You are on page 1of 20

Subject-Matter Content: How Does It Assist the Interactional and Linguistic Needs of

Classroom Language Learners?


Author(s): Teresa Pica
Source: The Modern Language Journal, Vol. 86, No. 1 (Spring, 2002), pp. 1-19
Published by: Blackwell Publishing on behalf of the National Federation of Modern Language Teachers
Associations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1192766 .
Accessed: 04/10/2011 11:16

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Blackwell Publishing and National Federation of Modern Language Teachers Associations are collaborating
with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Modern Language Journal.

http://www.jstor.org
Subject-Matter
Content: How Does It
Assist Interactional and
the
Linguistic Needs of Classroom
Language Learners?
TERESA PICA
Universityof Pennsylvania
3700 WalnutSt.
Philadelphia,PA 19104-6216
upenn.edu
teresap@gse.

This study focused on the role of subject-matter content in second language (L2) learning. It
sought to identify ways in which teachers modified classroom interaction about subject-matter
content in order to assist the input, feedback, and production needs of L2 learners, and to
promote their attention to developmentally difficult relationships of L2 form and meaning
that they had not fully acquired. Data were collected from 6 preacademic English L2 classes
whose content consisted of thematic units on film and literature. Each class was composed of
10-15 high intermediate English L2 students and their teachers. Analysis of the data focused
on teacher-led discussions, because these were the predominant mode of interaction in each
of the classes, and on form-meaning relationships encoded in noun and verb forms for
purposes such as reference, retelling, argument, and speculation regarding film and literary
content. Results of the study revealed numerous contexts in which the discussion interaction
might have been modified for the kinds of input, feedback, or production that could draw
students' attention to developmentally difficult form-meaning relationships. However, there
were relatively few instances in which this actually occurred. Instead, the teachers and students
tended to exchange multiutterance texts, the comprehensibility of which provided little basis
for modified interaction and attention to form and meaning.

MORE THAN 15 YEARSHAVE PASSED SINCE portunities for students to receive L2 input and
Merrill Swain drew from her massive data base on produce modified output. In effect, immersion
French immersion students to express concerns classroom interaction had served as an excellent
about their comprehension strengths and pro- source of meaningful, comprehensible input for
duction shortcomings, and to point out discrep- the students to learn subject-matter content and
ancies between their solid achievements in sub- to improve their ability to understand spoken and
ject-matter content and their uneven mastery of written L2. However, Swain's data also indicated
second language (L2) structures. The factors and that this input was considerably greater in quan-
reasons for these findings were addressed by tity than the amount of output the students were
Swain herself at the time (Swain, 1985) and have asked to produce. Their low level of output was of
continued to interest second language acquisi- concern, Swain argued, because production of
tion (SLA) researchers and professionals ever modified, comprehensible output might have
since. been what they needed to broaden the scope and
What might have brought about this outcome,
accuracy of their L2 learning. In subsequent re-
Swain's data suggested, was an imbalance in op- search (Swain, 1988, 1991, 1995, 1996), Swain
noted another concern about classroom input.
TheModernLanguageJournal, 86, i, (2002) Her analysis revealed that the input adjustments
0026-7902/02/1-19 $1.50/0 teachers made to help students understand sub-
@2002 TheModernLanguageJournal
ject-matter content were limited in scope, sali-
2 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)

