You are on page 1of 11

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain: Brain changes and the
role of body representation
J. Foell1,2*, R. Bekrater-Bodmann1*, M. Diers1, H. Flor1
1 Department of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University,
Mannheim, Germany
2 Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, USA

Correspondence Abstract
Herta Flor
E-mail: herta.flor@zi-mannheim.de Background: Phantom limb pain (PLP) is a common consequence of
amputation and is difficult to treat. Mirror therapy (MT), a procedure
Funding sources utilizing the visual recreation of movement of a lost limb by moving the
This work was supported by the PHANTOM
intact limb in front of a mirror, has been shown to be effective in reducing
MIND project (Phantom phenomena: a
window to the mind and the brain), which
PLP. However, the neural correlates of this effect are not known.
receives research funding from the European Methods: We investigated the effects of daily mirror training over 4
Community’s Seventh Framework Programme weeks in 13 chronic PLP patients after unilateral arm amputation. Eleven
(FP7/2007–2013)/ERC Grant Agreement No. participants performed hand and lip movements during a functional
230249, and by a subproject of the magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measurement before and after MT.
Collaborative Research Project ‘Bionic-Hand’ The location of neural activity in primary somatosensory cortex during
funded by the Bundesministerium für Bildung
these tasks was used to assess brain changes related to treatment.
und Forschung (V4UKF02). This manuscript
reflects only the authors’ views, and the
Results: The treatment caused a significant reduction of PLP (average
Community is not liable for any use that may decrease of 27%). Treatment effects were predicted by a telescopic
be made of the information contained distortion of the phantom, with those patients who experienced a
therein. The authors have no other relevant telescope profiting less from treatment. fMRI data analyses revealed a
affiliations or financial involvement with any relationship between change in pain after MT and a reversal of
organization or entity with a financial interest dysfunctional cortical reorganization in primary somatosensory cortex.
in or financial conflict with the subject matter
Pain reduction after mirror training was also related to a decrease of
or materials discussed in the manuscript
apart from those disclosed. No writing
activity in the inferior parietal cortex (IPC).
assistance was utilized in the production of Conclusions: Experienced body appearance seems to be an important
this manuscript. predictor of mirror treatment effectiveness. Maladaptive changes in
cortical organization are reversed during mirror treatment, which also
Conflicts of interest alters activity in the IPC, a region involved in painful perceptions and in
None declared. the perceived relatedness to an observed limb.
*These authors contributed equally to this
work.

Accepted for publication


6 November 2013

doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00433.x

without proportional distortions. Additionally,


1. Introduction
60–90% (Jensen et al., 1983; Hanley et al., 2009) of
After the amputation of a limb, most patients report amputees report phantom limb pain (PLP), a sensation
awareness of a phantom (Giummarra et al., 2007), i.e., of pain located in the amputated limb, which has a
the continuing perception of the missing limb, with or high rate of chronicity and is difficult to treat (Weeks

© 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC®. Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 729
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain J. Foell et al.

What’s already known about this topic? of ownership (Lloyd, 2007). These results might be of
• It is known that mirror therapy provides relief for significance in MT, as perceived distortions of the
some patients with chronic phantom limb pain. phantom might complicate the integration of visual
• We also know that the intensity of this pain is and proprioceptive input into a coherent percept and
correlated with the extent of cortical reorganiza- reduce the effects of mirror treatment. This might be
tion in primary somatosensory cortex. one factor why about 40% of the PLP patients do not
benefit from MT (Weeks et al., 2010). The identifica-
tion of predictors that influence the effectiveness of
What does this study add?
MT would help to understand the mechanism of this
• This study shows that the pain relief induced by
treatment and could improve its clinical use.
mirror therapy is accompanied by a reversion of
This study was designed to (1) evaluate the effects of
cortical reorganization, and that the treatment
MT on pain in chronic PLP patients, (2) compare the
effect is dependent on properties of the phantom
brain changes related to treatment effects before and
limb.
after therapy and (3) identify predictors of treatment
success. Our hypotheses were (1) a subset of patients
will benefit from MT, and (2) these patients will
et al., 2010). Central changes have been proposed to display an individual reduction of dysfunctional corti-
contribute to PLP (Flor et al., 2006). Reorganization of cal shift, which will be stronger in patients with large
somatosensory (cf. Flor et al., 1995; Birbaumer et al., treatment benefit.
1997) and motor cortex (cf. Karl et al., 2001;
Koppelstaetter et al., 2007) – e.g., the invasion of areas 2. Materials and methods
neighbouring the representation of the amputated
limb into the cortical representation zone – was shown 2.1 Patients
to be related to PLP intensity, suggesting that it may be
The sample consisted of 13 patients with major unilateral
related to this change after amputation (Flor et al.,
upper limb amputation [four women, mean age 50.6, stan-
2006).
dard deviation (SD) = 15.8 years, range: 26–74 years] who
Since there is a strong influence of vision on the experienced PLP regularly at least once a week with an
perception (Hunter et al., 2003) and movement average intensity of at least 20 on a visual analogue scale
(Brodie et al., 2003) of a phantom, the use of experi- (VAS; ranging from 0 to 100) and who had been amputated
mental set-ups inducing the illusion of two intact for more than 2 years to rule out acute PLP (average time
limbs might influence these central alterations since amputation 21.3, SD = 12.7 years, range: 6–49 years).
(Ramachandran and Rogers-Ramachandran, 1996). All subjects reported feeling a phantom limb. Supporting
When persons with PLP are asked to place their Information Table S1 provides information on all partici-
healthy limb in front of a mirror such that its reflection pants, including cause of amputation, prosthesis use, pain
visually replaces the missing limb, this can result in medication, PLP frequency and intensity, and the experience
of telescoping. All participants gave written informed
pain alleviation in some patients. Chan et al. (2007)
consent prior to taking part in the study, and the Ethics
published a randomized placebo-controlled study
Committee of the Medical Faculty Mannheim of Heidelberg
showing significant PLP decrease with recently ampu- University approved the protocol, which adhered to the Dec-
tated leg amputees after 4 weeks of training. However, laration of Helsinki. Two patients were excluded from the
in subacute pain spontaneous recovery can occur functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measure-
(Schley et al., 2008), requiring a study of these effects ments or analysis (due to a tattoo in one case and an inci-
in chronic patients. dental finding in anatomic images in another); thus, 11
The mechanisms behind the mode of action of patients were included in the brain imaging part of the study
mirror therapy (MT) are not clear. One possible (four women, mean age 49.3 years, SD = 15.3, range: 26–68
mechanism is the representational restitution of the years; average time between amputation and measurement
missing limb in the brain (Foell et al., 2011) by the 21.3 years, SD = 13.8, range: 6–49 years), whereas all 13
patients participated in the mirror treatment. PLP intensity as
convergence of concurrent visual and proprioceptive
assessed by the German version of the West Haven-Yale
input. Additionally, previous evidence indicates that
Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI; Kerns et al., 1985;
an external object can be more easily integrated into Flor et al., 1990; modified to separately assess phantom and
one’s own body representation if there is a high degree residual limb pain) averaged 2.23 (SD = 0.72) for the entire
of congruence between sensory modalities (Tsakiris, group and 2.12 (SD = 0.70) for the fMRI subgroup. Twelve
2010). For example, deviations between the seen and patients reported that they experienced non-painful
felt position of an artificial limb impede the sensation phantom phenomena, such as a feeling of pressure or slight

