You are on page 1of 10

Psychol Stud

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-021-00602-1

TARGET ARTICLE (WITH PEER COMMENTARY)

Investigating How Intolerance of Uncertainty and Emotion


Regulation Predict Experiential Avoidance in Non-Clinical
Participants
Kisley Di Giuseppe1 • Alisdair J. G. Taylor1

Received: 11 August 2020 / Accepted: 26 April 2021


Ó National Academy of Psychology (NAOP) India 2021

Abstract Emotional regulation and intolerance of uncer- targeting both intolerance of uncertainty and appropriate
tainty have been independently implicated in the overuse use of emotion regulation may help enhance individuals’
of experiential avoidance in stress-related situations. self-awareness in relation to avoidant responses to prevent
However, little research has examined how this relation- the potential creation of the cycle that maintains anxiety
ship manifests altogether in a non-clinical sample. The disorders.
current study aimed to determine the predictive nature of
intolerance of uncertainty and the ability to regulate Keywords Emotion dysregulation 
emotions on experiential avoidance. A secondary aim was Experiential avoidance  Intolerance of uncertainty 
to establish the role of worry in these relationships. Par- Non-clinical sample  Worry
ticipants (N = 360) completed questionnaires that mea-
sured intolerance of uncertainty, emotion dysregulation,
worry and experiential avoidance. In stage one of the Introduction
analysis, a bivariate linear regression was conducted to see
if both intolerance of uncertainty and emotion regulations Experiential avoidance is a strategy deployed in situations
would significant predict avoidance. These were then where individuals face extreme unwelcome feelings in
entered into a multiple linear regression model with forced relation to an event. For example, a highly sensitive person
entry method to explain the variance in experiential refuses to see a doctor for a normal check-up because they
avoidance. Subsequently, a mediation analysis was con- fear they could be diagnosed with a serious life-threatening
ducted using worry, between intolerance of uncertainty and medical condition. This discomfort subsequently develops
experiential avoidance and between emotion regulation into an unwillingness to remain in connection with the
and experiential avoidance. As expected, individuals’ intimate bodily sensations, emotions, thoughts, memories
tendency to intolerance of uncertainty and poor emotion and behavioural predispositions (Hayes et al., 1996),
regulation individually predicted experiential avoidance. enhancing the intentional desire of control or escape
Furthermore, worry mediated these relationships. These (Santanello & Gardner, 2006). Experiential avoidance is
findings have implications for self-awareness responses in thus manifested by the individual’s intentional effort to
healthy individuals who show avoidant behaviours. alter the form and frequency of the particular event that
Inflexible use of emotion regulation and low tolerance of induces feelings of anxiety. As an apparent regulatory
uncertainty make a significant contribution to experiential strategy, it becomes maladaptive when implemented
avoidance. Therefore, therapeutic interventions specifically rigidly and inflexibly (Schmalz & Murrell, 2010). Partic-
ularly, by creating a pleasant sense of control due to the
decrease of unwanted internal feelings, it might lead to a
& Kisley Di Giuseppe biased self-rewarding amplifying loop (Hayes et al., 2004).
kdigiuseppe@freelanceresearchlab.com
Furthermore, the perpetuation of distorted beliefs through
1
Kingston University, Penrhyn Rd, Kingston upon Thames rigidly avoiding aversive stimuli has been shown to relate
KT1 2EE, UK to the development and persistence of psychopathology

123
Psychol Stud

within a different range of disorders (Kumpula et al., 2011; behavioural avoidance to increase their sense of control by
Spinhoven et al., 2017). Therefore, experiential avoidance reducing uncertainty. This showed an association between
is seen as functional due to the subsequent short-term relief the excessive responding as desire of predictability and the
it provides from aversive stimuli (Schreiber et al., 2012). inflexible use of avoidance. Others (Vander Haegen &
This research focuses on further understanding the con- Etienne, 2016) have highlighted a relationship between
struct of experiential avoidance. Past research has mainly these constructs while supporting the idea that intolerance
focussed on its explanatory nature in both clinical (Shorey of uncertainty has been found to play a central role in
et al., 2017; Varese et al., 2016) and non-clinical (Emerson developing negative problem orientation among parents of
et al., 2019) samples, yielding thus the need for research childhood cancer survivors. Similar research (Aktar et al.,
that focuses on its potential antecedents. Specifically, the 2017) has supported an association between parents’ levels
current study explored how experiential avoidance is of intolerance of uncertainty and experiential avoidance as
explained by intolerance of uncertainty and emotional contributing factors of the environmental pathways to
regulation. parent-to-child transmission of anxiety. Taken together, the
above findings highlight a strong relationship between
Intolerance of Uncertainty intolerance of uncertainty and experiential avoidance.

