You are on page 1of 16

Child Development, January/February 2008, Volume 79, Number 1, Pages 201 – 216

Content or Connectedness? Mother – Child Talk and


Early Social Understanding
Rosie Ensor and Claire Hughes
University of Cambridge

Despite much research into individual differences in social understanding among preschoolers, little is known
about corresponding individual differences within younger children. Likewise, although studies of preschoolers
highlight the importance of mental-state references, other aspects of talk have received less attention. The current
study involved 120 families with 2-year-olds; video-based transcripts of observations of family interaction were
coded for quantity, connectedness, and content of mothers’ and children’s talk. At 2, 3, and 4 years of age, children
completed social understanding and verbal ability tests. Mothers’ connected turns and mental-state reference
within connected turns showed independent associations with children’s social understanding (as did children’s
mental-state references, both overall and within connected turns). Connected conversations provide a fertile
context for children’s developing social understanding.

Theory of mind is the ability to impute mental states in However, in their everyday social interactions, chil-
order to predict and explain human behavior, that is, dren as young as 2 years engage in deception and
the use of beliefs and feelings as a lens on human trickery (Newton, Reddy, & Bull, 2000) and blame
action. Early research focused on developmental mile- others for their own transgressions (Dunn, 1988;
stones in children’s understanding of false beliefs (see Wilson, Smith, & Ross, 2003). Rather than being
Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001 for a meta-analysis), simple behavioral routines to achieve desired and
but the last decade has seen growing interest in the avoid undesired outcomes, such behaviors appear to
origins of individual differences in false belief compre- reflect young children’s deliberate attempts to create
hension (Hughes et al., 2005; Repacholi & Slaughter, false beliefs in others (Newton et al., 2000). If so,
2003). The current study contributes to this endeavor children’s poor performance on false belief tasks may
by addressing two hitherto neglected topics. First, reflect the significant peripheral linguistic and infor-
although progress has been made in identifying mation-processing demands of such tasks (Bloom &
precursors of false belief understanding (Meltzoff, German, 2000; Leslie & Polizzi, 1998; Siegal & Beattie,
Gopnik, & Repacholi, 1999; Wellman, Phillips, & 1991). In line with this proposition, Clements and
Rodriguez, 2000), the correlates of early individual Perner (1994) found that more than 90% of 3-year-olds
differences in social understanding have yet to be who failed an unexpected location false belief task
explored. Second, studies that document associations nevertheless looked at the correct location (see also
between preschoolers’ false belief comprehension and Garnham & Ruffman, 2001); moreover, 3-year-olds
family conversations typically focus on the content of are more likely to succeed on false belief tasks that
such speech (in particular, references to mental states). have improved ecological validity (Cassidy, 1998;
As a result, other aspects of family talk (e.g., quantity Chandler, Fritz, & Hala, 1989; Hala & Chandler,
and quality) have received much less research atten- 1996; Hala, Chandler, & Fritz, 1991; Wimmer &
tion. The current study examines these two issues. Perner, 1983). Together, these findings suggest that
Next, we outline the theoretical background to each of 3-year-olds have an implicit understanding of false
these two areas in turn. beliefs.
Other important elements of children’s early social
Early Social Understanding understanding include the detection of simple emo-
tions and the recognition of their causes. By 18
Most children do not pass standard false belief
tasks (commonly viewed as the litmus test of theory of months, children can recognize that someone might
want something that they themselves do not want
mind) until the age of 4 years (Wellman et al., 2001).
(Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997) and can predict how
someone else will feel if that person’s desire is
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Rosie Ensor, Centre for Family Research, Free School Lane, # 2008, Copyright the Author(s)
Cambridge, CB2 3RF, United Kingdom. Electronic mail may be Journal Compilation # 2008, Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
sent to rad35@cam.ac.uk. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2008/7901-0014
202 Ensor and Hughes

