
be enough to end corruption. However, the other levels
than a specific dimension to analyze the impact of
of analysis indicate that the phenomenon, and conse-
groups on corrupt behavior. Additionally, the authors,
quently the fight against it, tends to be more complex.
although seeking to understand the phenomenon by
In the macro view, it is assumed that the organiza-
means of levels of analysis, to a certain extent still seem
tional context may contribute towards corruption, inso-
to treat the phenomenon as an internal-external dichot-
far as social norms exist that are capable of creating
omy, seeing that they define the “external world” as the
corrupt organizations that reach beyond the individuals
location for the “extrinsic opportunities” (p. 66) of the
that constitute them. For example, organizations
individual, as well as stating that the proposal should
be seen as an “inner-to-outer-world approach” (p. 58). may create objectives that are difficult to achieve by
Furthermore, as in CICAF, there doesn’t seem to be any honest means and that, in conjunction with a culture

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
6 J. G. Modesto & R. Pilati

Figure 4. The Multilevel Analytical Model of Corruption.

of strong competitiveness, may favor corrupt prac-


we hereby propose the Analytical Model of Corruption
tices. The macro-level also includes, according to the
(AMC) that takes into account the various views that
authors, studies that assess national and international
should be considered when studying this subject
corruption indexes.
(Ashforth et al., 2008) as well as concern in linking
Seeing that corruption widely affects the social sys-
levels of analysis (Dimant & Schulte, 2016). The model
tem, Ashforth et al. (2008) propose that corruption also
can be seen in Figure 4.
be studied from a wider perspective consisting, for
According to the definition endorsed in the present
example, the understanding of the supply and demand
theoretical study, corruption should be construed as the
of corrupt actions. After all, where there are corrupted,
abuse of power for illegal gains (Andersson & Hey-
there are corruptors and a more extensive system that
wood, 2009). In light of this understanding, the analysis
favors the existence of these social roles.
of corrupt behavior should begin by looking at the
For Ashforth et al. (2008), researching corruption
positional dimension, which includes the opportunity
from a long-term view means understanding that cor-
to undertake an action due to the position occupied
ruption is an old phenomenon, by which it is necessary
by the individual. It should be noted that this concern
to consider its historical factors. Finally, corruption can
with the notion of position of power does not seem to
also be researched from a deep view, which means
have been included in previous models found in the
assuming an interdisciplinary proposal that includes
literature and is, nevertheless, an essential aspect of
contributions from different areas.
the AMC.

Analytical Model of Corruption


Positional Dimension
Considering the lack of theoretical perspectives to guide
Once in a position of power, potential corruption situ-
the study of corruption in an interdisciplinary manner
ations may occur by which the individual may, or may
(Judge et al., 2011), as well as the importance of under-
not, act in a corrupt manner. Taking the positional
standing the psychosocial processes necessary for
dimension into account, therefore, means accepting that
appropriate research of corruption (Zaloznaya, 2014),
the position an individual occupies in a particular case,
within a certain context, is relevant to the analysis of

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
Corruption Analytical Model 7