730 Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 © 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC®
J. Foell et al. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain

tingling, at least several times per week. The mean intensity and downwards; (4) sequential converging of fingertips and
for these sensations was 48.38 (SD = 26.58, range: 0–92) on thumb, palm towards the mirror, without actual contact
VAS from 0 = no sensation to 100 = very intense sensation between the fingertips; and (5) tracing figures with the index
(fMRI subgroup: mean 46.27, SD = 26.09, range: 0–92). finger in the manner of a concert conductor.
Patients experiencing a telescope (n = 8) reported a mean All these tasks were designed for maximum visibility of
intensity of 80.0 (SD = 26.4, range: 23–100) for this sensa- the movement while excluding any kind of tactile feedback.
tion, measured using VAS, ranging from 0 = no telescope to Patients were instructed to perform each task for 3 min
100 = very intense (fMRI subgroup: mean 89.50, SD = 9.01, (total: 15 min daily) and to record the actual amount of time
range: 77–100). they needed to perform each of the tasks on each day.
Over the course of the study, one patient (patient 1) used Patients were advised to keep the frequency of the hand
a steady amount of pain medication (a non-steroidal anti- movements constant (at approximately 0.5 Hz), but this was
inflammatory drug) everyday for 8 weeks of the study. Three not enforced; instead, patients were free to use the frequency
patients (3, 7 and 13) used analgesics occasionally, i.e., on 5 that best enabled them to relate the observed movement to
or fewer days spread over the entire 8 weeks of the study the movement of their phantom limb. They were instructed
(patient 3: an opioid, patients 7 and 13: a non-steroidal to abort any movement task if it intensified their pain level
anti-inflammatory drug). and to document if this happened. After explanation and
demonstration of all tasks, the patients practiced the tasks
themselves and received a detailed written explanation of
2.2 Design the movement tasks including colour photographs. No
patient reported a lack of understanding of or an inability to
Each patient gave daily reports of pain (using VAS) for a total
perform the mirrored movement task.
of 8 weeks, commencing 2 weeks prior to therapy (pre-
Patients received a large transportable mirror (30 × 50 cm
phase) to assess the baseline level of PLP, followed by a
or 12 × 20 in.) and were required to give a daily report on
4-week training phase with daily mirror exercises and con-
the intensity of PLP on that day in general (on VAS with the
cluding with 2 weeks without any treatment (but with daily
endpoints ‘no pain’ and ‘very intense pain’, ranging from 0
pain ratings; post-phase) to evaluate possible long-term
to 100) and the occurrence and intensity of PLP during the
effects of the therapy. On the day before the first training
training (on a scale from 0 for ‘none at all’ to 6 for ‘most
unit as well as after the last one, the patients’ brain activation
intense pain ever experienced’, taken from the MPI; Flor
was assessed: Patients performed (1) mirrored hand move-
et al., 1990). They were also asked daily about the degree to
ments and (2) a lip-pursing task during fMRI measurement
which they were able to relate the movement seen in the
(cf. Lotze et al., 2001; MacIver et al., 2008). During the same
mirror to their phantom limb, on a scale from 0 (not at all)
visit, patients were asked about the frequency, intensity and
to 6 (as vivid as a real perceptual experience), and whether
relieving factors for current and past PLP as well as current
they felt movement in the phantom limb (on the same scale
and past non-painful phantom sensations (including tele-
as the item before). Patients were contacted by phone at least
scoping) using a structured interview (Winter et al., 2001).
once a week to check for questions or problems, and were
For everyday of the study (pre-phase, training phase and
instructed to contact the experimenters if any problems
post-phase), patients were asked about any pain medication
should arise.
they were using.
The entire procedure of the study, except for the time
spent in the MRI scanner and the explanation of the fMRI
2.3 Mirror training measurement procedure, was identical for the patients with
and without fMRI data.
Patients received specific verbal and written instructions for
4-week mirror training during a one-on-one appointment
prior to the training phase. Patients were instructed to try to
2.4 MRI scanning session
consciously relate the movement observed in the mirror to
their phantom at any point during the training and to keep The MRI measurements took place at the beginning and at
their attention focused on the task. Each instruction was the end of the 4-week training period. During the first
explained verbally, demonstrated by a therapist and per- appointment, patients were informed about the purpose of
formed by the patient in front of her or him. Patients then the study and the exact movement tasks to perform in the
trained on their own everyday for a period of 4 weeks. Five MRI scanner. Before the measurement, patients were trained
different movement tasks were devised to improve patient to execute the requested hand movements with the intact
compliance and to reduce the risk of straining. The tasks hand without producing muscle activity in the residual arm
included (1) opening and closing of fingers: repeated con- (using an electromyography feedback procedure prior to the
verging of the fingertips, starting with a loosely opened first measurement; see Lotze et al., 2001). Lip and hand
hand, palm towards the mirror, but without any tactile movements were demonstrated to the patients and practiced
contact among the fingers or between fingertips and palm; by the patients before the measurement. During the fMRI
(2) stretching of fingers, with palm towards the mirror; (3) scan, the patients’ gaze was redirected using a mirror
turning the hand, so that the palm alternately faced upwards attached to the MRI head coil. This way, they could easily