satisfied or not satisfied (Wellman & Banerjee, 1991). lary necessary for labeling mental states enables
By the age of 2 – 3 years, children can identify how children to represent and reflect on abstract mental-
someone will feel, even in situations where they state concepts (Bartsch & Wellman, 1995; Olson, 1988).
would probably feel differently (Denham, 1986; Well- From this perspective, individual differences in social
man & Banerjee, 1991). Reflecting these findings, in understanding are closely tied to variation in child-
this study, we view early social understanding as ren’s own language skills. Second, conversations
a broad theoretical construct that includes under- provide contexts in which children are confronted
standing of both beliefs and emotions. with the differences between others’ and their own
states of mind (Nelson, 2005); here, the origins of
individual differences in children’s social under-
Content and Connectedness of Conversation
standing are intrinsically social. What is not yet clear,
Four distinct areas of research highlight the however, is the features of conversation that are
importance of conversation for individual differences particularly important for children’s early social
in children’s social understanding. First, a seminal understanding. For example, do conversational refer-
series of studies by Dunn and colleagues revealed that ences to specific types of mental states differ in
children from families characterized by high frequen- importance? In the only relevant study to include
cies of causal talk and/or talk about feelings were experimental tasks, Ruffman et al. (2002) found that
more likely than their peers to succeed on false belief an aggregate of mothers’ talk about mental states was
and emotion understanding tasks 7 months later the most consistent correlate of children’s perfor-
(Dunn, Brown, & Beardsall, 1991; Dunn, Brown, mance on false belief tasks. However, given that
Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991). Subsequent references to mental states are relatively infrequent,
longitudinal studies have shown that talk about this finding may reflect the difficulty of conducting
mental states with both friends (Hughes & Dunn, a fine-grained analysis without extended conversa-
1998) and mothers (Ruffman, Slade, & Crowe, 2002) tional excerpts. For example, the detailed study of
predict improvements in children’s false belief com- Bartsch and Wellman (1995) involving lengthy obser-
prehension. Second, training studies highlight the vations of a small number (4 – 10) of children revealed
importance of explanation within conversations, that mothers’ talk about desires predicted pre-
rather than presentation of counterevidence, for schoolers’ later talk about cognitions more strongly
children’s success on false belief tasks (Appleton & than did mothers’ talk about cognitions (see also
Reddy, 1996; Lohmann & Tomasello, 2003). Third, Ruffman et al., 2002). In contrast, Jenkins, Turrell,
a recent large-scale study (involving 1,116 pairs of Kogushi, Lollis, and Ross (2003) found that family talk
5-year-old twins) showed that shared environmental about cognitions (but not desires) predicted 4-year-
factors had a substantial influence on individual differ- old children’s talk about cognitions. This finding is
ences in children’s understanding of false beliefs; consistent with theoretical accounts that emphasize
bivariate model-fitting analyses showed that these the mind-to-world fit of cognitions (Searle, 1983). In
factors had a common impact on children’s verbal other words, compared with references to desires,
ability (Hughes et al., 2005). In other words, environ- references to cognitions are more likely to highlight
mental influences upon children’s early language skills, the subjective nature of mental states.
such as the quantity (Clarke-Stewart, 1973; Hart & Studies of normative individual differences in
Risley, 1992, 1995; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer, children’s understanding of false beliefs have, as
& Lyons, 1991) and quality (Tomasello & Barton, 1994; indicated above, focused heavily on the literal content
Tomasello & Farrar, 1986; Tomasello & Todd, 1983) of of family talk. This is surprising because a core tenet of
family talk, also appear likely to influence children’s theoretical analyses of conversation is that the literal
false belief comprehension. Fourth, several studies content of an utterance does not necessarily reveal the
demonstrate that, compared with native-signing deaf speaker’s meaning. That is, according to Grice (1975),
children, late-signing deaf children (i.e., deaf children even in ordinary conversational exchanges, interloc-
born to hearing parents) show marked delays in false utors must navigate their way through not only
belief understanding, suggesting that restricted family conventional implicatures (where there is a clear
conversations can delay false belief comprehension and mutually understood correspondence between
(Peterson, 2002, 2004; Peterson & Siegal, 1995, 2000; the ordinary and implicated meanings of words) but
Woolfe, Want, & Siegal, 2002). also conversational implicatures (for which the mean-
Why should conversations matter for children’s ing of the utterance depends on shared understand-
social understanding? Two distinct mechanisms have ing of conversational principles; de Rosnay &
been proposed: First, the acquisition of the vocabu- Hughes, 2006). In other words, beyond asking, ‘‘what
Mother – Child Talk and Early Social Understanding 203

