You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/350389395

How do employees appraise challenge and hindrance stressors? Uncovering


the double-edged effect of conscientiousness

Article  in  Journal of Occupational Health Psychology · March 2021


DOI: 10.1037/ocp0000275

CITATIONS READS

3 1,064

4 authors:

Jie Ma Cong Liu


Jinan University (Guangzhou, China) Hofstra University
20 PUBLICATIONS   93 CITATIONS    37 PUBLICATIONS   1,033 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Yisheng Peng Xiaohong Violet Xu


George Washington University University of Texas at San Antonio
27 PUBLICATIONS   284 CITATIONS    48 PUBLICATIONS   452 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Workplace Ostracism Project I View project

Understanding human behavior through attachment. View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jie Ma on 26 March 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Occupational Health Psychology
How Do Employees Appraise Challenge and Hindrance Stressors?
Uncovering the Double-Edged Effect of Conscientiousness
Jie Ma, Cong Liu, Yisheng Peng, and Xiaohong Xu
Online First Publication, March 25, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000275

CITATION
Ma, J., Liu, C., Peng, Y., & Xu, X. (2021, March 25). How Do Employees Appraise Challenge and Hindrance Stressors?
Uncovering the Double-Edged Effect of Conscientiousness. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. Advance online
publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000275
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology
© 2021 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 1076-8998 https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000275

How Do Employees Appraise Challenge and Hindrance Stressors?


Uncovering the Double-Edged Effect of Conscientiousness
Jie Ma1, Cong Liu2, Yisheng Peng3, and Xiaohong Xu4
1
School of Management, Jinan University
2
Department of Psychology, Hofstra University
3
Department of Organizational Sciences and Communication, George Washington University
4
Department of Psychology, Old Dominion University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The challenge–hindrance model deems primary appraisal the central mechanism underlying the effects
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of challenge and hindrance stressors on employee outcomes. However, the literature has reported
conflicting findings on the relationships between challenge/hindrance stressors and challenge/
hindrance appraisals. Drawing upon transactional theory (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), the current
study aims to address these conflicting findings by investigating the moderating effect of conscien-
tiousness on stressor–appraisal relationships. On this basis, we further demonstrate when challenge and
hindrance appraisals mediate the effects of challenge and hindrance stressors on work motivation
(i.e., work engagement) and job strain (i.e., job-related anxiety). We conducted two substudies to
examine the research model at the between-person level (Study 1) and the within-person level (Study 2).
The results of both studies were highly convergent. Challenge stressors were more positively related to
both challenge and hindrance appraisals for employees high in conscientiousness. Hindrance stressors
were also more positively related to hindrance appraisal for employees high in conscientiousness.
By exacerbating the stressor–appraisal relationships, conscientiousness was found to strengthen the
indirect relationship of challenge stressors with work engagement via challenge appraisal and the
indirect relationships of challenge and hindrance stressors with job-related anxiety via hindrance
appraisal. We conclude that conscientiousness functions as a double-edged sword in the process of
making primary appraisals.

Keywords: challenge and hindrance stressors, primary appraisals, conscientiousness, work engagement,
job strain

The challenge–hindrance model (CHM; Cavanaugh et al., 2000) Scholars have thus called for “moving away from the CHM in favor
distinguishes between challenge stressors (e.g., time pressure and of a more appraisal-based approach” (Mazzola & Disselhorst, 2019,
job complexity) and hindrance stressors (e.g., situational constraints p. 949), considering that challenge and hindrance stressors may
and role ambiguity). The CHM posits that although both types of differentially affect outcomes via primary appraisals, such as chal-
stressors increase job strain, challenge stressors can motivate em- lenge appraisal and hindrance appraisal (LePine et al., 2016; Liu &
ployees and promote work outcomes, whereas hindrance stressors Li, 2018; Mazzola & Disselhorst, 2019; O’Brien & Beehr, 2019).
are demotivating and detrimental to work outcomes (e.g., LePine The challenge–hindrance research has long acknowledged that
et al., 2005). However, the proposed differential effects of challenge primary appraisals serve as the conceptual ground that differentiates
and hindrance stressors on work outcomes (e.g., motivation, between challenge and hindrance stressors and as the central
engagement, and job performance) have found mixed support in mechanism linking stressors to outcomes (e.g., LePine et al.,
empirical research, including meta-analyses (Abbas & Raja, 2018; 2016; Liu & Li, 2018; Prem et al., 2017; Webster et al., 2011).
LePine et al., 2016; Liu & Li, 2018; Mazzola & Disselhorst, 2019). However, the relationships between challenge/hindrance stressors
and challenge/hindrance appraisals are inconsistently theorized,
and the associated empirical results are conflicting. Prior research
has proposed that challenge/hindrance stressors are both positively
related to challenge and hindrance appraisals (Nelson & Simmons,
Jie Ma https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0477-7717 2003; Prem et al., 2017; Webster et al., 2011). Whereas some
This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation studies reported a positive relationship between challenge stressors
of China (grant number: 72002090) and by the Project of Humanities and and challenge appraisal (e.g., LePine et al., 2016; Liu & Li, 2018;
Social Sciences, Chinese Ministry of Education (grant number: Searle & Auton, 2015), others reported a null relationship (Rosen
20XJC630005). et al., 2020). Additionally, past research found the challenge
An early version of this study on the mediating effect of appraisals was stressor–hindrance appraisal relationship to be positive (Prem
presented at the 79th Academy of Management Annual Meeting in Boston, et al., 2017), negative (Kim & Beehr, 2019), or null (LePine et al.,
Massachusetts, USA, 2019. 2016). Similarly, the hindrance stressor–challenge appraisal rela-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jie Ma, tionship was also found to be positive (Webster et al., 2011),
School of Management, Jinan University, Guangzhou, Guangdong, China, negative (LePine et al., 2016), or null (Searle & Auton, 2015).
510630. Email: yonasma028@gmail.com These conflicting findings make it unclear when employees appraise

1
2 MA, LIU, PENG, AND XU

challenge and hindrance stressors as challenging and hindering. This However, most psychological theories, including transactional the-
not only lowers the ability of the appraisal approach to elucidate the ory, propose within-person relationships (e.g., Curran & Bauer,
effects of those stressors, but also may calls into question the 2011; Voelkle et al., 2014; Wang & Maxwell, 2015). For instance,
theoretical validity of the distinction between challenge and hin- Curran and Bauer (2011) lamented, “Despite the fact that the majority
drance stressors. It is thus imperative to address and reconcile the of psychological theories posit within-person processes, the research
conflicting findings. conducted to empirically evaluate these theories often involves the
To this end, we examine the moderating effect of conscientious- collection and analysis of strictly between-person data” (p. 584).
ness on the relationships between challenge/hindrance stressors and Therefore, we contribute to the literature by examining the relation-
challenge/hindrance appraisals. The transactional theory holds that ships among stressors, appraisals, and outcomes at the within-person
how stressors are appraised depends on the characteristics of the and between-person levels and by providing robust testing of con-
stressors and the individual (e.g., personality). Personality, in par- scientiousness as a boundary condition for both the within- and
ticular, determines the extent to which a stressor is personally between-person variations in stressor–appraisal relationships using
significant to an individual, which is the key to an individual’s a rigorous methodology (e.g., a time-lagged design and a daily diary
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

primary appraisals (Kilby et al., 2018; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). design).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Challenge and hindrance stressors are distinguished based on their


