You are on page 1of 11

Published for the British Institute of Learning Disabilities

Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 2012, 25, 531–541

Young Children’s Attitudes Toward Peers with


Intellectual Disabilities: Effect of the Type of
School
Maria Georgiadi*, Efrosini Kalyva†, Elias Kourkoutas* and Vlastaris Tsakiris‡
*University of Crete, Crete, Greece; †The International Faculty of the University of Sheffield, City College, Thessaloniki, Greece; ‡Special Needs
Educator, Thessaloniki, Greece

Accepted for publication 12 July 2012

Background This study explored typically developing children from non-inclusive settings. Girls and students
children’s attitudes towards peers with intellectual who expressed more positive social, emotional and
disabilities, with special reference to the type of school overall attitudes towards students with intellectual
they attended. disabilities chose more positive adjectives to describe a
Materials and Methods Two hundred and fifty-six Greek child with intellectual disabilities. It was also found
children aged 9–10 (135 in inclusive settings) completed that children from inclusive settings drew children with
a questionnaire and an adjective list by Gash (European intellectual disabilities as more similar to a child with
Journal of Special Needs Education 1993; 8, 106) and drew Down syndrome in comparison with children from
a child with intellectual disabilities, commenting also on non-inclusive settings.
their drawings. Conclusions Effective inclusive practices should be
Results Typically developing children expressed over- promoted to foster social acceptance of students with
all neutral attitudes towards peers with intellectual intellectual disabilities.
disabilities. Type of school differentiated their attitudes,
with children from inclusive settings being more Keywords: attitudes, inclusion, Intellectual disabilities,
positive towards peers with intellectual disabilities and peers
choosing less negative adjectives to describe them than

of diversity that no one will be left out from the very


Introduction
beginning’ (McLeskey & Waldron 2007, p. 166).
Inclusion is defined as access to mainstream settings, Peer acceptance constitutes an important determinant
where children with special educational needs are of successful inclusion (Lewis 1995), because social
educated together with their typically developing rejection may contribute to the development of various
classmates through an array of useful and appropriate emotional and behavioural problems (Hay et al. 2004).
activities (Blamires 1999). Despite the fact inclusion is Many primary schools students with intellectual
common practice nowadays in a variety of countries disabilities feel rejected by their typically developing
(Van Kraayenoord 2003), it seems that only a small peers and are rarely chosen as friends (Roberts &
percentage of students with intellectual disabilities are Zubrick 1992; Sale & Carey 1995). Siperstein et al.
fully included in regular education classrooms (11% in (2007a, p. 134) concluded from their review of relevant
the USA in 2002–2003 according to Smith & O’Brien studies that ‘these findings are dramatic in that
2007). ‘An underlying assumption of successful inclusive children with intellectual disabilities were found to be
programs is that all children will be included in the equally significantly more often rejected than their non-
learning and social communities of the school and disabled peers in both integrated and segregated
that classrooms in these schools will be so accepting settings’.

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2012.00699.x


532 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

The social rejection of children with intellectual first inclusion law in Greece was implemented in 2001
disabilities can be partly attributed to their lack of basic (law 2817/2000, Ministry of Education 2001). It
interpersonal/social skills, to their inability to exhibit recognized the rights of all children with disabilities to
sophisticated psychosocial skills (Vaughn & Elbaum 1999; be educated in the least restrictive environments, and it
Pearl & Donahue 2004) or to their deficits in the cognitive was revised again in 2008 (law 3699/2008, Ministry of
component of the social domain of adaptive behaviour Education 2008). The legislation 102357/G6 (2002)
that prevents them from making a judgment when provided detailed information about the implementation
confronted with conflicting information in social challenges of inclusion practices, and it provided guidelines about
(Leffert et al. 2010). Another variable that could account for which school children with disabilities should attend.
the social rejection experienced by children with Children with mild disabilities (including children with
intellectual disabilities is the negative attitudes that intellectual disabilities) can be integrated in the
typically developing children tend to express towards mainstream school classroom (sometimes with the
them, with Siperstein et al. (1988) recording a strong parallel support of an educator) or can attend inclusion
connection between the negative attitudes of typically classes during the first teaching hours and then return
developing children towards children with intellectual to the mainstream school classroom. Children with
disabilities and the actual social rejection of these intellectual disabilities attend inclusion classes for no
children. In fact, typically developing children express more than 10 h per week (typically two teaching hours
more negative attitudes towards peers with intellectual per day) with other children with various disabilities,
disabilities than peers with other disabilities (Lewis while their main education takes place in the
1995; Kendall 2000; Georgiadi 2002). mainstream school classroom together with their
Positive peer attitudes towards children with typically developing peers. Inclusion classes provide
intellectual disabilities, on the other hand, can facilitate children with intellectual disabilities the opportunity to
the normalization process (Antonak & Livneh 1991; study some difficult units, such as language or maths,
Henry et al. 1996) and the integration in mainstream with the help of specialized instructors in small groups or
schools (Gash & Coffey 1995). The following factors are individually, but they do not substitute the mainstream
consistently found to affect typically developing classroom where these children belong. Children with
children’s attitudes towards their peers with intellectual moderate or severe disabilities attend independent
disabilities: age – with older children expressing more special schools – there are some separate special
negative attitudes (Townsend et al. 1993; Tang et al. 2000); schools that accommodate a specific type of disability,
gender – with girls being more favourable (Gash 1993; but not in all prefectures (Agorastou et al. 2009). In
Antonak & Harth 1994); attitudes of parents and teachers the academic year 2005–2006, there were 11 094
towards individuals with intellectual disabilities children with disabilities in primary education (1.9%
(Gollnick & Chinn 2002; McDougall et al. 2004); and of the total primary schoolchildren) – almost one-fifth
contact with children with intellectual disabilities – (21.7%) attended normal or inclusion primary classes,
children in contact with a child with intellectual while the rest (78.3%) were in special primary schools.
disabilities express more positive attitudes, but have The total number of students with intellectual
more difficulty engaging in a personal interaction with disabilities was 2389 (19.1% of all children with
the specific child (Gash 1993; Manetti et al. 2001), the disabilities) in pre-school and primary education
presence of labels and the behavioural characteristics (Ministry of Education 2008).
of the child with intellectual disabilities (Siperstein The limited evidence suggests that in Greece, people
et al. 2007a,b). with intellectual disabilities seem to face many social
The vast majority of relevant studies was conducted disapproval and numerous barriers to social participation.
mainly in North America, with some studies taking It has been suggested that many Greeks tend to believe
place amongst schoolchildren in Zambia (Nabuzoka & that the birth of an individual with intellectual
Ronning 1997), the United Kingdom (Furnham & Gibbs disabilities may be a divine sign that the parents did
1984), Italy (Manetti et al. 2001) and China (Siperstein something bad (Room et al. 2001). Another study
et al. 2011). There are many differences in the inclusion conducted on attitudes towards disabilities in a
practices of these countries, while Downs & Williams multicultural society found that members of the Greek-
(1994) found that even students from Western countries Australian community were among the least receptive
differ in the nature of the attitudes that they express of people with disabilities, especially those with
towards their peers with intellectual disabilities. The intellectual disabilities (Westbrook et al. 1993). Given the