ence, complexity, and functionality of L2 mor- In their relation to academic skills and overall
phosyntax, also considered crucial for interlan- proficiency, these views of L2 learning are appro-
guage development. priate to many of the instructional goals of L2
In spite of these shortcomings, however, im- and content integration and to the ways in which
mersion and other classroom approaches ori- L2 learning must be evaluated for purposes of
ented toward integration of subject-matter con- pedagogy and policy. Of concern, however, is
tent and L2 learning have continued to thrive in that, as they overlook the learning of L2 forms
number and variety. These include what are and structures that encode subject-matter con-
known popularly as sheltered, adjunct, theme- tent, these views have the capacity to hold L2
based, and language for specific purposes (LSP) learners to criteria that meet grade-level stan-
approaches, as well as less explicitly labeled varie- dards for reading and writing, but disadvantage
ties characterized by spoken or written activities them in more competitive domains of oral com-
conjoined with students' reading of texts, viewing munication with native speakers (NSs).
of video or film, and experiences in the commu- Yet another concern with assessment and evalu-
nity (Brinton, 2000; Carson, Taylor, & Fredella, ation relates to the groups with whom students'
1997; Mohan, 1979; Stoller & Grabe, 1997; learning of L2 and content are compared. As was
Zuengler & Brinton, 1997). Across academic and illustrated in Pica (1997) and Pica et al. (1998),
professional arenas, these and other incarnations control and comparison groups used as a basis for
of content-based L2 approaches aim to support
evaluating students' L2 learning have tended to
students in learning the L2 they need for current, come from foreign language (FL) classrooms.
concurrent, or future success at school, in the Those groups used as a basis for evaluating con-
workplace, and across broader social contexts. As tent learning have been NSs who share the same
they attempt to address these aims, instructional
L1 as the L2-content students, but who are en-
approaches that integrate L2 and subject-matter rolled in mainstream classes in the L1. As Swain
content have grounded evaluation of their ac-
has noted with respect to immersion programs in
complishments in measures of global proficiency Canada, the emphasis on NS comparisons in the
and skill application. These practices raise addi-
evaluation of content outcomes reflects the value
tional concerns.
and emphasis given to content mastery among
parents, institutional administrators, and policy
EVALUATIONOF L2 LEARNINGAND makers (Swain, 1995). This emphasis in evalu-
CONTENT LEARNING ation is evident in Genesee et al. (1977),
Concerns about L2 content integration in the Hauptman et al. (1988), Ho (1982), Sternfeld
areas of student assessment and program evalu- (1988), and Swain (1991). It should be noted,
ation pertain to options for setting L2 learning however, that there is only a small sample of stud-
criteria and for selecting comparison learners ies on which to draw in this area because much of
and controls. Although it is possible to base crite- the immersion research was designed to answer
ria on the acquisition of linguistic forms and theoretical questions or address policy issues re-
structures, sociolinguistic units, or features of garding L2 development and has not examined
text and discourse, it has been more typical to students' content learning (Swain, 1991; Swain &
base them on global dimensions of L2 proficiency Carroll, 1987; Swain & Lapkin, 1989; Wesche,
or on skills for reading, writing, listening, and 1985, 1992).
The learning of the L2, although recognized as
speaking. Surveys by Pica (1997) and Pica, Wash-
burn, Evans, and Jo (1998) have identified this an important goal of content-based instruction,
pattern across a range of approaches to L2 con- has seldom been subjected to NS level criteria.
tent integration, including immersion (Genesee, Instead, the performance of L2 learners in regu-
Polich, & Stanley, 1977; Hart & Lapkin, 1989; Ho, lar FL classrooms has been used in this regard
1982; Sternfeld, 1988; Swain, 1991; Wesche, (Hart & Lapkin, 1989; Hauptman, Wesche, &
1992); sheltered (Freeman, Freeman, & Gon- Ready, 1988; Ho, 1982; Sternfeld, 1989). This
zalez, 1987; Hauptman, Wesche, & Ready, 1988; comparison is somewhat imbalanced, however,
Lafayette & Buscaglia, 1985; Sternfeld 1989; and because FL program students might differ con-
Wesche, 1985); adjunct (Brinton, Snow, & siderably from their content-based counterparts
Wesche, 1989; Snow & Brinton, 1988); theme- in terms of motivations, home environments and
based (Giauque, 1987; Leaver & Stryker, 1989); resources, and time spent on language study both
and LSP (Graham & Beardsley, 1986; Hudson, in and out of class. As a result, the wider context
1991; Peck, 1987). of language study might be as accountable for
TeresaPica 3
students' L2 learning as the actual content-based hensible, often drawing on emergent morphosyn-
or FL curricula to which they are exposed. tax to do so.
Several studies have actually looked at NS and One of the most comprehensive discussions of
FL learner populations as comparison groups input needs appears in Long (1996). According
(Genesee, Polich, & Stanley, 1977; Sternfeld, to Long, learners need access to input that pro-
1988), whereas others have structured their com- vides positive evidence or data on L2 form as it
parison between content classroom L2 learners encodes message meaning. Sources of positive
and NSs (Spilka, 1976; Wesche, 1985, 1992). evidence include spoken and written texts that
These studies have revealed significantly higher are in their authentic state, as well as those that
achievement among the NSs. Among those stud- have been modified for comprehensibility
ies that Harley (1993) reviews is one designed through simplification, redundancy, and elabora-
and implemented by Harley herself (Harley, tion of their linguistic features, interlocutor ex-
1989), in which she found that French immersion pectations, or communicative goals.
students, after many years of L2 content instruc- As Long argues, such input is an excellent
tion, still differed from NSs of French in their source of data about L2 form and meaning, but
expression of the imparfait and passe compose. it is also an insufficient source of evidence when
These learners continued to exhibit English L1 learners need to master L2 forms that are devel-
transfer in their production of these verbs, espe- opmentally difficult because they are too com-
cially in complex or socially distinctive discourse plex, highly redundant, or have little or no per-
environments. ceptual saliency for the complete scope of their
There is considerable confidence in the use of functional roles to be noticed. These linguistic
subject-matter content as an aid to L2 learning elements often occur in reduced syllables, as
and a range of classroom approaches that inte- bound, grammatical morphemes that distinguish
grate content and language have emerged in re- complex relationships of verb tense and aspect or
sponse to learners' needs and interests. There is sentence modality. Also difficult for learners to
also a good deal of evidence from assessment and access are noun phrase articles, determiners, or
evaluation studies that content-based approaches gender markings that carry low semantic weight
promote L2 proficiency and facilitate skill learn- as they encode message meaning.
ing in ways that are relevant and important to the When learners have difficulty in noticing these
academic and professional goals of L2 learners. A forms, there is a tendency for them to develop
remaining concern for SLA research, however, is incomplete or incorrect representations in their
that classroom experiences with subject-matter interlanguage development, and thereby substi-
content might not provide sufficient access to the tute incorrect versions for correct ones, or omit
kinds of input, feedback, and production of out- them altogether. As Long explains, this tendency
put that learners need to assist their learning reveals why learners are believed to need addi-
beyond the areas of global L2 proficiency and tional, negative evidence about what is not in the
skill application. These kinds of input, feedback, L2. Such evidence can be accessed in a variety of
and production, which are described in the fol- ways, including formal instruction on L2 rules,
lowing section, were the basis for the research explicit correction of specific features, and im-
questions of the present study. plicit feedback from requests for message clarifi-
cation and confirmation and from interlocutor
INPUT, FEEDBACK,AND PRODUCTION responses that paraphrase or recast their errone-
NEEDS OF L2 LEARNERS ous utterances.
In addition to the positive and negative evi-
The theoretical, and in many cases, empirically dence that comes from modified input, feedback,
documented needs of L2 learners have been de- and formal instruction, Swain (1985) argues that
scribed and discussed in syntheses of Ellis (1994), learners' own production can provide a basis for
Gass and Selinker (1994), Lightbown and Spada their learning of L2 form to encode message
(1993), Long (1996), Pica (1994), and Swain meaning. When learners are asked to modify
(1995), among others. What this work has re- their message production toward greater com-
vealed is that learners need to access L2 input prehensibility or accuracy, they have an opportu-
that is modified for comprehensibility, illustrative nity to move from their rudimentary interlan-
of relationships among L2 form, meaning, and guage grammar, with its communicative tendency
function, and responsive to differences between toward semantic processing and juxtaposition of
their interlanguage and their L2 target. In addi- constituent features, to more advanced, syntactic
tion, learners need to make their output compre- processing and message organization. How learn-
4 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)
ers accomplish this task is not fully understood. responses can be encoded through simple utter-
However, as Swain puts forth, the need to aim ances as well, including open signals of "what"or
toward message clarity and to repeat and reor- "please repeat," and brief responses of "yes"or
ganize original messages often pushes learners to "no" (Pica, Holliday, Lewis, Berducci, & Newman,
modify syntactically what was originally a mean- 1991; Pica, Holliday, Lewis, & Morgenthaler,
ingful message, but whose form was wanting in 1989). These modifications promote message
scope, complexity, and target-like standards of comprehensibility as well as the saliency of form-
acceptability (Linnell, 1995; Swain, 1985, 1995, meaning relationships in the message. Example
1996). 1, below, and Examples 2-4 to follow, have been
Empirical studies have shown that many L2 composed from patterns in the data of these
needs can be addressed during the course of in- three earlier studies (Pica, 1992; Pica et al., 1989;
formal conversation, open-ended communica- Pica et al., 1991). These examples illustrate the
tion, and the exchange of message meaning. Ex- kinds of modifications and interactional features
perientially oriented classrooms often make this that were identified in these studies.
assumption when they engage learners in role
plays, opinion exchanges, and other types of com-
municative activities (Pica & Doughty, 1985a, Example 1
1985b). However, these kinds of meaning-based
interactions can lend themselves to an even flow EnglishL2 Learner NS English
of communication, with little need for learners to
focus on form-meaning relationships in input, or (la) the boysarriveat Whatdid you sayabout
to move beyond their current level of L2 develop- station the boys?
ment during production of modified L2 output. (lb) (Trigger) (NegotiationSignal)
As a result, their attention needs to be focused they arriveat station oh, really
(Response) (TopicContinuation)
only on message meaning. Learners engaged in
interaction with meaningful, subject-matter con- Here, the NS uttered a signal that indicated
tent must therefore be challenged to attend to difficulty in understanding the learner's message
the form in which meaning is encoded and to meaning, and also modified the learner's mes-
notice more developmentally advanced and diffi- sage through segmentation of the phrase the boys
cult relationships of form and meaning. As will be and its incorporation into the prepositional
discussed below, interactions that involve negotia- phrase aboutthe boys.In so doing, the NS demon-
tion of meaning and form-focused intervention strated to that learner that the boyscould appear
and instruction can help them meet such chal- as both the subject of the learner's statement and
lenges. object of the NS's preposition. The NS signal thus
provided negative evidence on the incomprehen-
sibility of the learner's message meaning and
INTERACTIONS THAT ADDRESS INPUT,
positive evidence about the form of its noun
FEEDBACK,AND PRODUCTION NEEDS OF
L2 LEARNERS phrase grammar. The learner responded with
modified production of the original trigger,
Negotiationof Meaning through substitution of the pronoun they. This
modification was made to the target-like portion
Negotiation of meaning occurs during commu- of the trigger but not to the verb arrive,which was
nicative interaction, when one interlocutor's mes- the constituent that required greater morphosyn-
sage appears to another interlocutor to be un- tactic accuracy.
clear, incomprehensible, or incomplete in its The linguistic modifications that occurred in
meaning. This lack of clarity serves as a triggerfor this exchange illustrate how negotiation can pro-
which the other interlocutor utters a signal. The vide positive L2 evidence, negative evidence, and
other interlocutor is then expected to respond. modified learner output on relationships of form
The signals and responses of negotiation are and meaning. These adjustments also illustrate
often modified linguistically through repetition, the inexactness of negotiation in targeting learn-
reduction, or addition to trigger utterances (Pica, ers' L2 needs. Here, the NS signal alerted the
1992). Modifications of signals and responses also learner to deficiencies in message comprehensi-
include extraction or segmentation of words, bility, but not to the lack of clarity in any specific
phrases, and clauses from previous utterances, relationship of form and meaning. As numerous
and lexical adjustments through use of para- studies have shown, modification of form is abun-
phrase, synonyms, and descriptors. Signals and dant within negotiation (Long, 1996; Pica 1994).
TeresaPica 5
However, it is often embedded within segmenta- and other attention-focusing devices that reveal
tion and movement of phrase and sentence con- to learners differences between their own inter-
stituents rather than targeted toward specific en- language and the requirements of their L2 target
codings in learner output. Some researchers have (Long, 1996). Example 2 illustrates instances of a
taken the position that large amounts of negotia- recast (2a) and implicit corrective feedback (2b).
tion are sociolinguistically inappropriate to L2
learners (Aston, 1986), or are in themselves not a
Example 2
guarantee of L2 learning (Foster, 1998). How-
ever, neither of these positions captures the fun- EnglishL2 Learner NS English
damental concern of researchers who have car-
ried out studies of the negotiation construct.
(2a) the boysarriveat station the boysarrivedat
Rather, it is the inexactness of negotiation, when the station
drawing learners' attention to form and mean- (Trigger) (Recast)
ing, that limits its sufficiency as a condition for L2 (2b) the boysarriveat station arrive?do you mean
learning. Researchers in the field have continued arrived?
to emphasize this point (Long, 1985, 1996; Pica,
(Trigger) (CorrectiveFeedback)
1994; Sato, 1986).
The recast preserved the lexical items of the
Form-Focused
Intervention learner's utterance, but inserted the before the
noun station and modified the verb with an ap-
Form-focused intervention occurs when con- propriate ending. Although Example 2a was
versational interaction becomes modified to more targeted than the negotiation signal in Ex-
achieve message comprehensibility, and does so ample la in drawing the learner's attention to the
in ways that draw the learner's attention to rela- interlocutor's difficulty with understanding mes-
tionships of L2 form and meaning, through a sage meaning, neither of these utterances pro-
focus onform (Doughty & Williams, 1998; Long & vided optimal linguistic data to the learner. The
Robinson, 1998). Focus on form, as defined by inexactness of a negotiation signal, such as la,
Long & Robinson (1998), is viewed as "an occa- has been discussed above. Recasts such as 2a have
sional shift in attention to linguistic code features been shown to be effective vehicles for negative
... triggered by perceived problems with compre- evidence in experimental contexts (Long, Ina-
hension or production" (p. 23). Other re- gaki, & Ortega, 1996), including those that are
searchers have used the term in ways that empha- carried out in experimental content-based class-
size its attentional component. A focus on form rooms (Doughty & Varela, 1998). However, they
need not be triggered by communication prob- pose potential ambiguity to learners in class-
lems, but might anticipate them through learner rooms that emphasize communication of content
directed models (Doughty & Williams, 1998). and the exchange of message meaning. As Lyster
Form-focused intervention can occur within (1998) has shown, recasts are similar in form and
negotiation, as the need to repair conversational occurrence to teachers' follow up utterances in-
breakdowns brings interlocutors to shift attention tended to express acceptance and approval of the
from a sole emphasis on the exchange of message students' responses to their questions. Often, stu-
meaning to the perceptual or structural shape dents have no obvious way to distinguish the func-
that encodes the meaning. This shift of attention tion of a recast based on its form.
is in keeping with the meaning of Long and Rob-
inson's focus on form (1998). Not all negotiation
Form-Focused
Instruction
involves such a focus on form, however. For exam-
ple, one interlocutor might fail to interpret the Form-focused instruction has been defined as
meaning another interlocutor intended due to transmission of information about language code
differences in message content expectations or and use of corrective feedback within the context
culturally grounded world views. Such misinter- of communicative activities (Lightbown & Spada,
pretation might lead to a negotiation of message 1993, 1999; White, Lightbown, Spada, & Ranta,
meaning, even though the linguistic form of the 1991). Interactional features can include teacher
message is acceptable, appropriate, and not the use of display or evaluation questions, metalin-
focus of the conversational repair. guistic statements, and explicit corrective feed-
In previous research, instances of form-focused back. Example 3 highlights some of these instruc-
intervention have included interlocutor recasts tional features, as the NS response utterances
of learner utterances, as well as models, feedback, provide relevant information about English
6 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)