© 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 731
Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain J. Foell et al.

view their intact hand lying on the abdomen, as well as its brain activation during lip-pursing and hand movement
reflection, which was produced using a mirror placed on the tasks, both before and after treatment, was observed, and
patient’s body. For the hand movement condition, patients differences in activation during hand movement, before and
were instructed to close their hand to a fist (yet without any after treatment, in areas related to the processing of body-
actual touching of the fingertips to the palm or between the related information and pain [primary and secondary soma-
fingers) and to open it again whenever they heard a sound tosensory cortex (S1 and S2), primary motor cortex (M1),
signal. This signal was presented with a frequency of 0.5 Hz anterior cingulate cortex, insula, parietal cortex] were cor-
during activity phases (on-block; duration: six scans or related with the benefit from therapy.
19.8 s), whereas no sound was given during the resting
phase (off-block; duration: six scans or 19.8 s), for which
patients were instructed to lie still, watching their hand and 2.6 Statistical analysis of clinical and
the mirror image. For the on-block, they were explicitly behavioural data
instructed to imagine their phantom hand moving in accor-
The average daily pain ratings were aggregated into weekly
dance with the reflected intact hand. For the lip-pursing
averages, and the mean ratings before training were then
condition, also assessed with an fMRI measurement, patients
compared with those after training using a one-tailed paired-
were asked to lie still with their eyes closed and to purse their
sample t-test. As a measure of effect size, Cohen’s d was
lips whenever they heard the sound signal (which was again
calculated using the means and SDs of the pain ratings before
presented with a frequency of 0.5 Hz during activity phases).
and after treatment. Connections between non-painful
phantom phenomena (i.e., telescoping and the feeling of
relatedness between phantom hand and mirrored hand, time
2.5 Image acquisition and analysis since amputation) and PLP as well as individual treatment
The fMRI scans were conducted with a 3 Tesla Siemens Trio effect were calculated using Spearman’s rank correlations.
MR (Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany) scanner using echo- Missing values caused by a patient skipping some daily
planar imaging (matrix 64 × 64, TE 45 ms, TR 3.3 s) and 40 ratings were replaced by the averaged value of the day before
slices of 2.3 mm thickness, tilted in accordance with the and the day after.
transverse plane adjusted to include all frontal, central, pari- Ratings about the relatedness between the mirror image
etal and occipital cortical areas as well as upper parts of the and the phantom hand were gathered after the performance
temporal cortex and the cerebellum. Seventy-eight whole- of each of the five tasks. For the depiction of results, these
brain scans including six blocks of hand or lip movements values were summed up over 3 days each. The averaged
with six scans each paired with seven blocks of six scans of value for the first 3 days of the training was taken as an
rest were gathered per condition, and the first three volumes indicator for the individual trait of the patient. A 3-day
were excluded from the analysis to allow for signal stability period was used in order to ensure that the rating was not
following onset transients. For anatomical reference, a influenced by either an initial first-day surprise effect or a
T1-weighted anatomical data set (magnetization-prepared training effect caused by the mirror treatment. Scores were
rapid acquisition with gradient echo; slice thickness 1.1 mm, averaged over the five tasks.
TR 2300 ms, TE 2.98 ms, flip angle 9°) was obtained.
fMRI data were evaluated using SPM8 (Wellcome Insti-
2.7 Brain changes related to treatment
tute of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK) implemented in
Matlab 7.1 (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). The echo- Several measures were used to assess treatment effects on
planar images of each subject were coregistered to the indi- neuronal activity. For both hand and lip stimulation, brain
vidual anatomical data sets after the anterior commissure activity during the task was observed before and after treat-
had been manually defined as the reference point. Further ment on a whole-brain level and when anatomical regions of
pre-processing included motion– and slice–time correction, interest (ROIs) were used. For the identification of the lip
normalization to the standard space using a template [Mon- and hand areas in S1 and M1, the functional ROIs defined by
treal Neurological Institute (MNI)] and smoothing with a MacIver et al. (2008) were used (MNI coordinates: lip S1:
Gaussian kernel of 8 mm3 (full width at half maximum) to ±58, −18, 24; lip M1: ±52, −8, 36; hand S1: ±34, −30, 58;
decrease spatial noise. For group-level analyses, the images hand M1: ±34, −34, 52). Differences in activation related to
acquired from patients with a right-hand amputation were MT were assessed using family-wise error correction. In
flipped (cf. Lotze et al., 2001; MacIver et al., 2008; Diers et al., addition, a multiple regression was performed, using pain
2010) so that the hemisphere unaffected by (i.e., ipsilateral reduction after treatment as a covariate in order to reveal
to) the amputation is always shown on the left. For the differences in activation related to treatment effects.
individual analyses of cortical reorganization (lip movement For the investigation of a shift in cortical lip representa-
data), this flip was not performed. The hemispheres of the tion, the analysis of the fMRI data of the lip-pursing task was
patients’ brains that were ipsilateral to the amputation used to determine the location of activity peaks in S1 and M1
served as control for the measured changes in cortical activ- using pre- and post-central ROIs (as defined by the Wake
ity since no pain-specific alterations were expected on the Forest University PickAtlas toolbox version 2.4 for SPM8;
intact hemisphere as a consequence of the study. Whole- Maldjian et al., 2003), both on the hemisphere affected by