psychosocial processes (Pereira & Araú jo, 2013). For


that women are less corrupt than men (Breen et al., 2017;
example, in the case of corruption, the differences and Swamy et al., 2001). According to the lower indexes of
similarities between processes that interfere in the cor-
corruption identified in certain studies, some authors,
rupt act may be tested when evaluating the corrupted
and even the World Bank, suggest that the presence of
(someone in a position of power that allow themselves
women in positions of power would be a good strategy
to be corrupted) and the corruptor (individual that
for better governance and a better approach to the fight
benefits from the illegal actions of the corrupted). In
against corruption (Swamy et al., 2001; World Bank,
some studies, this difference has already been consid-
2001). Not only researchers, but also the general popu-
ered at the time the scenarios being researched are
lation seem to agree that women in power may contrib-
elaborated (Bai et al., 2016), pointing to the existence
ute towards the fight against corruption, especially
of possible specific psychosocial processes for under-
because of the belief that women are more averse to
standing corrupt behavior when analyzing the various risk, aside from being considered as outsiders in politics
positions occupied. Another aspect in the analysis of
(Barnes et al., 2018). However, when analyzing gender
this phenomenon, taking into account the positional
together with the opportunity for corruption in the
dimension, involves researching to what level power
public sector, facts do not seem to be enough to state
itself, inherent to the position occupied, is capable of
that women are effectively less corrupt (Alhassan-
specifically favoring corrupt acts, as well as antisocial Alolo, 2007). In truth, it can be argued that
practices in general. In this regard, there is evidence that governments that have more women in positions of
power may favor behavior aimed at obtaining personal power are only less corrupt due to a liberal democracy
gains. However, this effect tends to occur among indi- that favors their election, and that, in truth, its liberal
viduals with low moral identity (DeCelles et al., 2012). It democracy itself that is associated with lower
can also be seen that in circumstances where power is corruption indices (Sung, 2003). Research on gender
perceived to be a way of influencing others, the position seems to point towards the need to identify moderators
of power may favor higher levels of aggressiveness and for the relationship between gender and corruption,
exploitation towards subordinates (Cislak et al., 2018). as well as if more
It is important to note that, although the analysis of
“typical” forms of corruption exist when taking gender
the positional dimension is considered to be the “gate-
into account.
way” to understanding corrupt behavior, it should be Aside from gender, other individual characteristics
considered as a common factor between all dimensions may be evaluated to understand the dispositional fac-
of the model (as indicated by the two-way arrows in the tors of corruption. In a research paper on white-collar
image). For example, when personality traits and their crime, it was found that those who committed a crime
impact on corruption are analyzed, we recommend that showed higher indices of hedonism, narcissism and
this impact be researched taking into account the posi- conscientiousness, and lower indices of self-control
tion of the corrupted and the corruptor. This is also (Blickle et al., 2006). White-collar criminals also seem
valid for the group dimension: Will group impact will to have lower indices of social consciousness when
be similar when the situation involves corrupted when compared to an individual in a position of power that
compared to corruptors? We believe that the positional did not commit the same crimes (Collins & Schmidt,
dimension will offer greater clarity to corruption pre- 2006).
dictors, which represents an important advantage of Still, regarding intraindividual factors, researchers
the AMC over previous models. have also evaluated the just world beliefs as predictors
of corruption. It was found that a greater perception of
Micro-level: Intraindividual Aspects of Corruption justice, based on the belief of a just world, was associ-
ated with lower intention of corruption indices (Bai
After understanding the positional dimension, let's et al., 2016).
analyze the AMC intraindividual factors (micro-level)
Despite evidence originating from specific research
which consist of elements that are equivalent to the on corruption, results from unethical behavior and dis-
micro view of Ashforth et al. (2008) and the internal honesty studies that point to possible variables relevant
world of CICAF (Collier, 2002) and Dimant and Schulte to understanding corrupt behavior may also be consid-
(2016). In this dimension, we propose that individual ered (see Lehnert et al., 2015; Treviñ o et al., 2006). It
characteristics (dispositional traits) be analyzed, together can be seen that, for example, the lower an
with the understanding of the mechanisms by which individual’s
persons evaluate a potential corruption situation (infor-
moral cognitive development, the greater the probabil-
mation processing). ity they will behave unethically (Knoll et al., 2016),
Regarding individual characteristics, an element that where individual differences in the perception of guilt
is commonly investigated is gender. There is evidence moderate that relationship (Johnson & Connelly, 2016).
Aside from the referred dispositional factors, some
research has also attempted to identify the skills of a

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
8 J. G. Modesto & R. Pilati