differential implications for personal growth and work goals
(LePine et al., 2005). As a personality trait, conscientiousness Challenge Stressors, Hindrance Stressors, and Primary
orients an employee toward personal growth and work accomplish- Appraisals
ment, thereby increasing the significance and perceived value of Transactional theory (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) considers pri-
these outcomes (Costa et al., 1991; Mount & Barrick, 1995). It is mary appraisals as the central mechanism linking stressors to stress
thus theoretically justified to deem conscientiousness a meaningful outcomes. Primary appraisals refer to an individual’s evaluation of
boundary condition for primary appraisals of challenge and hin- the implications of a stressor for his/her valued goals and usually
drance stressors. In practical terms, conscientiousness is more involve challenge appraisal and hindrance appraisal (LePine et al.,
frequently included as a criterion in personnel selection than other 2016; Webster et al., 2011). Challenge appraisal is the anticipation
personality traits, such as emotional stability, neuroticism, or self- that the stressor will facilitate one’s valued goals, such as work
efficacy (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Thus, considering the preva- accomplishment or personal development, whereas hindrance ap-
lence of the use of conscientiousness in personnel selection, exam- praisal is the anticipation that the stressor will thwart these valued
ining the effect of conscientiousness on employee appraisals of job goals (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; LePine et al., 2016; Webster
stressors could more broadly impact the human resource practices et al., 2011). In other words, challenge appraisal is concerned with
than focusing on other personality traits that are less frequently future gains, whereas hindrance appraisal is concerned with future
included as criteria. In addition, we further examine whether, by losses (O’Brien & Beehr, 2019). Consequently, primary appraisals
affecting stressor–appraisal relationships, conscientiousness mod- shape an individual’s subsequent reactions to stressors (Lazarus &
erates the indirect effects of challenge and hindrance stressors on Folkman, 1984).
work motivation (i.e., work engagement) and job strain (i.e., job- Early research on the CHM followed the premise of primary
related anxiety) via challenge and hindrance appraisals. Finally, job appraisals and employed a priori-categorization approach in
stressors can vary at the between- and within-person levels (Prem making the distinction between challenge and hindrance stressors.
et al., 2017; Sonnentag & Ilies, 2011). To strengthen the validity of Specifically, this approach relies on subject matter experts’ judg-
our research model, we conducted two studies to test the hypotheses ments of the implications of a given stressor for personal gain and
at both the between- and within-person levels. work accomplishment (Cavanaugh et al., 2000; LePine et al.,
Our research contributes to the literature by making the first 2005). Accordingly, challenge stressors are job stressors judged
attempt to address the role of conscientiousness as a boundary to facilitate personal growth and work accomplishment, and
condition of the stressor–appraisal relationship. It thus helps hindrance stressors are those judged to constrain these outcomes
reconcile the conflicting findings regarding challenge/hindrance (Cavanaugh et al., 2000; LePine et al., 2005). The studies em-
stressor–challenge/hindrance appraisal relationships and furthers ploying this priori-categorization approach implied that challenge
our understanding of when primary appraisals mediate the effects stressors are uniformly appraised as challenging and hindrance
of challenge and hindrance stressors on employee outcomes. Addi- stressors are uniformly appraised as hindering; thus, these stres-
tionally, we respond to the call for further knowledge of individual sors are differentially related to work outcomes (e.g., motivation,
differences in primary appraisals of challenge and hindrance stres- attitudes, and job performance) (LePine et al., 2005; Podsakoff
sors (González-Morales & Neves, 2015; O’Brien & Beehr, 2019). et al., 2007). Although intuitive, those studies did not explicitly
On this basis, we address the concern that the CHM is incompatible assess employees’ own appraisals and failed to acknowledge that
with a large body of research demonstrating the power of individual a stressor may be appraised as both challenging and hindering
differences on stress processes (Griffin & Clarke, 2010). By revealing (Webster et al., 2011).
that challenge and hindrance appraisals are not inherent in challenge To address the limitations of the priori-categorization approach, a
and hindrance stressors, we further underscore the importance of growing number of studies have explicitly measured employees’
measuring the appraisals in the challenge–hindrance research, which primary appraisals and hypothesized that both challenge and
has long been debated in this literature (O’Brien & Beehr, 2019). hindrance stressors are related to challenge appraisal as well as
Finally, previous research has largely examined the relationships hindrance appraisal (e.g., Prem et al., 2017; Webster et al., 2011).
among stressors, appraisals, and outcomes at the between-person This theoretical position is consistent with the transactional theory,
level (Liu & Li, 2018; LePine et al., 2016; Webster et al., 2011). which suggests that challenge and hindrance appraisals are not
STRESSORS, APPRAISALS, AND CONSCIENTIOUSNESS 3

mutually exclusive (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Unfortunately, value the most at work and compatible with their desired goals.
research has generated conflicting findings regarding these relation- Given this, employees high in conscientiousness should be more
ships, reaching equivocal conclusions on how employees actually likely to appraise challenge stressors as challenging. In contrast,
appraise challenge and hindrance stressors (e.g., Kim & Beehr, 2019; employees low in conscientiousness care less about personal
LePine et al., 2016; Liu & Li, 2018; Prem et al., 2017; Rosen et al., growth or work success, and challenge stressors are less valuable
2020; Searle & Auton, 2015; Tuckey et al., 2015). and rewarding to them. They are thus less likely to make challenge
We assert that overlooking the impact of personality on primary appraisal.
appraisals of stressors is a key reason for these conflicting findings
(Kilby et al., 2018; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Penley & Tomaka, Hypothesis 1a: Conscientiousness moderates the relationship
2002). Personality predisposes an individual to certain goals, values, between challenge stressors and challenge appraisal such that
and preferences such that differences in personality can prompt the relationship is more positive for employees high in consci-
individuals to value different things and to attach different values to entiousness.
the same thing (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). According to transac-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tional theory, personality determines what is at stake to an individual In contrast, hindrance stressors, such as red tape and situational
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in a stressful situation and in turn affects whether and to what extent constraints, are devoid of opportunities for personal gains and
a stressor is appraised as promoting (i.e., challenge appraisal) or accomplishment (LePine et al., 2005). Coping with hindrance
thwarting (i.e., hindrance appraisal) something valuable to oneself stressors allows an employee to lessen interference in work tasks
(Kilby et al., 2018; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Likewise, research at best and does not foster growth and greater success (Dawson
indicates that personality represents an individual’s dispositional et al., 2016; LePine et al., 2005; Tuckey et al., 2015). Higher
cognitive schema for constructing the subjective meanings of a levels of hindrance stressors thus indicate fewer possibilities for
stressor (Kilby et al., 2018). Differences in personality may thus growth and success, leading to a negative relationship between
render a stressor more challenging or hindering to some individuals hindrance stressors and challenge appraisal (LePine et al., 2016;
than to others, leading to interindividual variations in stressor– Searle & Auton, 2015). Employees high in conscientiousness are
appraisal relationships (Mitchell et al., 2019). oriented toward greater success and work accomplishment, mak-
Challenge and hindrance stressors are distinguished based on ing them more sensitive and susceptible to reduced opportunities
their opposite potential with respect to personal growth, task
for gaining these outcomes. For this reason, we can expect a
completion, and work accomplishment (LePine et al., 2005). We
stronger negative relationship between hindrance stressors and
argue that the subjective value and significance of these outcomes—
challenge appraisal for employees high in conscientiousness. In
the content of primary appraisals—depends upon the level of an
contrast, employees low in conscientiousness attach less value to
employee’s conscientiousness. As a personality trait, conscientious-
achievement and growth and are less sensitive to reduced gain
ness involves the disposition to be achievement-oriented, depend-
potential. Given this, the relationship between hindrance stressors
able, organized, and self-disciplined (Barrick & Mount, 1991).
and challenge appraisal should be weaker for employees low in
These dispositions translate into achievement striving (e.g., chasing
conscientiousness.
growth and accomplishment) and dependability (e.g., focusing on
completing work tasks and fulfilling responsibilities) (Costa et al.,
Hypothesis 1b: Conscientiousness moderates the relationship
1991; Hough, 1992; Judge et al., 2002; Moon, 2001; Mount &
between hindrance stressors and challenge appraisal such that
Barrick, 1995). Compared to those low in conscientiousness, em-
the relationship is more negative for employees high in con-
ployees high in conscientiousness value personal growth, job
responsibility and work accomplishment to a greater extent scientiousness.
(Abbas & Raja, 2018; Lin et al., 2015). Therefore, employees
with different levels of conscientiousness should appraise challenge Both challenge and hindrance stressors are suggested to relate
and hindrance stressors differently. positively to hindrance appraisal (e.g., Prem et al., 2017; Searle &
Auton, 2015; Webster et al., 2011). Hindrance stressors primarily
bring employees’ goal pursuit to a standstill and hamper their
The Moderating Effect of Conscientiousness on the personal growth (LePine et al., 2016; Liu & Li, 2018; Webster
Stressor–Appraisal Relationships et al., 2011). Although challenge stressors have the potential to lead
Challenge stressors allow employees to tap into their potential to greater achievement and personal gains, they still pose a risk to
(Kim & Beehr, 2019). Addressing challenge stressors can improve these favorable outcomes because employees might not be able to
work competency and advance work accomplishment, serving successfully achieve the results (Rodell & Judge, 2009). Challenge
as opportunities to progress in one’s work. For this reason, chal- stressors also call for higher levels of effort expenditure and
lenge stressors are suggested to be positively related to challenge investment of personal resources, rendering employees less able
appraisal (LePine et al., 2016; Webster et al., 2011). The transac- to handle work demands in other important areas (Kim & Beehr,
tional theory suggests that an individual is more likely to make 2019; Prem et al., 2017). For example, high levels of time pressure
challenge appraisal when the stressor can promote something of might prompt employees to speed up work to meet a deadline at the
high value to the individual (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Employ- cost of work quality. Transactional theory indicates that an individ-
ees high in conscientiousness attach greater importance to personal ual is more likely to appraise a stressor as hindering when it thwarts
growth and work success than those low in conscientiousness something important and valuable to oneself (Lazarus & Folkman,
(Costa et al., 1991; Liu et al., 2013). For employees high in 1984). The hindering potential of challenge and hindrance stressors
conscientiousness, challenge stressors are beneficial to what they should be more salient and frustrating to employees high in
4 MA, LIU, PENG, AND XU