verbs, as well as corrective feedback on what the form-focused instruction as they participate in
learner should do to produce them more accu- activities involving subject-matter content?
rately. 2. If such modified interaction is found to oc-
cur, to what extent does it provide the kinds of
input, feedback, and production of modified out-
Example 3 put that draw attention to developmentally diffi-
NS English cult relationships of L2 form and meaning?
EnglishL2 Learner
The methodology of the study is described next,
Whathappenedto the together with operational definitions and exam-
boys?Wheredid they ples of the variables that were under investigation.
firstarrive?
(DisplayQuestions)
(3a) the boysarriveat I thinkyou mean arrived METHOD
station becausethis happened
Contentand Activities
(Responseto Question) lastweek.Youhaveto
add the -edending to show Data for the study came from two advanced
pasttime. level, content-based classes in a university-based
(MetalinguisticStatements) English language institute, which comprised nu-
(3b) they at stationlast Yes,I understandwhat merous programs in academic English, English in
week you'resaying,but to be business, law, and medical fields, and conversa-
(Responseto Question) correct,you should say tional English. Both content-based classes em-
arrivednot arrive.
phasized cultural, thematic content over linguis-
(ExplicitCorrectiveFeedback) tic form. One class focused on literature and
In form-focused instruction, whether immedi- culture, with students reading and responding to
ate or delayed, there is usually a reference to authentic American English literary texts. The
other class focused on film and American culture,
problems with form, especially the ways in which
such problems can interfere with the communi- using videotapes of recent movies, along with re-
cation of meaning. There is no immediate com- views and summaries of the movies as its content.
munication problem, as there is during negotia- The classes were two of a wide range of electives
tion, but interlocutors can refer to problems with available to students at the institute.
Each class followed a detailed curriculum
meaning and form as a preface to, or within, the
implementation of form-focused instruction. guide designed by the language institute direc-
tors and instructors, two of whom also partici-
pated in the study, and other members of the
Summary institute staff. Both the literature and film curric-
ula covered a broad variety of interactional activi-
This section has summarized similarities and ties and formats, consisting of teacher-led and
distinctions among negotiation of meaning, student-to-student debate and discussion, dia-
form-focused intervention, and form-focused in-
struction with respect to their interactional fea- logue journals, at home projects and papers, and
in-class presentations. Classes met daily, for 1
tures and theoretical roles in assisting the input,
hour, over the course of a 7 week session.
feedback, and production needs of L2 learners.
Does classroom use of subject-matter content
promote these kinds of interaction? Do these in- Participants
teractions draw learners' attention to difficult
forms and structures that encode content mean- Participants were two highly experienced, En-
ing? These general questions were the basis for glish as a Second Language (ESL) female instruc-
the following research questions and study of the tors and their classes of 10-15 high intermediate
interaction in six content-focused L2 classrooms. ESL students. The teachers held advanced de-
grees in applied linguistics and had over a decade
of teaching experience in L2 and FL settings.
RESEARCHQUESTIONS Each teacher had played a key role in the design
of the courses under study and had already
1. To what extent do learners and their teach- taught the courses several times. The teacher of
ers modify their interaction through negotiation the literature course had originated the course,
of meaning, form-focused intervention, and grounding it in principles of whole language and
TeresaPica 7
communicative language pedagogy, and was her- erature curriculum taught by the teachers who
self carrying out research on student empower- were to participate in the study were observed by
ment in her classroom over a series of 7-week the researcher and a team of graduate student re-
sessions, including the session of the present searchers throughout two 7-week sessions prior to
study. The teacher of the film course had been the actual data collection in order to identify com-
closely involved with course development, with parable interactional activities that could be stud-
the institute curriculum as a whole, and was coor- ied across classes and to determine whether or not
dinator of the intensive English program at the there were interactional contexts for expression
institute. Both teachers believed strongly in the and understanding of reference, sequence, mo-
integral connection between language and cul- dality, and information structure.
ture, and thus regarded culture learning as a This period of observation led to the following
major contributor to L2 learning. results: With respect to identification of compara-
Students in the literature class came from a ble activities to study, teacher-directed discussion
wide range of Asian and European L1 back- of prior viewings of film or reading of texts was
grounds and ethnicities. Students in the film class found to be the dominant interactional activity
were predominantly of Asian L1 backgrounds throughout the observation period. Each discus-
and ethnicities. Students with Asian backgrounds sion followed a consistent pattern: It was charac-
came primarily from Korea, Taiwan, and Japan. terized by utterances that began with frames such
The European students came from Eastern and as, "I'd like to talk about" or "Let's go on to."
Western Europe. All were adults who presented These frames served as the initial boundary of
academic backgrounds and goals and held at the discussion. The final boundary was marked
least a bachelor's degree. Most were engaged in either by the end of the class meeting or a teacher
full-time English language study and were plan- utterance such as, "Ok, let's move on to." Dura-
ning to remain in the United States for further tion of each discussion varied from half to three-
education once they completed their English lan- fourths of each one hour class meeting time, be-
guage studies. cause other portions of class time were used for
Results of placement and proficiency tests, in- classroom management and periodic text reread-
cluding the Michigan and TOEFL tests and profi- ing or film reviewing in order to support opinions
ciency interviews, as well as reports and observa- and answers.
tions of teachers and program administrators, Classroom observation also confirmed an
revealed an overall level of communicative profi- abundance of contexts that required the, a, and
ciency for students, that was consistent with their zeroarticles, as teachers and students referred to
placements in their respective classrooms. Despite characters, places, and events in the films or sto-
their overall level of communicative proficiency, ries, as well as in reviews, critiques, and summa-
however, the students also revealed imprecisions ries. There were also numerous contexts that re-
and inconsistencies of form in their spoken and quired inflectional and functor morpheme
written expression of meaning in areas such as marking of lexical and modal verbs for time, as-
making reference to places, people, and events, se- pect, and modality in relating story lines, express-
quencing activities, conditions, and events, as well ing experiences and opinions, advancing argu-
as foregrounding and backgrounding informa- ments, and making speculations. These features
tion, and asserting claims and opinions. were also consistent with the nature of course
These difficulties with form in the encoding of content and the discourse requirements of the
reference, sequence, modality, and information discussion activity.
structure were characterized by underuse or Data were collected through audio- and
overuse of articles, inappropriate verb tense and videotapings of class meetings over the 7-week
aspect marking, and modal verb misselection. As duration of each course. Six sustained, teacher-
was noted above with respect to Long (1996), led classroom discussions about the cultural, the-
such developmentally lingering imprecisions are matic, or story content of a literary text or film
not unusual in the encoding of form and mean- were chosen at random from a sample of more
ing in areas of low salience such as these. than 30 such activities, each using frames such as
those noted above.
Data Collection
Data Coding
Several procedures were followed in the precol-
lection, collection, coding, and analysisof the data The data from the discussions were coded and
for the study. Classes that followed the film and lit- quantified with respect to teacher and student
8 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)