Intolerance of uncertainty has been conceptualised as a Emotion Regulation


cognitive bias towards a set of negative beliefs about
unpredictability, compromising the individual’s assessment Emotion regulation is a multi-dimensional construct,
(inaccurate threat appraisal) and related behavioural responsible for modifying, monitoring and evaluating
response (inability to cope) in relation to an unknown individuals’ responses (Gross et al., 2006). The regulation
environment (Buhr & Dugas, 2002). It has been thought to of emotion involves the reduction of negative affect due to
encompass both prospective (discomforting cognitive individuals’ innate tendency to seek instant satisfaction to
appraisals) and inhibitory (apprehension, avoidance and decrease short-term discomfort when facing distress (Tice
immobilisation) responses due to the unpredictable future et al., 2001). The use of specific regulatory strategies has
(Shihata et al., 2017). Furthermore, the need for pre- different social survival advantages. However, the lack of
dictability has been interpreted as a coping response to flexibility in implementing strategies to regulate emotions
alleviate distress (Pawluk & Koerner, 2013), and suggests contributes to increase undesirable emotional feelings,
an innate desire for controllability (Mushtaq et al., 2011), while evoking ‘‘escaping’’ maladaptive responses and
contributing to avoidance. Most recently, intolerance of compromising long-term goals (Campbell-Sills et al.,
uncertainty has been defined as ‘‘an individual’s disposi- 2015). Hayes et al. (2004) found that the over-reliance on
tional incapacity to endure the aversive response triggered emotional suppression in the presence of distressing per-
by the perceived absence of salient, key, or sufficient sonal experiences is perceived to function as a form of
information, and sustained by the associated perception of experiential avoidance of emotions.
uncertainty’’ (Carleton, 2016; p. 31). Evidence from clinical research (Chapman et al., 2011)
The construct is central in the development of poor proposed poor emotion regulation to be a key factor in
problem orientation, a cognitive dysfunction that causes increasing the plausibility of experiential avoidance in
perceived threats and doubts in problem-solving capability borderline personality disorder. Non-clinical studies (Shi
(Robichaud & Dugas, 2005). Poor problem orientation et al., 2016) found that students with less ability to regulate
interferes with the individual’s metacognitive and attitu- their emotions would present a greater tendency to engage
dinal processes in relation to his or her own problem- in experiential avoidance. Others (Mohammadkhani et al.,
solving techniques, leading to unacceptable distressing 2016) observed that emotion regulation plays an important
foreseen outcomes (Dugas et al., 1998). Based on this line mediating role between experiential avoidance and anxi-
of thinking, it is reasonable to assume that intolerance of ety-related symptomatology, leading vulnerable individu-
uncertainty could relate to experiential avoidance. als to potential unmanageable reactions (Yook et al., 2010).
Previous experimental research (Jacoby et al., 2016) has This unmanageable reaction might manifest in the form of
shown an association between distress during responding in experiential avoidance. Taken together, the above findings
uncertain situations and self-report inhibitory intolerance highlight a strong relationship between emotion regulation
of uncertainty (a construct that measures avoidance when and experiential avoidance.
facing uncertainty). The above shows how paralysis might
have an impact on how highly vulnerable individuals
respond to the unknown. Moreover, Flores et al. (2018)
found that high-intolerant participants engaged in