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 533

impact of culture in shaping views, attitudes and The majority of the participants, who were recruited
understanding of difference and disability (Vlachou from primary schools in the wider area of Crete in
1997; Tang et al. 2000), the present study aims to explore Greece, had previous experience with an individual
the attitudes of typically developing children towards with intellectual disabilities (64.5%). Crete is the biggest
peers with intellectual disabilities in Greece after the island of Greece with a population of approximately
implementation of the most recent inclusion law. The 600 000 (6% of the Greek population). There are 40
effect of type of school (inclusive or non-inclusive setting) primary schools with inclusion classes and 12 primary
on attitude formation towards peers with intellectual special schools in a total of 1523 primary schools in the
disabilities will be examined, as well as the effect of whole island. The researchers drew first a list of the 40
gender or previous experience with an individual with primary schools with inclusion classes in Crete
intellectual disabilities. (inclusive setting) and randomly selected one from each
Previous studies have used exclusively questionnaires of the four prefectures of the island. The next step was
or adjective lists to explore the attitudes of children to draw separate lists with the primary schools in each
towards their peers with intellectual disabilities. We prefecture that did not have inclusion classes or any
wanted to compliment the data that they derived from other form of special education (non-inclusive setting)
using these traditional means and to avoid imposing and to select one from each. Then, a class from the third
adult-centred meaning on children’s perceptions (Sapkota and the fourth grade was randomly picked from each
& Sharma 1996). So, we asked the typically developing school, and the students were given informed consent
children to draw a child with intellectual disabilities and for their parents to fill out, to authorize their
to write something about their drawings, to avoid the participation. Of the 300 children who were initially
difficult situation of asking them what their drawing was identified, the parents of 261 children consented to their
about (Punch 2002). The use of this ‘draw and write participation. The attrition was small (13%) and the
technique’ will be used to supplement and enrich the return rate satisfactory.
data collected through the questionnaire and the adjective
list.
Materials
The participants were asked to complete the Gash (1993)
Materials and methods
questionnaire on attitudes towards inclusion that
consists of three parts. It has been used with children
Participants
from other European countries to evaluate attitudes
The sample of the present study consisted of a total of 256 towards intellectual disabilities (Gash et al. 2000) and
typically developing children (135 boys and 152 girls) with children from Greece to evaluate attitudes towards
aged 9–10 years old attending third (56.3%) and fourth physical disabilities (Nikolaraizi & DeReybekiel 2001;
(43.8%) grade. Approximately half of these children Kalyva & Agaliotis 2009). The first part of the
(52.7%) were in inclusive settings, while the remain- questionnaire started with the hypothetical scenario that
ing 47.3% were in non-inclusive settings. Typically a new student with intellectual disabilities would come
developing children in inclusive settings attended to their class: ‘I would like you to pretend that a new
mainstream schools where an inclusion classroom was child (it could be a boy or a girl) came to your class. He
operating, but they were not in the same class with a or she has mental retardation (MR). Here are some
student with intellectual disabilities. Children with questions for you to answer; there are no right or wrong
intellectual disabilities spent some time in the inclusion answers’. Consequently, based on this scenario, they
classrooms, but the majority of their day they attended completed the questionnaire that contained 16 questions
their mainstream classrooms. Children with intellectual relating to the sociability (10 items) and the schooling
disabilities participated in school and classroom activities (6 items) of children with MR. The schoolchildren had
together with their typically developing peers. Typically to state their agreement by choosing one option on a
developing children in non-inclusive settings attended 4-point Likert scale, where 1 = definitely yes and
mainstream schools where no inclusion classroom or any 4 = definitely no. In the second part of the questionnaire,
other form of special education or inclusion practice was they were asked to describe the new student with MR to
taking place and there was no diagnosed student with a friend choosing among a list of 34 adjectives that are
intellectual disabilities in their classroom or in their either positively (e.g. clever) or negatively (e.g. stupid)
school. rated according to Gash (1993). They rated each adjective

