You are on page 1of 5

Accepted: 7 July 2016

DOI: 10.1111/ane.12652

REVIEW ARTICLE

Differential diagnosis, discerning depression from cognition

E. Portaccio

Department of NEUROFARBA, University of
Florence, Florence, Italy
Cognitive impairment is common in multiple sclerosis (MS), affecting up to 70% of
patients. One of the most important possible confounders to cognitive assessment is
Correspondence the occurrence of depression, a common consequence of MS. Cognition and depres-
E. Portaccio, Department of NEUROFARBA,
University of Florence, Florence, Italy. sion have been linked in recent neuropsychiatric research that proposed a number of
Email: portilio@tin.it neurocognitive models of mood disorders. According to these models, primary failure
of key brain regions of emotional processing and regulation or abnormal connectivity
between them contributes to the adoption of maladaptive cognitive strategies and the
development of mood disorders. In MS, a similar interplay between cognitive function
and depression has been reported. In particular, depression seems to alter attentional
capacity in terms of deficits in working memory and, more specifically, deficits in the
executive control. However, cognitive impairment in MS does exist also in the absence
of depression and it is more likely that depression exacerbates existing cognitive dif-
ficulties rather than cause them per se. On the other hand, it is possible that a dysex-
ecutive syndrome secondary to MS might in turn precipitate depression.

KEYWORDS
cognitive impairment, depression, multiple sclerosis

Cognitive impairment is a common and disabling consequence of administration and interpretation by experienced neuropsychologists.
1,2
multiple sclerosis (MS), affecting up to 70% of patients. Cognitive More recently, brief assessment tools have been developed. In par-
functions most frequently involved are attention and information pro- ticular, a Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS15 has been
cessing speed, learning and memory, executive functions. Cognitive recommended as an international, validated and standardized brief
impairment is detectable at all stages of the disease, even in the “pre- cognitive test.
3
symptomatic” radiologically isolated syndromes and in the pediatric
cases.4–6 It tends to progress over time, independently of the accu-
mulation of physical disability, and is one of the major determinants of 1. |  CONFOUND ERS TO
the disease burden.7 Indeed, it is a predictor of health-­related quality COGNITIVE EVALUATION
of life and unemployment and reduces competence in daily activities,
coping, symptom management and medication adherence.7 While A number of confounders must be taken into account when ­assessing
the correlations between cognitive impairment and clinical variables cognitive evaluation in MS. In the seminal consensus approach, held
are relatively poor in both adult and pediatric patients with MS,8–10 in 2002, proposing the MACFIMS,14 the expert panel identified the
quantitative magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) parameters of brain main factors affecting neuropsychological test interpretation. They
tissue damage and atrophy provide stronger relationships.11,12 Over include premorbid intellectual ability, fatigue, neurological sensory-­
the last decades, several neuropsychological batteries have been pro- motor impairments, comorbidities, side effects of therapies and
posed for cognitive assessment in MS. The most commonly applied depression.14
13
are the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological tests and the As for the role of premorbid ability, recent research pointed to a
Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in MS (MACFIMS)14 While twofold relationship with cognitive functioning. On the one hand, as
these batteries are highly accurate in MS patients, their implementa- underscored by the consensus panel, premorbid intellectual ability can
tion in clinical practice is limited as they are time-­consuming and need be considered as a confounder. At this regard, the administration of

14  | © 2016 John Wiley & Sons A/S.


wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ane Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 2016; 134 (Suppl. 200): 14–18
Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Portaccio |
      15