dishonest person. For example, there is evidence that intuitive and automated decisions. Although efficient
more creative people have higher indices of dishonest
behavior (Gino & Ariely, 2012). According to the
authors, creative people have a greater capacity to
justify their actions, thus increasing their chances of
behaving dishonestly.
Together with research on individual characteristics,
the AMC micro-level assumes that it is important to
analyze how information relating to a potential corrupt
act is processed and, consequently, how it influences
decision-making. Based on the tradition of research into
the dual process model of information processing
(Evans & Stanovich, 2013), the AMC foresees that a
corrupt action may be governed by controlled and/or
automatic processes. In this aspect, the AMC is aligned
with research into moral cognition that has been used
to analyze to what extent behavior for personal gain is
more intuitive or deliberate (Hallsson et al., 2018).
There is evidence suggesting that, in situations
where an individual has sufficient time and cognitive
resources to make a careful decision, the controlled
processes will prevail (Evans & Stanovich, 2013). In
such a case, an individual can carefully analyze the risks
and benefits of a corrupt act. This understanding is
consis- tent with what is posited by CICAF, and the
decision-
making cost-benefit models in general. Based specifi-
cally on the General Theory of Crime (Becker, 1968), it
is expected that, among other factors, when making
a
rational decision, the individual assesses the gains that
may be obtained by corruption in comparison to those
obtained by legal means; the risks of being found out
and punished; as well as the degree of punishment
should he/she be found out. In this sense, there is
evidence that greater perception of the risk will
decrease the intention of acting corruptly (Bai et al.,
2016).
In addition to the risks and benefits analysis, a more
controlled and conscious process may favor dishonest
practices according to the use of justifications that legit-
imate the behavior. In situations where the individual
can justify his/her actions as “legal” or by considering
them as broader moral benefits (Gino & Pierce, 2009) it is
more probable that the dishonest behavior will occur.
According to the Theory of Self-concept Maintenance,
the use of justifications is favorable since the individual
keeps the external rewards obtained through
dishonesty and at the same time they reduce the
intrinsic discomfort of having acted dishonestly (Mazar
et al., 2008).
Although it is relevant to evaluate the variables
linked to a more deliberate and controlled process, the
decision in a political or economic scope is not always
conscious as assumed in the cost-benefit models. There
is evidence
that our rational capacity is limited during the financial
decision-making process (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979),
and on certain occasions may contribute with more
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
in many circumstances, automatic processes may be
associated with errors in judgment and perception,
see- ing that, for example, people do not always have
the capacity to perceive a certain situation as unethical
as they lack the cognitive resources for a more
systematic evaluation (Darley, 2005), or the individual
may not have been capable of deliberately applying
self-control due to psychological exhaustion (Mead et
al., 2009).
Another important aspect regarding automatic pro-
cesses in corruption evaluation involves the analysis of
cognitive biases. Loss aversion, for example, is consid-
ered to be a cognitive bias that may favor the
occurrence of automated dishonest decision-making
(Schindler & Pfattheicher, 2017). Individuals, due to
loss aversion, will be more inclined to act
dishonestly to avoid
financial loss.
Furthermore, when processing information, empha-
sis should be placed on the importance of emotions
when analyzing decision-making in risk situations
(Reyna, 2004). More specifically, regarding
dishonesty, there is evidence that individuals that
act dishonestly
tend to gradually escalate their dishonest actions
seeing that repeated exposure to these actions
influences the amygdala, a region of the brain that is
directly linked to emotions (Garrett, Lazzaro, Ariely,
& Sharot, 2016).
In this sense, according to the authors, repeated expo-
sure to dishonest actions reduces emotional response
to those same actions, which may favor an increase in
dishonesty. Other studies also point to a relationship
between the amygdala and immoral acts (Shenhav &
Greene, 2014), which restates the importance of emo-
tions in the processing of information and decision-
making in the broader scope of unethical behavior.
The intraindividual process dimension of the
AMC proposes that cognitive processes should be
evaluated, and not only the demographic and
dispositional vari- ables, which may be predictors of
corrupt decision- making. It is necessary to
understand which factors influence corrupt behavior
when an automatic process
prevails and when the controlled processing of infor-
mation prevails. It is also necessary to understand
how dispositional variables influence the types of
cognitive processing, and as a result of this produce a
model that
includes intraindividual factors as the antecedents of
corrupt behavior.

Meso level: Group Processes and Corruption


The group process dimension is related to the
intraindi- vidual dimension and refers to the meso-
level of analy- sis. At this level, it is possible to
investigate the role played by groups in corrupt
decisions, which is some- thing that has been
neglected in empirical research and previous
theoretical models, as has already been mentioned.
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
Corruption Analytical Model 9