utterances. Random samples of the data were with the standard variety of English that was the
coded by the researcher and three trained target of the students' L2 studies. This procedure
coders, each with backgrounds in applied linguis- was carried out in order to determine whether
tics. Interitem reliability was .98 for utterances, an utterance of negotiation or form-focused in-
and ranged between .80 and .99 for all other tervention or instruction conveyed positive or
features described below. All teacher and student negative evidence. The distinction is shown in
utterances were further coded for the following Example 4. In 4a, which displays positive L2 evi-
interactional and linguistic features. Examples of dence, the interlocutor extracted the already tar-
these features were described, discussed, and il- get-like a moviefrom the learner's utterance and
lustrated in Examples 1-3 above, and are opera- continued to use it in a target-like way. In 4b,
tionally defined below: which shows the provision of negative evidence,
the interlocutor offered target versions of the
1. Negotiation signal utterances: questions,
learner's nontarget production of the past form
statements, commands, and phrases from one in- of watch.
terlocutor, which indicated difficulty in following
the other's prior, that is, trigger, utterance, and
requested clarification or confirmation of it. Example 4
2. Negotiation trigger utterances: utterances of
the other interlocutor that immediately preceded EnglishLanguageLearner NS English
or occurred no more than five utterances prior to
a signal utterance. (4a Illustrationof PositiveL2 Evidence)
I watcha movielastweek a movie?
3. Negotiation response utterances: utterances
of the trigger producer that immediately fol- (NegotiationSignal)
a movielastweek
lowed a negotiation signal.
4. Form-focused intervention utterances: re- (Recast)
Canyou thinkof other
casts, which simultaneously modified one or
more nontarget features of an interlocutor's ut- waysyou can referto a
movie?Thinkof the review
terances, but preserved utterance meaning and
declarative intonation. Recasts produced with ris- you read.It used a differ-
ent term.
ing intonation and clarification or confirmation
(Form-Focused Instruction)
request functions were coded as negotiation sig-
nals. (4b) Illustrationof NegativeEvidence
I watcha movielastweek you watched?
5. Form-focused instruction utterances: ques-
tions that asked students to display information (NegotiationSignal)
you watcheda movielast
known already to their questioner, metalinguistic
week
statements, statements, and phrases of correction
and rejection, and lesson-related statements and (Recast)
Can you thinkof another
questions of elicitation and evaluation.
6. Topic switch and topic continuation utter- wayto saywatchwhenyou
watcha moviein the past?
ances: utterances that introduced, or switched to,
new discussion topics or continued and sustained Canyou add an ending
like -ed?
current topics.
(Form-Focused
Instruction)
Coding of form-meaning relationships focused
on identification of contexts for the following: RESULTS
1. References to characters, places, and events
In answer to Research Question 1, the data
in film and literary content or reactions thereto
revealed a low amount of interaction modified by
that required the, a, and zeroarticles.
2. Inflectional and functor morpheme marking negotiation and negligible amounts of interac-
tion involving form-focused intervention or in-
of lexical and modal verbs for time, aspect, and
struction. With respect to Question 2, the data
modality in relating story lines, expressing expe-
revealed input, feedback, and learner-modified
riences and opinions, making speculations, ad-
output that contained relatively large amounts of
vancing arguments, and supporting opinions.
positive L2 evidence and low amounts of negative
Also noted were whether form-meaning en- evidence on the relationships of L2 form and
codings in such contexts were target-like in their meaning under study. One of the most striking
grammatical features, that is, were consistent findings of the study was that the majority of
TeresaPica 9

student nontarget utterances went unaddressed ing learners and other nonnative speakers with
in any direct way.These findings are discussed in NSs outside the classroom (Long, 1985), and no
more detail below in relation to the research better than that found in communicative class-
questions of the study. room discussions (Pica & Doughty, 1985a, 1985b;
Pica & Long, 1986). However, as was argued by
Pica, Kanagy,and Falodun (1993), on the basis of
Resultson QuestionI
findings from these and similar studies whose
data came from discussion, opinion sharing, and
Question 1 asked whether learners and their
teachers engaged in negotiation of meaning, other tasks that allowed for divergent views and
form-focused intervention, and form-focused in- outcomes, the low incidence of negotiation in the
struction as they participated in discussions in- present data may have been more related to the
volving subject-matter content. The classroom open-endedness of the discussion activity, rather
data revealed that only a small portion of discus- than to the subject-matter content under discus-
sion discourse was characterized by these forms sion. Closed-ended, problem-solving, and infor-
of interaction. Table 1 displays the frequencies mation gap tasks might have required greater
and proportions of negotiation signal and re- comprehensibility and accuracy of subject con-
sponse utterances, form-focused intervention re- tent than the film or literature discussions and
casts, and form-focused instruction utterances might, therefore, have generated more negotia-
that were found. tion on the students' part.
As shown in Table 1, there were only 358 nego- What had been revealed, however, in earlier
tiation utterances from teachers and students out observation of film and literature classroom in-
of a total of 4,008 utterances. This small figure, teraction, as well as that carried out during the
which constituted 9%1 of the total utterances, present study,was a relative absence of such tasks.
represented most of the interaction under inves- Given the high level of students' interest in cine-
tigation, given that there were 17 recasts, 25 utter- matic and literary content and their teachers'
ances of code transmission, and 5 utterances of belief in the connections between language
code correction. Together, these three types of learning and culture learning, the discussion
utterances constituted only 1% of the total utter- seemed to be an inevitable choice as a promi-
ances of modified interaction of the teachers and nent, highly interactive activity that could engage
students during their discussions. the students' views. That there was indeed a con-
Although at 9% the proportion of negotiation siderable amount of unmodified interaction
utterances was considerably higher than that of around subject content is evident from the near
the utterances of form-focused intervention or equal distribution of teacher and student utter-
instruction, this figure was still quite low relative ances during the discussions. As displayed in Ta-
to that found for negotiation in situations involv- ble 1, there were 2,142 teacher utterances and

TABLE1
Negotiation, Form-FocusedIntervention, Form-FocusedInstruction
Teachers Students Totals
% Total % Total % Total
n Utterances n Utterances n Utterances
Negotiation Signal Utterances 133 6% 66 3% 199 5%
Negotiation Response Utterances 42 2% 117 6% 159 4%
Total Negotiation Signal &
Response Utterances 175 8% 183 10% 358 9%
Form-FocusedIntervention
Utterances (Recasts) 17 0% 0 0% 17 0%
Form-FocusedInstruction Utterances:
Code Transmission 24 1% 1 0% 25 1%
Code Correction 5 0% 0 0% 5 0%
Total Negotiation, Form-Focused
Intervention, & Instruction
Utterances 221 10% 184 10% 405 10%
Total Utterances 2,142 53% 1,866 47% 4,008 100%
10 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)