is being talked about?’’ it is important also to ask, The present study involved a socially diverse sam-
‘‘how is it being talked about?’’ (Harrist & Waugh, ple of 120 families with 2-year-olds, filmed during their
2002). We propose that one special feature of the everyday routines. Mothers’ and children’s utterance
‘‘how’’ is the alignment of one interlocutor to another were then transcribed and coded; children completed
(Fogel, 1993). We term this alignment ‘‘connected- social understanding tasks at ages of 2, 3, and 4 years.
ness’’ and, following the example of Dunn and The data were used, in sum, to address three questions.
colleagues (Dunn & Brophy, 2005; Dunn & Cutting, First, are mothers and children particularly likely to
1999; Slomkowski & Dunn, 1996), employ a simple refer to desires, cognitions and/or emotions within
operational definition: the frequency with which each connected utterances? Second, do mothers’ and child-
speaker’s utterances are semantically related to ren’s mental-state references and connected turns
another speaker’s prior utterance (Gottman, 1983). show independent relations with children’s social
In this way, the construct of connectedness provides understanding? Third, does connectedness moderate
an index of how well interlocutors are tuned in to each associations between mothers’ and children’s mental-
other’s talk (Bruner, 1983). Findings from two sepa- state references and children’s social understanding?
rate studies show that children who perform well on
false belief tasks also engage in frequent and extensive
connected conversations with their friends (Dunn & Method
Cutting, 1999; Slomkowski & Dunn, 1996). Individual
differences in the connectedness of mother – child Recruitment and Participants
conversations are also striking (Dunn, 1993) and, in A socially diverse sample was recruited via
a study of ‘‘hard-to-manage’’ preschoolers, Dunn and mother – toddler groups in low-income neighborhoods
Brophy (2005) found that low frequencies of mother – and support groups for mothers in Cambridge, UK.
child connected conversations were related to poor Core inclusion criteria included toddlers aged between
false belief understanding. 24 and 36 months at the first wave of data collection and
Despite the promise of the previously described English as a home language. Of the 192 eligible
early findings, associations between connectedness of families, 140 (73%) gave informed consent and took
conversations and children’s social understanding part in the first wave of the study; of these, 120 (75 boys
have been relatively little researched. This is surpris- and 45 girls) participated fully at all three study time
ing given the likely overlap between mental-state and points. Reflecting the local population, all but four of
connected talk; it seems probable that references to the children were Caucasian. With regard to parental
mental states occur predominantly during connected occupation, 44% of the heads of households were
conversations. If so, two possibilities arise. First, in professional/managerial occupations, 26% were in
associations between mental-state references and skilled nonmanual occupations, and 30% were in
social understanding may actually reflect overlap- manual occupations or unemployed. With regard to
ping relations with connectedness. The independence parental education, 29% of mothers and 17% of fathers
of mental-state and connected talk as correlates of had college degrees, 34% of mothers and 15% of fathers
social understanding, however, has yet to be estab- had A-levels (equivalent to high school diploma), 25%
lished. Second, children’s emerging social under- of mothers and 32% of fathers had General Certificates
standing may be particularly strongly associated of Secondary Education (GCSEs) or equivalent, and
with references to mental states that occur within 12% of mothers and 35% of fathers had no educational
connected conversations. That is, how mental states qualifications. In terms of family structure, 22% of
are referred to may be more important than their mothers were lone parents and 11% of the children
simple frequency. A similar moderation effect has were singletons, 54% had one sibling, 21% had two
been found for mother – child causal talk: Children siblings, 10% had three siblings, and 4% had four or
whose mothers used causal talk chiefly during dis- five siblings (93% of all siblings were older siblings).
putes did poorly on subsequent assessments of emo-
tion understanding, whereas children whose
Procedures
mothers’ causal talk was within the context of shared
play, comforting or joking were more successful on At the first two time points (M ages for target
the later emotion understanding tests (Dunn & children 5 2.38 years, SD 5 32 days at Time 1 and 3.45
Brown, 1993). To assess their relative independence years, SD 5 35 days at Time 2), two researchers visited
and interplay, we examine both mother – child men- each family for 2 hr (typically on a weekday evening).
tal-state and connected talks in relation to children’s During each visit, mothers completed a question-
concurrent and later social understanding. naire-based interview with one researcher, while
204 Ensor and Hughes