Concerning the role of groups, some researchers con-


social psychology), the greatest body of evidence can
sider the possibility (Schikora, 2011) that the existence of
be found in the contextual factors dimension, relating
groups inhibits corrupt behavior. This understanding
to the causes and consequences of corruption (Tanzi,
favored the development of the Four Eyes Principle
1998), considering the proposed distinction between
(4EP), which posits that a corrupt individual will be less
corruption, unethical behavior and dishonest behavior.
inclined to act dishonestly in front of other people.
One can highlight, for example, how the political and
Thus,
economic system and cultural aspect influence corrupt
the name “four eyes”, indicating the existence of “more practices.
eyes” would favor inspecting and the inhibition of cor- Concerning the political system, there is evidence
rupt behavior. Although intuitive, there is evidence that that a democratic government is associated with lower
4EP is not efficient (Schikora, 2011). Some phenomena
indi- ces of corruption (Pellegrini, 2011). It can,
typically researched under the scope of social psychol-
however, be seen, that this effect tends to occur in more
ogy contribute towards the understanding of the non-
consolidated democracies, with democratically elected
effectiveness of 4EP.
governments that have been in power during a
One of the group processes that helps in understand-
minimum uninter- rupted period of 10 years for the
ing the reason behind the non-effectiveness of 4EP is
impact on corruption to be perceived. One of the ways
social influence. In cases where a strong group identity
to understand these
exists and one of the members of the group acts dishon-
findings is that, in mature democracies, there is an
estly, that could create a group norm and cause the
expectation that corrupt politicians are not get re-
other members of the group to also act dishonestly
elected, which, consequently, would reduce the general
(Gino et al., 2009). Furthermore, under circumstances
corruption indices of the country. Additionally, the
where it is possible to act dishonestly under the pretext
impact democracy has in reducing corruption tends to
of increased benefits for the group, there is a greater
occur only in countries where the per capita Gross
prob- Domestic Product is approximately more than 2,000
ability of dishonesty taking place (Gino et al., 2013; Gino
dollars. In countries that fall below this level, democra-
& Pierce, 2009). In other words, the group may, under
tization does not reduce corruption insofar as it arises
certain circumstances, contribute towards dishonest
as an opportunity to obtain more concrete gains in light
behavior instead of inhibiting it.
of the economic situation of the country (Jetter et al.,
Group influence is one of the ways of understanding 2015). Aside from the direct effect of economy, there is
what drives an individual to act dishonestly. According
evidence of interaction between a democratic system
to the Social Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1981), we have
and a liberal economic model with regards to the reduc-
various social identities according to the groups we
tion of corruption (Saha et al., 2009), seeing that a
integrate. In light of this, an individual may have a
liberal structure would reduce the opportunity public
moral personal identity and still act in a corrupt
agents have of obtaining gains by means of kickbacks
manner in certain situations based on other identities
and bribes. Nevertheless, this data should be analyzed
that are called upon depending on the context and the
with caution considering that there is evidence that
group in question (Darley, 2005).
compet- itiveness, which is typical of a liberal
As such, the AMC meso-level allows group corrup-
economic model,
tion processes to be investigated. In that respect, a
may favor corruption (Søreide, 2009).
research design that compares individual and group
In addition to the analysis of political and
decisions, studies that evaluate pressure to conform, economic models, commonly investigated within the
intergroup bias, among other group processes that fall field of polit- ical science and economics, it is important
under the scope of corruption, can be developed. A to understand the impact culture has on corruption.
specific line of research into this dimension is Based on Hostede’s Model (Hofstede, 1980), various
important, studies have focused on the impact of the model’s
considering that many corrupt actions take place within
cultural dimensions on corruption, especially with
the context of groups and teams, as is the case of orga- regards to the individualism-
nizations and politics. collectivism, power distance, masculinity-femininity,
and uncertainty avoidance dimensions.
Macro-level: Contextual Aspects of Corruption The individualism/collectivism dimension applies to
the understanding of the relationship pattern of the
There is a proximity of the AMC macro-level to the individuals with the groups to which they belong. In
external world of CICAF, to the meso and external societies with higher indices of individualism, there is a
world of Dimant and Schulte (2016), as well as to the greater tendency for individuals to establish personal
macro and long-term view of Ashforth et al. (2008), to goals and to consider themselves to be responsible
the extent that it gives context to the contextual factors of for their actions. There is also an understanding, as
corruption. Based on a more advanced stage of econom- opposed to countries with higher indices of
ics in the study of the phenomenon (when compared to collectivism, that rules and laws should apply in general
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
to the whole

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
10 J. G. Modesto & R. Pilati