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


534 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

as 1 = not representative, 2 = quite representative and by two independent researchers specialized in


3 = very representative. Then, the participants had to interpreting children’s drawings. The categorization
state whether there were any children with MR in their of the drawings resulted in four themes (semantic
school and whether they knew someone who had MR. categories), which were formed after a systematic process
The first question helped the researchers to cross-check of coding the verbal and non-verbal data: those connected
that the participants were aware of the operation of the with the physical appearance (monsters, disfigurement,
inclusion classroom in their school. The second question Down syndrome), those showing problems in rela-
measured the experience/inexperience of the partici- tionships and in activities (relationships), the size of the
pants with individuals with MR. Although the term figure (small figures) and those showing unpleasant
intellectual disabilities has replaced the term MR faces (unhappy). Once the categories were drawn,
(Schalock et al. 2007), the researchers chose to use the each was rated on a dichotomous yes/no according to
term MR when addressing the typically the participants, whether it possessed a certain characteristic or not.
because this is the term most typically used in Greece. Inter-rater reliability was very high for all four
Finally, the ‘draw and write technique’, which is themes – between 0.95 and 0.96. Two representative
widely employed to explore children’s beliefs about drawings with accompanying comments are shown in
health (Pridmore & Bendelow 1995; Bradding & Figure 1A and B.
Horstman 1999), was also used in the present study.
Participants were given an A4 sheet of paper and a
Procedure
pencil and were instructed to draw a child with MR
and to write a comment about the drawing that they The researchers went into the schools and gave out the
made. The advantage of using drawings is not only that questionnaires and the paper and pencils. Then, they
they are fun and creative, but also that children have read out loud the instructions to the students and
the necessary time to think about the theme that they reminded them that they had the right to withdraw
want to portray. They also have the time that they need from the study without any penalty. They asked for
to make any changes and additions to produce the their oral assent before starting the study, and five
desirable drawing (Shaver et al. 1993). In this study, the children who did not want to participate were
researchers used drawings in an exploratory manner to excluded, leaving thus a sample of 256 students. The
discover how children portray children with intellectual order of administration of the measures was counter-
disabilities. The drawings of the children were analysed balanced. So, half of the children filled in the

(A) (B)

Figure 1 (A) Boy 9 years old, attends a non-inclusive school. He wrote: ‘This child would be very upset with himself. He would not
be very good with lessons, but I would not make fun of him. We will take care of this child and look after him’ and (B) Boy 10.5
years old, attends an inclusive school. He wrote: “Children with mental retardation, we must not consider them stupid, because we
all know that they have a knot in their brain. This child (in the drawing) tries to run away from school and to run away from the
children who hit him”.

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 535

questionnaire and then did the drawing, while the other Table 1 Standardized loadings for three-factor confirmatory
half drew first and then completed the questionnaire. model of attitudes towards peers with intellectual disabilities
The whole procedure lasted approximately 40–45 min for typically developing peers (n = 256)
and took place in the classroom and in the presence of
the researchers. Factor Factor Factor
Items 1 2 3

Results Factor 1: Social attitudes


Would you invite him/her to your 0.76
All the typically developing children who participated house to play in the evenings?
in this study expressed overall neutral attitudes towards Would you invite him/her to your 0.77
their peers with intellectual disabilities (M = 26.32, birthday party with your other
SD = 6.65). The scores are interpreted on a continuum friends?
with 14 representing extremely positive attitudes and 56 Would you make him/her your 0.74
representing extremely negative attitudes. Therefore, the best friend?
mean obtained in the study is close to the middle of this Would you ask him/her to sit 0.73
beside you?
continuum and represents neutral attitudes (Gash 1993;
Would you chat to him/her at 0.73
Gash et al. 2000; Kalyva & Agaliotis 2009). Moreover, all
break time?
the typically developing children attending inclusion
Do you think children with MR 0.60
settings (N = 135) were aware that there were children should be taught in the same
with intellectual disabilities in their school. classroom as you?
The factorability of the 16 items of the questionnaire Would you pick him/her on your 0.59
on attitudes towards the inclusion of students with team in a competition?
intellectual disabilities questionnaire was examined. Do children with MR prefer other 0.57
Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was children with MR as friends?
0.82, which is very good and Bartlett’s test of sphericity Factor 2: Educational attitudes
was significant (v278 = 765.97, P = 0.000). Finally, the Should children with MR have 0.74
their own special school where
communalities were all above 0.3, further confirming
all the children have MR?
that each item shared common variance with other
Should children with MR have 0.68
items. Given these overall indicators, factor analysis was
their own special classroom in
conducted with all 16 items. your school?
Principal component analysis was used because the Do you think that he/she would 0.62
primary purpose was to identify and compute composing have the same hobbies as the
characteristics for the factors underlying the attitudes other children in the class?
questionnaire. The initial eigenvalues showed that the Factor 3: Emotional attitudes
first factor explained 28.7% of the variance, the second Would you feel angry if he/she 0.71
factor 12.8% of the variance and the third factor 10.8% of did not keep the rules of your
the variance. Two and three-factor solutions were games at play time?
Would you care if other children 0.70
examined, using both varimax and oblimin rotations of
made fun of the child with MR?
the factor loading matrix. The three-factor solution was
Would you feel afraid of him/her 0.62
preferred, although it was not consistent with Gash’s
because they had MR?
(1993) subscales, because of the ‘levelling off’ of
eigenvalues and the clear theoretical distinction between
the three factors. During several steps, two items ‘Would loading matrix for this final solution is presented in
you smile at him/her on the first day?’ and ‘Later on, Table 1.
would you tell him/her secrets that you usually keep for Internal consistency for each of the scales was
your friends?’ were eliminated because they did not examined using Cronbach’s alpha, and the results are as
contribute to a simple factor structure and failed to meet a follows: Factor 1 – social attitudes towards students
minimum criterion of having a primary factor loading of with intellectual disabilities (8 items) = 0.83; Factor 2 –
0.4 or above. So, a principal components factor analysis educational attitudes towards students with intellectual
was conducted on the remaining 14 items, with the three disabilities (3 items) = 0.72; Factor 3 – emotional
factors explaining 52.3% of the variance. The factor attitudes towards students with intellectual disabilities