123
Psychol Stud

The Role of Worry as a Mediator Theoretical Perspectives

Past research sees worry to be related with the constructs The study’s overarching theoretical foundation is based on
under investigation, particularly, worry has been identified the combination of Hillen et al.’s (2017) idea of uncertainty
as an anticipatory strategy useful to decrease internal dis- tolerance processes, and Grupe and Nitschke’s (2013)
tress by avoiding unpleasant internal experiences (Buhr & model of anxiety on neural responses to uncertainty.
Dugas, 2012). The significant relationship between expe- Within Hillen and colleagues’ multi-dimensional concep-
riential avoidance and worry is consistent with previous tual model, uncertainty is identified as consisting of indi-
findings from non-clinical studies (Roemer et al., 2005), viduals’ metacognitive awareness triggered by stimuli
with experiential avoidance to be also a partial mediator in perceived to be unknown. The way uncertainty is perceived
the relationship between worry and maladaptive perfec- is sourced by the level of unreliability presented due to
tionism (Santanello & Gardner, 2006). Despite their evi- ambiguous or missing information, and difficult to predict
dent similarities, worry is considered to be a distinct in view of the randomness of its probability. This would
variable to experiential avoidance. As mentioned above, further create the condition where individuals would have
experiential avoidance is a behavioural strategy used to limiting comprehension of its causal determinants and
avoid uncomfortable internal experiences (Hayes et al., effects. As a result, the conscious awareness of the
1996). Vice versa, worry as a cognitive process comprises unknown leads to a set of psychological reactions: cogni-
uncontrollable chain of images and thoughts towards the tive (a continuum of negative or positive appraisals varying
unknown (Borkovec et al., 1983) with a probable function for example from threat to opportunity); emotional (a
of avoiding threats (Wells, 2005). Furthermore, it provides continuum of negative or positive states varying for
individuals with a momentary sense of control (Freeston example from worry to curiosity); and behavioural (con-
et al., 1994). Evidence of the association between worry scious or unconscious responses aiming at avoiding or
and poor emotion regulation has also been highlighted altering the uncertain situation). According to this model,
(Salters-Pedneault et al., 2006), particularly, the role when uncertainty is perceived as a threat, this is implicitly
played by the confidence in problem-solving abilities in aversive resulting in negative affective states. Anderson
male participants in reducing levels of worry (Zlomke & et al. (2019) maintain that emotions perform an adaptive
Hahn, 2010). Furthermore, the mediating role of emotion function in guiding attention, cognition and action.
dysregulation within the relationship between worry and Therefore, the way an uncertain situation is perceived is a
anger rumination has also been indicated (Besharat & fundamental determinant of what specific emotion
Ramesh, 2017). responses are induced.
Intolerance of uncertainty-based models of worry have Specifically, when looking at the aversive effects of
hypothesised that high-intolerant individuals are more uncertainty in anxiety-related studies, Grupe and
susceptible to engage in worry, particularly, intolerance of Nitschke’s (2013) model proposes the existence of inter-
uncertainty has been found to play a central role in connected set of neurobiological and psychological pro-
developing positive beliefs about worrying (Vander Hae- cesses essential for adaptive anticipatory responses to
gen & Etienne, 2016). Intolerance of uncertainty is also uncertainty. It is further suggested that neurological alter-
seen to mediate the relationship between negative affec- ations in any of these processes underlie maladaptive
tivity and worry in non-clinical teenagers (Boelen et al., responses as these create the condition for increased threat
2010). According to Dugas et al.’s (1998) intolerance of expectancies, which exacerbates physiological and beha-
uncertainty model, cognitive bias due to the avoidance vioural reactivity under conditions of uncertainty. This
process of emotional stimuli negatively reinforces the toxic condition results in anticipatory maladaptive reactions in
chain of chronic worrying. Here, worrying functions as a order to avoid or reduce the impact of a perceived threat.
relief mechanism that alleviates self-predicted disappoint- For instance, biased pessimistic expectations resulting in
ment through negative reinforcement (Dugas et al., 1998). substantial anticipatory distress leads to elevated threat
Additionally, intolerance of uncertainty has been associ- expectancies. Consequently, this altogether increases both
ated with worry (Birrell et al., 2011), which in turn have vigilance and a biased interpretation towards ambiguity.
been associated with avoidance (Stapinski et al., 2010). In The following combination impedes discriminative analy-
light of the above evidence on the associations between the sis of cause and affect, which would further increase
current study constructs and worry, it appears logical for avoidance. Furthermore, engaging in avoidance prevents
the explanatory nature of this research to investigate the individuals from identifying and executing adapted
mediating role of worry in the relationships between these responses, while allowing negative predictions to persist.
constructs. Previous research (Carleton, 2016) has found similar
alterations specific to individuals’ self-reporting of

123
Psychol Stud

intolerance of uncertainty, showing that amplified beha- whether their behaviours potentially link to intolerance of
vioural and physiological responsiveness under uncertainty uncertainty and emotion regulation.
causes unsettling to cognitive, behavioural and emotional The current study also explored the potential mediating
functioning. role of worry in the relationship of intolerance of uncer-
In this study, it is hypothesised that in situations of tainty and experiential avoidance, given that the contribu-
uncertainty, individuals who are predisposed to aversive tion of beliefs about worry is underscored in the intolerance
vulnerable psychological reactivity to the unknown and of uncertainty model (Dugas et al., 1998). Similarly, as the
unable to regulate their emotion in response to it, would be model has also argued that the perpetuation of worry is a
more likely to be vigilant and distressed in relation to a form of cognitive avoidance in order to decrease the dis-
self-perceived threat, presenting therefore inefficient mal- tress of emotionally processed situations, it would be
adaptive avoidant responses as preventative strategies. interesting to explore how worry mediates the relationship
between emotion regulation and experiential avoidance.
Intolerance of Uncertainty, Emotional Regulation This exploratory analysis will extend the aforementioned
and Experiential Avoidance research and inform us the extent to which worry is
involved in each relationship.
The current study explored the way unpleasant uncertainty-
related feelings motivate individuals to engage in avoidant
coping regulatory strategies, especially in those who pre- Method
sent an intolerance towards the unknown. Specifically, we
endeavoured to explain the variability of experiential Participants
avoidance (outcome variable) as a function of both intol-
erance of uncertainty and emotion regulation (explanatory There were 360 participants (M = 27.2, SD = 8.2) after 19
variables). The overarching relationship between uncer- were excluded due to incomplete responses. The majority
tainty, avoidance and how emotions are regulated in situa- of participants were female (58.3%), Caucasian (77.5%)
tions of distress is evident (Jacoby et al., 2016). and drawn from a university student population (73.1%).
Specifically, the adoption of inadequate emotional regula- Some participants were given a paper version of the survey
tory strategies during uncertainty may lead some individ- (N = 69), while others completed it using Qualtrics
uals to use rigid avoidant coping mechanisms (Behar et al., (N = 291). There was no significant difference between the
2009). This may provide individuals with a momentary average age of participants in the two samples: paper vs
sense of relief benefit and control over future consequences online (M = 26.88, SD = 7.279 vs M = 27.27, SD = 8.383,
of an unknown eventuality (Dugas et al., 1998). Both t(358) = 0.350, p = 0.726). Response method was factored
intolerance of uncertainty and emotional regulation have in as a control to ensure it was not confounding the data.
been independently associated with experiential avoidance This did not significantly affect the models, nor predict the
(Lee et al., 2010; Shi et al., 2016). However, to our outcome variables.
knowledge no research has looked at how this relationship
manifests altogether in a large non-clinical sample. More- Measures and Procedure
over, the second aim of the study was to determine the
stronger predictor when comparing the two explanatory The study’s protocol was approved by the university’s
variables. In line with past research (Grupe & Nitschke, ethics committee. Participation was voluntary, with a clear
2013; Hillen, et al., 2017), it is expected that both intol- option to withdraw at any time. Participants completed a
erance of uncertainty and emotion regulation will inde- battery of questionnaires (see below). The order of the
pendently significantly predict experiential avoidance. scales was counterbalanced across participants. A debrief
Specifically, individuals who score high on intolerance of form was provided along with contact details of the
uncertainty and emotion dysregulation would score high on research team. The study took approximately fifteen min-
experiential avoidance. These findings will be beneficial to utes to complete.
be considered in community settings such as workplace,
particularly for those who show avoidant signs and those Experiential Avoidance
who support such individuals in situations of employment
uncertainty-related stress (Mentler et al., 2005). The study The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II;
outcomes could also be potentially enlightening with Bond et al., 2011) measured experiential avoidance. It
healthy university students who use rigid avoidance coping consists of seven items (e.g. ‘‘I’m afraid of my feelings’’)
strategy (Bello & Gumarao, 2016), in order to understand measured on a seven-point Likert scale (1 = ‘‘never true’’
to 7 = ‘‘always true’’), with higher scores indicating greater