target children completed false belief and emotion to have.’’ References to mental states that were conver-
understanding tasks with the other researcher (who sational, repetitive (within an utterance), and imitative
also filmed them playing with a sibling). At the end of were excluded.
the visit, families were observed for 30 min during The transcripts were divided into conversational
meal preparation and/or a meal; researchers fol- turns; a turn was defined as the utterances of one
lowed the target child and did not participate in and speaker bounded by another speaker’s utterances
responded as little as possible to family interaction. (Shatz & Gelman, 1973) or a significant silence (usually
Families received £20 as a token of thanks for their 5 s or more). Adopting procedures from previous
time. Following the home visits, the videos were studies (e.g., Dunn & Cutting, 1999), each conversa-
transcribed professionally. Researchers then checked tional turn that mothers directed to the target child was
that the speakers and addressees, the division of assigned to one of the following five quality codes:
speech into turns, and the content of speech were
correct and added any nonverbal contextual informa- 1. Connected: Speaker’s utterance is semantically
tion needed to understand the speech. related to the other interlocutor’s previous turn.
Approximately 6 weeks after the home visit (Time 2. Initiation: Speaker initiates a new topic that
1 M interval 5 39 days, SD 5 32 days, range 5 26 – 95 is both unrelated to the other interlocutor’s
days; Time 2 M interval 5 39 days, SD 5 35 days, previous turn and successful in eliciting
range 5 19 – 92 days), mother – toddler dyads visited a semantically related response from the other
the lab in pairs for a 2-hr session. During this visit, interlocutor.
each child completed verbal ability assessments. 3. Failed: Speaker’s turn is directed (either explic-
Taxis/parking refunds were provided for all families, itly or implicitly) to the other interlocutor but
as well as a video of the visit. At the third time point fails to elicit a semantically related response.
(M age of target children at Time 3 5 4.19 years, SD 5 4. Conflict: Speaker’s utterance includes a prohibi-
37 days), researchers visited each child at play group tion, threat, or insult.
or nursery school for 2 hr. During this visit, each child 5. Unclear: Speaker’s utterance was inaudible and,
completed false belief and emotion understanding thus, is not transcribed sufficiently to code.
tasks as well as a verbal ability assessment; families
received a DVD of the visit. A trumping system was used such that turns that
could be categorized as either conflict or connected
were always coded as connected.
Observational Coding
In previous studies, the quality of interaction has
Time 1 transcripts (i.e., when the target children were also been indexed by the length of connected epi-
2 years old) were coded using the scheme designed by sodes, that is, the average number of turns in each
Brown, Donelan-McCall, and Dunn (1996), for refer- connected conversation (Slomkowski & Dunn, 1996)
ences to three categories of mental states: cognitions, or the number of connected conversations that con-
desires, and feelings. References to cognitive states tinued for at least three turns (Dunn & Cutting, 1999).
included terms used to denote thoughts, memories, or These approaches were not adopted in the current
knowledge of the mother, child, or a third party. The study; instead (as in one other study—Brophy &
most common cognitive terms recorded were think, Dunn, 2002), the quality of each turn (i.e., con-
know, forgot, mean, and variations. Other cognitive terms nected/initiatory/failed/conflictual/unclear) was
that occurred were assure, believe, concentrate, distract, coded. Such turn-based coding has at least two
expect, figure, guess, idea, ignore, imagine, interest, learn, advantages: First, the problem of reliably identifying
pretend, recognize, trust, understand, wonder, and varia- the point where one connected conversation ends and
tions. The desire terms observed were fancy, keen, hope, another begins is circumvented; second, the quality of
want, wish, would like, would love, and variations com- each turn containing a mental-state reference can be
monly used to capture both desires and goals. For calculated, providing indices of the frequencies of
example, want was coded when the term referred to connected, initiatory, failed, and conflictual turns con-
a goal-directed behavior. References to feelings taining references to desires, cognitions, and emotions.
included those that referred to an emotional state. The Table 1 shows a conversational excerpt, coded for
feeling terms noted were cross, disgust, enjoy, excited, fed quantity, content, and quality. Turns 1 and 4 were
up, frightened, fun, grumpy, happy, like, love, pleased, proud, coded as failed because the child did not respond to
scared, surprise, upset, worried, and variations. For exam- the mother’s utterances. Turn 2 was coded as an
ple, like was only coded when the term referred to initiation because the child responds to the mother’s
a state of enjoyment and not when it indicated ‘‘to want second utterance. Turn 3 could have been coded as
Mother – Child Talk and Early Social Understanding 205

Table 1
Coding of Example Conversational Excerpt

Quantity

No. turns No. words Content Quality (of turn)

1. M: Would you like a slice of ham now or will you wait? 1 12 Desire reference Failed
2. M: Do you want a slice of ham? 2 7 Desire reference Initiation
3. C: No. I bumped my nose 1 5 — Connected
4. M: You’re so tired 3 4 — Failed
5. C: Piglet down 2 2 — Failed
6. C: Bumped my nose 3 3 — Initiation
7. M: You did bump your nose didn’t you? 4 20 Cognitive reference Connected
But I think it’s all better now though, isn’t it?