a culturally appropriate measure is recommended, such as the North


2. |  MOOD DISORDERS,
American Adult Reading Test and the National Adult Reading Test in
DEPRESSION AND MS
the UK and Australia, selected verbal subtests from the WAIS.14
On the other hand, premorbid intellectual ability and intellectu-
Emotional disorders are highly prevalent in MS.27 Mental illness in
al enrichment can be considered as a protective factor, in the con-
MS patients has been related to reduced functioning, and quality of
text of the cognitive reserve theory. The concept of reserve has been
life, decreased treatment adherence and increased risk of suicide.27
developed to account for the discrepancies between the degree of
Relative to the general population, lifetime prevalence rates are
brain damage or pathology and its clinical manifestations.16 Recently,
elevated for major depressive disorder (36%–54% vs 16.2%), bipo-
reserve has been classified into passive and active models. Brain
lar disorder (13% vs 1%–4.5%), anxiety disorders (35.7% vs 28.8%),
reserve17 is an example of a passive model, where reserve derives
adjustment disorders (22% vs 0.2%–2.3%), and psychotic disorders
from brain size or neuronal count. Active models, such as the theory
(2%–3% vs 1.8%), whereas suicide may be at least twice as common.27
of cognitive reserve, rely on the idea that individual differences in how
The etiology of mood disorders in MS is multifactorial. Factors that
tasks are processed or neural networks are used can allow some peo-
may contribute to depression include MS-­related physical disability,
ple to cope better than others with brain pathology.16 In adult-­onset
the disease course, pain, fatigue, anxiety, alcohol abuse, and cognitive
MS, several cross-­sectional studies showed that both heritable (larg-
function.28
er maximal lifetime brain growth—brain reserve) and environmental
(greater intellectual enrichment—cognitive reserve) factors may atten-
uate the negative effects of MS on cognitive status.18,19 Moreover, in
a longitudinal study, although higher cognitive reserve can mediate 3. |  DEPRESSION AND
between cognitive performance and brain atrophy with disease pro- COGNITIVE FUNCTION
gression, the protective effect of cognitive reserve can be suppressed
by accumulating brain atrophy.20 Similar results have been obtained in Beyond MS, the overlap between cognition and neural function in
21
pediatric-­onset MS as well. In a 5-­year follow-­up longitudinal study, mechanisms underpinning mood disorders has been increasingly rec-
37.6% of patients had deteriorating cognitive performance, particu- ognized in recent research.29 Maladaptive cognition is the primary tar-
larly in subjects with lower levels of cognitive reserve. The protective get in cognitive behavioral therapy for mood disorders. In depression,
role of cognitive reserve disappeared when focusing on subjects who the cognitive model proposed by Beck is grounded in the theory that
were cognitively impaired at baseline, whereas it was stronger in sub- depressed individuals have maladaptive schemas that is beliefs about
jects classified as cognitively preserved, who started out the study oneself involving hopelessness, rejection, failure, and worthlessness.
with higher cognitive reserve. Therefore, cognitive reserve appears These schemas act as a filter through which depressed individuals
to operate particularly when cognitive functioning is still relatively process the external word, reinforcing their negative beliefs about
preserved. 21
themselves.29 These are not neurobiological cognitive impairments
As for fatigue, its impact is difficult to distinguish due to extensive per se, but instead dysfunctional thought patterns sustained by mood,
influence by other MS-­related factors. Fatigue is one of the most com- congruent with prevailing emotions. An alternative concept is that of
22
mon and disabling symptoms of MS, affecting up to 90% of patients. emotion regulation, which recognizes cognition as an important con-
It has been demonstrated that disease severity, depression, and tributory component of mood disorders.30 Emotion can be viewed
23
sleep disturbance all independently predicted fatigue in MS. As for as a perception–valuation–action sequence, in which input from the
the relationship with cognitive functioning, several studies provided external or internal world is perceived and valued and then triggers
strong evidence of an association with alertness/vigilance, whereas an action that alters the external or internal world.30 Neuroimaging
the relation was weak or absent with memory performance, cogni- studies consistently showed that, at the neural level, this process
tive speed, selective attention, language, visuospatial processing, and involves highly evolutionarily conserved subcortical systems, such as
working memory.24 On the basis of these data, it has been recently the amygdala, ventral striatum and periaqueductal gray, as well as a
proposed that fatigue may be considered as a feeling that becomes set of cortical regions (more elaborated in primates) that include the
behaviorally evident when a patient is forced to rely on specific cogni- anterior insula and dorsal anterior cingulated30 (Fig. 1). Emotion regu-
24
tive processes, in particular on alerting/vigilance tasks. lation refers to implementation of conscious or non-­conscious modu-
Among other potential confounders to cognitive performance lation of the trajectory of an emotional response. As for emotional
in MS, the possibility of transient cognitive decline has to be taken reactions, emotion regulation involves a perception–valuation–action
into account. It has been demonstrated that cognitive ability tran- sequence, triggered when the emotional response itself becomes the
siently declined during a relapse without cognitive characteristics.25 target of valuation.30 Explicit regulation of emotion requires conscious
More recently, the occurrence of isolated cognitive relapses has been effort and active monitoring and thus is associated with some level of
26
described. In asymptomatic patients, without any change in physical insight and awareness. Meta-­analyses of neuroimaging studies have
examination, an at least partially reversible cognitive worsening was found that this type of emotion regulation is associated with activa-
found, in association with gadolinium enhancing lesions in frontopari- tion of various brain regions, namely the frontoparietal executive net-
etal regions at brain MRI scans.26 work—including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the ventrolateral
|
16       Portaccio