123
Psychol Stud

experiential avoidance. The AAQ-II showed excellent with forced entry method to explain the variance in expe-
internal consistency in this sample (a = 0.91), in line with riential avoidance. All relevant statistical assumptions were
previous non-clinical studies (a = 0.89, Lewis & Naugle, met. Subsequently, mediation analyses were conducted
2017). using worry as a mediator between intolerance of uncer-
tainty and experiential avoidance and separately between
Intolerance of Uncertainty emotion regulation and experiential avoidance. Mediation
in both models were performed conducting a path analysis
The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale Short Form (IUS-12; to test the models in line with Baron and Kenny (1986).
Carleton et al., 2007) consists of twelve items (e.g. ‘‘Un- Firstly, the predictors had to be significantly associated
foreseen events upset me greatly’’) measured on a five- with the outcome variable (path c). Secondly, the predic-
point Likert scale (1 = ‘‘Not at all characteristic of me’’ to tors had to be significantly related to the mediator (path a).
5 = ‘‘Entirely characteristic of me’’), with higher scores Thirdly, the mediator had to be significantly related to the
indicating greater tendencies to intolerance. Excellent outcome variable (path b). Finally, the relationship of the
psychometric properties were shown in this sample predictor and outcome variable should attenuate when
(a = 0.90), in line with previous non-clinical studies controlling for the mediator (path c’). The mediation
(a = 0.91, Carleton et al., 2007). analyses were conducted using the PROCESS v.3.3 macro
model (Hayes, 2017). SPSS 24.0 software was used to
Emotion Regulation conduct these analyses.

Emotion regulation was measured using the Difficulties in


Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, Results
2004). It consists of 36 items (e.g. ‘‘When I’m upset, I have
difficulty concentrating’’) measured on a five-point Likert Altogether IUS, DERS and PSWQ significantly positively
scale (1 = almost never, to 5 = almost always). Items 2, 6, correlated with AAQ (all r’s [ 0.596, p \ 0.001; see
8, 10, 17, 20, 24, 34 are scored reversely, with higher Table1). A bivariate linear regression showed that IUS
scores indicating less ability to regulate emotions. The significantly predicted the variance in AAQ F(1,
DERS has shown excellent internal consistency in non- 358) = 198.067, p \ 0.001. Similarly, DERS significantly
clinical sample (a = 0.93, Salters-Pedneault et al., 2006), predicted AAQ F(1, 358) = 427.935, p \ 0.001. There-
and in the current sample (a = 0.95). fore, both IUS and DERS were entered into a multiple
regression model in order to explain the variance in AAQ.
Worry As expected, a multiple linear regression showed that the
model comprised of IUS and DERS significantly explained
The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., 60% of the variance in AAQ F(2, 357) = 271.617,
1990) consists of 16 items measured on a five-point Likert p \ 0.001. The strongest predictor of AAQ was DERS
scale (1 = ‘‘not at all typical of me’’ to 5 = ‘‘very typical of (b = 0.586, t = 14.919, p \ 0.001), for everyone unit
me’’). Items are worded positively and negatively (e.g. increase in DERS, AAQ increases by 0.236. Furthermore,
‘‘Many situations make me worry’’) with items 1, 3, 8, 10, IUS significantly predicted AAQ (b = 0.286, t = 7.285,
11 scored reversely (e.g. ‘‘I never worry about anything’’). p \ 0.001), for every one unit increase in IUS, AAQ
Higher scores suggest worrisome issues. The PSWQ increases by 0.290 (see Table 1).
demonstrated strong internal consistency both in the pre- As both the variables predicted the AAQ, they were
sent sample (a = 0.93;), and across studies with students entered in two separate mediation analyses to determine the
(a = 0.93, Zlomke & Hahn, 2010). role of PSWQ in their relationship with AAQ. The first
mediation analysis showed that IUS significantly predicted
Statistical Analysis PSWQ (b = 0.596, t = 14.041, p \ 0.001). Moreover,
PSWQ significantly predicted AAQ (b = 0.401, t = 8.272,
Descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients were p \ 0.001). Finally, there was a significant indirect effect
calculated for the various measures (see Table 1). In stage of IUS on AAQ through PSWQ and this represented a
one of the analysis, a bivariate linear regression was con- relatively small effect, k2 = 0.242, 95% BCa CI [0.179,
ducted to see if intolerance of uncertainty would signifi- 0.308]. Moreover, a Sobel test supported the above full
cantly predict avoidance and if so then would qualify for mediation effect z = 7.066, p \ 0.001. The second medi-
the proceeding multiple regression. Similarly, this was also ation analysis showed that DERS significantly predicted
done for DERS. Since both predictors were significant, PSWQ (b = 0.529, t = 11.798, p \ 0.001). Moreover,
they were entered into a multiple linear regression model PSWQ significantly predicted AAQ (b = 0.311, t = 8.015,