of society, and not vary for the benefit of certain groups. social, political and economic norms on corruption
Based on the understanding that laws apply to every- was already explicitly shown in the model, to the extent
one, aside from the fact that collectivist societies tend to that the norms, especially descriptive norms, guide the
prioritize informal relationships (which may contribute behavior according to the context. In this regard, it can
towards personal advantages such as corruption), var-
be seen that descriptive norms influence the intention
ious studies have shown that countries with higher
of
indices of individualism tend to have lower levels of
corruption, by which this relationship is partially medi-
corruption (Davis & Ruhe, 2003; Jha & Panda, 2017; ated by the level of moral disengagement of the indi-
Yeganeh, 2014). vidual (Zhao et al., 2019). According to the authors,
The power distance dimension refers to the degree to descriptive norms tend to indicate that acting in a cor-
which people accept the fact that power is unevenly rupt manner is something normal within that context.
distributed. We are thus dealing with the acceptance
This understanding activates moral disengagement
of the hierarchy of social structures as if it were a
mechanisms that serve to justify and legitimate a
natural and “unchangeable” process. Therefore, higher corrupt act.
indices The macro level, therefore, favors the understanding
of power distance tend to be related to higher indices of of contextual factors of corruption. On this level, studies
corruption (Soeharto & Nugroho, 2018) seeing that, can be developed to compare different political and
based on structured hierarchies, it will not be likely that economic models and their impact on corruption.
individuals will oppose the behavior of those in power How corruption is expressed (i.e.: Cronyism, bribery,
(even if this behavior is corrupt). Aside from this, white-collar crime) within different cultural contexts
greater power distance favors the exchange of favors, may also be analyzed, in order to identify whether or
kickbacks and bribes by those in power in detriment of not more typical forms of expression exist depending
those who it is unlikely will have access to those posi- on the culture. Furthermore, from a macro perspective,
tions (Davis & Ruhe, 2003; Yeganeh, 2014). Neverthe- it is essential to undertake studies that analyze the
less, there is evidence of a positive relationship repercus- sions of corruption. The analysis of this
between the power distance levels of a country and the dimension, to the extent found in literature, has
perceived degree of reputation of its organizations been privileged by researchers, especially when
(Deephouse et al., 2016). research in economics and political science was taken
The masculinity-femininity dimension involves the into account, and was included in the AMC multilevel
evaluation of aspects such as the degree of competitive- proposal.
ness and obtaining material rewards in detriment of
cooperation and quality of life. The higher the mascu-
linity index, the greater the competitiveness and mate- Concluding remarks
rialism, while higher levels of femininity are associated
The objective of this work was to propose the Analytical
with being careful and cooperative. In masculine cul-
Model of Corruption as a multilevel model for interdis-
tures success and money are thus prioritized, which ciplinary research. The main advantages of the AMC,
favors its association with corruption (Davis & Ruhe, when compared with the few previous models found in
2003; Yeganeh, 2014).
literature, are that its structure is based on a clear defi-
The uncertainty avoidance dimension deals with the
nition of corruption that, in that sense, take into account
cultural aspect, which involves the degree to which
the importance of analyzing the positional dimension to
people in certain places are tolerant of the fact that the
understand corruption. Moreover, contrary to previous
future is unpredictable. Countries with higher indices of
models, it highlights the role played by groups in cor-
uncertainty avoidance tend to seek out ways of “con- ruption, which is an aspect that was neglected in other
trol” that allow for greater predictability in the world. models.
In light of this, the development of bureaucratic
It is also important to emphasize the multilevel
structures
aspect of the AMC. Despite the distinction made
that favor corruption is observed (Davis & Ruhe, 2003).
between the positional, micro, meso and macro
Furthermore, corruption can be seen as being safer in
dimensions, the model proposes a mutual interaction
such a context, providing immediate rewards and guar-
between them. As such, it is foreseen in the model
antees, which also helps to understand the relationship
that an individual
between this cultural dimension and corrupt practices
in a position of power should make decisions. The
(Park, 2003).
tendency to make a corrupt decision is influenced by
Still at a macro level, aside from culture, another
dispositional factors, as well as by how a situation is
variable of interest to social psychology that
evaluated. Should it be evaluated in a rational and
contributes towards the understanding of corrupt
behavior is the social norm. Despite the limitations deliberate manner, if the benefits outweigh the risks, it
already discussed regarding CICAF (Collier, 2002), the is more likely that the corrupt behavior will take place.
impact of the When automatic processes prevail, some biases also
tend to favor corrupt behavior, as is the case with loss
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
Corruption Analytical Model 11