key regions or to abnormalities in the connectivity and coordination


Explicit regulation Implicit regulation
vlPFC vACC between regulatory and emotional-­reactivity regions.29,30
dlPFC vmPFC It is worth noting that the involvement of the same regions has
Parietal cortex been consistently related to depressed mood in MS subjects. Indeed,
Pre-SMA and SMA depression has been related to increased lesion load in arcuate fas-
ciculus area, dominant medial inferior frontal regions and dominant
anterior temporal areas, atrophy in frontal lobes and third and lateral
ventricles, smaller volume of the hippocampus in the dentate gyrus.28
Emotional processing Two studies are particular relevant to what has been reported
Amygdala
in neuropsychiatric research. In the first, 12 relapsing-­remitting MS
Insula
dACC patients without depression or cognitive impairment and 12 healthy
PAG subjects underwent brain functional MRI during an emotional pro-
cessing task.31 The Authors’ goal was to unmask differences in emo-
F I G U R E   1   Brain regions involved in emotion regulation. vlPFC, tional processing and adaptive reorganizations in MS patients without
ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; affective and cognitive disorders. In contrast to controls, MS subjects
SMA, supplementary motor area; vACC, ventral anterior cingulated displayed a lack of functional connectivity between amygdala and
cortex; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; dACC, dorsal anterior
both the ventrolateral and prefrontal cortices, while increased regional
cingulated cortex; PAG, periacqueductal gray
activation was seen in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, possibly as a
compensatory strategy.31
prefrontal cortex and the parietal cortex—as well as the insula, sup- In the second study on 69 cognitively intact patients with MS and
plementary motor area and presupplementary motor area30 (Fig. 1). 42 matched controls, Rocca MA and Colleagues aimed at assessing the
Implicit regulation is characterized by the absence of an explicit mechanisms leading to hippocampal dysfunction in MS and its rele-
instruction and conscious monitoring and can happen without insight vance for depression.32 The working hypothesis was that focal lesions
30
and awareness. In these paradigms, neural activation is consistently in the white matter may contribute, through a disconnection mecha-
observed in the ventral anterior cingulated cortex and the ventrome- nism, to hippocampal dysfunction and depression.
30
dial prefrontal cortex (Fig. 1). They found that, compared to healthy controls, patients with MS
In recent years, neurocognition has been intensively studied using showed significant atrophy of the whole brain and left hippocampus,
functional neuroimaging and several different models have been as well as a distributed pattern of decreased resting-­state functional
proposed.29 These models rely on a number of cognitive process- connectivity between the hippocampi and several cortical–subcortical
es involved in mood disorders, such as reappraisal and suppression, regions.
attention, and rumination. Reduced hippocampal resting-­state functional connectivity with
Reappraisal derives from an explicit alteration of the self-­relevant regions of the default-­mode network was strongly correlated with
meaning (an appraisal) of an emotion-­inducing stimulus decreasing the higher lesion volume, longer disease duration, and the severity of
29
level of negative affect associated with the stimulus. On the oth- depression and disability, suggesting a disconnection syndrome.32
er hand, the attempt to control the emotional response by inhibiting
the expression of emotion rather than to change its value is defined
as suppression.29 In the field of mood disorders, attention can be 4. |  DEPRESSION AND COGNITIVE
conceptualized as two separate but related components: attentional FUNCTION IN MS
deployment and attentional control. Attentional deployment refers to
the allocation of attention with the aim of influencing an emotional From the previous sections, it emerges the broad interplay between cog-
state or response. Patients with mood disorders selectively attend to nition and emotional disorders. Moreover, brain regions involved in dys-
29
negatively valenced stimuli. The top-­down regulation of attention functional emotion regulation and mood disorders greatly overlap with
(attentional control) is central for attention shifting from emotional to key regions affected in MS-­related cognitive impairment.11 With this
29
non-­emotional stimuli. Attention deficits are likely to contribute to background, it is straightforward to wonder whether there is a relation-
the extended processing of negative information in depressed patients. ship between depression and cognitive functioning in MS and whether
Strictly related to the prevailing processing of negative stimuli is the there is a specific profile of depression-­related cognitive impairment.
process of rumination, defined as persistently focusing on one’s inter- As for the first question, in 2008 Arnett PA and Colleagues reviewed
nal state as a means of coping with distress.29 Cognitive strategies can the main 22 studies investigating on the relationship between cog-
be broadly grouped as maladaptive (for instance suppression, rumina- nitive dysfunction and depression in MS patients.33 On the whole,
tion) and adaptive (for instance reappraisal). Within mood disorders, twelve of those had negative findings. However, these studies were
individuals increasingly use maladaptive cognitive strategies, instead conducted on small samples, using scales with overlapping vegeta-
of adaptive emotion regulation strategies. It is not clear whether emo- tive symptoms and cognitive tests with low sensitivity. Patients with
tion regulatory impairments are primarily due to a failure to activate moderate-­to-­severe depression were also excluded. In contrast, the
Portaccio |
      17