732 Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 © 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC®
J. Foell et al. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain

the amputation and on the opposing hemisphere. The shift the training per day, and no patient dropped out of
in cortical organization caused by the amputation was treatment. Three participants skipped individual train-
defined as the Euclidean distance between the peak activity ing days because of time constraints or illness (patients
locations under the assumption that both locations lie in the 7 and 13 missed 1 day, patient 12 missed 2 non-
same hemisphere; this was carried out by multiplying the
consecutive days). Patients consistently performed all
x-coordinate of the left activity peak with −1, thereby effec-
five movement tasks for 3 min each with only minor
tively flipping the coordinates of the left side in MNI space
onto the right side (cf. MacIver et al., 2008). This procedure
deviations in training duration (patients 7, 11 and 12,
allowed us to estimate the position of the mouth represen- range of duration 2–4 min per task).
tation under healthy conditions and to calculate the distance
between this position and the affected position (see Elbert
et al., 1994; Karl et al., 2001). The calculation resulted in a
3.2 Pain
measure of the shift that had occurred on the contralateral
side of the cortex relative to the ipsilateral side represented as The patients reported stable pain ratings 2 weeks
the distance in millimetres, where the distance was calcu- before the training, with a decline after the first week
lated using the Euclidean distance formula. The individual of daily training. The average pain rating in the week
amount of this cortical shift was correlated with the indi-
after the training was significantly lower than that
vidual intensity of pain using Spearman’s rank correlation.
from the week before start of the training (week 1:
The degree of cortical shift as described above immediately
before the 4-week training was compared with the same
M = 28.21, SD = 11.52; week 2: M = 28.26,
value immediately after the training as a measure for the SD = 16.27; week 7: M = 20.60, SD = 12.80; week 8:
degree of change in cortical organization, which might have M = 23.44, SD = 13.04; week 2 vs. week 7: t12 = 1.78,
occurred as a result of MT. Cortical reorganization during the p = 0.05). Cohen’s d was 0.52, indicating a medium-
training phase was defined as the Euclidean distance between sized effect of treatment on pain scores. Individual
left and right activity peaks (as described above) before train- pain ratings before and after the mirror treatment are
ing minus that same distance after training. This results in the given in Supporting Information Table S2 and illus-
amount of relative change in location during the training trated in Fig. 1A; Fig. 1B shows the average pain
period given in millimetres and creates a measure of activity ratings before, during and after the mirror treatment.
shift on the affected side back towards a healthy state. Spear-
man’s rank correlation was used to investigate the connection
between this measure and pain ratings.
In addition, we calculated the cortical shift from the first to 3.2.1 fMRI data: Group-level analysis
the second measurement in the S1 and M1 regions of the
hemisphere ipsilateral to the amputation. This shift was then Lip movement task: At both time points, patients
checked for correlations with benefit from treatment. Since showed significant bilateral activation during lip
we hypothesized a specific effect of MT only on the affected pursing in S1, M1 and insular cortex. Significant brain
hemisphere, we expected no significant correlation for the activations both before and after treatment are
hemisphere ipsilateral to amputation site. described in Supporting Information Table S3.
In order to assess the contribution of the hand movement Mirrored hand movement task: The mirrored hand
(cf. Makin et al., 2013), we also defined functional hand ROIs movements caused significant bilateral activation in
using group means of M1 and S1 during hand movement
somatosensory and motor regions before and after
(contralateral to amputation site) and defined 5-mm spheri-
treatment, as well as in the insular cortex and, at the
cal ROIs around these coordinates. We used these ROIs to
extract the percentage signal change in these areas (using the
first time point, in the inferior parietal cortex (IPC)
REX ROI extraction toolbox version 2.1 for SPM8; and thalamus (Supporting Information Table S4).
Whitfield-Gabrieli, 2009). We correlated the intensity of acti- Both before and after treatment, activation in S1
vation at the first measurement with pain levels before cortex was less intense on the hemisphere affected by
therapy, the activation at the second measurement with pain the amputation. A paired-sample t-test revealed no
levels after therapy and the difference in activation intensi- significant pre-post change in the mirror task in S1 or
ties with benefit from treatment. M1. The multiple regression analysis showed a signifi-
cant connection between the individual amount of
pain reduction during the treatment and a decrease of
3. Results
activity in the IPC on the hemisphere affected by the
amputation over the course of therapy (p = 0.001,
3.1 Mirror training performance
uncorrected, peak activity: T = 7.31, Z = 4.08, MNI
The patients did not report any problems in perform- coordinates: x = 52, y = −38, z = 46; see Supporting
ing the mirrored movements or with the duration of Information Fig. S1).

© 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 733
Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain J. Foell et al.

A (Cohen’s d) of treatment showed no significant result


for M1 (r = 0.15, p = 0.34), whereas the amount of
Pre-training reduction in the cortical shift of S1 was found to be
Post-training
significantly positively correlated with the reduction
Pain rating (VAS, 0–100)

in PLP measured as the difference between the pre-


and post-phases (r = 0.75, p < 0.01). Fig. 3 shows a
scatter plot of this relationship.
The cortical shift in the control regions, i.e., S1 and
M1 in the hemisphere ipsilateral to the amputation,
was not significantly correlated with treatment benefit
(S1: r10 = 0.18, p = 0.59; M1: r10 = 0.16, p = 0.64).
The intensity of activation in the phantom hand
region during mirrored hand movements before the
Subject # training was not significantly correlated with
B pre-training pain (S1: r10 = −0.24, p = 0.48; M1:
Pre-phase Training phase Post-phase r10 = −0.37, p = 0.26), the same activation after the
Pain rating (VAS, 0–100)

A Pre
All patients (N = 11)
Post

Week

Figure 1 (A) Individual pre- and post-training pain ratings, ordered from
most to least benefit. Triangles denote individual patients without a tele-
scopic distortion of the phantom. (B) Daily pain ratings averaged over
weeks and over all participants. Error bars depict standard error.