123
Psychol Stud

Table 1 Hierarchical multiple regression, correlations coefficients and descriptive statistics


Variables Hierarchical multiple regression
B SE B b DR2 F

.601 271.617*
Constant -7.102 1.318
IUS .290 .040 .286*
DERS .236 .016 .586*
Summary of correlations
AAQ IUS DERS M SD
1. AQQ – 22.57 9.84
2. IUS .597* – 30.91 9.72
3. DERS .738* .530* – 87.63 24.40
4. PSWQ .614* .596* .529* 52.10 13.77
In the Summary of Correlations higher scores are indicative of more extreme responding in the direction of the construct assessed
AAQ Experiential avoidance; IUS Intolerance of uncertainty; DERS Difficulties in emotion regulation; PSWQ Worry
* p \ .001

p \ 0.001). Finally, there was a significant indirect effect regulate emotion and level of intolerance in relation to
of DERS on AAQ through PSWQ and this represented a uncertainty. Furthermore, as part of its exploratory nature,
very small effect size, k2 = 0.066, 95% BCa CI [0.048, the current study also investigated the possible mediational
0.086]. Also, a Sobel test supported the above full media- role of worry in the above relationships. Findings showed
tion effect z = 6.608, p \ 0.001 (See Fig. 1). that Worry mediated the linkages of both intolerance of
uncertainty and emotion dysregulation with experiential
avoidance.
Discussion In line with the first hypothesis, the model comprising
intolerance of uncertainty and emotion dysregulation sig-
The current study explored the relationship between nificantly predicted individual levels of experiential
intolerance of uncertainty and emotion dysregulation as avoidance. Impressively, these variables accounted for
possible predictors of experiential avoidance. As expected, nearly two third of the variance in avoidance. The current
individuals’ tendency to high intolerance of uncertainty findings add to the body of evidence where both emotion
and poor emotion regulation both predicted experiential regulation difficulties and intolerance of uncertainty have
avoidance. This is the first study showing that non-clinical traditionally been independently implicated in the overuse
individuals’ avoidance is influenced by their ability to of experiential avoidance (Hayes et al., 2004; Lee et al.,

Fig. 1 Illustration of the


mediating effects of worry
(PSWQ) on the relationships
PSWQ
E = .596, CI[.726, .963]* E = .401, CI[.218, .354]*
between intolerance of
uncertainty (IUS) and
experiential avoidance (AAQ),
and between emotion
dysregulation (DERS) and E = .597, CI[.520, .689]*
experiential avoidance (AAQ); IUS
* p \ .001 k2 = .242, CI[.179, .308]*
AAQ
E = .738, CI[.269, .326]*
DERS k2 = .066, CI[.048, .086]*