ten studies that did show a relationship between cognitive dysfunc- Depression Dysexecutive
tion and depression had greater sample size included a more hetero- syndrome
geneous group of MS patients and compared depressed patients with
non-­depressed. The review authors concluded that mood disorders do F I G U R E   2   The relationship between cognition and depression in
indeed impact cognitive function in MS.33 multiple sclerosis
As for the second question, that is whether the impact of mood
disorders on cognition has a specific profile in MS, the majority of ventral anterior cingulate and ventromedial prefrontal cortex), or to an
information is due to the elegant studies by Arnett PA and Colleagues. abnormal connectivity between these areas.29,30 Similar findings have
The starting hypothesis came from the results of an old meta-­analysis been reported in patients with MS: depression and emotion dysregula-
of ten studies looking for an association between depression and cog- tion have been associated with both primary involvement of key brain
nitive performance.34 The Authors found that depressed MS patients regions (atrophy) and reduced functional connectivity between them
performed worse on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, suggest- secondary to white matter focal lesion load (disconnection syndrome)28
ing that depression either slowed speed of information processing With appropriate patient selection, depression and cognitive perfor-
34
and/or impaired working memory. In a first study, Arnett PA and mance were found to be strictly related in MS as it occurs in neuropsy-
Colleagues expanded on this assessing 20 depressed MS patients, 41 chiatric research. At the behavioral level, depression seems to alter
non-­depressed MS patients and eight non-­depressed healthy controls attentional capacity in terms of deficits in working memory and, more
on a battery of capacity-­demanding and non-­capacity-­demanding specifically, deficits in executive function (Fig. 2).36 This interaction is
tasks.35 not surprising considering the overlap between emotional regulatory
Results showed that depressed patients with MS performed sig- regions and the executive network and the results of neuroimaging
nificantly worse than both non-­depressed groups on some capacity-­ studies in depressed patients with MS reporting an involvement of
demanding tasks (namely the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, the frontal areas.28,30 On the other hand, it is possible that the converse
Symbol Digit Modalities Test and the Visual Elevator Task) but neither occurs: a dysexecutive syndrome secondary to MS might in turn pre-
on the other capacity-­demanding tasks (the California Verbal Learning cipitate depression altering the regulation of emotional processing
Test and the 7/24 Spatial Recall Test) nor on non-­capacity-­demanding thus favouring the persistent use of maladaptive cognitive strategies
tasks.35 Possible explanations of these findings were as follows: (i) that (Fig. 2).36 Indeed, it has to be noted that cognitive impairment in MS
cognitive capacity in general is not affected by depression in patients does exist also in the absence of depression and that it is more likely
with MS; (ii) that depression may be associated with an involvement of that depression exacerbate existing cognitive difficulties rather than
discrete cognitive functions such as verbal working memory capacity cause them per se. In the majority of studies assessing cognitive func-
or executive dysfunction reducing the access to verbal working mem- tion in MS, the presence of significant mood disorders is considered as
ory; and (iii) that the findings were the consequence of psychomotor an exclusion criterion. In these conditions, cognitive impairment affects
slowing.35,36 In a further study involving the same groups of subjects, up to 70% of patients with involvement of attention, information pro-
the authors added tasks specifically evaluating working memory cessing speed, memory and executive function as well.1,2
with verbal stimuli, tracking in particular the function of the central The interplay between cognition and depression in MS raises an
37
executive subsystem (namely the Reading Span Test). In this study, interesting, yet unanswered, question: Does the treatment of one of
depressed MS patients performed significantly worse than the oth- the two actors improve the other? For instance, successful treatment
er two groups on the working memory test. Moreover, scores on this of depression might ameliorate some aspects of cognitive dysfunction,
task were related to that on the three capacity-­demanding tests found as it has been reported in patients with traumatic brain injury.36,38
to be affected in depressed patients with MS in previous assessment. Nevertheless, the clinicians must be aware of the burden secondary to
These results point to a direct impact of depression on a discrete cog- cognitive impairment and depression in MS subjects providing prompt
nitive capacity that is on the central executive component of working diagnosis and treatment, considering both pharmacological and non-­
memory.37 pharmacological (rehabilitation, psychotherapy) approaches.