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


536 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

(3 items) = 0.68; and whole scale (14 items) = 0.78. No attitudes (F1,254 = 0.09, P = 0.766) towards students with
substantial changes in alpha for any of the scales could intellectual disabilities.
have been achieved by eliminating more items. The factorability of the 34 adjectives that typically
Composite scores were created for each of the three developing children could choose to describe students
factors, based on the mean of the items, which had their with intellectual disabilities questionnaire was examined.
loadings on each factor. Higher scores indicated less Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was
positive attitudes towards students with intellectual 0.86, which is very good and Bartlett’s test of sphericity
disabilities. was significant (v2351 = 2401.15, P < 0.001). Finally, the
Attitudes towards students with intellectual disabilities communalities were all above 0.3, further confirming that
were regressed on gender, type of school and previous each item shared common variance with other items.
experience with an individual with intellectual Given these overall indicators, factor analysis was
disabilities. These three predictors accounted for almost a conducted with all 34 items.
quarter of the variance in attitudes towards students with Principal component analysis was used because the
intellectual disabilities (R2 = 0.28). Type of school primary purpose was to identify and compute
(b = 0.18, P = 0.004) demonstrated significant effects on composing characteristics for the factors underlying the
attitudes towards students with intellectual disabilities, adjectives chosen to describe a child with intellectual
while gender and previous experience with an individual disabilities. The initial eigenvalues showed that the first
with intellectual disabilities did not have an effect on factor explained 24.4% of the variance and the second
attitudes towards students with intellectual disabilities factor 19% of the variance. Two, three and four factor
(b = 0.05, P = 0.447) and (b = 0.08, P = 0.210), respec- solutions were examined, using both varimax and
tively. MANOVA was carried out to determine differences in oblimin rotations of the factor loading matrix. The two-
the three factors that were considered important for the factor solution was preferred because of its theoretical
attitudes towards students with intellectual disabilities support, the ‘levelling off’ of eigenvalues and the clear
according to typically developing children from inclusive theoretical distinction between the two factors. During
and non-inclusive settings, as shown in Table 2. MANOVA several steps, seven adjectives were eliminated (spa,
showed that schoolchildren attending inclusive settings twit, different, shy, sloppy, sad, ashamed and shy)
expressed more positive social attitudes (F1,254 = 8.37, because they did not contribute to a simple factor
P = 0.004) and overall attitudes (F1,254 = 8.29, P = 0.004) structure and failed to meet a minimum criterion of
towards students with intellectual disabilities than having a primary factor loading of 0.4 or above. So, a
schoolchildren attending non-inclusive settings. There principal components factor analysis was conducted on
was no difference between schoolchildren attending the remaining 27 items, with the two factors explaining
inclusive and non-inclusive settings in their educational 43.4% of the variance. The factor loading matrix for this
attitudes (F1,254 = 2.05, P = 0.154) and in their emotional final solution is presented in Table 3.
Internal consistency for each of the scales was
Table 2 Means and standard deviations of attitudes of examined using Cronbach’s alpha, and the results are as
typically developing primary schoolchildren towards peers follows: Factor 1 – negative adjectives (17 items) = 0.77
with intellectual disabilities according to the type of school and Factor 2 – positive adjectives (10 items) = 0.68. No
substantial changes in alpha for any of the scales could
Non- have been achieved by eliminating more items.
Inclusive inclusive Total Composite scores were created for each of the two
Attitudes M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F1,254 factors, based on the mean of the items, which had their
loadings on each factor.
Social1 12.55 (4.67) 14.2 (4.48) 13.42 (4.69) 8.37*
The number of negative adjectives chosen to
Educational2 7.19 (2.49) 7.62 (2.33) 7.42 (2.42) 2.05
describe a child with intellectual disabilities was
Emotional2 5.45 (1.91) 5.53 (2.23) 5.49 (2.06) 0.09
Overall 25.26 (5.99) 27.27 (5.17) 26.32 (5.65) 8.29* regressed on gender, type of school, previous experience
attitudes2 with an individual with intellectual disabilities, social,
educational, emotional and overall attitudes towards
Lower scores indicate more positive attitudes. students with intellectual disabilities. These seven
*P < 0.005. predictors accounted for more than a third of the
1
Score range: 8–32. variance in the number of negative adjectives chosen to
2
Score range: 4–16. describe a child with intellectual disabilities (R2 = 0.41).