E = .596, CI[.249, .348]* E = .574, CI[.167, .276]*


PSWQ

123
Psychol Stud

2010; Shihata et al., 2017). This is not surprising. Past emotion dysregulation within the relationship between
evidence has demonstrated that in presence of distress, worry and anger management (Besharat & Ramesh, 2017).
factors such as uncertainty, avoidance and how people Here, few possible explanations could be advanced. In
regulate their emotions are interrelated (Jacoby et al., unknown situations, worry has an avoidant function to con-
2016). Two possible explanations can be offered to explain trol distress, presenting itself as a fundamental element along
the above relationships. First, negative emotional responses with intolerance of uncertainty (Dugas et al., 1998; Stapinski
to a problem-solving situation may contribute to intoler- et al., 2010). Also, worry as a cognitive process comprises
ance of uncertainty by increasing its ambiguous elements uncontrollable chain threatening thoughts towards the
as threatening (Robichaud & Dugas, 2005). Second, unknown (Borkovec et al., 1983). In this situation, the indi-
intolerance of uncertainty may engender poor emotional vidual’s cognitive capability to regulate their emotions is
problem orientation (Dugas et al., 1998). affected, while limiting their ability to deal with the reduc-
As hypothesised, intolerance of uncertainty predicted tion of negative affect (Tice et al., 2001). The individual’s
experiential avoidance as this is considered as a personality increasing undesirable emotional feelings would lead to a
trait where future unknown negative events are inadmissible further attempt to suppress unpleasant intimate bodily sen-
and should be avoided (Vander Haegen & Etienne, 2016). sations, which would consequently involve the use of avoi-
High-intolerant individuals perceive ambiguity as threaten- dant maladaptive responses (Santanello & Gardner, 2006).
ing (Birrell et al., 2011), while reinforcing the toxic chain of Furthermore, from the current study’s results, it could be
chronic worry (Dugas et al., 1998). This potentially leads to assumed that worrisome issues may result from difficulties
unmanageable reactions (Yook et al., 2010), due to a poor engaging in goal-directed behaviours. In this instance, poor
perceived sense of control over the environment (Borkovec problem orientation may interfere with the individual’s
et al., 1983). Consequently, it can be asserted that in the case metacognitive and attitudinal processes in relation to their
of ambiguity, experiential avoidance might represent an own problem-solving strategies. The above supposition has
attempt to decrease the distress of emotionally processed been previously explored by Zlomke and Hahn (2010), who
threatening situations. Its function is to give the individual a investigated the role played by confidence in problem-
pleasant sense of control due the short-term relief (Hayes solving abilities in reducing levels of worry. Further research
et al., 2004; Schreiber et al., 2012). should consider the role of poor problem solving associated
This study also found a significant relationship between with emotion responses in relation to uncertainty. This would
emotion dysregulation and experiential avoidance, as help explore the potential negative consequences regarding
expected. This supports past research’s evidence about the the individual’s belief in their own ability to succeed.
long-term effects caused by the above relationship in Various limitations should be appreciated when con-
developing anxiety-related symptomatology (Campbell- sidering the implications of the current study. For instance,
Sills et al., 2015; Mohammadkhani et al., 2016). Further- self-report methodology brings some risk of bias such as
more, previous studies have demonstrated individuals’ social desirability. However, participants’ anonymity
innate tendencies to seek instant satisfaction when facing aimed at reducing this effect. Finally, the mixed use of
distress. Here, emotional avoidance is perceived as a online Qualtrics and face-to-face data collection may have
necessity to regulate emotions in the form of immediate amplified some variation, however the standard deviations
relief from aversive stimuli (Schreiber et al., 2012). The for both paper and online were very similar. Future
current findings explain how emotion dysregulation may research should address these limitations by using multi-
compromise individuals’ ability to cope when facing method designs. For instance, additional information on the
uncertainty. Individuals vulnerable to the emotional con- relationships among the current study’s constructs could be
sequences of not being able to predict specific events might obtained by comparing high/low intolerant individuals with
perceive these as potentially threatening and respond different levels of emotion regulation in experimental
inflexibly. Consequently, this would evoke ‘‘escaping’’ designs using anxiety-related objective measures.
maladaptive responses (Campbell-Sills et al., 2015). For
example, developing a drinking habit while dealing with a
situation of grief as a response of someone’s loss will Conclusions
prevent people from processing their emotions.
The findings showed that worry mediated both relation- The current study was innovative in investigating the
ships between intolerance of uncertainty and experiential processing in individuals potentially at risk for developing
avoidance and between emotion dysregulation and experi- excessive experiential avoidance by virtue of presenting
ential avoidance. Past research has highlighted both the high levels of intolerance of uncertainty and inability to
relevance of intolerance of uncertainty to the phenomenol- properly regulate emotion. Inflexible use of emotion reg-
ogy of worry (Birrell et al., 2011), and the mediating role of ulation and low tolerance of uncertainty make a significant