1,866 student utterances. As such, these utter- desired to refrain from indicating incomprehen-
ances constituted 53% and 47% of the total of sion and thereby adhere to classroom norms for
4,008 utterances gathered during the six discus- deference. This minimization of signals, in turn,
sions. However, these utterances seldom required resulted in few teacher responses available to stu-
adjustments to be understood. As shown in Ex- dents as vehicles of positive L2 evidence. The 117
cerpt 12, and to be discussed below, classroom utterances of student response shown in Table 1
discussions moved smoothly, as students commu- suggest that the teacher and student signals were
nicated message meaning, with little apparent potentially effective in generating contexts for
need to attend to the form used to encode it. student production of modified output. However,
Of additional note was the finding that negotia- this possibility was somewhat mitigated by further
tion and form-focused intervention and instruc- analysis of the student response data, which re-
tion were largely teacher-provided. Recasts and vealed mainly repetition of already target-like seg-
code transmission and correction utterances ments of prior utterances or brief answers of yes
were barely evident in the student data. With or no.2
respect to negotiation, the students' contribu- Taken together, results of data analysis with
tions were mainly responses to their teachers, respect to Question 1 revealed a paucity of the
with nearly twice as many student responses (117) kinds of interaction considered helpful to learn-
as signal utterances (66). Additionally, as will be ers' input, feedback, and production needs, and
shown in the excerpts below, many of the stu- a lack of suppliance with respect to the L2 evi-
dents' responses were simple acknowledgments dence such interactions are known to generate.
of yesor denials of no, and therefore were not the However, the question remained as to whether or
kind of responses needed to "push"students to- not the evidence, infrequent as it was, was never-
ward greater syntactic processing of their mes- theless targeted toward the form-meaning rela-
sages (Swain, 1985). tionships that were so crucial to the L2 develop-
Observation of the classes as a whole indicated ment of the learners in the study. This was what
that much of the code transmission and correc- Question 2 aimed to answer. As such, Question 2
tion the students received was not integrated into focused on the extent to which the input, feed-
class discussions. Instead, the teachers provided back, and modified production generated by ne-
lessons on specific structural rules and lexical gotiation and form-focused intervention and in-
meanings, in response to students' imprecisions struction provided positive and negative evidence
on their written homework assignments or contri- of developmentally difficult and complex rela-
butions in prior classes. Statements and explana- tionships of L2 form and meaning the students
tions of rules were also provided when students could produce, but had yet to master. Results of
asked questions about grammar in their journal data analysis for this question are discussed next.
entries. Such attention to L2 form lent further
support to the possibility that it was the discussion Resultson Question2
activity specifically, rather than the subject-matter
content in general, that was responsible for the For the form-meaning relationships under
low amount of form-focused instruction found in study, the data for Question 2 revealed input,
the data. feedback, and student production of modified
Also shown in Table 1, 199 utterances were output that contained both positive and negative
provided as signals of message incomprehensibil- evidence on L2 form as it was used to encode
ity. These constituted 6% of teacher and 3% of message meaning, Positive L2 evidence was found
student utterances. These signals contained most in negotiation signals or responses, form-focused
of the negative evidence available during discus- recasts, or form-focused instruction utterances
sion. Given the inexactness of negotiation signals that relocated, added, deleted, or substituted a
in drawing learners' attention to specific areas noun article, a verb tense, an aspect morpheme
of difference between a target L2 version and or a modal verb that had been used in a different,
their own production, these results suggest that but target-like, manner to express a prior utter-
the availability of negative evidence during dis- ance. Negative evidence was found when these
cussion was minimal and was primarily teacher- modifications occurred in a nontarget form in a
supplied. prior utterance. This distinction was illustrated in
Students provided few signals to teachers or to Example 4.
each other. As speculated elsewhere (Pica, 1987), As shown in Table 2, during negotiation there
their lack of signaling suggested either that they were 55 utterances with noun article modifica-
comprehended messages with ease, or that they tions and 27 with verb tense and aspect modifica-
TeresaPica 11
TABLE2
Teachers' Negotiation, with Modificationof Students' Target and Nontarget Productions
Modificationof Students' Production n % Negotiation Utterances
Target Noun Articles 53 30%
Nontarget Noun Articles 2 1%
TargetVerbTense/Aspect 22 13%
Nontarget VerbTense/Aspect 5 3%
Target Modal Verbs 7 4%
Nontarget Modal Verbs 0 0%
Target Noun Articles,and/or VerbTense/Aspect,
and/or Modal Verbs 5 3%
Nontarget Noun Articles,and/or VerbTense/Aspect,
and/or ModalVerbs 0 0%
Totals
Total Target Modifications 87 50%
Total Nontarget Modifications 7 4%
OverallTotal Modifications 94 54%

tions, 7 utteranceswithmodalverbmodifications, Cantonesetherewere


and 5 utteranceswith some combinationthereof. no captions? no
Together, these 94 utterances constituted over (NegotiationSignal) (Responseto Negotiation
54%of the positiveand negativeevidencethatwas (FilmClass) Signal)
availableto studentsduringnegotiation.Thus,al-
As can also be seen in Table 2, however, 87 of
though there were not manyutterancesof nego-
tiation during the discussion activitiesof these the 94 signal and response utterances involved
modification of students' already target-like pro-
classrooms,a good portion of their modification
involvedcrucialform-meaningrelationshipsthat duction. Such exchanges were in keeping with the
the studentsneeded to acquire. message-oriented purpose of negotiation. How-
This aspect of negotiationis shownin Excerpt ever, the evidence provided with respect to L2
1, which, along with Excerpts 2-14 below, was form was largely positive, serving to reinforce stu-
taken from the actual data of the study.In Ex- dents' already target-like productions. Typically,
the teacher would signal incomprehension of the
cerpt 1, the teacherrequestedclarificationof the
student'smessage regardinghis reaction to the student's preceding utterance, but in so doing,
film Dim Sum.In so doing, the teacher incorpo- simply extract from it a target-like noun or verb
rated the student'stargetproductionof speaking phrase. The nontarget form within the student's
and providedpositiveL2 evidenceof verb aspect preceding utterance was often omitted from this
morphology.The teacheralsorecastthe student's signaling feature or follow-up move.
nontargetverbinflectionsof arethroughsubstitu- This pattern can be seen in the following ex-
tion of the more temporallyappropriate were. changes from the data. In Excerpt 2, the teacher's
This modificationoffered negative evidence re- signal substituted the student's possessive pro-
garding differences between the student's pro- noun herwith the noun article a, and in so doing,
duction and a targetversionfor markingtime. modified the student's alreadytarget-likeutterance.
In Excerpt 3, the teacher's response to the stu-
dent's signal confirmed the form of the student's
three uses of the article thein themeaningof thename
Excerpt1
of themovie?albeit in the context of a slightly differ-
Teacher Student ent version of the student's utterance.
In Excerpt 4, the teacher's signal repeated the
whatis yourbasicreactionActuallyI didn't student's already target-like expression of form
to DimSum?good!OK understand becausewhen and meaning. The student's utterance, thething is
theyarespeakingCantonese tooslow,was target-like in form with respect to use
therearenotcaptionsthere of the, but apparently incomprehensible with re-
so theycan'tunderstandit spect to message meaning. This situation seemed
(Trigger) to warrant a more general, message-focused con-
whentheywerespeaking firmation.
12 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)

Excerpt 2 phrase, missed the last part, by extracting it from a


longer utterance. However, she did not modify
Teacher Student the nontarget watch, and thereby missed the op-
hershadow portunity to add an appropriate time inflection to
(Trigger) this form. In Excerpt 7, the teacher confirmed
like a shadow? yeah the meaning of the student's message through
(Negotiation Signal) (Response to Negotiation paraphrase. However, she did not signal regard-
Signal) ing the nontarget people does.
Mmhm
(Topic Continuation Utterance)
(Literature class) Excerpt 5

Teacher Student
Excerpt 3 does my feeling was about the
Teacher Student knittingis pitiful or or miserable
(Trigger)
... so the idea of standing
the knitting? yes,
and steady and testing and
(Negotiation Signal) (Response to Negotiation
producing information or
Signal)
knowledge is all wrapped and and in this time he he uh
up in this title of this movie I feel that
-Stand and Deliver is that the meaningof the
name of the movie? (Topic Continuation)
(Literature class)
(Negotiation Signal)
all of these meanings are
the meanings of the name
Excerpt 6
of the movie
(Response to Negotiation Signal) Teacher Student
(Film class) did you watch it? I watchit but I missedthelast
part
Excerpt 4 uhhuh I watchit
(Trigger)
Teacher Student
you missed the last part? yeah
the thing is too slow (Negotiation Signal) (Response to Negotiation
(Trigger) Signal)
the theme? these themes? no no no no no (Film class)
(Negotiation Signal) (Response to Negotiation
Signal)
Wh- wha- that's the right Excerpt 7
meaning but what's the
right word? Anybody know? Teacher Student
the something was too slow
(Negotiation Signal) I think the message of the
(Film class) author is that we must try to
do what we think its good not
During many negotiated exchanges, the teach- what other peopledoesonly
ers' signals and responses provided only positive because they do it. because its
evidence when, in fact, negative evidence might not perhaps superficially we
have been even more crucial to students' notic- will be better but our con-
ing of form and meaning. This pattern can be science will not eh, we will
seen in Excerpts 5-7. In Excerpt 5, the teacher's not accept it. I don't know.
extraction of the noun phrase the knitting con- (Trigger)
firmed student's target production of article the, so we shouldn'tsuccumb? yeah
but the teacher did not modify other nontarget (Negotiation Signal) (Response to Negotiation
features such as the student's use of does. In Ex- Signal)
cerpt 6, the teacher modified the student's verb (Literature class)
TeresaPica 13
TABLE3
Teachers' Recastsof Students' Target and Nontarget Productions
Teachers' Recastsof Students' Production n % Recast Utterances
TargetNoun Articles 2 12%
Nontarget Noun Articles 3 18%
TargetVerbTense/Aspect 0 0%
Nontarget Verb Tense/Aspect 5 29%
Target Modal Verbs 0 0%
Nontarget Modal Verbs 0 0%
Target Noun Articles, and/or VerbTense/Aspect, and/or
Modal Verbs 1 1%
Nontarget Noun Articles, and/or VerbTense/Aspect Form,
and/or Modal Verbs 0 0%
Other Teacher Recasts 6 35%
Total Teacher Recasts 17 100%