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 537

Table 3 Standardized loadings for two-factor confirmatory inclusive settings chose less negative adjectives (M =
model of adjectives chosen to describe children with intellectual 28.18, SD = 2.48) to describe students with intellectual
disabilities by typically developing peers (n = 256) disabilities (t1,254 = 5.94, P = 0.015) than schoolchil-
dren attending non-inclusive settings (M = 28.18,
Items Factor 1 Factor 2 SD = 2.48).
The number of positive adjectives chosen to describe
Factor 1: Negative adjectives
a child with intellectual disabilities was regressed on
Dumb 0.78
Untidy 0.77 gender, type of school, previous experience with an
Dirty 0.75 individual with intellectual disabilities, social,
Idiot 0.74 educational, emotional and overall attitudes towards
Thick 0.73 students with intellectual disabilities. These seven
Rough 0.71 predictors accounted for approximately one third of the
Stupid 0.70 variance in the number of positive adjectives chosen to
Crazy 0.68 describe a child with intellectual disabilities (R2 = 0.34).
Moron 0.62 Emotional attitudes (b = 0.36, P = 0.000), overall
Scary 0.59
attitudes (b = 0.28, P = 0.000) and social attitudes
Retarded 0.58
(b = 0.15, P = 0.026) towards students with intellectual
Bold 0.57
disabilities demonstrated significant effects on the
Simple 0.56
Freak 0.56 number of positive adjectives chosen to describe a child
Nerd 0.49 with intellectual disabilities, while gender, type of
Geek 0.48 school, previous experience with an individual with
Dork 0.47 intellectual disabilities and educational attitudes
Factor 2: Positive adjectives towards students with intellectual disabilities did not
Friendly 0.72 have an effect on the number of positive adjectives
Kind 0.68 chosen to describe a child with intellectual disabilities
Careful 0.67 (b = 0.03, P = 0.653), (b = 0.12, P = 0.052),
Special 0.63
(b = 0.09, P = 0.165) and (b = 0.05, P = 0.190),
Tender 0.57
respectively. More specifically children who expressed
Lovable 0.48
more positive social, emotional and overall attitudes
Neat 0.47
Clever 0.45 towards students with intellectual disabilities tended to
Happy 0.43 choose more positive adjectives to describe students
Nice 0.42 with intellectual disabilities.
Analysis with chi square showed that there were
differences in the drawings of the participants
according to the type of school that they attended, but
Gender (b = 0.17, P = 0.007) and type of school not according to their gender or previous experience
(b = 0.15, P = 0.020) demonstrated significant effects with an individual with intellectual disabilities
on the number of negative adjectives chosen to describe (Table 4). Schoolchildren attending inclusive settings
a child with intellectual disabilities, while previous drew a child with intellectual disabilities as less
experience with an individual with intellectual similar to a monster (v2 = 5.55, d.f. = 1, P = 0.018) and
disabilities, social, educational, emotional and overall as more similar to a child with Down syndrome
attitudes towards students with intellectual disabilities (v2 = 19.47, d.f. = 1, P < 0.001) than their peers
did not have an effect on the number of negative attending non-inclusive settings. Finally, analysis with
adjectives chosen to describe a child with intellectual chi square showed that there were no differences in
disabilities (b = 0.01, P = 0.965), (b = 0.03, P = 0.756), the comments of the drawings of the participants
(b = 0.02, P = 0.810), (b = 0.07, P = 0.386) and (b = 0.01, according to the type of school that they attended. It
P = 0.960), respectively. Post hoc analysis showed that should be noted that the comments that children
(i) boys chose more negative adjectives (M = 29.15, made about their drawings revolved around
SD = 2.12) to describe students with intellectual emotional, social and academic themes, while in the
disabilities (t1,254 = 3.03, P = 0.003) than girls comment of Drawing B, there is some evidence of
(M = 28.20, SD = 2.79); and (ii) schoolchildren attending bullying.

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


538 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

Table 4 Percentages of drawings and comments made for a disabilities have been generalized to other settings and
child with intellectual disabilities by typically developing actually practiced – that is, whether children would
primary schoolchildren according to their type of school actually interact with peers with intellectual disabilities in
and out of school and consider them as their friends
Type of school (Magiati et al. 2002).
The type of school (inclusive versus non-inclusive
Inclusive (%) Non-inclusive (%) v2
setting) was found to differentiate typically developing
Drawings children’s attitudes towards peers with intellectual
Monsters 13.5 25.2 5.55* disabilities. More specifically, it was found that children
Disfigurement 72.9 66.4 1.28 from inclusive settings tend to hold overall more
Small figures 27.1 32.8 0.98 positive attitudes towards their peers with intellectual
Down syndrome 36.8 12.6 19.47* disabilities than children from non-inclusive settings.
Unhappy 25.6 34.5 2.37 This finding is supported also by studies on a range of
Comments disabilities (Diamond & Carpenter 2000; Favazza et al.
Love 45.9 43.7 0.12
2000; Krajewski & Hyde 2000). It could be partly
Unhappy 11.3 17.6 2.11
explained by the fact that children from inclusive
Special school 26.3 19.3 1.73
settings in the current study were in the same school
Relationships 19.5 11.8 2.85
Negative 17.3 10.9 2.08 with peers with mild intellectual disabilities attending
Feel sorry 6.8 10.1 0.91 inclusion classes, who tend to receive more positive
Different 7.5 10.9 1.94 social feedback than children with more severe
intellectual disabilities (Calhoun & Elliott 1977; Casey
*P < 0.05. et al. 1988; Manetti et al. 2001).
Typically developing children attending inclusive
settings expressed more positive social attitudes
towards their peers with intellectual disabilities in
Discussion
comparison with children from non-inclusive settings,
The aim of the present study was to explore typically while no differences were observed in educational or
developing children’s attitudes towards their peers with emotional attitudes. This finding is really important
intellectual disabilities and to identify any differences given the documented social rejection experienced by
according to the type of school that they attended children with intellectual disabilities (Siperstein et al.
(inclusive versus non-inclusive setting), as well as their 2007a,b). Previous experience with an individual with
gender and previous experience with an individual with intellectual disabilities did not have any significant
intellectual disabilities. It was found that typically effect on the attitudes or the choice of adjectives as was
developing children expressed overall neutral attitudes the case in other studies (Gash 1993; Manetti et al. 2001).
towards their peers with intellectual disabilities. This This could be attributed to methodological limitations
finding is contradictory to this of Siperstein et al.’s (1988, resulting from measuring contact with just one question
2007a,b) who reported that typically developing children and might be resolved in future studies with the use of
tend to express negative attitudes towards their peers more elaborate measures of type or frequency of
with intellectual disabilities. It is also quite surprising previous experience (Avramidis & Kalyva 2006). Gender
given the reported negative general perception of did not have an effect on attitudes towards students
individuals with intellectual disabilities in Greece (Room with intellectual disabilities, but girls chose more
et al. 2001). It could be attributed, although, to the fact positive adjectives to describe a child with intellectual
that Greek teachers express similar attitudes towards disabilities and this is in line with studies suggesting
students with intellectual disabilities (Avramidis & that girls are more favourable (Gash 1993; Antonak &
Kalyva 2007) and teachers’ attitudes tend to influence Harth 1994).
their students’ (McDougall et al. 2004). It is also likely that Although it cannot be argued that the mere placement
as many of these children had some contact with an of children with and without disabilities in the same
individual with intellectual disabilities, they held less school can automatically foster positive social interactions
negative attitudes (Siperstein et al. 2007a,b). However, (Dore et al. 2002; Gash et al. 2004), it is possible to
there is no indication as to which extent the attitudes that decrease negative sociability that may be caused by
were expressed towards children with intellectual ignorance, fear of the unknown or feelings of insecurity