conscientiousness because it obstructs their path to achievement Hypothesis 2a: Challenge appraisal is positively related to work
and fulfilling job responsibilities (Lin et al., 2015; Liu et al., engagement.
2013). Research also suggests that employees high in conscien-
tiousness are more vulnerable to risks to or constraints on work On the other hand, hindrance appraisal should be positively
goals and are more sensitive to potential threats to task comple- related to job-related anxiety, which is a common psychological
tion (Boyce et al., 2016; Lin et al., 2015). Thus, employees high strain experienced at work (Nelson & Simmons, 2003). It repre-
in conscientiousness are more likely to make hindrance appraisal sents the degree to which an employee feels anxious, worried, and
of both challenge and hindrance stressors. In contrast, those low tense about work (Warr, 1990). It often stems from worry about
in conscientiousness emphasize growth and accomplishment to a possible failure or a sense of uncertainty about attaining work
lesser extent (Abbas & Raja, 2018; Lin et al., 2015; Liu et al., goals (Lazarus, 1991; Rodell & Judge, 2009; Webster et al.,
2013) and are apt to detach their attention from job stressors 2011). Hindrance appraisal indicates that the current situation
(Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007). The hindering aspect of challenge is going to block one’s work goals and personal development, and
and hindrance stressors thus becomes less salient and significant future resource losses are possible. Such anticipation can burden
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to them, which makes them less likely to make hindrance an employee psychologically and lead to job-related anxiety. The
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

appraisal. extant literature has documented the positive association between


hindrance appraisal and various types of job strain (e.g., anger,
Hypothesis 1c and 1d: Conscientiousness moderates the rela- emotional exhaustion, and physical symptoms) (Searle & Auton,
tionship between challenge stressors and hindrance appraisal 2015; Tuckey et al., 2015; Webster et al., 2011), including anxi-
(c) and the relationship between hindrance stressors and hin- ety (Mawritz et al., 2014). We thus propose the following
drance appraisal (d) such that the relationships are more positive hypothesis:
for employees high in conscientiousness.
Hypothesis 2b: Hindrance appraisal is positively related to job-
related anxiety.
Primary Appraisals and Stress Outcomes
The transactional theory holds that primary appraisals stimulate The Moderated Mediating Effects
subsequent psychological and behavioral reactions to stressors
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Recent studies have found distinct Considering the aforementioned arguments together, we sug-
effects of challenge and hindrance appraisals on stress outcomes. gest that challenge stressors can indirectly facilitate work engage-
Specifically, challenge appraisal was found to elicit positive stress ment by eliciting employees’ challenge appraisal, while
outcomes, including a sense of learning (Prem et al., 2017), hindrance stressors can indirectly undermine work engagement
intrinsic motivation (Parker et al., 2019), and problem-focused by reducing employees’ challenge appraisal. For this reason,
coping (Searle & Auton, 2015). Then, hindrance appraisal was challenge appraisal should act as the central mechanism of the
differential effects of challenge and hindrance stressors on work
found to elicit negative stress outcomes such as burnout (Li et al.,
engagement. Nevertheless, we argue that this mediating effect of
2020), anger (Searle & Auton, 2015), and fatigue (Tuckey et al.,
challenge appraisal is stronger or more evident for employees
2015). The findings are in line with the physiological underpin-
high in conscientiousness. Challenge stressors should be more
nings of challenge and hindrance appraisals: Challenge appraisal
positively related to challenge appraisal for employees high in
activates the sympathetic adrenal medullary axis, which serves as
conscientiousness. This stronger propensity should in turn make
the energy mobilization system, whereas hindrance appraisal
these employees more engaged. In contrast, hindrance stressors
activates the pituitary-adrenal cortical axis, which acts as the
should be more negatively related to challenge appraisal for
distress system (Turner et al., 2012). In short, challenge and
employees high in conscientiousness, which should make them
hindrance appraisals are responsible for distinct types of stress
less engaged.
outcomes.
We propose that challenge appraisal should be positively related Hypotheses 3a and 3b: Conscientiousness moderates the indi-
to work engagement, an affective-motivational state characterized rect relationships of challenge (a) and hindrance (b) with work
by vigor (i.e., feeling energetic), dedication (i.e., being enthusiastic engagement via challenge appraisal such that the relationships
and proud of one’s work), and absorption (i.e., being highly are stronger for employees high in conscientiousness.
engrossed in one’s work) (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2004). The transac-
tional theory maintains that challenge appraisal is indicative of a Both challenge and hindrance stressors can indirectly increase job-
positive stress transaction between an individual and a situation and related anxiety by eliciting hindrance appraisal. Hindrance appraisal
thus can heighten the individual’s morale and enthusiasm for coping thus functions as the central mechanism transmitting the effects of
(Lazarus, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Similarly, because both types of stressors on job-related anxiety. Employees high in
challenge appraisal reflects employees’ anticipation of future conscientiousness are more likely to make hindrance appraisal of
resource gains (e.g., work accomplishment) (Dawson et al., 2016), challenge and hindrance stressors. This stronger propensity should
it can prompt them to be engaged in their work for the sake of make such employees more vulnerable to job-related anxiety. Given
accelerating resource gains. Indeed, research has reported a positive this, we argue that conscientiousness should intensify the mediating
relationship of challenge appraisal with motivation to work, task effect of hindrance appraisal on the relationships of challenge and
persistence, vitality, and proactivity (Liu & Li, 2018; Ohly & Fritz, hindrance stressors with job-related anxiety. The theoretical model of
2010; Van den Broeck et al., 2010). the study is presented in Figure 1.
STRESSORS, APPRAISALS, AND CONSCIENTIOUSNESS 5

Hypotheses 3c and 3d: Conscientiousness moderates the indi- Measures


rect relationships of challenge (c) and hindrance stressors
Unless otherwise noted, respondents rated their level of agree-
(d) with job-related anxiety via hindrance appraisal such that
ment with each scale item based on a 5-point Likert scale from 1
the relationships are stronger for employees high in conscien-
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The original scales were in
tiousness.
English and were translated into Chinese using back-translation
methods by two graduate students who were fluent in both English
Study 1: Method and Chinese (Brislin, 1970).
Challenge and hindrance stressors were assessed using a modified
Participants and Procedure version of Cavanaugh et al.’s (2000) 11-item scale (Webster et al.,
We recruited the participants from a large commercial bank in 2010). Challenge stressors were assessed with six items (e.g.,
China. To reduce common method variance and to establish a “I have a considerable number of projects and assignments to
temporal order between the study variables, we used web-based accomplish”) (α = .80), and hindrance stressors were assessed
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

surveys to collect data in two phases, 4 weeks apart. Data on with five items (e.g., “It is not clear to me what is expected of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

challenge and hindrance stressors, challenge and hindrance apprai- me on the job”) (α = .78).
sals, conscientiousness, and basic demographic information were
collected at Time 1. Data on work engagement and job-related
Challenge and Hindrance Appraisals
anxiety were collected at Time 2. As suggested by Dawson et al.
(2016), the 4-week interval enables researchers to investigate the In the challenge–hindrance literature, there is no consensus on the
effects of job demands on job strains while reducing potential best way to measure primary appraisals. Some studies measure
confounds due to the variability in the work environment. Employ- employees’ challenge and hindrance appraisals of every single
ees received approximately $0.75 for completing the Time 1 survey stressor separately (e.g., Liu & Li, 2018; Webster et al., 2011),
and approximately $1.50 for completing the Time 2 survey. Those while others follow a global approach to measuring challenge and
who completed both surveys were eligible to win four gift cards hindrance appraisals of job stressors in general using a multi-item
($15–$75) in a lottery. The data collection was conducted in scale (e.g., LePine et al., 2016; Prem et al., 2017; Tuckey et al.,
compliance with the APA ethical guidelines and was approved 2015). We choose the latter approach in the present study. First, a
by the affiliated institute of the first author. job demand does not exist in isolation but within a constellation of
We emailed 450 employees at Time 1 and received responses multiple demands (Van Woerkom et al., 2016). Accordingly, an
from 322 employees (71%), 288 of whom completed the Time 2 employee’s appraisals of a stressor are not in isolation from other
survey (63%). In the final sample, 52% of respondents were male, stressors. For example, an employee’s appraisals of job complexity
86% had a bachelor’s degree and above, and the average age with a low level of time pressure can be different from his/her
and job tenure (in years) were 34.18 (SD = 9.45) and 6.01 appraisals of job complexity with a high level of time pressure.
(SD = 5.52), respectively. Regarding job positions, 30% of re- Thus, measuring employees’ challenge and hindrance appraisals of
spondents were bank clerks, 18.4% were customer managers, job demands in general seems to be more informative than measur-
11.2% were product/financial advisors, 9.9% were loan and ing their appraisals of each job demand separately. Second, asses-
mortgage specialists, 9.9% were accountants, 8.9% were opera- sing appraisals of every single stressor separately increases the
tional specialists, 6.9% were support/operational/HR personnel, length of a survey, especially when assessing challenge and hin-
3.6% were marketing staff, and 1.3% were senior managers/ drance stressors- a higher-order categorization of various job stres-
executives. sors. Long surveys tend to burden participants, posing a threat to

Figure 1
Theoretical Model
6 MA, LIU, PENG, AND XU

response quality. In this study, we used LePine et al.’s (2016) fit index (CFI) = .91, root mean square error of approximation
instrument to measure challenge (e.g., “In general, I feel that my job (RMSEA) = 0.05, standardized root mean square residual
promotes my personal accomplishment”) (α = .80) and hindrance (SRMR) = .06]. The model fit was better than that of the five-factor
(e.g., “In general, I feel that my job hinders my personal accom- model (a) [Δχ2 (11) = 836.45, p < .001] and that of the alternative
plishment”) (α = .81) appraisals with three items each. five-factor model (b) [Δχ2 (11) = 449.75, p < .001].