Note. C5child; M5mother.

connected (as the child replied to the mother’s ques- back, bringing both hands forward, keeping hand
tion) or failed (as the mother did not respond to the closed, and keeping coin hidden. Scores for this task
child’s comment about her nose). As connected have a possible range of 0 – 12 points.
trumps all other codes, turn 3 was coded as con- Picture book false belief (Hughes, 1998). The standard
nected. Turn 5 is coded as failed because the mother false belief task was made more engaging by using
did not reply to the child’s comment about piglet. The peep-through picture books with deceptive elements
mother responded to the child’s second comment (e.g., what appears to be an eye peeping through
about her nose; therefore, Turn 6 was coded as an a hole in the page turns out to be a spot on a snake’s
initiation and Turn 7 was coded as connected. back; what looks like an orange turns out to be the
As part of a summer research internship, a psychol- setting sun). The same book (Moerbeek, 1994) was
ogy undergraduate student was trained and then inde- used at 2 and 4 years of age; at 3 years of age a differ-
pendently coded 20% of transcripts. Interrater ent book (Balas, 2003) was used to ensure that the
agreement with the first author was established using deceptive element was not affected by children’s
Cohen’s kappa and was excellent for both references to memories of the previous testing session. At each
mental states (j 5 .86) and quality of utterances (j 5 .97). time point, children were asked two forced-choice test
questions tapping memory for own false belief,
‘‘Before we turned the page, what did you think this
Experimental Tasks
would be, an eye or a spot?’’ and prediction of other’s
At ages 2, 3, and 4 years, children completed tasks false belief, ‘‘Look, this is Charlie (show puppet).
tapping simple skills of deception and awareness of Charlie has never seen this book before. If we show
mistaken beliefs. We first describe the tasks that were him this picture, what will he think it is, an eye or
administered at all three time points: a penny hiding a spot?’’ We also asked two corresponding forced-
deception game, a picture book false belief task, and an choice reality control questions, ‘‘What is it really, an
emotion understanding (identification and perspective eye or a spot?’’ Children were credited with success
taking) puppet game. Next, we describe an object on the test questions only if they also passed the
transfer false belief task given at ages 3 and 4 years corresponding reality control question. Possible
and a deceptive content false belief task given at age 3 scores on these tasks range from 0 to 2 points.
years that, because of time constraints in the school/ Emotion understanding (Denham, 1986). In this task,
nursery visits, could not be included at age 4 years. a puppet (Johnny for boys, Nancy for girls) with
Tasks were presented in a counterbalanced order. a blank face and four felt faces portraying happy,
Deception (Hughes, 1998). In this penny hiding sad, angry, and scared expressions was used. In the
game, the researcher hides a coin behind her back emotion labeling subtask, children were asked to
and, bringing both hands forward with the coin name how the puppet felt when each face was placed
concealed in one hand, asks the child to guess which in turn on the puppet; children were then told how
hand has the coin. After two more display trials, the Johnny or Nancy felt and asked to choose the appro-
researcher invites the child to hide the coin. For each priate face. Vignettes (8 at age 2 years of age; 16 at ages
of the following three test trials, one point is awarded of 3 and 4 years) associated with feelings of happiness,
for each of the following: hiding both hands behind sadness, anger, or fear were then enacted (2 of each
206 Ensor and Hughes