123
Psychol Stud

contribution to experiential avoidance in healthy individ- Bello, D. L., & Gumarao, M. (2016). Stress, coping strategies, and
uals. Individuals presenting intolerance to prospective academic performance of dentistry students. AUP Research
Journal, 2, 37–40.
uncertainty tend to exhibit goal-directed behaviour diffi- Besharat, M., & Ramesh, S. (2017). The relationship between worry
culties. Uncertainty stimulates avoidance, since suppress- and anger rumination with adjustment problems to heart disease:
ing unpredictable realities helps disengage from anxiety- the mediating role of difficulties in emotion regulation. Heart
related processing. Moreover, worry’s mediating role and Mind, 1, 141. https://doi.org/10.4103/hm.hm_7_18
Birrell, J., Meares, K., Wilkinson, A., & Freeston, M. (2011). Toward
affirms its relevance to theoretical perspectives of intoler- a definition of intolerance of uncertainty: a review of factor
ance of uncertainty and its relation to experiential avoid- analytical studies of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale.
ance. The current findings would benefit those who support Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 1198–1208.
healthy individuals showing avoidant behaviour to better https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.009
Boelen, P. A., Vrinssen, I., & van Tulder, F. (2010). Intolerance of
understand the particularity of this phenomenon. Thera- uncertainty in adolescents. The Journal of Nervous and Mental
peutic interventions (e.g. Cognitive Behavioural Therapy) Disease, 198, 194–200. https://doi.org/10.1097/nmd.0b0
specifically targeting both intolerance of uncertainty and 13e3181d143de
appropriate use of emotion regulation may help enhance Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole,
N., et al. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the
individuals’ self-awareness in relation to avoidant respon- acceptance and action questionnaire–II: a revised measure of
ses to prevent the potential creation of the cycle that psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior
maintains anxiety disorders. Therapy, 42, 676–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.20
11.03.007
Borkovec, T., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & Depree, J. A. (1983).
Preliminary exploration of worry: some characteristics and
Authors’ contributions All authors contributed to the study con-
processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 9–16.
ception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis
https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3
were performed by Kisley Di Giuseppe and Alisdair J. G. Taylor. The
Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. (2002). The intolerance of uncertainty scale:
first draft of the manuscript was written by Kisley Di Giuseppe and all
psychometric properties of the English version. Behaviour
authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All
Research and Therapy, 40, 931–945. https://doi.org/10.1016/s00
authors read and approved the final manuscript/
057967(01)00092-4
Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. J. (2012). Fear of emotions, experiential
Availability of data and material Data transparency. avoidance, and intolerance of uncertainty in worry and gener-
alized anxiety disorder. International Journal of Cognitive
Code availability Software application. Therapy, 5(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2012.5.1.1
Campbell-Sills, L., Ellard, K. K., & Barlow, D. H. (2015). Emotion
Declarations regulation in anxiety disorders. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of
emotion regulation. (2nd ed., pp. 203–218). The Guilford Press.
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of Carleton, R. N. (2016). Into the unknown: a review and synthesis of
interest. contemporary models involving uncertainty. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 39, 30–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.20
Ethics approval Approval was obtained from the ethics committee 16.02.007
of Kingston University. The procedures used in this study adhere to Carleton, R. N., Norton, M. P., & Asmundson, G. J. (2007). Fearing
the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki the unknown: a short version of the intolerance of uncertainty
and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. scale. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 105–117.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.014
Chapman, A. L., Dixon-Gordon, K. L., & Walters, K. N. (2011).
Experiential avoidance and emotion regulation in borderline
References personality disorder. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cogni-
tive-Behavior Therapy, 29, 35–52. https://doi.org/10.10
Aktar, E., Nikolić, M., & Bögels, S. M. (2017). Environmental 07/s10942-011-0124-6
transmission of generalized anxiety disorder from parents to Dugas, M. J., Gagnon, F., Ladouceur, R., & Freeston, M. H. (1998).
children: worries, experiential avoidance, and intolerance of Generalized anxiety disorder: a preliminary test of a conceptual
uncertainty. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 19, 137–146. model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 215–226.
Anderson, E. C., Carleton, R. N., Diefenbach, M., & Han, P. K. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(97)00070-3
(2019). The relationship between uncertainty and affect. Fron- Emerson, L.-M., Ogielda, C., & Rowse, G. (2019). The role of
tiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02504 experiential avoidance and parental control in the association
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator between parent and child anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology.
variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00262
strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality Flores, A., López, F. J., Vervliet, B., & Cobos, P. L. (2018).
and Social Psychology, 51, 1173. Intolerance of uncertainty as a vulnerability factor for excessive
Behar, E., Dimarco, I. D., Hekler, E. B., Mohlman, J., & Staples, A. and inflexible avoidance behavior. Behaviour Research and
M. (2009). Current theoretical models of generalized anxiety Therapy, 104, 34–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.02.008
disorder (GAD): conceptual review and treatment implications. Freeston, M. H., Rhéaume, J., Letarte, H., Dugas, M. J., & Ladoucer,
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1011–1023. https://doi.org/ R. (1994). Why do people worry? Personal and Individual
10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.006 Difference, 17, 791–802.