Teacher recasts provided consistent negative you thinkuh whatcan be the


evidence. As shown in Table 3, there were 11 bestwayfor parentsto growup
teacher recasts concerning the relationships of theirchildren?
form and meaning under study. These consti- raisetheirchildren whatcan parentsdo to make
tuted 65% of the recast data and, as shown in the (Recast) them educatedor successful?
examples below, were occasionally effective in (Responseto Recast)
alerting students to their imprecisions. However, the best wayto
they were not numerous in frequency among educatethem educatethem in the society,yeah
other teacher utterances. (Rephrasing) (Responseto Rephrasing)
Recasts appeared both during and outside of
As with the signals and responses of negotia-
negotiation. A negotiation recast was illustrated
in Excerpt 1, when the teacher requested clarifi- tion, teacher recasts of student nontarget utter-
cation of the student's message, and in so doing ances did not alwaysfocus on nontarget features.
modified the student's nontarget verb inflections This type of feedback is shown in Excerpt 10, in
of are through substitution of the more accurate which the target-like mustn't show his humiliation
were.Excerpts 8-9 are illustrative of the teachers' of a student's prior utterance is recast into a new
recast utterances through their comments and utterance. However, the nontarget by don't give
responses, as they recast "go back China," "grow moneyis not.
up their children," and "make them educated" in
the students' speech. Excerpt 10
Teacher Student
Excerpt 8
Teacher Student yeah if he's stillproudhe mustn't
showhis humiliationby don't
There'sanothercon- give money
flict in the mother. rightit's humiliation
somethingelse is- thatwouldshow
the motheris thinking (Recast)
a lot about
With respect to student production of modified
go backChina
going back to China output, results were similar to those of teacher
is one thing utterances, with reincorporation of target-like
(Recast) segments of their original utterances. This rein-
(FilmClass) corporation is shown in Table 4. As with the
teacher utterances, there were more student ut-
terances with noun article modification than verb
Excerpt 9 morpheme modification, and hardly any modal
or combined modification for a distribution of 41,
Teacher Student
21, 2, and 5 modified utterances, respectively.
whatdo you thinkuh whatdo Student-to-student negotiation, though limited in
14 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)
TABLE4
Student Responses of Modified Production
Students'ModifiedProduction n % Student Response Utterances
Target Noun Articles 40 34%
Nontarget Noun Articles 1 1%
TargetVerbTense/Aspect 21 18%
Nontarget VerbTense/Aspect 0 0%
Target Modal Verbs 2 2%
Nontarget Modal Verbs 0 0%
Target Noun Article, and/or VerbTense/Aspect, and/or
Modal Verbs 5 4%
Nontarget Noun Articles,and/or VerbTense/Aspect,
and/or Modal Verb 0 0%
Total Student Modified Production 69 59%
Total Student Response Utterances 117 100%

amount, nevertheless, revealed patterns of modi- pus of discussion data, there were few among
fication not unlike those of teacher-student inter- these that addressed the L2 form and meaning
action. In the student-to-student negotiated ex- relationships under study. Only 1 code transmis-
change of Excerpt 11, in which a student sion and 3 code correction utterances drew stu-
modified after to pull don't wear the clothes,but it dents' attention to these features. It is interesting
happen,such a pattern is apparent. to note, however, that 25 utterances provided
form-focused instruction on content-related lexi-
cal items.
Excerpt 11
Student Student
OTHER FINDINGS
and oh aftertopulldon'twearthe
A notable feature of the discussion interaction
clothesbut it happen
was that most of the student utterances with non-
(Trigger)
so what? What? target production of the form-meaning relation-
ships under study were not given a direct re-
(NegotiationSignal) (NegotiationSignal)
afterwhat? sponse of positive or negative evidence. Instead
they were followed by utterances of topic continu-
(NegotiationSignal) ation or topic switch. This pattern was especially
Takeyourclothes
evident when student utterances were embedded
off afteryou are
in lengthy, but generally comprehensible, texts
finishedwearing
that the teacher and peer interlocutors did not
denim ...
interrupt with utterances of negotiation, recast
(Responseto Nego-
tiationSignal) through form-focused intervention, or address
through form-focused instruction. Instead, they
Another typical outcome was for students sim- actually prolonged the nontarget discourse
ply to acknowledge the teachers' signals with vari- through back channel utterances and topic sus-
ations of yesor no. This acknowledgment was illus- taining moves.
trated in Excerpts 1-2 and 5-7. As shown, Thus, as shown in Table 5, 170 of students'
teachers' signals substituted more target-like nontarget utterances with contexts for noun,
forms while retaining lexical items in the students' verb, and modal suppliance were followed by
original utterances. In so doing, the teachers re- teacher utterances of topic switch or continu-
stricted the students' need to recode their original ation. They constituted 9% of the students' total
utterances further or to draw on their limited in- utterances. As also shown, less than 1% of the
terlanguage resources to enhance or modify their students' utterances with these features were fol-
contributions in further ways (Pica et al., 1989). lowed by responses that carried negative evi-
Finally, not only were very few code transmis- dence through negotiation or recasting. Many
sion and correction utterances found in the cor- of the students' nontarget utterances appeared
TeresaPica 15

TABLE5
Students' Nontarget Production in Contexts for Form-Meaning Encoding and Teachers' Following
Utterances
Teachers'Following Utterances
Topic Switchor
Continuation Negotiation Recast
% Total Student %Total Student % Total Student
Students' Production n Utterances n Utterances n Utterances
Noun Articles 52 3% 2 0% 3 0%
VerbTense/Aspect 117 6% 5 0% 5 0%
Modal Verbs 1 0% 0 0% 0 0%
Total 170 9% 7 0% 8 0%

in long texts without any teacher intervention Excerpt 13


at all.
Student Student
Excerpts 12 and 13 illustrate these phenomena
in both teacher-to-student and student-to-student
interaction during discussion. The italicized I'm not sure if her motherwant
to get marriedwiththe uncle or
phrases reveal the unaddressed, nontarget pro-
ductions. In Excerpt 12, the teacher acknowl- not becauseI thinkin the movie
edged the student's contributions through back probablyher motherhesitate to
channeling, and followed them up with an ex- get marriedwithwithwith
pression of her own opinion. At the same time, yes I thinkso
She hesitates
however, the student uttered numerous nontar-
yes the uncle she refuse
just because
get forms, especially with respect to verbs used to
relate the story line, without receiving any inter- I don't understandwhyshe ask
vention at all. her daughterif I can get married
with the uncle or not and finally
her daughtercried
Excerpt 12 (FilmClass)

Teacher Student
CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS
the daughterhave a prettygood
The use of subject-matter content to support
but she also hopeto get married
classroom L2 learning has been recognized theo-
but she thinkabouther mother.
so they areworriedeach other retically, empirically, and pedagogically for its con-
tributions to global L2 proficiency and academic
you knowso theypretend they think skill development across a broad spectrum of
mm-hmm theyreallyhavea good life at learners. However, concerns have lingered among
that time mm-hmm but when
L2 teachers and researchers about the effective-
the her mothergo to Chinaback
ness of a content focus for development and mas-
and her mother changechange
his un thinkingand being and tery of L2 features whose limited saliency often re-
then uh her daughterthinkthat quires attention to form. The present study was an
then she can get marriedand attempt to address those concerns through exami-
nation of classroom interaction involving subject-
her mothercan independon
matter content, particularlywith respect to its role
others
in providing negotiation of meaning, form-fo-
really?I had a very
cused intervention, and form-focused instruction.
differentpoint of
view. These interactions are known to provide the kinds
of input, feedback, and learner production of
(Filmclass)
modified output that draw students' attention to
These patterns of back channeling and non-inter- form in relation to content meaning. Of addi-
vention can also be seen in the student-to-student tional interest was whether attention was given to
discourse in Excerpt 13. noun articles, verb tense and aspect, and modal
16 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)