AC KNOW L ED G EM ENTS
5. |  CONCLUSIONS
None.
Depression and cognitive impairment are commonly reported in patients
with MS.1,27 Recently, neuropsychiatric research has developed several
CO NFL I C TS O F I NT ER ES T
neurocognitive models of mood disorders, underscoring the role of dys-
functional cognition in their pathophysiology.29 Neuroimaging studies Emilio Portaccio has served on scientific advisory boards for Biogen
have pointed to a direct involvement of regions processing emotional Idec, Merck Serono, Bayer and Genzyme, received speaker’s hono-
reactions (such as the amygdala, ventral striatum and periaqueductal raria from Biogen Idec, TEVA, Novartis and Genzyme, and received
gray, the anterior insula and dorsal anterior cingulate) and regulatory research support from Merck Serono. No source of funding was
regions (frontoparietal executive network, supplemental motor area, received to assist with the preparation of this article.
|
18       Portaccio

REFERENCES 21. Pasto L, Portaccio E, Goretti B, et al. The cognitive reserve theory in
the setting of pediatric-­onset multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2016; doi:
1. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclero- 10.1177/1352458516629559. [Epub ahead of print].
sis. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7:1139–1151. 22. Krupp LB. Fatigue in multiple sclerosis: definition, pathophysiology
2. Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Cognitive changes in multiple sclero- and treatment. CNS Drugs. 2003;17:225–234.
sis. Expert Rev Neurother. 2008;8:1585–1596. 23. Strober LB, Arnett PA. An examination of four models predicting
3. Labiano-Fontcuberta A, Martinez-Gines ML, Aladro Y, et al. A compar- fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;20:631–
ison study of cognitive deficits in radiologically and clinically isolated 646.
syndromes. Mult Scler. 2016;22:250–253. 24. Hanken K, Eling P, Hildebrandt H. Is there a cognitive signature for
4. Amato MP, Goretti B, Ghezzi A, et al. Cognitive and psychosocial MS-­related fatigue? Mult Scler. 2015;21:376–381.
features of childhood and juvenile MS. Neurology. 2008;70:1891– 25. Benedict RH, Morrow S, Rodgers J, et al. Characterizing cognitive func-
1897. tion during relapse in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2014;20:1745–
5. Amato MP, Goretti B, Ghezzi A, et al. Cognitive and psychosocial fea- 1752.
tures in childhood and juvenile MS: two-­year follow-­up. Neurology. 26. Pardini M, Uccelli A, Grafman J, et al. Isolated cognitive relapses
2010;75:1134–1140. in multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2014;85:1035–
6. Amato MP, Goretti B, Ghezzi A, et al. Neuropsychological features in 1037.
childhood and juvenile multiple sclerosis: five-­year follow-­up. Neurol- 27. Minden SL, Feinstein A, Kalb RC, et al. Evidence-­based guideline:
ogy. 2014;83:1432–1438. assessment and management of psychiatric disorders in individu-
7. Langdon DW. Cognition in multiple sclerosis. Curr Opin Neurol. als with MS: report of the Guideline Development Subcommittee
2011;24:244–249. of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2014;82:174–
8. Banwell B, Ghezzi A, Bar-Or A, et al. Multiple sclerosis in children: 181.
clinical diagnosis, therapeutic strategies, and future directions. Lancet 28. Feinstein A, Magalhaes S, Richard JF, et al. The link between multiple
Neurol. 2007;6:887–902. sclerosis and depression. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10:507–517.