aversion. However, this decision is limited by group approach to measuring corruption. Political Studies, 57(4),
and contextual factors. The presence of a group may 746–767. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2008.00758.x
inhibit corrupt behavior, as long as the group has Ashforth, B. E., Gioia, D. A., Robinson, S. L., & Treviño, L. K.
norms to prevent corrupt behavior and some level of (2008). Re-viewing organizational corruption. Academy of
group identity exists. However, if the group has norms Management Review, 33(3), 670–684. https://doi.org/
that induce corrupt behavior, the presence of more 10.5465/AMR.2008.32465714
indi- viduals may favor corruption. This interaction Ayal, S., Gino, F., Barkan, R., & Ariely, D. (2015). Three
is also principles to REVISE people’s unethical behavior.
influenced by contextual factors. Social, political and Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(6), 738–741. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1745691615598512
economic, as well as cultural norms will influence the
Bai, B., Liu, X., & Kou, Y. (2016). Belief in a just world lowers
tendency to behave corruptly. Even though the context
bribery intention. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19(1),
does not favor corrupt behavior, excuses may be used 66–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12108
to justify the behavior, breaking with the social norm. Barnes, T. D., Beaulieu, E., & Saxton, G. W. (2018). Restoring
Based on the interactive nature of the AMC, despite trust in the police: Why female officers reduce suspicions of
the influence of groups and contextual factors on indi- corruption. Governance, 31(1), 143–161. https://doi.org/
vidual behavior, individuals also influence group and 10.1111/gove.12281
contextual dynamics. A single individual is capable of Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: An economic
influencing an entire group, a typical process in the approach. Journal of Political Economy, 76(2), 169–217.
https://doi.org/10.1086/259394
phenomenon of leadership (see Tanzi, 1998, regarding
Blickle, G., Schlegel, A., Fassbender, P., & Klein, U. (2006).
how the role of a leader may be an example to prevent
Some personality correlates of business white-collar crime.
corrupt behavior). Additionally, various corrupt indi-
Applied Psychology, 55(2), 220–233. https://doi.org/10.1111/
vidual behaviors, if they go unpunished, may gradually
j.1464-0597.2006.00226.x
become the norm, thus creating a “culture of corrup- Breen, M., Gillanders, R., Mcnulty, G., & Suzuki, A.
tion”. This perspective of interaction favors the AMC to (2017). Gender and corruption in business. The Journal of
be used as a structure that could guide interdisciplinary Development Studies, 53(9), 1486–1501. https://doi.org/
studies, which is essential for a comprehensive and 10.1080/00220388.2016.1234036
efficient analysis of the antecedents of corruption Cislak, A., Cichocka, A., Wojcik, A. D., & Frankowska, N.
(Ashforth et al., 2008; Judge et al., 2011). (2018). Power corrupts, but control does not: What stands
The AMC was not developed with the intent to behind the effects of holding high positions. Personality and
exhaust the relevant variable set used to understand Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(6), 944–957. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0146167218757456
corrupt behavior. Its purpose is to help guide and orga-
Collier, M. W. (2002). Explaining corruption: An institutional
nize large sets of variables relating to corruption and
choice approach. Crime, Law and Social Change, 38(1), 1–32.
dishonest behavior antecedents. As already mentioned,
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1019802614530
it is based on a multilevel premise and on the under- Collins, J. M., & Schmidt, F. L. (2006). Personality, integrity,
standing of the multideterminism of human behavior, and white collar crime: A construct validity study. Personnel
the analysis of factors in isolated levels of analysis is Psychology, 46(2), 295–311. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-
not sufficient to fully understand corrupt behavior. As 6570.1993.tb00875.x
such, the AMC proposes an organizational structure Darley, J. M. (2005). The cognitive and social psychology of
that serves as a means for researchers and contagious organizational corruption. Brooklyn Law Review,
professionals interested in fighting and preventing 70(4), 1177–1194.
Davis, J. H., & Ruhe, J. A. (2003). Perceptions of country
corruption to develop strategies aimed at
understanding the factors corruption: Antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Business
Ethics, 43(4), 275–288. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
and variables of specific situations or, otherwise,
1023038901080
develop strategies to prevent and mitigate undesirable DeCelles, K. A., DeRue, D. S., Margolis, J. D., & Ceranic, T. L.
conduct related with dishonesty. Thus, the AMC proves (2012). Does power corrupt or enable? When and why
to be, from a multilevel standpoint, as a useful contrib- power facilitates self-interested behavior. Journal of Applied
utor to the fields of expertise of various professionals, Psychology, 97(3), 681–689. https://doi.org/10.1037/
such as lawyers, political scientists, sociologists, econo- a0026811
mists and psychologists that are involved in research- Deephouse, D. L., Newburry, W., & Soleimani, A. (2016). The
ing, preventing and fighting corruption. effects of institutional development and national culture on
cross-national differences in corporate reputation. Journal of
References World Business, 51(3), 463–473. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.jwb.2015.12.005
Alhassan-Alolo, N. (2007). Gender and corruption: Testing
Dimant, E., & Schulte, T. (2016). The nature of corruption: An
the new consensus. Public Administration and Development,
interdisciplinary perspective. German Law Journal, 17(1),
27(3), 227–237. https://doi.org/10.1002/pad.455
53–72. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2071832200019684
Andersson, S., & Heywood, P. M. (2009). The politics of
perception: Use and abuse of Transparency International’s