3.2.2 fMRI data: Individual analysis


The initial measurement of cortical activation during B
the lip-pursing task showed an average dysfunctional Pre-training
cortical shift of 15.4 mm (SD = 9.6 mm) in S1 as well Post-training

as an average shift of 16.6 mm (SD = 6.7 mm) in M1.


The comparison of the cortical shift before and after
training revealed an average reduction of this shift of
2.9 mm (SD = 11.4 mm) for S1 and 1.5 mm
(SD = 10.5 mm) for M1. This difference is not statisti-
cally significant on a group level (Z = −0.889,
p = 0.37). Fig. 2 displays the difference in distribution
of cortical activity for all patients during the lip-
pursing task in S1 on the hemisphere that is affected
by the amputation before and after treatment
(Fig. 2A), as well as individual data for the amount of
dysfunctional shift in S1 before and after treatment Figure 2 (A) Neuronal activity in somatosensory cortex during lip-
(Fig. 2B). pursing task before (red) and after (blue) mirror therapy. (B) Amount of
The correlation between the individual amount of dysfunctional shift for individual patients before (red) and after (blue)
reduction in the shift and the individual effect size training, ordered from most to least benefit.

734 Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 © 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC®
J. Foell et al. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain

in pain over all patients was almost exclusively caused


by five participants without a telescope.
The feeling of being able to relate the observed
movement to the phantom limb did not significantly
differ between the five training tasks, with mean
values ranging from 1.74 to 1.95 (on a scale from 0 for
‘not at all’ to 6 for ‘as vivid as a real perceptual expe-
rience’), while SDs ranged from 1.61 to 1.77. Indi-
vidual participants differed in the mean value of this
item, with a range across nearly the entire scale (0.00–
5.91). Overall, the average intensity of this sensation
rose over the course of the training from 1.40
(SD = 1.62) on the first day of the training to 2.15
(SD = 1.89) on the last one; a difference bordering on
statistical significance (t12 = 1.76; p = 0.05 one tailed)
indicating a medium-sized effect (Cohen’s d = 0.43).
Figure 3 Correlation between cortical reorganization in S1 (positive The initial starting point of the patients for this item
values indicate shift in expected direction, i.e., towards healthy location) (measured as its average for the first 3 days of training)
and effect size of treatment. Empty circles indicate patients with telescop- varied widely, with a range from 0.00 to 5.80. There
ing, and filled circles patients without telescoping. was a strong positive correlation between individual
values of this 3-day average and the amount of pain
relief reported by the patients as a result of the training
training was not significantly correlated with (r = 0.873, p < 0.001).
post-training pain (S1: r10 = −0.40, p = 0.22; M1: At the beginning of training, those patients without
r10 = −0.46, p = 0.16), and the difference between the a telescopic distortion started out with a slightly higher
activations before and after was not significantly cor- intensity (not statistically significant) of the sensation
related with treatment benefit (S1: r10 = −0.24, of relatedness as described above, after which they
p = 0.47; M1: r10 = −0.17, p = 0.62). reported no significant change from the beginning to
the end of training (first 3 days: M = 2.29, SD = 2.16;
last 3 days: M = 2.36, SD = 2.38; t4 = −0.22, p = 0.42
3.2.3 Predictors of treatment effects
one tailed; Cohen’s d = 0.03). Patients with a tele-
There was no significant correlation between benefit scope, however, displayed a gradual increase in the
from treatment and time since amputation (r = −0.27,
p = 0.42). Eight of the 13 patients reported a telescopic
distortion of the phantom limb (see Supporting Infor-
Pre-phase Treatment phase Post-phase
mation Table S1). The perceived intensity of this tele-
scope was negatively correlated with the treatment
effect (r = −0.94, p < 0.01). Apart from the intensity of
Pain intensity (VAS 0–100)

the telescope, the mere presence of this phenomenon


had an influence on pain alleviation: Five patients
without a telescopic distortion reported an average of
51.23% decrease in PLP (pre-phase: M = 32.01,
SD = 14.34 vs. post-phase: M = 15.61, SD = 6.64;
t4 = 2.20, p < 0.05 one tailed; Cohen’s d = 1.64),
whereas the training had almost no effect (changes in
PLP ratings <1%) on the averaged PLP ratings of
patients with a telescope (pre-phase: M = 25.88,
SD = 12.60 vs. post-phase: M = 26.03, SD = 14.09, Week

t7 = −0.07, p = 0.47 one tailed; Cohen’s d = 0.01). Con-


sequently, both groups differed significantly in their
reduction in pain intensity after training (t11 = 2.14, Figure 4 Difference in pain development of patients with and without
p < 0.05 one tailed; Cohen’s d = 1.62). This difference telescopic distortion of the phantom limb. Error bars indicate standard
(shown in Fig. 4) indicates that the average alleviation error.

© 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 735
Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain J. Foell et al.