verbs, as students and teachers in the study re- Given the popularity and interest generated by
ferred to characters and incidents, advanced story the discussion in content-based classrooms, two
lines, gave reactions, made speculations, and pre- approaches might be taken to preserve its place,
sented arguments during their classroom interac- yet modify its application. One approach would
tion. These form-meaning relationships were im- guide teachers in modifying their responses to
portant, because they were low in saliency, students' multiutterance contributions in ways
developmentally difficult, and had not yet been that would generate more input, feedback, and
mastered by the students. production of student output. The other ap-
Overall, results suggested that subject-matter proach would encourage teachers to use the dis-
content in the film and literature classrooms pro- cussion as an initial activity to introduce or review
vided a meaningful context for students' expo- content, and then follow it with interactive, form-
sure to the form and meaning relationships they focusing tasks that promote opportunities for
had yet to master. However, the discussion, as the more targeted input, feedback, and student pro-
most frequently implemented interactional activ- duction of modified output.
ity in these classrooms, did not promote the kinds In implementing the first approach, teachers'
of interaction that could draw attention to these modified responses could include planned inter-
relationships. Instead, it provided a context for vention strategies that would prompt students to
the students to sustain lengthy, multi-utterance speak at length, and at the same time recast their
texts, whose comprehensibility of message mean- nontarget encodings of form as they advanced
ing provided little basis for negotiation, form-fo- message meaning. This approach would be pat-
cused intervention, and form-focused instruc- terned on the work of Doughty and Varela
tion. (1998), whose research revealed ways in which
The discussions were interesting and meaning- teacher recasts of student responses in science
ful with respect to subject-matter content. How- classrooms were able to advance their develop-
ever, as open-ended communication activities, ment of verb form and meaning. Using this ap-
they drew attention away from students' need for proach, a text such as that shown in Excerpt 12
input and feedback that contained negative evi- might resemble the following, shown here as Ex-
dence on crucial form-meaning relationships in cerpt 14:
their L2 development. The discussions involved
teachers and students in using language to discuss
Excerpt 14
content, but did not focus on the L2 form used to
encode content meaning, particularly when the Teacher Student
students' own production of form was itself not
target-like. Although there were only two teachers the daughter have a prettygood
who participated in the present study, other re- but she also hopeto get married
search has noted similar results for discussion ac- but she thinkabouther mother.
tivities (Pica, Kanagy,& Falodun, 1993). Together, so they areworriedeach other
these studies suggest that, in order to address you knowso theypretend they think
learners' L2 needs, content-based teachers need mm-hmm, yes she
to find additional ways to promote L2 learning hopedto get married,
through content in the classroom. but she thoughtabout
In spite of the limitations of the discussion as a hermother theyreallyhavea good life at
classroom practice, first in the context of commu- thattime mm-hmm but when
nicative language classrooms, and now here, with the her mothergoto Chinaback
respect to film and literature L2 classes, this activ- and her mother changechange
ity continues to dominate the discourse in many his un thinkingand being and
classrooms. This decision makes sense in light of then uh her daughterthinkthat
its attraction to students' interests, as well as its then she can get marriedand
reliability for teachers in their preparation and her mothercan independon
coverage of subject-matter content. Thus, the dis- others
cussion activity appears to be efficient in terms of really? Thedaughter
curriculum decisions and classroom conventions. thoughtthat hermother
However, as a task for L2 learning, it falls short of coulddependon others?
meeting conditions that satisfy learners' needs I had a verydifferent
for positive, and particularly, negative evidence, point of view.
relevant to L2 learning. (FilmClass)
TeresaPica 17

As the italicized segments show, the teacher's discussion, if it is to serve students' many needs
two responses recast the student's use of think and goals. Collaborative, form-focused tasks can
into a more target form. In so doing, she pre- be easily produced and incorporated into a cur-
served the discussion format, but provided im- riculum organized around subject-matter con-
plicit negative evidence and target versions of the tent. Grounded in theory and research on L2
student's think.Because teacher recasts would fol- learning and teaching, these tasks can not only
low utterances that students generated them- enrich content learning, but also broaden per-
selves, rather than produced in response to spectives on the role of content in classroom L2
teacher display questions, they would be less learning.
likely to be confused with the approval function
that is characteristic of teacher follow-up utter-
ances in lessons (Lyster, 1998). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
In addition to inserting recasts to convey im-
plicit negative evidence during discussion activi- Manythanksto the followingresearchassistantsand
ties, teachers could also employ classroom tasks associateswho helped with the data collection and
that require precision of form and content and re- analysis: Sharon Bode, Ruth Boyd-Kletzander, Bruce
sponses of negative evidence as necessities for Evans, Daniel Jackson, Victoria Jo, and Jess Unger.
their completion. Close-ended information ex- Thanks also to my colleagues, Gay Washburn, Kristine
change tasks would be especially conducive to this Billmyer, Mara Blake-Ward, Lyn Buchheit, Mary Ann
outcome. For example, students might be asked to Julian, Sharon Nicolary, Jack Sullivan, and Helen Yung,
reconstruct a scene from a film or story by pooling for their feedback and support throughout this and
other related projects.
individual story lines in strip story format, which
would then need to be placed in order of occur-
rence. Alternatively,they might be asked to partici-
NOTES
pate in a dictogloss task, taking notes on a passage
or scene, then using the notes for collaborative re-
construction of it. Research (Swain, 1995) has 1
Percentage figures that show 0% on frequency data
shown that as students collaborate on reconstruc- have been rounded to the nearest percent.
2 This finding is
tion tasks they are able to provide each other with displayed in Table 4 and will be
discussed with the information in Table 4.
negative evidence and use this evidence as a basis
for modifying their imprecise production.
In light of these possible directions, additional
research is now underway by the researcher and REFERENCES
administrators of the present study, along with
newly assigned teachers in content classes. Form- Aston, G. (1986). Trouble-shooting in interaction with
focusing tasks in six categories have been devel- learners: The more the merrier? AppliedLinguis-
oped for the same film curriculum that was used in tics, 7, 128-143.
the present study. These tasks draw on the same Brinton, D. (2000). Out of the mouths of babes: Novice
teacher insights into content-based instruction. In
scripts, reviews, and summaries that were the basis
of discussion activities in the study (Pica et al., L. Kasper, M. Babbitt, & R. Mlynarczyk (Eds.),
Content-basedcollegeESL instruction (pp. 48-70).
2001). Task categories include "Spot the Differ-
Mahwah, NewJersey: Erlbaum.
ence" (Crookes & Rulon, 1988; Long, 1981), "Dic-
Brinton, D., Snow, M. A., & Wesche, M. (1989). Content-
togloss" (Swain, 1998; Wajnryb, 1990), '"Jig-Saw based second language instruction.New York: New-
Story Construction" (Pica, Lincoln-Porter, Pani- bury House.
nos, & Linnell, 1996), and several different ap- Carson, J., Taylor,J., & Fredella, L. (1997). The role of
proaches to the "GrammarBased Communication content in task-based EAP instruction. In M. A.
Task" (Ellis, 1998; Fotos, 1994; Loschky & Bley- Snow & D. Brinton (Eds.), The content-based class-
Vroman, 1993). Preliminary data collection has room:Perspectiveson integratinglanguageand content
revealed students actively engaged in drawing (pp. 367-370). WhitePlains,NY:Longman.
each others' attention to form as they advance Crookes, G., & Rulon, K. A. (1988). Topic and feedback
in native speaker/non-native speaker conversa-
their message meaning, using the very scripts and tion. TESOLQuarterly,22, 675-681.
reviews that had failed to inspire their attention to
Doughty, C., & Varela, E. (1998). Communicative focus
form during discussion (Pica et al., 2001). on form. In C. Doughty &J. Williams (Eds.), Focus
The content-based classroom has much to of- on form in secondlanguageclassroom(pp. 114-138).
fer students in their L2 learning experience, but New York:Cambridge University Press.
it needs a broader repertoire of activities than the Doughty, C., & Williams, J. (Eds.). (1998). Focus on form
18 TheModernLanguageJournal 86 (2002)
in secondlanguage classroom.New York:Cambridge Long, M. (1981). Input, interaction, and second lan-
University Press. guage acquisition. In H. Winitz (Ed.), Native lan-
Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second language acquisition. guage and foreign language acquisition (pp.
Oxford: Oxford University Press. 259-278). Annals of the New York Academy of
Ellis, R. (1998). Teaching and research: Options in Sciences, Vol. 379.
grammar teaching. TESOLQuarterly,32, 39-60. Long, M. (1985). Input and second language acquisi-
Foster, P. (1998). A classroom perspective on the nego- tion theory. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input
tiation of meaning. AppliedLinguistics,9, 1-23. in second language acquisition (pp. 377-393).
Fotos, S. (1994). Integrating grammar instruction and Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
communicative language use through grammar Long, M. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment
consciousness-raising tasks. TESOL Quarterly,28, in second language acquisition. In W. C. Ritchie &
323-351. T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbookof language acquisi-
Freeman, D., Freeman, Y., & Gonzalez, R. (1987). Suc- tion: Vol. 2. Second language acquisition (pp.
cess for LEPs: The sunnyside sheltered English 413-468). New York:Academic Press.
program. TESOLQuarterly,21, 361-367. Long, M., Inagaki, S, & Ortega, L. (1998). The role of
Gass, S., & Selinker, L. (1994). Secondlanguage acquisi- implicit negative evidence in SLA: Models and
tion:An introductorycourse.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence recasts in Japanese and Spanish. ModernLanguage
Erlbaum. Journal, 82, 357-371.
Genesee, F., Polich, E., & Stanley, M. (1977). An experi- Long, M., & Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on form: The-
mental French immersion program at the secon- ory, research, and practice. In C. Doughty & J.
dary school level 1969 to 1974. Canadian Modern Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in second language
LanguageReview,33, 318-332. classroom(pp. 15-41). New York:Cambridge Uni-
Giauque, G. (1987). Teaching for content in a skills versity Press.
course: Greek mythology in French. ForeignLan- Loschky, L., & Bley-Vroman, R. (1993). Creating struc-
guage Annals, 20, 565-569. ture-based communication tasks for second lan-
Graham, J. G., & Beardsley, R. S. (1986). English for guage development. In G. Crookes & S. Gass
specific purposes: Content, language, and com- (Eds.), Tasksand languagelearning:integratingtheory
munication in a pharmacy course model. TESOL and practice (pp. 123-167). Clevedon, England:
Quarterly,20, 227-245. Multilingual Matters.
Harley, B. (1989). Functional grammar in French im- Lyster, R. (1998). Recasts, repetition, and ambiguity in
mersion: A classroom experiment. AppliedLinguis- L2 classroom discourse. Studiesin SecondLanguage
tics, 10, 331-359. Acquisition,20, 51-82.
Harley, B. (1993). Instructional strategies and second Mohan, B. A. (1979). Relating language teaching and
language acquisition in early French immersion. content teaching. TESOLQuarterly,13, 171-182.
Studiesin SecondLanguageAcquisition,15, 245-260. Peck, S. (1987). Spanish for social workers:An interme-
Hart, D., & Lapkin, S. (1989). Frenchimmersionat thesecon- diate level communicative course with content lec-
dary/postsecondary interface:Reportto theOntarioMin- tures. ModernLanguageJournal, 71, 402-409.
istry of Collegesand Universities.Modern Language Pica, T. (1987). Second language acquisition, social in-
Center, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education. teraction, and the classroom. AppliedLinguistics,8,
Hauptman, P. C., Wesche, M., & Ready, D. (1988). Sec- 3-21.
ond-language acquisition through subject-matter Pica, T. (1992). The textual outcomes of native
learning: A follow-up study at the University of speaker-non-native speaker negotiation. In C.
Ottawa. LanguageLearning,38, 433-461. Kramsch & S. McConnell-Ginet (Eds.), Text and
Ho, K. K. (1982). Effect of language of instruction on context: Cross-disciplinaryperspectiveson language
physics achievement. Journal of Researchin Science study (pp. 198-237). Lexington, MA: Heath.
Teaching,19, 761-767. Pica, T. (1994). Research on negotiation: What does it
Hudson, T. (1991). A content comprehension approach reveal about second language learning conditions,
to reading English for science and technology. processes, and outcomes? Language Learning, 44,
TESOLQuarterly,25, 77-104. 493-527.
Lafayette, R., & Buscaglia, M. (1985). Students learn Pica, T. (1997, October). Focus on form in contentbased
language via a civilization course: A comparison of secondlanguage classrooms.Paper presented at An-
second language classroom environments. Studies nual Second Language Research Forum, East
in SecondLanguageAcquisition,7, 323-342. Lansing, MI.
Leaver, B. L., & Stryker, S. B. (1989). Content-based Pica, T., Billmyer, K., Julian, M., Blake-Ward,M., Buch-
instruction for foreign language classroom. Foreign heit, L., Nicolary, S., & Sullivan,J. (2001, February
LanguageAnnals, 22, 269-275. 24). Fromcontent-based textstoform-focusedtasks:An
Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (1993). How languages are integrationof secondlanguagetheory,research,and ped-
learned.Oxford: Oxford University Press. agogy. Paper presented at Annual AAAL Confer-
Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (1999). How languages are ence, St. Louis, MO.
learned(Rev.Ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Pica, T., & Doughty, C. (1985a). Input and interaction in
Linnell,J. (1995). Negotiationas an aid to syntacticization. the communicative language classroom: A com-
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of parison of teacher-fronted and group activities. In
Pennsylvania. S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in secondlan-
TeresaPica 19