9. Amato MP, Ponziani G, Pracucci G, et al. Cognitive impairment in 29. Malhi GS, Byrow Y, Fritz K, et al. Mood disorders: neurocognitive
early-­onset multiple sclerosis. Pattern, predictors, and impact on models. Bipolar Disord. 2015;17(Suppl 2):3–20.
every­day life in a 4-­year follow-­up. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:168–172. 30. Etkin A, Buchel C, Gross JJ. The neural bases of emotion regulation.
10. Amato MP, Ponziani G, Siracusa G, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:693–700.
early-­onset multiple sclerosis: a reappraisal after 10 years. Arch Neu- 31. Passamonti L, Cerasa A, Liguori M, et al. Neurobiological mechanisms
rol. 2001;58:1602–1606. underlying emotional processing in relapsing-­remitting multiple scle-
11. Filippi M, Rocca MA, Benedict RH, et al. The contribution of MRI rosis. Brain. 2009;132:3380–3391.
in assessing cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Neurology. 32. Rocca MA, Pravata E, Valsasina P, et al. Hippocampal-­DMN discon-
2010;75:2121–2128. nectivity in MS is related to WM lesions and depression. Hum Brain
12. Till C, Ghassemi R, Aubert-Broche B, et al. MRI correlates of cognitive Mapp. 2015;36:5051–5063.
impairment in childhood-­onset multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology. 33. Arnett PA, Barwick FH, Beeney JE. Depression in multiple sclerosis:
2011;25:319–332. review and theoretical proposal. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008;14:691–
13. Rao SM. A Manual for the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological 724.
Tests in Multiple Sclerosis. Milwaukee, WI: Medical College of Wiscon- 34. Thornton AE, Raz N. Memory impairment in multiple sclerosis: a
sin; 1990. quantitative review. Neuropsychology. 1997;11:357–366.
14. Benedict RH, Fischer JS, Archibald CJ, et al. Minimal neuropsycholog- 35. Arnett PA, Higginson CI, Voss WD, et al. Depressed mood in multiple
ical assessment of MS patients: a consensus approach. Clin Neuropsy- sclerosis: relationship to capacity-­demanding memory and attentional
chol. 2002;16:381–397. functioning. Neuropsychology. 1999;13:434–446.
15. Langdon DW, Amato MP, Boringa J, et al. Recommendations for 36. Feinstein A. Mood disorders in multiple sclerosis and the effects on
a Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis cognition. J Neurol Sci. 2006;245:63–66.
(BICAMS). Mult Scler. 2012;18:891–898. 37. Arnett PA, Higginson CI, Voss WD, et al. Depression in multiple scle-
16. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028. rosis: relationship to working memory capacity. Neuropsychology.
17. Katzman R. Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzhei- 1999;13:546–556.
mer’s disease. Neurology. 1993;43:13–20. 38. Fann JR, Uomoto JM, Katon WJ. Cognitive improvement with treat-
18. Sumowski JF, Leavitt VM. Cognitive reserve in multiple sclerosis. Mult ment of depression following mild traumatic brain injury. Psychoso-
Scler. 2013;19:1122–1127. matics. 2001;42:48–54.
19. Sumowski JF, Rocca MA, Leavitt VM, et al. Brain reserve and cognitive
reserve in multiple sclerosis: what you’ve got and how you use it. Neu-
rology. 2013;80:2186–2193.
How to cite this article: Portaccio, E. (2016), Differential diagnosis,
20. Amato MP, Razzolini L, Goretti B, et al. Cognitive reserve and cor-
discerning depression from cognition. Acta Neurologica
tical atrophy in multiple sclerosis: a longitudinal study. Neurology.
Scandinavica, 134(Suppl. 200):14–18. doi: 10.1111/ane.12652
2013;80:1728–1733.

You might also like