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 539

(Ward et al. 1994). Siperstein et al. (2007a,b) actually the particular inclusive settings may have influenced
reported that typically developing youths’ perceptions of the drawing of the children attending them (Siperstein
the abilities of children with intellectual disabilities et al. 2007a,b).
significantly influence their willingness to interact with these The comments that children made about their
peers. This speculation is further supported by another drawings revolved around emotional, social and
finding of this study – namely that children attending academic themes, matching closely the three categories
inclusive settings chose less negative adjectives to that derived from the factor analysis of the questionnaire
describe their peers with intellectual disabilities than that they answered. This finding is very interesting
children from non-inclusive settings. The stronger keeping in mind that half of the participants completed
evidence in favour of Siperstein et al.’s (2007a,b) claim is this task before actually filling in the questionnaire. It
that students who expressed positive, social, emotional could lend further support to the notion that attitudes
and overall attitudes towards students with intellectual towards peers with intellectual disabilities do not refer
disabilities were more likely to choose positive adjectives only to academic skills, but also to emotional and social
to describe students with intellectual disabilities. dimensions (Antonak & Harth 1994) and that the nature
However, the choice of positive adjectives is not of interactions between students with and without
necessarily translated into positive behaviour towards disabilities depends on several factors, such as the
children with intellectual disabilities in real life. In fact, instructional format, the proximity of general and special
the comment that was made by a child in inclusive educators, and curricular areas (Carter et al. 2008). No
setting and is presented in Drawing B indicates differences were detected in children’s comments
potential bullying behaviour against children with according to the type of school that they attended.
intellectual disabilities, and it draws attention to the It is important to remember that it is positive and
discrepancy between reported attitudes and actual not neutral attitudes that foster peer inclusion and
behaviours. It also stresses the need to implement acceptance, but positive attitudes are taught through
triangulation methods and more projective techniques to planned social contact between the students and
measure attitudes, especially with children who are appropriate teacher training and are not achieved by
more prone to offer socially desirable answers random placement (Zielger 2001). Moreover, it should
(Avramidis & Kalyva 2006). Manetti et al. (2001) who be noted that the extent to which contact is voluntary or
compared expressed attitudes to sociometric measures imposed might affect the formation of attitudes towards
found significant discrepancies between the two. To peers with intellectual disabilities, while an unpleasant
partly overcome this limitation, the researchers in this experience with a person with intellectual disabilities,
study asked the typically developing children to draw a especially in childhood, is strongly associated with
child with intellectual disabilities and to write a negative attitudes in adulthood (Tachibana 2005). These
comment about their drawing, but further studies are factors that could be further explored in future
should use more indirect measures of attitudes. studies and should be taken into consideration when
Children’s drawings were analysed in terms of the contemplating this study’s findings,
themes that arose (their size, facial expression and The present study explores the attitudes of typically
similarity to a monster, a disfigured person or a developing children towards their peers with intellectual
person with Down syndrome). It is important to note disabilities in a context where inclusion practices were
that these categories derived from the children’s recently introduced and enforced, while there is also a
drawings without any intervention or guidance from stigmatizing tendency towards individuals with
the researchers. It was found that children from intellectual disabilities. It was found that typically
inclusive settings depicted the child with intellectual developing children express overall neutral attitudes
disabilities as more similar to a child with Down towards their peers with intellectual disabilities, with
syndrome and as less likely to a monster than children from inclusive settings being more accepting
children from non-inclusive settings. Children in than children from non-inclusive settings. Girls chose
inclusive settings in this study were as previously more positive adjectives to describe students with
mentioned in contact with peers with mild intellectual intellectual disabilities than boys, and students who
disabilities, and this could have made their portrayal expressed positive, social, emotional and overall attitudes
less frightening. Moreover, the use of the label ‘child towards students with intellectual disabilities were more
with Down syndrome’ used by teaching staff to likely to choose positive adjectives to describe students
describe actual students with intellectual disabilities in with intellectual disabilities.