guage acquisition(pp. 115-132). Rowley, MA: New- tiveson integratinglanguageand content(pp. 78-84).
bury House. New York:Addison Wesley Longman.
Pica, T., & Doughty, C. (1985b). The role of group work Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some
in classroom second language acquisition. Studies roles of comprehensible input and comprehensi-
in SecondLanguageAcquisition,7, 233-248. ble output in its development. In S. Gass & C.
Pica, T., Holliday, L., Lewis, N., Berducci, D., & Newman, Madden (Eds.), Input in secondlanguageacquisition
J. (1991). Language learning through interaction: (pp. 235-253). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
What role does gender play? Studiesin SecondLan- Swain, M. (1988). Manipulating and complementing
guage Acquisition,13, 343-76. content teaching to maximize second language
Pica, T., Holliday, L., Lewis, N., & Morgenthaler, L. learning. TESLCanadaJournal, 6, 68-83.
(1989). Comprehensible output as an outcome of Swain, M. (1991). French immersion and its offshoots:
linguistic demands on the learner. Studiesin Second Getting two for one. In B. Freed (Ed.), Foreign
LanguageAcquisition,11, 63-90. languageacquisitionand the classroom(pp. 91-103).
Pica, T., Kanagy,R., & Falodun,J. (1993). Choosing and Lexington, MA: Heath.
using communication tasks for second language Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output in second
instruction. In G. Crookes & S. Gass (Eds.), Tasks language learning. In G. Cook & B. Seidlhofer
and language learning (pp. 9-34). Clevedon, En- (Eds.), For H. G. Widdowson:Principlesand practice
gland: Multilingual Matters. in thestudyof language(pp. 125-144). Oxford: Ox-
Pica, T., Lincoln-Porter, F., Paninos, D., & Linnell, J. ford University Press.
(1996). Language learners' interaction: How does Swain, M. (1996). Discovering successful second lan-
it address the input, output, and feedback needs guage teaching strategies and practices: From pro-
of L2 learners? TESOLQuarterly,30, 59-84. gram evaluation to classroom experimentation.
Pica, T., & Long, M. (1986). The linguistic and conver- Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Develop-
sational performance of experienced and inexpe- ment, 17, 89-104.
rienced teachers. In R. Day (Ed.), Talkingto learn: Swain, M. (1998). Focus on form through conscious
Conversationin second language acquisition (pp. reflection. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.),
85-98). Rowley, MA:Newbury House. Focusonform in classroomsecondlanguageacquisition
Pica, T., Washburn, G, Evans, B., & Jo, V. (1998, Janu- (pp. 64-82). New York: Cambridge University
ary). Negativefeedbackin content-basedsecond lan- Press.
guage classroominteraction:How does it contributeto Swain, M., & Carroll, S. (1987). The immersion observa-
second language learning? Annual Pacific Second tion study. In B. Harley, P.Allen,J. Cummins, & M.
Language Research Forum, Tokyo,Japan. Swain (Eds.), Thedevelopment of bilingualproficiency.
Sato, C. (1986). Conversation and interlanguage devel- Final report.Vol. II (pp. 190-263). Toronto: The
opment: Rethinking the connection. In R. Day Institute for Studies in Education.
(Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversationin second lan- Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (1989). Canadian immersion
guage acquisition(pp. 5-22). Rowley, MA:Newbury and adult second language teaching: What's the
House. connection? Modern Language Journal, 73,
Snow, M. A., & Brinton, D. M. (1988). Content-based 150-159.
language instruction: Investigating the effective- Wajnryb, R. (1990). Grammardictation.Oxford: Oxford
ness of the adjunct model. TESOLQuarterly,22, University Press.
553-574. Wesche, M. (1985). Immersion and the universities. The
Snow, M. A., & Brinton, D. M. (1997). The content-based CanadianModernLanguageReview,41, 931-940.
classroom: on integratinglanguageand con-
Perspectives Wesche, M. (1992). French immersion graduates at uni-
tent.New York:Addison Wesley Longman. versity and beyond: What difference has it made?
Spilka, I. (1976). Assessment of second-language per- In J. Alatis (Ed.), Georgetown UniversityRound Table
formance in immersion program. Canadian Mod- on Languagesand Linguistics 1992: Language, com-
ern LanguageReview,32, 543-561. munication, and social meaning (pp. 208-240).
Sternfeld, S. (1988). The applicability of the immersion Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
approach to college foreign language instruction. White, L., Spada, N., Lightbown, P., & Ranta, L. (1991).
ForeignLanguageAnnals, 21, 221-226. Input enhancement and L2 question formation.
Sternfeld, S. (1989). The University of Utah's Immer- AppliedLinguistics,12, 416-432.
sion/Multiliteracy Program: An example of an Zuengler,J., & Brinton, D. (1997). Linguistic form, prag-
area studies approach to the design of first-year matic function: Relevant research from content-
college foreign language instruction. ForeignLan- based instruction. In M. Snow & D. Brinton (Eds.),
guage Annals, 22, 341-352. Thecontent-based classroom:Perspectiveson integrating
Stoller, F., & Grabbe, W. (1997). A six-t's approach to language and content(pp. 268-73). New York:Ad-
content-based instruction. In M. Snow & D. M. dison Wesley Longman.
Brinton (Eds.), The content-basedclassroom:Perspec-

You might also like