123
Psychol Stud

Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of Mushtaq, F., Bland, A. R., & Schaefer, A. (2011). Uncertainty and
emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–14.
structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00249
regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Pawluk, E. J., & Koerner, N. (2013). A preliminary investigation of
Assessment, 26, 41–54. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:joba.000 impulsivity in generalized anxiety disorder. Personality and
0007455.08539.94 Individual Differences, 54, 732–737. https://doi.org/10.10
Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation 16/j.paid.2012.11.027
in everyday life. In: D. K. Snyder, J. Simpson, & J. N. Hughes Robichaud, M., & Dugas, M. J. (2005). Negative problem orientation
(Eds.), Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to (Part II): construct validity and specificity to worry. Behaviour
dysfunction and health (pp. 13–35). Washington: American Research and Therapy, 43(3), 403–412. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11468-001 j.brat.2004.02.008
Grupe, D. W., & Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation Roemer, L., Salters, K., Raffa, S. D., & Orsillo, S. M. (2005). Fear
in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological and avoidance of internal experiences in GAD: preliminary tests
perspective. Natural Review Neuroscience, 14, 488–501. of a conceptual model. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29,
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3524 71–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-005-1650-2
Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and Salters-Pedneault, K., Roemer, L., Tull, M. T., Rucker, L., & Mennin,
conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. D. S. (2006). Evidence of broad deficits in emotion regulation
Guilford Publications. associated with chronic worry and generalized anxiety disorder.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 469–480.
et al. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: a preliminary https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9055-4
test of a working model. The Psychological Record, 54, Santanello, A. W., & Gardner, F. L. (2006). The role of experiential
553–578. avoidance in the relationship between maladaptive perfectionism
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & and worry. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 319–332.
Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9000-6
disorders: a functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and Schmalz, J. E., & Murrell, A. R. (2010). Measuring experiential
treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, avoidance in adults: the avoidance and fusion questionnaire.
1152–1168. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.64.6.1152 International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy,
Hillen, M. A., Gutheil, C. M., Strout, T. D., Smets, E. M., & Han, P. 6, 198–213. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100908
K. (2017). Tolerance of uncertainty: conceptual analysis, Schreiber, L. R., Grant, J. E., & Odlaug, B. L. (2012). Emotion
integrative model, and implications for healthcare. Social regulation and impulsivity in young adults. Journal of Psychi-
Science and Medicine, 180, 62–75. https://doi.org/10.10 atric Research, 46, 651–658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsy
16/j.socscimed.2017.03.024 chires.2012.02.005
Jacoby, R. J., Abramowitz, J. S., Reuman, L., & Blakey, S. M. (2016). Shi, R., Zhang, S., Zhang, Q., Fu, S., & Wang, Z. (2016). Experiential
Enhancing the ecological validity of the beads task as a avoidance mediates the association between emotion regulation
behavioral measure of intolerance of uncertainty. Journal of abilities and loneliness. PLoS ONE, 11, 1–11.
Anxiety Disorders, 41, 43–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ja https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168536
nxdis.2016.02.003 Shihata, S., Mcevoy, P. M., & Mullan, B. A. (2017). Pathways from
Kumpula, M. J., Orcutt, H. K., Bardeen, J. R., & Varkovitzky, R. L. uncertainty to anxiety: an evaluation of a hierarchical model of
(2011). Peritraumatic dissociation and experiential avoidance as trait and disorder-specific intolerance of uncertainty on anxiety
prospective predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Journal disorder symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 45, 72–79.
of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 617–627. https://doi.org/10.10 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.12.001
37/a0023927 Shorey, R. C., Gawrysiak, M. J., Elmquist, J., Brem, M., Anderson,
Lee, J. K., Orsillo, S. M., Roemer, L., & Allen, L. B. (2010). Distress S., & Stuart, G. L. (2017). Experiential avoidance, distress
and avoidance in generalized anxiety disorder: exploring the tolerance, and substance use cravings among adults in residential
relationships with intolerance of uncertainty and worry. Cogni- treatment for substance use disorders. Journal of Addictive
tive Behaviour Therapy, 39, 126–136. https://doi.org/10.1080/1 Diseases, 36(3), 151–157. https://doi.org/10.1080/10550
6506070902966918 887.2017.1302661
Lewis, M., & Naugle, A. (2017). Measuring experiential avoidance: Spinhoven, P., Hemert, A. M., & Penninx, B. W. (2017). Experiential
evidence toward multidimensional predictors of trauma seque- avoidance and bordering psychological constructs as predictors
lae. Behavioral Sciences, 7, 9. https://doi.org/10.339 of the onset, relapse and maintenance of anxiety disorders: one
0/bs7010009 or many? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(6), 867–880.
Mantler, J., Matheson, K., Anisman, H., & Matejicek, A. (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608017-9856-7
Coping with employment uncertainty: strategies of employed Stapinski, L. A., Abbott, M. J., & Rapee, R. M. (2010). Fear and
and unemployed workers. Journal of Occupational Health perceived uncontrollability of emotion: evaluating the unique
Psychology, 10, 200–209. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-899 contribution of emotion appraisal variables to prediction of
8.10.3.200 worry and generalised anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and
Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Therapy, 48, 1097–1104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.0
Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Question- 7.012
naire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495. Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional
https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6 distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you
Mohammadkhani, P., Abasi, I., Pourshahbaz, A., Mohammadi, A., & feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
Fatehi, M. (2016). The role of neuroticism and experiential 53–67. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.80.1.53
avoidance in predicting anxiety and depression symptoms: Vander Haegen, M., & Etienne, A. (2016). Cognitive processes across
mediating effect of emotion regulation. Iranian Journal of anxiety disorders related to intolerance of uncertainty: clinical
Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 10, 1–8. review. Cogent Psychology, 3, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/23
https://doi.org/10.17795/ijpbs-5047 311908.2016.1215773

123
Psychol Stud

Varese, F., Morrison, A. P., Beck, R., Heffernan, S., Law, H., & and generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,
Bentall, R. P. (2016). Experiential avoidance and appraisals of 24, 623–628. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.04.003
voices as predictors of voice-related distress. British Journal of Zlomke, K. R., & Hahn, K. S. (2010). Cognitive emotion regulation
Clinical Psychology, 55, 320–331. https://doi.org/10.1111/b strategies: gender differences and associations to worry. Per-
c.12102 sonality and Individual Differences, 48, 408–413.
Wells, A. (2005). The metacognitive model of GAD: assessment of https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.007
meta-worry and relationship with DSM-IV generalized anxiety
disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 107–121. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-005-1652-0 jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Yook, K., Kim, K., Suh, S. Y., & Lee, K. S. (2010). Intolerance of
uncertainty, worry, and rumination in major depressive disorder

123

You might also like