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


540 Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities

Correspondence European comparison. Adaptive Physical Activity Quarterly 11,


32–43.
Any correspondence should be directed to Efrosini Favazza P. C., Phillipsen L. & Kumar P. (2000) Measuring and
Kalyva, City College, 24 Proxenou Koromila Street, 546 22, promoting acceptance of young children with disabilities.
Thessaloniki, Greece (e-mail: kalyva@city.academic.gr). Exceptional Children 66, 491–508.
Furnham A. & Gibbs M. (1984) School children’s attitudes
towards the handicapped. Journal of Adolescence 7, 99–117.
References
Gash H. (1993) A constructivist attempt to change attitudes
Agorastou M., Kalyva E., Kaderoglou E. & Stefanidis F. towards children with special needs. European Journal of
(2009) Attitudes towards inclusion in primary education in Special Needs Education 8, 106–125.
Greece. In: Disabilities: Insights from Across Fields and Around Gash H. & Coffey D. (1995) Influences on attitudes towards
the World, Vol. 1. (eds C. A. Marshall, E. Kendall, M. children with mental handicap. European Journal of Special
Banks & R. M. S Gover), pp. 316–352. Praeger Press, Needs Education 10, 1–16.
Westport, CT. Gash H., Gonzales G., Pires M. & Rault C. (2000) Attitudes towards
Antonak R. F. & Harth R. (1994) Psychometric analysis and Down Syndrome: a national comparative study: france, Ireland,
revision of the Mental Retardation Attitude Inventory. Mental Portugal, and Spain. Irish Journal of Psychology 21, 203–214.
Retardation 32, 272–280. Gash H., Illán Romeu N. & López Pina N. (2004) A Qualitative
Antonak R. F. & Livneh H. (1991) Survey research on attitudes. In: Approach to Perception of Down Syndrome in Ireland and Spain.
Handbook of Mental Retardation, 2nd edn (eds J. L. Matson & J. A. Available at: http://www.riverbendds.org/index.htm?page=
Mulick), pp. 552–568. Pergamon Press, New York. perception.html (accessed on 27 January 2012).
Avramidis E. & Kalyva E. (2006) Methodi erevnas stin idiki agogi: Georgiadi M. (2002) Attitudes of primary school students (fifth
Theoria ke praksi [Research methods in special needs education: and sixth grade) towards children with chronic illnesses.
Theory and practice]. Papazisis, Athens. Education Sciences 3, 59–76.
Avramidis E. & Kalyva E. (2007) The influence of teaching Gollnick P. & Chinn P. (2002) Multicultural Education in a
experience and professional development on Greek teachers’ Pluralistic Society. Merril Prentice Hall, Columbus, OH.
attitudes towards inclusion. European Journal of Special Needs Hay D. F., Payne A. & Chadwick A. (2004) Peer relations in
Education 22, 367–389. childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 84–108.
Blamires M. (1999) Universal design for learning: Re-establishing Henry D., Keys C., Jopp D. & Balcazar F. (1996) The Community
differentiation as part of the inclusion agenda. Support for Living Attitudes Scale, Mental Retardation Form: development,
Learning 14, 158–163. and psychometric properties. Mental Retardation 34, 149–158.
Bradding A. & Horstman M. (1999) Using the draw and writer Kalyva E. & Agaliotis I. (2009) Can contact affect Greek
technique with children. European Journal on Oncology Nursing children’s understanding of and attitudes towards peers with
3, 170–175. physical disabilities? European Journal of Special Needs
Calhoun G. & Elliott R. N. (1977) Self-concept and academic Education 24, 213–220.
achievement of educable retarded and emotionally disturbed Kendall P. C. (2000) Child & Adolescent Therapy: Cognitive-
pupils. Exceptional Children 43, 279–380. Behavioural Procedures, 2nd edn. Guilford, New York.
Carter E. W., Sisco L. G., Brown L., Brickham D. & Al-Khabbaz Krajewski J. J. & Hyde M. S. (2000) Comparison of teen
Z. A. (2008) Peer interactions and academic engagement of attitudes toward individuals with mental retardation between
youth with developmental disabilities in inclusive middle 1987 and 1998: has inclusion made a difference? Education
and high school classrooms. American Journal on Mental and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental
Retardation 113, 479–494. Disabilities 35, 284–293.
Casey W., Jones D., Kugler B. & Watkins B. (1988) Leffert J. S., Siperstein G. N. & Widaman K. F. (2010) Social
Integration of Down’s Syndrome children in the primary perception in children with intellectual disabilities: the
school: a longitudinal study of cognitive development and interpretation of insincere benign intentions. Journal of
academic attainments. British Journal of Educational Psychology Intellectual Disability Research 54, 168–180.
38, 279–286. Lewis A. (1995) Children’s Understanding of Disabilities. David
Diamond K. & Carpenter C. (2000) The influence of inclusive Fulton, London.
preschool programs on children’s sensitivity to the needs of Magiati I., Dockrell J. E. & Logotheti A. E. (2002) Young
others. Journal of Early Intervention 23, 81–91. children’s understanding of disabilities: the influence of
Dore R., Dion E., Wagner S. & Brunet J.-P. (2002) Highschool development, context, and cognition. Journal of Applied
inclusion of adolescents with mental retardation: a multiple Developmental Psychology 23, 409–430.
case study. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Manetti M., Schneider B. H. & Siperstein G. (2001) Social
Developmental Disabilities 37, 253–261. acceptance of children with mental retardation: testing the
Downs P. & Williams T. (1994) Student attitudes toward contact hypothesis with an Italian sample. International
integration of people with disabilities in activity settings: a Journal of Behavioural Development 25, 279–286.