Conscientiousness
Testing the Hypotheses
Conscientiousness was assessed with the nine-item scale of the
Big Five Inventory (John & Srivastava, 1999) (e.g., “I am someone The means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations
who perseveres until the task is finished”) (α = .82). among the variables are presented in Table 1. To test the hypothe-
ses, we followed the path analytical procedures outlined by Preacher
et al. (2007) and estimated all path parameters simultaneously with
Work Engagement
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Mplus 7.2. The predictors (i.e., challenge and hindrance stressors,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Work engagement was assessed with the nine-item Utrecht Work conscientiousness) were grand-mean centered prior to analysis. Two
Engagement Scale (Schaufeli et al., 2006) (e.g., “I am immersed in unhypothesized cross-linked paths (i.e., the path between challenge
my work”) (α = .88). The scale assesses three factors of work appraisal and job-related anxiety and that between hindrance ap-
engagement, and a composite score based on dedication, vitality, praisal and work engagement) were examined and controlled. We
and absorption is used to indicate overall work engagement. estimated the indirect effect using the bootstrapping method.
Table 2 presents the results.
Job-Related Anxiety Consistent with our prediction, conscientiousness significantly
moderated the challenge stressor–challenge appraisal relationship
Job-related anxiety was assessed with the three-item job-related (b = .24, p = .01). We plot this moderating effect in Figure 2a.
anxiety scale by Warr (1990) (α = .83). Employees evaluated the Simple slope analysis revealed that challenge stressors were more
extent to which their job had made them feel anxious, tense and positively related to challenge appraisal for employees high in
worried in the past month on a 5-point scale from 1 (never) to 5 (all conscientiousness (+1SD) (b = .38, p < .001) than for those low
of the time). in conscientiousness (−1SD) (b = .12, p = .17). Hypothesis 1a was
supported. Contrary to our prediction, conscientiousness failed to
Study 1: Results and Discussion moderate the hindrance stressor–challenge appraisal relationship
(b = −.13, p = .09). Hypothesis 1b was therefore not supported.
Evidence for Discriminant Validity
We further found that conscientiousness significantly moderated
We performed a series of confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) the challenge stressor–hindrance appraisal relationship (b = .29,
to test the hypothesized seven-factor model. We compared the p = .03) such that the relationship was more positive for employees
model with two alternative models: A five-factor model (a) in high in conscientiousness (b = .31, p = .001) than for those low in
which the items for challenge stressors and hindrance stressors conscientiousness (b = .01, p = .90) (Figure 2b). Finally, conscien-
were combined into one factor and the items for challenge and tiousness significantly moderated the hindrance stressor–hindrance
hindrance appraisals were combined into another factor and appraisal relationship (b = .21, p = .04) such that the relationship
another five-factor model (b) in which the items for challenge was more positive for employees high in conscientiousness
stressors and challenge appraisal were combined into one factor (b = .57, p < .001) than for those low in conscientiousness
and the items for hindrance stressors and hindrance appraisals (b = .34, p < .001) (Figure 2c). Hypotheses 1c and 1d were there-
were combined in another factor. The proposed seven-factor fore supported.
model demonstrated an acceptable fit to the data [χ2 (641) = In accordance with our propositions, we found that chal-
1,043.78, p < .001, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) = .91, comparative lenge appraisal was positively related to work engagement

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and Correlations for Variables in Study 1

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Age 33.68 9.60 —


2. Job tenure 6.09 5.62 .57 —
3. Challenge stressors 3.69 .54 −.09 −.09 (.80)
4. Hindrance stressors 2.52 .69 −.10 −.14* .07 (.78)
5. Challenge appraisal 3.84 .53 −.05 −.07 .28** −.28** (.80)
6. Hindrance appraisal 2.24 .74 −.04 −.02 .16** .52** −.34** (.81)
7. Conscientiousness 4.05 .55 .29** .13* .13* −.53** .21** −.39** (.82)
8. Work engagement 4.02 .66 −.06 .04 .13* −.24** .36** −.16** .25** (.88)
9. Job-related anxiety 2.21 .92 −.17* −.03 .14* .44** −.16** .39** −.53** −.14** (.83)
Note. N = 288. M = mean, SD = standard deviation. Internal consistency reliabilities are in italic and appear on the diagonal within parentheses.
* p < .05. ** p < .01.
STRESSORS, APPRAISALS, AND CONSCIENTIOUSNESS 7

Table 2
The Results of Path Estimates, Indirect Effects and Conditional Indirect Effects in Study 1

Mediator Outcome
Challenge appraisal Hindrance appraisal Work engagement Job-related anxiety
Predictor Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE)

Challenge stressors .25*** (.05) .15* (.07) .09 (.07) .16 (.09)
Hindrance stressors −.24*** (.05) .46*** (.07) −.17** (.06) .40*** (.08)
Conscientiousness .04 (.07) −.24** (.09)
Challenge stressors × Conscientiousness .24* (.07) .29* (.13)
Hindrance stressors × Conscientiousness −.13 (.08) .21* (.10)
Challenge appraisal (mediator) .31*** (.07) −.05 (.10)
Hindrance appraisal (mediator) −.01 (.06) .25** (.08)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Indirect Effect Estimate Boostrap 95% CI


Challenge stressors → challenge appraisal → work engagement
Low conscientiousness (−1SD) .04 [−.01, .11]
Medium conscientiousness .08 [.02, .15]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) .12 [.04, .22]
Difference (high vs. low conscientiousness) .08 [.02, .20]
Challenge stressors → hindrance appraisal → job-related anxiety
Low conscientiousness (−1SD) .002 [−.05, .07]
Medium conscientiousness .04 [.01, .10]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) .08 [.03, .17]
Difference (high vs. low conscientiousness) .08 [.01, .20]
Hindrance stressors → challenge appraisal → work engagement
Low conscientiousness (−1SD) −.05 [−.12, −.01]
Medium conscientiousness −.07 [−.14, −.03]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) −.09 [−.18, −.03]
Difference (high vs. low conscientiousness) −.04 [−.12, .001]
Hindrance stressors → hindrance appraisal → job-related anxiety
Low conscientiousness (−1SD) .08 [.03, .13]
Medium conscientiousness .11 [.03, .21]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) .14 [.04, .20]
Difference (high vs. low conscientiousness) .06 [.03, .17]
Note. N = 288. Bootstrapping sample = 10,000.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

(b = .31, p < .001), and hindrance appraisal was positively related conscientiousness (IE = .14, CI95% [.04, .20]) than for employees
to job-related anxiety (b = .25, p = .002). Hypotheses 2a and 2b low in conscientiousness (IE = .08, CI95% [.03, .13]). We thus
were thus supported. The conditional indirect effects are summa- found support for Hypotheses 3c and 3d.
rized in Table 2. The results showed that, with 10,000 bootstrapping We conducted Study 1 to examine the moderating effects of
replications, conscientiousness significantly moderated the indirect conscientiousness at the between-person level. The stress process
relationship between challenge stressors and work engagement via can also manifest as a within-person phenomenon that varies
challenge appraisal such that the indirect relationship was stronger from day to day for each employee, and these daily fluctuations
for employees high in conscientiousness (indirect effect [IE] = .12, carry critical information about employees’ stress experiences
CI95% [.04, .22]) than for employees low in conscientiousness that cannot be obtained from between-person-level investiga-
(IE = .04, CI95% [−.01, .11]). Hypothesis 3a was therefore tions (e.g., Prem et al., 2017; Sonnentag & Ilies, 2011). Addi-
supported. However, conscientiousness failed to moderate the tionally, a cognitive appraisal is a transient response that is
indirect relationship between hindrance stressors and work engage- optimally measured in real-time. A more frequent and real-
ment via challenge appraisal. Therefore, Hypothesis 3b was not time assessment of employee cognitive appraisal will strengthen
supported. the validity of our theoretical model. In view of these limitations,
Additionally, conscientiousness significantly moderated the we used a daily diary design over five consecutive workdays in
indirect relationship between challenge stressors and job-related Study 2.
anxiety via hindrance appraisal such that the indirect relationship
was stronger for employees high in conscientiousness (IE = .08, Study 2: Method
CI95% [.03, .17]) than for employees low in conscientiousness
Participants and Procedure
(IE = .002, CI95% [−.05, .07]). Similarly, conscientiousness sig-
nificantly moderated the indirect relationship between hindrance We recruited 145 employees from an architectural design con-
stressors and job-related anxiety via hindrance appraisal such that sulting firm located in northern China. With the help and approval of
the indirect relationship was stronger for employees high in the organization, we sent a study invitation letter, a statement
8 MA, LIU, PENG, AND XU