sensation, ranging from 0.73 (SD = 0.26) on the first 3 tion. In addition, input from the periphery was shown
days of training to 1.67 (SD = 0.59) on the last 3 days. to increase cortical reorganization especially when it is
The change in group average described above was random (Spitzer, 1997; Mackert et al., 2003). Overall,
almost entirely caused by patients with a telescope: these issues lead us to assume that methodological
For this group, there was a significant difference differences kept the effects of cortical reorganization
between the first and the last 3 days of training from being found by that group. Their further finding
(t7 = −2.21, p < 0.05 one tailed; Cohen’s d = 2.06). relating PLP to disrupted connectivity and grey matter
volume is highly interesting and promotes further dis-
cussion about the mechanisms behind the disorder.
4. Discussion
In accordance with previous work (Weeks et al.,
One aim was to determine whether MT is effective in 2010; Darnall and Li, 2012), not all patients reported
chronic PLP patients. Overall, treatment led to a pain pain alleviation after treatment. Our data suggest that
reduction of 27%, a medium-sized effect. This extends this is connected to the ability to relate the mirrored
earlier findings in acute amputees (Chan et al., 2007) movement to their phantom: Although the patients,
to chronic PLP. However, our results also suggest that on average, reported an increase in the intensity of
only some patients profit from MT, and that individual this sensation during training, they started out very
differences determine treatment effectiveness. differently. This suggests that the ability to relate the
Treatment effects were connected to cortical reorga- mirror image to the phantom is a characteristic that
nization: As PLP decreased, the representation in the patients bring into the therapy. The concept of the
somatosensory cortices of both hemispheres became ‘body matrix’ (Moseley et al., 2012) proposes that
more similar, partially recreating their presumed activity in specific cortical regions (including S1) is
normal state. For motor cortex, no connection was related to the ability to subjectively incorporate a limb
found. MacIver et al. (2008) investigated the effects of not belonging to one’s body. This embodiment has
mental imagery on PLP and found similar changes, been found to vary between individuals while being
including more concise activation after treatment. stable over time (Bekrater-Bodmann et al., 2012). It
Makin et al. (2013) recently hypothesized that pre- has also been argued that integration of a foreign body
served hand function rather than dysfunctional reor- part, rather than movement of the phantom, is nec-
ganization (Flor et al., 1995) in S1 is positively essary for pain relief (Schmalzl et al., 2011). Here, the
associated with PLP, suggesting that an effective PLP relatedness towards the mirrored hand at the begin-
treatment should be accompanied by reduced activity ning of treatment was predictive of pain relief, also
in the hand representation in S1 rather than a reloca- suggesting that the incorporation of the mirrored
tion of cortical lip representation. However, we found hand, i.e., the feeling of observing one’s own hand, is
no significant alterations in S1 activity representing the necessary for treatment benefit.
hand after training, and no significant correlation Farrer et al. (2003) investigated agency (‘the feeling
between activation changes in the hand region and the that leads us to attribute an action to ourselves’) over a
treatment effect. Instead, we found the already virtual hand and examined neural correlates. When
reported shift in cortical lip localization. Makin et al. the feeling of relatedness was reduced by distorting the
had patients perform lip movements comparable with image, activation in IPC was present. The intensity of
the ones in this study and found no connection this activation increased along as reported relatedness
between PLP and neural activation. Their finding of decreased. Interrupting agency elicits activation in the
continuing activation in the region previously repre- same area (Yomogida et al., 2010). Our results support
senting the now-amputated hand seems to contradict these findings, since we observed activation in this
the model of maladaptive cortical reorganization. region to be higher before treatment, when relatedness
However, a combination of conceptual and method- to the mirror image was lowest. We found this change
ological factors suggests that these viewpoints are in activation to be directly associated with pain relief.
complementary rather than contradictory (Flor et al., The same area has been connected to pain processing
2013). Makin et al. (2013) used the intensity of activa- (Dunckley et al., 2005; Cauda et al., 2009; Uematsu
tion, whereas cortical reorganization mainly relates to et al., 2011) and may be part of a neuronal network
its location. Furthermore, the direction of this shift can involved in the perpetuation of neuropathic pain.
vary between persons, diminishing the reliability of an These results identify IPC as being connected both to
averaged approach: In their analysis, activation during disruption of agency over a body part and painful
movement in PLP patients was weaker and less spread somatic sensations and emphasize its role regarding the
out, consistent with averaged heterogeneous activa- resiliency of PLP and treatment effectiveness. A reduc-

736 Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 © 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC®
J. Foell et al. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain

tion of IPC activation by providing the brain with activity suggest a mechanism of MT that goes beyond
agency over the mirrored hand may thus be an alter- a placebo effect. Also, it is unclear what might be an
native or additional neuronal mechanism of MT. appropriate placebo control: It is interesting to note
However, these mechanisms require that patients that mental visualization, which was found to be an
accept the mirrored hand as their own. We found a effective form of treatment by MacIver et al. (2008)
large variation in this acceptance, and our results and Mercier and Sirigu (2009), was used as a control
suggest a possible reason: Patients with a telescope had by Chan et al. (2007). Moreover, in a previous study
virtually no benefit from treatment, whereas those on sensory discrimination training, an attention
without a telescope reported an average pain allevia- placebo condition was ineffective (Flor et al., 2001).
tion of over 50%, and all three patients with a major For these reasons, even though the effect that we
treatment effect (i.e., pain reduction of more than a found for cortical reorganization was in line with prior
third from baseline) belonged to this group. Also, tele- findings in terms of its effect size, directionality and
scope vividness was negatively correlated with treat- location on the cortex, and even though its theoretical
ment benefit. The possible interaction of telescoping mechanism was corroborated by the findings regard-
and MT has not yet been studied, but it seems plau- ing parietal activation and telescoping, the influence
sible that a distortion or displacement of the phantom of a possible placebo effect cannot be excluded. We
limb on a proprioceptive level causes a contradiction if assumed that the hemisphere ipsilateral to the ampu-
the patient is confronted with an intact visual repre- tation would be unimpaired and did not undergo
sentation of that limb. We recently found that incon- changes due to cortical reorganization. Subjects thus
gruence between a mirrored and an actual arm can acted as their own controls. This use of the unimpaired
lead to an illusory third arm (Foell et al., 2013), sug- hemisphere as a control is a limitation, since it may be
gesting that the incongruent mirror image is not per- possible that the treatment causes changes on both
ceived as a representation of the hidden arm. In hemispheres. However, we found no significant cor-
amputees, the difference between a deformed relation between the cortical shift in the unimpaired
phantom and the non-deformed mirror image of an hemisphere and treatment benefit, suggesting that
intact arm may likewise lead to non-integration, with any changes in this hemisphere did not systematically
the mirror image perceived as an additional entity, influence our baseline. The sample sizes for behav-
unrelated to the phantom. This non-integration might ioural and fMRI measurements that were used were
impede treatment benefit. Telescoping might thus be a relatively small. However, we found significant effects
predictor for the effectiveness of MT. for both, with a medium overall effect size for treat-
It might be possible to circumvent this influence of ment effectiveness. A study using a larger sample size
a telescope by introducing a training system that takes might detect additional differences in response to MT.
the distortion into account. One solution is a virtual This study shows that mirror treatment for PLP has
reality (VR) set-up capable of fitting the virtual image a measurable effect on pain ratings of chronic PLP
to the distorted phantom. Since we have shown that patients, and that this benefit is correlated with
the ability to include the mirrored hand increases changes in neural activation, particularly with a
during training in patients with a telescope, we reduction of dysfunctional cortical reorganization in
assume that this ability can be improved. Recently, our S1. Furthermore, we found that treatment effective-
group introduced and tested a VR-based controllable ness depended on an ability to relate the mirror image
hand for use inside an MRI scanner, which induces a to one’s phantom. This finding is supported by a
feeling of ownership (Bach et al., 2012; Trojan et al., in decrease in cortical activity in IPC, an area connected
press). The role of telescoping, however, needs to be to a feeling of agency and pain generation. A telescopic
further examined. It should be noted that dividing our distortion of the phantom seems to be a determining
sample according to telescoping creates very small factor for treatment benefit.
groups and has happened post hoc. Further investiga-
tions need to replicate these findings.
This study has some limitations. No control group Author contributions
without treatment or with an alternate treatment was
All authors of this paper were involved in the conception and
used. Thus, placebo effects or spontaneous changes of design of this study as well as the analysis and interpretation
PLP cannot be excluded (although the latter are of data. Further, all authors revised the manuscript for
unlikely given the chronic condition of the patients). important intellectual content and gave final approval of the
However, our results on reorganizational alterations in version to be published. Results were discussed among all
S1 and the correlation between pain relief and IPC authors, and all authors commented on the manuscript.