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541


Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 541

McDougall J., Dewit D. J., King G., Miller L T. & Killip S. Planning. ERG Technical Notes series No.2. Liverpool School
(2004) High school-aged youth’s attitudes toward their peers of Tropical Medicine, Liverpool.
with disabilities: the role of school and student interpersonal Siperstein G. N., Bak J. J. & O’Keefe P. (1988) Relationship
factors. International Journal of Disability, Development, and between children’s attitudes toward and their social
Education 51, 287–313. acceptance of mentally retarded peers. American Journal on
McLeskey J. & Waldron N. (2007) Making differences ordinary Mental Retardation 93, 24–27.
in inclusive classrooms. Intervention in School and Clinic 42, Siperstein G. N., Norins J. & Mohler A. (2007a) Social acceptance
162–168. and attitude change: Fifty years of research. In: Handbook of
Ministry of Education (2001) Special Needs Education in Greece. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (eds J. W. Jacobson & J.
Available at: http://www.ypepth.gr (accessed on 31 January A. Mulick), pp. 133–154. Kluewer/Plenum, New York.
2012). Siperstein G. N., Parker R. C., Bardon J. N. & Widaman K. F.
Ministry of Education (2008) Revised Law on Special Needs (2007b) A national study of youth attitudes toward the
Education in Greece. Available at: http://www.ypepth.gr inclusion of students with intellectual disabilities. Exceptional
(accessed on 31 January 2012). Children 73, 435–455.
Nabuzoka D. & Ronning J. A. (1997) Social acceptance of Siperstein G. N., Parker R. C., Norins J. & Widaman K. F.
children with intellectual disabilities in a school setting in (2011) A national study of Chinese youths’ attitudes towards
Zambia: a pilot study. International Journal of Disability, students with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual
Development, and Education 44, 105–115. Disability Research 5, 370–384.
Nikolaraizi M. & DeReybekiel N. (2001) A comparative study of Smith P. & O’Brien J. (2007) Have we made any progress?
children’s attitudes towards deaf children, children with Including students with intellectual disabilities in regular
wheelchairs and blind children in Greece and in the UK. education classrooms. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities
European Journal of Special Needs Education 16, 167–182. 45, 297–309.
Pearl R. & Donahue M. (2004) Peer relationships and Tachibana T. (2005) Attitudes of Japanese adults toward
learning disabilities. In: Learning about Learning Disabilities, 3rd persons with intellectual disability: an exploratory analysis of
edn (ed. B. Wong), pp. 133–156. Academic Press, San Diego. respondents’ experiences and opinions. Education and Training
Pridmore P. & Bendelow G. (1995) Images of health: exploring in Developmental Disabilities 40, 352–359.
children’s beliefs using the draw and write technique. Health Tang C. S., Davis C., Wu A. & Oliver C. (2000) Chinese
Education Journal 54, 473–488. children’s attitudes toward mental retardation. Journal of
Punch S. (2002) Research with children: the same or different Developmental and Physical Disabilities 12, 73–87.
from research with adults? Childhood 9, 321–341. Townsend M. A. R., Wilton K. M. & Vakilirad T. (1993)
Roberts C. & Zubrick S. (1992) Factors influencing the social Children’s attitudes toward peers with intellectual disability.
status of children with mild academic disabilities in regular Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 37, 405–411.
classrooms. Exceptional Children 59, 192–202. Van Kraayenoord C. (2003) The task of professional
Room R., Rehm J., Trotter R. T., Paglia A. & Ustun T. B. (2001) development. International Journal of Disability, Development,
Cross-cultural views on stigma, valuation, parity, and societal and Education 50, 363–365.
values towards disability. In: Disability and Culture: Universalism Vaughn S. & Elbaum B. (1999) The self-concept and friendships
and Diversity (eds T. Ustun, B. S. Chatterji, J. E. Bickenbach, R. T. of students with learning disabilities: A developmental
Trotter, R. Room, J. Rehm & S. Saxena), pp. 247–292. Hogrefe perspective. In: Developmental Perspectives on Children with
and Huber Publishers, Seattle. High-Incidence Disabilities (eds R. Gallimore, L. P. Bernheimer,
Sale P. & Carey D. M. (1995) The sociometric status of children D. MacMillan, D. Speece & S. Vaughn), pp. 81–107. Lawrence
with disabilities in a full-inclusion school. Exceptional Children Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey.
59, 192–202. Vlachou A. (1997) Struggles for Inclusive Education. Open
Sapkota P. & Sharma J. (1996) Participatory Interactions with University Press, Buckingham.
Children in Nepal. PLA Notes Number 25. International Ward D. J., Center Y. & Bochner S. (1994) A question of
Institute for Environment and Development, London. attitudes: integrating children with disabilities into regular
Schalock R. L., Luckasson R. A., Shogren K. A., Borthwick- classrooms? British Journal of Special Education 21, 34–39.
Duffy S., Bradley V., Buntinx W. H. E. & Yeager M. H. (2007) Westbrook M. T., Legge V. & Pennay M. (1993) Attitudes
The renaming of Mental Retardation: understanding the towards disabilities in a multicultural society. Social Science
change to the term Intellectual Disability. Intellectual and and Medicine 36, 615–623.
Developmental Disabilities 45, 116–124. Zielger R. J. (2001) A critical analysis of the literature surrounding
Shaver T., Francis V. & Barnett L. (1993) Drawing as Dialogue: A attitudes toward people with disabilities. Unpublished Master
Qualitative Approach to Needs Assessment for Health Education Thesis. University of Wisconsin-Stout, Menomonie, Wisconsin.

© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 25, 531–541

You might also like