Figure 2 describing the study purpose and the data confidentiality policy,
(a) The Moderating Effect of Conscientiousness on the Challenge survey instructions, and a web link for a one-time general survey of
Stressor–Challenge Appraisal Relationship (Study 1), (b) The Moder- employees. A total of 143 employees completed the general survey,
ating Effect of Conscientiousness on the Challenge Stressor–Hindrance which collected their basic demographic information and assessed
Appraisal Relationship (Study 1), and (c) The Moderating Effect of their conscientiousness. A week later, employees were asked to fill
Conscientiousness on the Hindrance Stressor–Hindrance Appraisal out two surveys per day for 5 consecutive days (i.e., Monday to
Relationship (Study 1) Friday). To establish temporal precedence from daily stressors/
appraisals to daily outcomes, we sent the first survey, which
(a) assessed daily stressors and appraisals, at 12:00 p.m. and the second
survey, which assessed daily work engagement and job-related
anxiety, at 5:30 p.m. Employees were instructed to complete the
survey within 3 hr after each survey was sent. Although each survey
was available all day, we retained the data only from surveys
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

completed during the instructed time.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

We used a personal identification code to link employees’ first


and second daily surveys. To ensure a sufficient amount of within-
person variance, we retained only the data from employees who
completed surveys for more than 2 days. Ultimately, we obtained
597 matched daily surveys from 132 employees. The data collection
was conducted following the APA ethical guidelines and was
approved by the affiliated institute of the first author. Among the
132 employees, 62% were architectural designers, 29% were archi-
tectural engineers, 10% were operational and support staff, 7% were
accounting and financial management personnel, and 2% were
senior executives. All respondents had a bachelor’s degree or above.
(b) The average age and job tenure (in years) were 33.35 (SD = 8.72)
and 10.45 (SD = 9.59), respectively.

Measures
Unless otherwise noted, respondents rated their level of agree-
ment with each scale item based on a 5-point Likert scale from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Similar back-translation
methods were followed to translate the original measures into
Chinese (Brislin, 1970).

Daily Challenge and Hindrance Stressors


Consistent with previous daily-level studies investigating chal-
lenge and hindrance stressors (Binnewies & Wörnlein, 2011; Prem
et al., 2017; Widmer et al., 2012), we assessed time pressure and
situational constraints as a daily challenge and hindrance stressors,
(c) respectively. Daily time pressure was assessed with the three-item
scale developed by Ohly and Fritz (2010) (e.g., “Today, I need to
work faster than usual to get my work done”) (average α = .88).
Daily situational constraints were assessed with three items from the
situational constraint scale by Semmer (1984) (e.g., “Today, I had to
work with materials and information that were incomplete and
outdated”) (average α = .83).

Daily Challenge and Hindrance Appraisals, Work


Engagement, Job-Related Anxiety, and Conscientiousness
Daily challenge and hindrance appraisals, work engagement, job-
related anxiety, and conscientiousness were assessed with the same
measures used in Study 1. We modified the measures of daily
variables to reflect daily experiences (e.g., “Today, I am immersed
in my work”). The average Cronbach’s alpha of each scale is
presented in Table 3.
STRESSORS, APPRAISALS, AND CONSCIENTIOUSNESS 9

appraisal was positively related to daily job-related anxiety


Analytical Strategy
(γ = .32, p < .001). Hypotheses 2a and 2b were thus supported.
The daily data were nested within each person. To justify the use of The conditional indirect effects at the daily level are summarized in
multilevel modeling, we calculated the Intraclass Correlation Coeffi- Table 4. Using the Monte Carlo approach, we found that conscien-
cient(1) and the percentage of within-person variance for each daily tiousness significantly moderated the indirect relationship between
variable. As Table 3 shows, the percentage of within-person variance daily challenge stressors and daily work engagement via daily
over the total variance ranged from 34% to 56% for each daily variable, challenge appraisal such that the indirect relationship was stronger
suggesting the within-person variance was sufficient for each daily for employees high in conscientiousness (IE = .12, CI95% [.07, .18])
variable. The use of multilevel modeling to test our hypotheses is thus than for employees low in conscientiousness (IE = .06, CI95% [.03,
warranted. We tested all the hypotheses simultaneously in a two-level .09]). Hypothesis 3a was therefore supported. However, conscien-
path model with Mplus 7.2. As in Study 1, the unhypothesized cross- tiousness failed to moderate the indirect relationship between daily
linked paths (i.e., the path between challenge appraisal and job-related hindrance stressors and daily work engagement via daily challenge
anxiety and that between hindrance appraisal and work engagement) appraisal. Thus, Hypothesis 3b was not supported.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

were controlled. We estimated the 95% confidence intervals of the Furthermore, conscientiousness significantly moderated the indi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

indirect and conditional indirect effects using the Monte Carlo approach rect relationship between daily challenge stressors and daily job-
with the RMediation package. related anxiety via daily hindrance appraisal such that the indirect
relationship was stronger for employees high in conscientiousness
(IE = .07, CI95% [.02, .13]) than for employees low in conscien-
Study 2: Results and Discussion
tiousness (IE = −.01, CI95% [−.05, .03]). Finally, conscientiousness
By adding two cross-level interaction terms to the model, we significantly moderated the indirect relationship between daily
found that conscientiousness significantly moderated the daily hindrance stressors and daily job-related anxiety via daily hindrance
challenge stressor–daily challenge appraisal relationship (γ = .25, appraisal such that the indirect relationship was stronger for employ-
p = .02) such that the relationship was more positive for employees ees high in conscientiousness (IE = .07, CI95% [.03, .13]) than for
high in conscientiousness (γ = .42, p < .001) than for those low in employees low in conscientiousness (IE = .02, CI95% [−.02, .06]).
conscientiousness (γ = .21, p < .001) (Figure 3a). However, we Hypotheses 3c and 3d were therefore fully supported at the within-
failed to find a significant moderating effect of conscientiousness on person level.
the daily hindrance stressor–daily challenge appraisal relationship By using the experience sampling method (ESM) in a daily diary
(γ = −.18, p = .06). Furthermore, conscientiousness significantly design, we replicated the findings of Study 1 at the daily level and
moderated the daily challenge stressor–daily hindrance appraisal examined how conscientiousness affects employees’ daily stress
relationship (γ = .28, p = .01) such that the relationship was more processes. The between-level (Study 1) and within-level (Study 2)
positive for employees high in conscientiousness (γ = .22, analyses were highly convergent, which provides a solid support for
p < .001) than for those low in conscientiousness (γ = −.02, the moderating effect of conscientiousness and the mediating effect
p = .92) (Figure 3b). Finally, conscientiousness significantly mod- of challenge and hindrance appraisals.
erated the daily hindrance stressor–daily hindrance appraisal rela-
tionship (γ = .21, p = .01) such that the relationship was more
General Discussion
positive for employees high in conscientiousness (γ = .23,
p < .001) than for those low in conscientiousness (γ = .05, We conducted two substudies to examine the moderating
p = .26) (Figure 3c). Together, these findings supported Hypothe- effects of conscientiousness on stressor–appraisal relationships.
ses 1a, 1b, and 1d, but not 1c. At both the between-person and within-person levels, conscien-
Consistent with the results of Study 1, the results of Study 2 tiousness strengthened the challenge stressor–challenge appraisal,
demonstrated that daily challenge appraisal was positively related to the challenge stressor–hindrance appraisal, and the hindrance
daily work engagement (γ = .28, p < .001) and that hindrance stressor–hindrance appraisal relationships. We also found that by

Table 3
Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and Correlations for Variables in Study 2

Variables M SD ICC 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Age 33.35 8.72 — —


2. Job tenure 10.45 9.59 — .97 —
3. Daily challenge stressors 3.56 .79 .44 −.10* −.10* (.88)
4. Daily hindrance stressors 2.96 .88 .51 .04 .01 .26** (.83)
5. Daily challenge appraisal 3.55 .67 .49 .09* −.08 .49** .01 (.83)
6. Daily hindrance appraisal 2.75 .70 .54 .03 .03 .21** .43** .18** (.75)
7. Conscientiousness 4.05 .43 — .13** .16** .21** −.09* .21** −.06 (.77)
8. Daily work engagement 3.73 .44 .66 .08* .10* .47** .01 .51** .01 .27** (.85)
9. Daily job-related anxiety 2.03 .99 .65 .06 .03 .19** .33** .04 .53** −.03 −.03 (.91)
Note. Nbetween = 132. Nwithin = 597. M = mean, SD = standard deviation. Average internal consistency reliabilities across five days are in italic and appear
on the diagonal within parentheses.
* p < .05. * p < .01.
10 MA, LIU, PENG, AND XU

Figure 3 exacerbating the aforementioned stressor–appraisal relationships,


(a) The Moderating Effect of Conscientiousness on the Daily Chal- conscientiousness strengthened the mediating effect of challenge
lenge Stressor–Daily Challenge Appraisal Relationship (Study 2), (b) appraisal on the relationship of challenge stressors with work
The Moderating Effect of Conscientiousness on the Daily Challenge engagement and the mediating effect of hindrance appraisal on
Stressor–Daily Hindrance Appraisal Relationship (Study 2), and (c) the relationships of challenge and hindrance stressors with job-
The Moderating Effect of Conscientiousness on the Daily Hindrance related anxiety.
Stressor–Daily Hindrance Appraisal Relationship (Study 2)