© 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 737
Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain J. Foell et al.

References Karl, A., Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., Cohen, L.G., Flor, H. (2001).
Reorganization of motor and somatosensory cortex in upper extremity
Bach, F., Cakmak, H., Maass, H., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Foell, J., Diers, amputees with phantom limb pain. J Neurosci 21, 3609–3618.
M., Trojan, J., Fuchs, X., Flor, H. (2012). Illusory hand ownership Kerns, R.D., Turk, D.C., Rudy, T.E. (1985). The West Haven-Yale Multi-
induced by an MRI compatible immersive virtual reality device. Biomed dimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI). Pain 23, 345–356.
Tech (Berl) 57, 718–720. Koppelstaetter, F., Siedentopf, C.M., Rhomberg, P., Lechner-Steinleitner,
Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Foell, J., Diers, M., Flor, H. (2012). The perceptual S., Mottaghy, F.M., Eisner, W., Golaszewski, S.M. (2007). [Functional
and neuronal stability of the rubber hand illusion across contexts and magnetic resonance imaging before motor cortex stimulation for
over time. Brain Res 1452, 130–139. phantom limb pain]. Nervenarzt 78, 1435–1439.
Birbaumer, N., Lutzenberger, W., Montoya, P., Larbig, W., Unertl, K., Lloyd, D.M. (2007). Spatial limits on referred touch to an alien limb may
Topfner, S., Grodd, W., Taub, E., Flor, H. (1997). Effects of regional reflect boundaries of visuo-tactile peripersonal space surrounding the
anesthesia on phantom limb pain are mirrored in changes in cortical hand. Brain Cogn 64, 104–109.
reorganization. J Neurosci 17, 5503–5508. Lotze, M., Flor, H., Grodd, W., Larbig, W., Birbaumer, N. (2001). Phantom
Brodie, E.E., Whyte, A., Waller, B. (2003). Increased motor control of a movements and pain. An fMRI study in upper limb amputees. Brain
phantom leg in humans results from the visual feedback of a virtual leg. 124, 2268–2277.
Neurosci Lett 341, 167–169. MacIver, K., Lloyd, D.M., Kelly, S., Roberts, N., Nurmikko, T. (2008).
Cauda, F., D’Agata, F., Sacco, K., Duca, S., Cocito, D., Paolasso, I., Isoardo, Phantom limb pain, cortical reorganization and the therapeutic effect of
G., Geminiani, G. (2009). Altered resting state attentional networks in mental imagery. Brain 131, 2181–2191.
diabetic neuropathic pain. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 81, 806–811. Mackert, B.M., Sappok, T., Grüsser, S., Flor, H., Curio, G. (2003). The
Chan, B.L., Witt, R., Charrow, A.P., Magee, A., Howard, R., Pasquina, P.F., eloquence of silent cortex: Analysis of afferent input to deafferented
Heilman, K.M., Tsao, J.W. (2007). Mirror therapy for phantom limb cortex in arm amputees. Neuroreport 14, 409–412.
pain. N Engl J Med 357, 2206–2207. Makin, T.R., Scholz, J., Filippini, N., Henderson Slater, D., Tracey, I.,
Darnall, B.D., Li, H. (2012). Home-based self-delivered mirror therapy for Johansen-Berg, H. (2013). Phantom pain is associated with preserved
phantom pain: A pilot-study. J Rehabil Med 44, 254–260. structure and function in the former hand area. Nat Commun 4, 1570.
Diers, M., Christmann, C., Koeppe, C., Ruf, M., Flor, H. (2010). Mirrored, doi: 10.1038/ncomms2571.
imagined and executed movements differentially activate sensorimotor Maldjian, J.A., Laurienti, P.J., Kraft, R.A., Burdette, J.H. (2003). An auto-
cortex in amputees with and without phantom limb pain. Pain 149, mated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based
296–304. interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 1233–1239.
Dunckley, P., Wise, R.G., Aziz, Q., Painter, D., Brooks, J., Tracey, I., Mercier, C., Sirigu, A. (2009). Training with virtual visual feedback to
Chang, L. (2005). Cortical processing of visceral and somatic stimula- alleviate phantom limb pain. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 23, 587–594.
tion: Differentiating pain intensity from unpleasantness. Neuroscience Moseley, G.L., Gallace, A., Spence, C. (2012). Bodily illusions in health
133, 533–542. and disease: Physiological and clinical perspectives and the concept of a
Elbert, T., Flor, H., Birbaumer, N., Knecht, S., Hampson, S., Larbig, W., cortical ‘body matrix’. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36, 34–46.
Taub, E. (1994). Extensive reorganization of the somatosensory cortex Ramachandran, V.S., Rogers-Ramachandran, D.C. (1996). Synaesthesia in
in adult humans after nervous system injury. Neuroreport 5, 2593–2597. phantom limbs induced with mirrors. Proc Biol Sci 263, 377–386.
Farrer, C., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Frith, C.D., Decety, J., Jeannerod, M. Schley, M.T., Wilms, P., Toepfner, S., Schaller, H.P., Schmelz, M., Konrad,
(2003). Modulating the experience of agency: A positron emission C.J., Birbaumer, N. (2008). Painful and nonpainful phantom and stump