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
12 J. G. Modesto & R. Pilati

Doise, W. (1980). Levels of explanation in the European


Social Psychology, 46(1), 65–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Journal of Social Psychology. European Journal of Social
jasp.12353
Psychology, 10(3), 213–231.
Lehnert, K., Park, Y., & Singh, N. (2015). Research note and
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420100302 review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature:
Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process
Boundary conditions and extensions. Journal of Business
theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate.
Ethics, 129(1), 195–219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241.
014-2147-2
https:// doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685
Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of
Garrett, N., Lazzaro, S. C., Ariely, D., & Sharot, T. (2016).
honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal
The brain adapts to dishonesty. Nature Neuroscience, 19,
of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633–644. https://doi.org/
1727–1732. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4426
10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633
Gino, F., & Ariely, D. (2012). The dark side of creativity:
Mazar, N., & Ariely, D. (2006). Dishonesty in everyday life
Original thinkers can be more dishonest. Journal of
and its policy implications. Journal of Public Policy &
Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 445–459.
Marketing, 25 (1), 117–126.
https://doi.org/ 10.1037/a0026406
https://doi.org/10.1509/jppm.25.1.117
Gino, F., Ayal, S., & Ariely, D. (2009). Contagion and
Mead, N. L., Baumeister, R. F., Gino, F., Schweitzer, M. E., &
differentiation in unethical behavior: The effect of one bad
Ariely, D. (2009). Too tired to tell the truth: Self-control
apple on the barrel. Psychological Science, 20(3), 393–398.
resource depletion and dishonesty. Journal of Experimental
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02306.x
Social Psychology, 45(3), 594–597. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Gino, F., & Pierce, L. (2009). Dishonesty in the name of
j.jesp.2009.02.004
equity. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1153–1160.
Park, H. (2003). Determinants of corruption: A cross‐national
https://doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02421.x
analysis. Multinational Business Review, 11(2), 29–48.
Gino, F., Ayal, S., & Ariely, D. (2013). Self-serving altruism?
https:// doi.org/10.1108/1525383X200300010
The lure of unethical actions that benefit others. Journal of
Pellegrini, L. (2011). Corruption, development and the environment.
Economic Behavior & Organization, 93, 285–292. https://
Springer Netherlands.
doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2013.04.005
Pereira, M. E., & Araújo, S. de F. (2013). Social cognition. In
Hallsson, B. G., Siebner, H. R., & Hulme, O. J. (2018). Fairness,
K. D. Keith (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Cross-Cultural Psychology
fast and slow: A review of dual process models of fairness.
(1st Ed., pp. 1191–1197). Wiley-Blackwell.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 89, 49–60. https://
Peters, J. G., & Welch, S. (1978). Political corruption in
doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.016
America: A search for definitions and a theory, or if political
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences comparing
corruption is in the mainstream of American politics why
values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across is it not in the mainstream of American politics research?
nations. Sage Publications. American Political Science Review, 72(3), 974–984.
Jetter, M., Montoya Agudelo, A. , & Ramírez Hassan, A. https:// doi.org/10.2307/1955115
(2015). The effect of democracy on corruption: Income is Pettigrew, T. F. (2018). The emergence of Contextual Social
key. World Development, 74, 286–304. Psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(7),
https://doi.org/10.1016/ 963–971. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218756033
j.worlddev.2015.05.016
Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and
Jha, C., & Panda, B. (2017). Individualism and corruption: A
theory. Praeger.
cross-country analysis. Economic Papers: A Journal of Applied
Reyna, V. F. (2004). How people make decisions that involve
Economics and Policy, 36(1), 60–74. risk: A dual-processes approach. Current Directions in
https://doi.org/10.1111/ 1759-3441.12163 Psychological Science, 13(2), 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Johnson, J. F., & Connelly, S. (2016). Moral disengagement
j.0963-7214.2004.00275.x
and ethical decision-making. Journal of Personnel Psychology,
Rilling, J. K., Gutman, D. A., Zeh, T. R., Pagnoni, G., Berns,
15(4), 184–189. https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/
G. S., & Kilts, C. D. (2002). A neural basis for social
a000166 cooperation. Neuron, 35(2), 395–405. https://doi.org/
Judge, W. Q., McNatt, D. B., & Xu, W. (2011). The
10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00755-9
antecedents and effects of national corruption: A meta-
Saha, S., Gounder, R., & Su, J.-J. (2009). The interaction
analysis. Journal of World Business, 46(1), 93–103.
effect of economic freedom and democracy on corruption: A
https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.jwb.2010.05.021
panel cross-country analysis. Economics Letters, 105(2), 173–
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An
analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–292. 176.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2009.07.010
https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185
Schikora, J. T. (2011, March 1). Bringing the four-eyes-
Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010).
principle to the lab (Munich Discussion Paper 2011–3).
Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic
Ludwig- Maximilians-Universitä t Mü nchen.
evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work.
http://epub.ub.uni-
Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 1–31. https://doi.org/ muenchen.de/12160/
10.1037/a0017103
Schindler, S., & Pfattheicher, S. (2017). The frame of the
Knoll, M., Lord, R. G., Petersen, L.-E., & Weigelt, O. (2016). game: Loss-framing increases dishonest behavior. Journal of
Examining the moral grey zone: The role of moral
Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 172–177. https://doi.org/
disengagement, authenticity, and situational strength in
10.1016/j.jesp.2016.09.009
predicting unethical managerial behavior. Journal of Applied
Scott, E. D., & Jehn, K. A. (1999). Ranking rank behaviors:
A comprehensive situation-based definition of dishonesty.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5
Corruption Analytical Model 13