(a) Theoretical Implications


The present study found conscientiousness to be an important
boundary condition for primary appraisals of challenge and hin-
drance stressors. This helps reconcile the conflicting findings on
the relationships between challenge/hindrance stressors and chal-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

lenge/hindrance appraisals. It has been suggested that challenge


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

stressors elicit challenge appraisal (LePine et al., 2005). However,


this relationship has been inconsistent (e.g., Rosen et al., 2020;
Webster et al., 2011). We posit that the potential for personal
growth and work accomplishment does not necessarily lead to a
positive relationship with challenge appraisal because some em-
ployees may attach less value to these outcomes. Supporting our
proposition, the results show that at both levels, challenge stres-
sors were more positively related to challenge appraisal for
employees high in conscientiousness, as these employees are
more achievement striving. The relationship was weaker for
employees low in conscientiousness, who attach less value to
(b) growth and accomplishment. Conscientiousness is thus the key to
affecting the extent to which an employee appraises challenge
stressors as challenging.
Past research has argued that both challenge and hindrance
stressors have a hindering potential to thwart task completion
and work goals (e.g., Prem et al., 2017; Webster et al., 2011).
The present study sheds light on when challenge and hindrance
stressors are more positively related to hindrance appraisal. Consis-
tent with our prediction, at both the between- and within-person
levels, challenge stressors were more positively related to hindrance
appraisal for employees high in conscientiousness, as these employ-
ees care more about fulfilling obligations and responsibilities. For
employees low in conscientiousness, who care less about work
duties, challenge stressors were unrelated to hindrance appraisal.
Similarly, at both levels, hindrance stressors were more positively
related to hindrance appraisal for employees high in conscientious-
ness. We thus reconcile the conflicting findings on the challenge
stressor–hindrance appraisal relationship (Kim & Beehr, 2019;
(c) LePine et al., 2016; Webster et al., 2011) and provide further insight
into the personal contingencies for the hindrance stressor–hindrance
appraisal relationship (LePine et al., 2016; Liu & Li, 2018).
It is worth noting that conscientiousness had a negative main
effect on hindrance appraisal. That is, employees high in conscien-
tiousness had, on average, lower levels of hindrance appraisal than
those low in conscientiousness, especially for low to medium levels
of challenge and hindrance stressors.1 Nevertheless, in accordance
with our reasoning, conscientiousness exacerbated the challenge/
hindrance stressor–hindrance appraisal relationships. This finding is
consistent with our proposition that employees high in conscien-
tiousness are more vulnerable to the hindering aspect of challenge
and hindrance stressors, rendering their hindrance appraisal more
“responsive” to the level of those stressors. In contrast, although

1
We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing this out.
STRESSORS, APPRAISALS, AND CONSCIENTIOUSNESS 11

Table 4
The Results of Two-Level Path Estimates, Indirect Effects and Conditional Indirect Effects in Study 2

Mediator Outcome
Challenge appraisal Hindrance appraisal Work engagement Job-related anxiety
Predictor Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE)

Challenge stressors .31*** (.04) .10* (.04) .20*** (.04) .09 (.06)
Hindrance stressors −.10* (.04) .14*** (.04) −.03 (.03) .12** (.04)
Conscientiousness .28* (.14) −.10 (.11)
Challenge stressors × Conscientiousness .25* (.10) .28* (.10)
Hindrance stressors × Conscientiousness −.18 (.10) .21* (.08)
Challenge appraisal (mediator) .28*** (.05) −.09 (.06)
Hindrance appraisal (mediator) −.07 (.04) .32** (.07)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Indirect effect Estimate Bayesian 95% CI


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Challenge stressors → challenge appraisal → work engagement


Conditioned indirect effect at low conscientiousness (−1SD) .06 [.03, .09]
Average indirect effect .09 [.05, .13]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) .12 [.07, .18]
Difference .06 [.01, .12]
Challenge stressors → hindrance appraisal → job-related anxiety
Conditioned indirect effect at low conscientiousness (−1SD) −.01 [−.05, .03]
Average indirect effect .03 [.004, .07]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) .07 [.02, .13]
Difference .08 [.02, .16]
Hindrance stressors → challenge appraisal → work engagement
Conditioned indirect effect at low conscientiousness (−1SD) −.01 [−.04, .02]
Average indirect effect −.03 [−.06, −.01]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) −.05 [−.09, −.01]
Difference −.04 [−.09, .001]
Hindrance stressors → hindrance appraisal → job-related anxiety
Conditioned indirect effect at low conscientiousness (−1SD) .02 [−.02, .06]
Average indirect effect .04 [.02, .08]
High conscientiousness (+1SD) .07 [.03, .13]
Difference .06 [.01, .11]
Note. Nbetween = 132, Nwithin = 597. The 95% CIs were calculated by Monte Carlo approach with 20,000 repetitions.
* p < .05. ** p < .01.

employees low in conscientiousness have higher levels of hindrance By responding to the call for further knowledge on individual
appraisal in general, their hindrance appraisal is less responsive to the differences in primary appraisals of challenge and hindrance stressors
level of those stressors. The finding further highlights that primary (O’Brien & Beehr, 2019), we contribute to an enhanced understanding of
appraisals stem from the interaction between the characteristics of why it is important to measure the appraisals directly in the
stressors and the properties of individuals (Lazarus & Folkman, challenge–hindrance research. Some studies assumed that each type
1984). Thus, to clarify when challenge and hindrance stressors are of the stressors is appraised consistently as challenging versus hindering
linked to challenge and hindrance appraisals, it is of great importance by most employees (e.g., Cavanaugh et al., 2000; LePine et al., 2005).
to account for the impact of conscientiousness. They thus have categorized challenge and hindrance stressors a priori and
In contrast to our expectations, conscientiousness did not moderate overridden the necessity of assessing primary appraisals. Others, how-
the negative relationship between hindrance stressors and challenge ever, have underscored the importance of measuring primary appraisals
appraisal. A possible explanation might be that employees high in for the reason each stressor can be appraised as both challenging or
conscientiousness have a stronger sense of self-control in stressful hindering to varying degrees (e.g., Webster et al., 2011). Despite the
situations (Penley & Tomaka, 2002). According to Lazarus and different conclusions, both lines of research seem to view primary
Folkman (1984), in some cases, maintaining one’s composure in appraisal as inherent in stressors. We agree with the necessity of
stressful situations may stimulate a sense of challenge. Although we measuring primary appraisals, albeit from a different perspective. Our
argued that employees high in conscientiousness are less likely than findings indicate that primary appraisals are not inherent in the stressors
those low in conscientiousness to make challenge appraisal when faced but influenced by employee conscientiousness. As being individual
with hindrance stressors, this effect may be neutralized by the stronger specific, primary appraisals should be directly assessed in order to better
sense of self-control associated with higher levels of conscientiousness. understand employees’ psychological experiences and predict their
Another explanation might be that the absence of potential for personal subsequent reactions in response to challenge and hindrance stressors.
growth and work accomplishment is too obvious to allow conscien- In contrast to the position that challenge and hindrance appraisals
tiousness to make a difference. In this case, regardless of whether an have differential effects on a given outcome (Prem et al., 2017;
employee is high or low in conscientiousness, this employee is less Webster et al., 2011), we found that they indeed influenced distinct
likely to make challenge appraisal of hindrance stressors. outcomes at both levels. In this study, challenge appraisal mainly
12 MA, LIU, PENG, AND XU

affected motivation (i.e., work engagement), whereas hindrance Practical Implications


appraisal mainly affected job strain (i.e., job-related anxiety).
We agree with Mazzola and Disselhorst’s (2019) recommenda-
This finding aligns with Tuckey et al. (2015), who reported that
tion that we should be cautious about increasing challenge stressors
job dedication was predicted only by challenge appraisal and fatigue
to motivate employees. Given our findings, challenge stressors are
only by hindrance appraisal. The results lend additional support to
more challenging and motivating to employees high in conscien-
the proposition that challenge and hindrance appraisals are not
tiousness. Managers should also be aware that these highly consci-
dichotomous but, rather, influence separate stress processes (Prem
entious employees are more likely to make hindrance appraisal and
et al., 2017; Searle & Auton, 2015). Given this, studies should not
are vulnerable to job strain. Organizations have been treating
link challenge stressors to only challenge appraisal and hindrance
conscientiousness as an important predictor in personnel selection
stressors only to hindrance appraisal (i.e., the single-appraisal
and tend to hire job candidates scoring high on this personality trait
approach) (LePine et al., 2016; Liu & Li, 2018), because this approach
(Liu et al., 2013). The prevalence of the use of conscientiousness as
reveals only a part of the appraisal mechanism underlying the effects of
a selection criterion along with the growing level of challenge
a given stressor. To reach a stronger conclusion about how appraisals
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

stressors (e.g., workload, time pressure, and job complexity) in


operate to affect employee outcomes, we should link a stressor to both
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