tomography study. Neuroimage 18, 324–333. sensations in acute traumatic amputees. J Trauma 65, 858–864.
Flor, H., Denke, C., Schaefer, M., Grusser, S. (2001). Effect of sensory Schmalzl, L., Thomke, E., Ragnö, C., Nilseryd, M., Stockselius, A.,
discrimination training on cortical reorganisation and phantom limb Ehrsson, H.H. (2011). ‘Pulling telescoped phantoms out of the stump’:
pain. Lancet 357, 1763–1764. Manipulating the perceived position of phantom limbs using a full-body
Flor, H., Diers, M., Andoh, J. (2013). The neural basis of phantom limb illusion. Front Hum Neurosci 5, 121.
pain. Trends Cogn Sci 17, 307–308. Spitzer, M. (1997). Noise-driven neuroplasticity in self-organizing feature
Flor, H., Elbert, T., Knecht, S., Wienbruch, C., Pantev, C., Birbaumer, N., maps: A neurocomputational model of phantom limbs. MD Comput 14,
Larbig, W., Taub, E. (1995). Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual corre- 192–199.
late of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature 375, Trojan, J., Diers, M., Fuchs, X., Bach, F., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Foell, J.,
482–484. Kamping, S., Rance, M., Maass, H., Flor, H. (in press). An augmented
Flor, H., Nikolajsen, L., Staehelin Jensen, T. (2006). Phantom limb pain: A reality home-training system based on the mirror and imagery treat-
case of maladaptive CNS plasticity? Nat Rev Neurosci 7, 873–881. ment approach. Behav Res Methods.
Flor, H., Rudy, T.E., Birbaumer, N., Streit, B., Schugens, M.M. (1990). Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: A neurocognitive model of
[The applicability of the West Haven-Yale multidimensional pain inven- body-ownership. Neuropsychologia 48, 703–712.
tory in German-speaking countries. Data on the reliability and validity Uematsu, H., Shibata, M., Miyauchi, S., Mashimo, T. (2011). Brain
of the MPI-D.]. Schmerz 4, 82–87. imaging of mechanically induced muscle versus cutaneous pain. Neu-
Foell, J., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Flor, H., Cole, J. (2011). Phantom limb rosci Res 70, 78–84.
pain after lower limb trauma: Origins and treatments. Int J Low Extrem Weeks, S.R., Anderson-Barnes, V.C., Tsao, J.W. (2010). Phantom limb
Wounds 10, 224–235. pain: Theories and therapies. Neurologist 16, 277–286.
Foell, J., Bekrater-Bodmann, R., McCabe, C., Flor, H. (2013). Sensorimo- Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2009). Region of Interest Extraction (REX) Toolbox
tor incongruence and body perception: An experimental investigation. (Boston, MA: MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences).
Front Hum Neurosci 7, 310. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00310. Winter, C., Fritsche, K., Karl, A., Huse, E., Labig, W., Grusser, S.M., Flor,
Giummarra, M.J., Gibson, S.J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Bradshaw, J.L. H. (2001). [The phantom and stump phenomena interview (PSPI)].
(2007). Central mechanisms in phantom limb perception: The past, Schmerz 15, 172–178.
present and future. Brain Res Rev 54, 219–232. Yomogida, Y., Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Wakusawa, K., Sekiguchi, A.,
Hanley, M.A., Ehde, D.M., Jensen, M., Czerniecki, J., Smith, D.G., Fukushima, A., Takeuchi, H., Horie, K., Sato, S., Kawashima, R. (2010).
Robinson, L.R. (2009). Chronic pain associated with upper-limb loss. The neural basis of agency: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 50, 198–207.
Am J Phys Med Rehabil 88, 742–751.
Hunter, J.P., Katz, J., Davis, K.D. (2003). The effect of tactile and visual
sensory inputs on phantom limb awareness. Brain 126, 579–589. Supporting Information
Jensen, T.S., Krebs, B., Nielsen, J., Rasmussen, P. (1983). Phantom limb,
phantom pain and stump pain in amputees during the first 6 months Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
following limb amputation. Pain 17, 243–256. online version of this article at the publisher’s web-site:

738 Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 © 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC®
J. Foell et al. Mirror therapy for phantom limb pain

Figure S1. Differences in cortical activity before and after Table S1. Details of participants.
treatment phase in correlation with the individual change in Table S2. Individual scores for phantom limb pain and dys-
pain ratings from pre- to post-therapy phase (p = 0.001 functional cortical shift before and after treatment.
uncorrected, voxel threshold: 10 voxels). MNI coordinates of Table S3. Activation during lip movement.
peak activity: x = 52, y = −38, z = 46; T = 7.31; Z = 4.08; Table S4. Activation during hand movement.
cluster size: 29 voxels.

© 2013 The Authors. European Journal of Pain published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of European Pain Federation - EFIC® Eur J Pain 18 (2014) 729–739 739

You might also like