Business & Society, 38(3), 296–325. Transparency International (2018). How do you define corruption?
https://doi.org/10.1177/ 000765039903800304 https://www.transparency.org/what-is-corruption
Shenhav, A., & Greene, J. D. (2014). Integrative moral Treviño, L. K., den Nieuwenboer, N. A., & Kish-Gephart, J. J.
judgment: Dissociating the roles of the amygdala and (2014). (Un)Ethical behavior in organizations. Annual
ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), Review of Psychology, 65(1), 635–660.
4741–4749. https://doi.org/10.1146/ annurev-psych-113011-143745
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3390-13.2014 Treviño, L. K., Weaver, G. R., & Reynolds, S. J. (2006).
Soeharto, I., & Nugroho, N. (2018). Are we culturally Behavioral ethics in organizations: A review. Journal of
corrupt? Revisiting the relationship between cultural Management, 32(6), 951–990. https://doi.org/10.1177/
dimensions and Corruption Perception Index. Asia Pacific 0149206306294258
Fraud Journal, 2(2), 143–149. World Bank (2001). Engendering development: Through gender
http://doi.org/10.21532/apfj.001.17.02.02.02 equality in rights, resources and voice. Oxford University Press.
Søreide, T. (2009). Too risk averse to stay honest? International Yeganeh, H. (2014). Culture and corruption. International
Review of Law and Economics, 29(4), 388–395. Journal of Development Issues, 13(1), 2–24. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.irle.2009.03.001 10.1108/IJDI-04-2013-0038
Sung, H.-E. (2003). Fairer sex or fairer system? Gender and Zaloznaya, M. (2014). The social psychology of corruption:
corruption revisited. Social Forces, 82(2), 703–723. https:// Why it does not exist and why it should. Sociology Compass, 8
doi.org/10.1353/sof.2004.0028 (2), 187–202. https://doi.org/10.1111/soc4.12120
Swamy, A., Knack, S., Lee, Y., & Azfar, O. (2001). Gender and Zhao, H., Zhang, H., & Xu, Y. (2019). Effects of perceived
corruption. Journal of Development Economics, 64(1), 25–55. descriptive norms on corrupt intention: The mediating role of
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3878(00)00123-1 moral disengagement. International Journal of Psychology, 54
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies (1), 93–101. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12401
in social psychology (1st Ed.). Cambridge University Press.
Tanzi, V. (1998). Corruption around the world: Causes,
consequences, scope, and cures. IMF Economic Review, 45(4),
559–594. https://doi.org/10.2307/3867585

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Massachusetts Amherst, on 28 May 2020 at 12:30:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2020.5

You might also like