modern organizations necessitates the implementation of stress


challenge and hindrance appraisals (i.e., the dual-appraisal approach).
management and health-promotion interventions in place, such as
As noted by Kilby et al. (2018), personality shapes an individual’s
relaxation exercises (Kaspereen, 2012), mindfulness-based stress
“predispositional stressor appraisal mechanism that may help research-
reduction programs (Shapiro et al., 2005), peer-support programs
ers to further explore interindividual differences in the stress response”
(Peterson et al., 2008), and workplace wellness programs (Tetrick &
(p. 98). Studying conscientiousness as the boundary condition not
Winslow, 2015). Evidence from different industries and occupations
only enriches the knowledge of individual differences in primary
shows that these interventions can mitigate job stress and facilitate
appraisals of challenge and hindrance stressors but also provides a
employee recovery from stress (for a review, see Tetrick &
clear account of when primary appraisals operate to intervene in the
Winslow, 2015). Regardless of the level of conscientiousness,
effects of challenge and hindrance stressors on employee outcomes. In
employees can benefit from these interventions.
the face of challenge stressors, employees high in conscientiousness
Organizations should also eliminate or at least reduce hindrance
are more likely to make challenge appraisal, which in turn makes them
stressors as much as possible. First, organizations should conduct a
more engaged in work than those low in conscientiousness. Challenge
comprehensive assessment to identify the occurrence of hindrance
appraisal is thus more likely to intervene in the positive effect of
stressors and the types and levels of these stressors that their
challenge stressors on work engagement for employees high in
employees experience. Then, meetings should be held to examine
conscientiousness. For these employees, challenge and hindrance
the causes of hindrance stressors, be they shortness of resources,
stressors are more positively related to hindrance appraisal, so these
lack of goal clarity, or interpersonal clashes. Finally, management
employees are more susceptible to job-related anxiety than their
teams should hold themselves accountable for developing and
counterparts low in conscientiousness. Given this, hindrance appraisal
implementing sound strategies to manage hindrance stressors.
is more likely to intervene in the positive effect of challenge and
For example, organizations could keep an eye out for unnecessary
hindrance stressors on job-related anxiety for employees high in
paperwork and approvals and then restructure the layers of man-
conscientiousness. We therefore contend that “moving away from
agement to reduce red tape. Role differentiation and clarification can
the CHM in favor of more appraisal-based approach” (Mazzola &
be useful to address role ambiguity or conflict.
Disselhorst, 2019, p. 949) should involve integrating employee per-
sonality (e.g., conscientiousness) into the appraisal processes. This
would allow for more accurate predictions regarding how employees
Limitations and Future Studies
react to challenge and hindrance stressors.
Finally, conscientiousness has been seen as a double-edged sword We acknowledge a few limitations of the current study. First, this
in the context of challenge and hindrance stressors because it study does not support a causal conclusion regarding the stressor–
exacerbates the positive effects of challenge stressors on job per- appraisal–outcome relationship. Future studies are encouraged to
formance (Lin et al., 2015) and the detrimental effects of challenge replicate our findings using an experimental design to identify
and hindrance stressors on well-being (Abbas & Raja, 2018; Lin causal effects. Additionally, we used the global approach to measure
et al., 2015). To explain why this is the case, past research has relied employees’ primary appraisals because this method can better
on the resource allocation framework and argued that conscientious- reduce survey constraints in field research. Nevertheless, it could
ness prompts employees to devote resources to better performance at be helpful to use other measurement approaches for assessing
the cost of well-being (Abbas & Raja, 2018; Lin et al., 2015). primary appraisals in lab studies to examine whether our findings
However, given the difficulty of measuring resource allocation, the remain consistent across measurement approaches and research
studies were unable to empirically test that mechanism. We dem- settings. Furthermore, because our participants were mainly knowl-
onstrate that conscientiousness exerts the double-edged effect on edge workers, the conclusions of our study may be sample-specific.
stressor–outcome relationships via primary appraisals. The conver- Although the results of the current study were consistent with
gent findings at both levels strengthen our conclusion. Nonetheless, theory and with empirical findings obtained from samples from
there may be other mechanisms responsible for the double-edged other industries (e.g., LePine et al., 2016; Searle & Auton, 2015),
effect of conscientiousness, such as stress coping (Zhang et al., it is helpful to replicate our study with more diversified samples
2019). We encourage future studies to delve further into this drawn from various industries, occupations, and organizations.
question to enrich our understanding of why conscientiousness is It would be interesting to investigate whether a given job
a double-edged sword in the context of job stressors. stressor (e.g., emotional demand) is a challenge in one occupation
STRESSORS, APPRAISALS, AND CONSCIENTIOUSNESS 13

(e.g., for emotional workers) but a hindrance in another (e.g., for Cavanaugh, M. A., Boswell, W. R., Roehling, M. V., & Boudreau, J. W.
knowledge workers). (2000). An empirical examination of self-reported work stress among US
We focused exclusively on conscientiousness as a personal factor managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 65–74. https://doi.org/10
affecting primary appraisals. However, we encourage future studies .1037/0021-9010.85.1.65
to consider the effects of organization-level variables on employees’ Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for
primary appraisal of challenge stressors (e.g., LePine et al., 2016). agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality
For example, organizational support for strength use (i.e., the extent Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 887–898. https://
to which an organization supports employees in utilizing their doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(91)90177-D
Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person and
personal strengths to do their best work) (Van Woerkom et al.,
between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual Review
2016) may affect stressor–appraisal relationships. Organizational
of Psychology, 62, 583–619. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych
support for strength use can serve as a powerful job resource that
.093008.100356
strengthens employees’ control over job stressors, which may
Dawson, K. M., O’Brien, K. E., & Beehr, T. A. (2016). The role of hindrance
increase challenge appraisal and the associated positive work out-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

stressors in the job demand–control–support model of occupational stress:


comes (e.g., work motivation) and reduce hindrance appraisal and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

A proposed theory revision. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 37,


the associated negative work outcomes (e.g., job strain). Addition- 397–415. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2049
ally, challenge stressors may seem less risky and devastating and González-Morales, M. G., & Neves, P. (2015). When stressors make you
more valuable and meaningful in organizations with a culture of work: Mechanisms linking challenge stressors to performance. Work and
“learning from” rather than assigning blame for failures (Madsen & Stress, 29, 213–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2015.1074628
Desai, 2010). The former culture emphasizes the positive side of Griffin, M. A., & Clarke, S. (2010). Stress and well-being at work. In S.
challenge stressors, prompting employees to appraise challenge Zedeck (Ed.), APA handbook of industrial & organizational psychology
stressors as more challenging and less hindering. It is also interesting (pp. 359–397). American Psychological Association.
to study whether this kind of organizational culture can mitigate the Hough, L. M. (1992). The ‘Big Five’ personality variables—construct confu-
sion: Description versus prediction. Human Performance, 5, 139–155.
downside of conscientiousness found in this study.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History,
Finally, we recommend that future studies explore other plausible measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John
mechanisms underlying the differential or uniform effects of chal- (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed.,
lenge and hindrance stressors on work outcomes. For instance, pp. 102–138). Guilford Press.
employees’ state regulatory focus (i.e., self-regulation strategies Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. W. (2002). Personality and
for achieving work goals) may mediate the effects of challenge leadership: A qualitative and quantitative review. Journal of Applied
and hindrance stressors (Zhang et al., 2019). It is likely that Psychology, 87, 765–780. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.765
challenge stressors trigger both promotion focus (i.e., regulating Kaspereen, D. (2012). Relaxation intervention for stress reduction among
oneself to approach accomplishment) and prevention focus (i.e., teachers and staff. International Journal of Stress Management, 19,
regulating oneself to avoid risks/failures), which lead to certain 238–250. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029195
Kilby, C. J., Sherman, K. A., & Wuthrich, V. (2018). Towards understanding
psychological and behavioral outcomes. Echoing this reasoning, in
interindividual differences in stressor appraisals: A systematic review.
their meta-analysis, Zhang et al. (2019) categorized coping beha- Personality and Individual Differences, 135, 92–100. https://doi.org/10
viors into promotion-focused coping and prevention-focused coping .1016/j.paid.2018.07.001
and concluded that these two coping strategies have differential Kim, M., & Beehr, T. A. (2019). Thriving on demand: Challenging work
impacts on employee performance and well-being. It would be results in employee flourishing through appraisals and resources. Interna-
interesting to integrate the perspectives of primary appraisal and tional Journal of Stress Management. Advance online publication. https://
self-regulation to elaborate the whole-chain effect of challenge and doi.org/10.1037/str0000135
hindrance stressors. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory
of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 819–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0003-066X.46.8.819
References Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping.
Springer.
Abbas, M., & Raja, U. (2018). Challenge-hindrance stressors and job LePine, J. A., Podsakoff, N. P., & Lepine, M. A. (2005). A meta-analytic test
outcomes: The moderating role of conscientiousness. Journal of Business of the challenge stressor–hindrance stressor framework: An explanation
and Psychology. Advance online publication. for inconsistent relationships among stressors and performance. Academy
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions of Management Journal, 48, 764–775. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2005
and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26. .18803921
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x LePine, M. A., Zhang, Y., Crawford, E. R., & Rich, B. L. (2016). Turning
Binnewies, C., & Wörnlein, S. C. (2011). What makes a creative day? A their pain to gain: Charismatic leader influence on follower stress appraisal
diary study on the interplay between affect, job stressors, and job control. and job performance. Academy of Management Journal, 59, 1036–1059.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 589–607. https://doi.org/10 https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2013.0778
.1002/job.731 Li, P., Taris, T. W., & Peeters, M. C. (2020). Challenge and hindrance
Boyce, C. J., Wood, A. M., & Ferguson, E. (2016). Individual differences in appraisals of job demands: One man’s meat, another man’s poison?
loss aversion: Conscientiousness predicts how life satisfaction responds to Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 33, 31–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/
losses versus gains in income. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10615806.2019.1673133
42, 471–484. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216634060 Lin, W., Ma, J., Wang, L., & Wang, M. (2015). A double-edged sword: The
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal moderating role of conscientiousness in the relationships between work
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185–216. https://doi.org/10.1177/ stressors, psychological strain, and job performance. Journal of Organi-
135910457000100301 zational Behavior, 36, 94–111. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1949
14 MA, LIU, PENG, AND XU

Liu, C., & Li, H. (2018). Stressors and stressor appraisals: The moderating Rodell, J. B., & Judge, T. A. (2009). Can “good” stressors spark “bad”
effect of task efficacy. Journal of Business and Psychology, 33, 141–154. behaviors? The mediating role of emotions in links of challenge and
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-016-9483-4 hindrance stressors with citizenship and counterproductive behaviors.
Liu, C., Liu, Y., Mills, M. J., & Fan, J. (2013). Job stressors, job perfor- Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 1438–1451. https://doi.org/10.1037/
mance, job dedication, and the moderating effect of conscientiousness: A a0016752
mixed-method approach. International Journal of Stress Management, 20, Rosen, C. C., Dimotakis, N., Cole, M. S., Taylor, S. G., Simon, L. S., Smith,
336–363. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034841 T. A., & Reina, C. S. (2020). When challenges hinder: An investigation of
Madsen, P. M., & Desai, V. (2010). Failing to learn? The effects of failure when and how challenge stressors impact employee outcomes. Journal of
and success on organizational learning in the global orbital launch vehicle Applied Psychology, 105, 1181–1206. Advance online publication. https://
industry. Academy of Management Journal, 53, 451–476. https://doi.org/ doi.org/10.1037/apl0000483
10.5465/amj.2010.51467631 Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and
Mawritz, M. B., Folger, R., & Latham, G. P. (2014). Supervisors’ exceed- their relationship with burnout and engagement: A multi-sample study.
ingly difficult goals and abusive supervision: The mediating effects of Journal of Organizational Behavior: The International Journal of Indus-
hindrance stress, anger, and anxiety. Journal of Organizational Behavior, trial. Occupational and Organizational Psychology and Behavior, 25,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

35, 358–372. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1879 293–315. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.248


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Mazzola, J. J., & Disselhorst, R. (2019). Should we be “challenging” Schaufeli, W. B., Bakker, A. B., & Salanova, M. (2006). The measurement of
employees?: A critical review and meta-analysis of the challenge-hin- work engagement with a short questionnaire: A cross-national study.
drance model of stress. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 40, 949–961. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, 701–716. https://
https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2412 doi.org/10.1177/0013164405282471
Mitchell, M. S., Greenbaum, R. L., Vogel, R. M., Mawritz, M. B., & Keating, Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection
D. J. (2019). Can you handle the pressure? The effect of performance methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of
pressure on stress appraisals, self-regulation, and behavior. Academy of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274.
Management Journal, 62, 531–552. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2016.0646 Searle, B. J., & Auton, J. C. (2015). The merits of measuring challenge and
Moon, H. (2001). The two faces of conscientiousness: Duty and achievement hindrance appraisals. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 28, 121–143. https://
striving in escalation of commitment dilemmas. Journal of Applied doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2014.931378
Psychology, 86, 533–540. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.535 Semmer, N. (1984). Streßbezogene Tätigkeitsanalyse [Stress-Oriented anal-
Mount, M. K., & Barrick, M. R. (1995). The Big Five personality dimensions: ysis task-analysis]. Beltz.
Implications for research and practice in human resources management. Shapiro, S. L., Astin, J. A., Bishop, S. R., & Cordova, M. (2005). Mindful-
Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, 13, 153–200. ness-based stress reduction for health care professionals: Results from a
Nelson, D. L., & Simmons, B. L. (2003). Health psychology and work stress: randomized trial. International Journal of Stress Management, 12,
A more positive approach. In J. C. Quick & L. E. Tetrick (Eds.), Handbook 164–176. https://doi.org/10.1037/1072-5245.12.2.164
of occupational health psychology (pp. 97–119). American Psychological Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The recovery experience questionnaire:
Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10474-005 Development and validation of a measure for assessing recuperation and
O’Brien, K. E., & Beehr, T. A. (2019). So far, so good: Up to now, the unwinding from work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12,
challenge–hindrance framework describes a practical and accurate dis- 204–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.12.3.204
tinction. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 40, 962–972. https:// Sonnentag, S., & Ilies, R. (2011). Intra-individual processes linking
doi.org/10.1002/job.2405 work and employee well-being: Introduction into the special issue. Journal
Ohly, S., & Fritz, C. (2010). Work characteristichallenge stressors, challenge of Organizational Behavior, 32, 521–525. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.757
appraisal, creativity, and proactive behavior: A multi-level study. Journal Tetrick, L. E., & Winslow, C. J. (2015). Workplace stress management
of Organizational Behavior, 31, 543–565. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.633 interventions and health promotion. Annual Review of Organizational
Parker, S. L., Bell, K., Gagné, M., Carey, K., & Hilpert, T. (2019). Collateral Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2, 583–603. https://doi.org/10
damage associated with performance-based pay: The role of stress ap- .1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111341
praisals. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 28, Tuckey, M. R., Searle, B., Boyd, C. M., Winefield, A. H., & Winefield, H. R.
691–707. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2019.1634549 (2015). Hindrances are not threats: Advancing the multidimensionality of
Penley, J. A., & Tomaka, J. (2002). Associations among the Big Five, work stress. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 20, 131–147.
emotional responses, and coping with acute stress. Personality and https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038280
Individual Differences, 32, 1215–1228. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191- Turner, M. J., Jones, M. V., Sheffield, D., & Cross, S. L. (2012). Cardio-
8869(01)00087-3 vascular indices of challenge and threat states predict competitive perfor-
Peterson, U., Bergström, G., Samuelsson, M., Åsberg, M., & Nygren, Å. mance. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 86, 48–57. https://
(2008). Reflecting peer-support groups in the prevention of stress and doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.004
burnout: Randomized controlled trial. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 63, Van den Broeck, A., De Cuyper, N., De Witte, H., & Vansteenkiste, M.
506–516. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04743.x (2010). Not all job demands are equal: Differentiating job hindrances and
Podsakoff, N. P., LePine, J. A., & LePine, M. A. (2007). Differential job challenges in the Job Demands–Resources model. European Journal
challenge stressor–hindrance stressor relationships with job attitudes, of Work and Organizational Psychology, 19, 735–759. https://doi.org/10
turnover intentions, turnover, and withdrawal behavior: A meta-analysis. .1080/13594320903223839
Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 438–454. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Van Woerkom, M., Bakker, A. B., & Nishii, L. H. (2016). Accumulative job
0021-9010.92.2.438 demands and support for strength use: Fine-Tuning the job demands-
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated resources model using conservation of resources theory. Journal of
mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behav- Applied Psychology, 101, 141–150. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000033
ioral Research, 42, 185–227. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273170701341316 Voelkle, M. C., Brose, A., Schmiedek, F., & Lindenberger, U. (2014).
Prem, R., Ohly, S., Kubicek, B., & Korunka, C. (2017). Thriving on Toward a unified framework for the study of between-person and
challenge stressors? Exploring time pressure and learning demands as within-person structures: Building a bridge between two research
antecedents of thriving at work. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 38, paradigms. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 49, 193–213. https://
108–123. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2115 doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.889593
STRESSORS, APPRAISALS, AND CONSCIENTIOUSNESS 15

Wang, L., & Maxwell, S. E. (2015). On disaggregating between-person and Widmer, P. S., Semmer, N. K., Kälin, W., Jacobshagen, N., & Meier,
within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel models. L. L. (2012). The ambivalence of challenge stressors: Time pressure
Psychological Methods, 20, 63–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000030 associated with both negative and positive well-being. Journal of
Warr, P. (1990). The measurement of well-being and other aspects of mental Vocational Behavior, 80, 422–433. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2011
health. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 193–210. https://doi.org/ .09.006
10.1111/j.2044-8325.1990.tb00521.x Zhang, Y., Zhang, Y., Ng, T. W., & Lam, S. S. (2019). Promotion-And
Webster, J. R., Beehr, T. A., & Christiansen, N. D. (2010). Toward a better prevention-focused coping: A meta-analytic examination of regulatory
understanding of the effects of hindrance and challenge stressors on work strategies in the work stress process. Journal of Applied Psychology, 104,
behavior. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76, 68–77. https://doi.org/10 1296–1323. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000404
.1016/j.jvb.2009.06.012
Webster, J. R., Beehr, T. A., & Love, K. (2011). Extending the challenge-
hindrance model of occupational stress: The role of appraisal. Journal of Received May 18, 2019
Vocational Behavior, 79, 505–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2011 Revision received October 25, 2020
.02.001 Accepted December 18, 2020 ▪
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

View publication stats

You might also like