type at 2 years of age; 4 of each type at 3 and 4 years of box, and children are asked to explain why she’s
age) with matching affective tone. In 8 vignettes, the looking in that box, as well as a second reality control
puppet’s emotions reflect what most children typi- question. Children are required to pass the corre-
cally feel in a given situation. In the other 8 vignettes, sponding reality control questions in order to receive
the puppet’s emotion differed from the emotion that credit on each of these two test questions—possible
the mother had stated her child would typically scores on this task range from 0 to 2 points.
express in that situation. Children were asked to
choose the face that matched how the puppet was
Verbal Ability Assessments
feeling in each vignette. Fully correct responses
received 2 points, whereas responses correct in A subtest from the British abilities scales (Elliott,
valence but not specific emotion (e.g., calling the Murray, & Pearson, 1983) was used to assess child-
angry face ‘‘sad’’) received 1 point. This task had ren’s verbal comprehension at 2 and 3 years of age.
a possible range of 0 – 24 points at 2 years of age and Standard scoring procedures were applied and gave
0 – 42 points at 3 and 4 years of age. a possible range of 0 – 27. The British picture vocab-
Object transfer false belief task. This task was given at ulary scale (Dunn, 1997) was used to assess children’s
3 and 4 years of age, using different characters and verbal comprehension at the age of 4 years.
props at each time point. At 3 years of age, the story
involved two well-known characters (Noddy and Big
Ears); props for this included toy figures, a set of
Results
miniature knives and forks, and a miniature drawer
and pot (with lid). At 4 years of age, the story was the We begin by describing the construction of the aggre-
classic Sally – Ann scenario (Wimmer & Perner, 1983), gate measures of social understanding and conversa-
enacted with props (two dolls, a basket, a box, and tion. We then report preliminary analyses concerning
a very small ball). In the Noddy story, Big Ears is doing (a) developmental change, stability of individual
the washing up and tidies the cutlery away in the pot differences, and sex differences in children’s social
before going out. Noddy then appears and moves the understanding and (b) associations between the social
cutlery into the drawer before also leaving. Big Ears understanding aggregate measures and children’s
then reappears, in need of the cutlery. In the Sally – verbal ability, mothers’ education, and the number
Ann story, Sally has a ball, which she places in a basket of mothers’ and children’s utterances. In the main
before going out. In her absence, Ann takes the ball out Results section, we present analyses for each of our
to play with it and replaces it in the box, before also study questions in turn. The first question concerns
leaving. Sally returns to the scene, wanting to play whether mothers and children are particularly likely
with the ball. For each story, children were asked two to refer to desires, cognitions, and/or emotions within
test questions: (a) prediction of search behavior, connected utterances. We address this question in two
‘‘Where will Noddy/Sally look for the knives/ball?’’ ways: by using repeated measures analysis of covari-
and (b) belief-based explanation, ‘‘Why did Noddy/ ance (ANCOVA) and by categorizing mothers and
Sally look there?’’ Children were asked both a memory children according to whether or not they referred to
control question, ‘‘Where did Noddy/Sally put the mental states preferentially within connected turns.
knives/ball first of all?’’ and a reality control question, The second question concerns whether or not moth-
‘‘Where are they/is really?’’ Children are credited ers’ and children’s mental-state references and con-
with success on the test questions only if they also nected turns are each significant, and independent,
respond to the reality control question—possible predictors of individual differences in children’s
scores on this task range from 0 to 2 points. social understanding. The third question concerns
Deceptive contents false belief task. In this task, given whether or not connectedness moderates associations
at the age of 3 years only, children are shown a pro- between mothers’ and children’s mental-state refer-
totypical container (an egg box) and a plain tube and ences and children’s social understanding. These
asked which they think contains an egg. The boxes are latter two questions are addressed using separate
then opened to reveal a realistic model egg in the plain correlations and hierarchical regression analyses.
tube. They are then asked to recall their false belief,
‘‘Before we looked, which box did you think had an
Data Reduction
egg inside?’’ and a reality control question, ‘‘Which
box really has an egg inside?’’ Next, they are intro- To achieve comparable measures of children’s
duced to a granny doll, who wants to make a cake and social understanding at all three time points, we
needs some eggs. The granny doll ‘‘walks’’ to the egg averaged standardized scores on the false belief,
Mother – Child Talk and Early Social Understanding 207

emotion understanding and deception tasks. Scores quality of their talk by calculating the frequencies of
on the false belief, emotion understanding, and connected, initiatory, failed, and conflictual turns.
deception tasks were significantly correlated at 2 Third, we indexed the content of mother – child talk
years of age, rmean(120) 5 .36, rmin(120) 5 .29, rmax(120) by calculating the frequencies of references to desires,
5 .40, p , .01; and at 3 years of age, rmean(120) 5 .35, cognitions, and emotions. Table 2 presents raw scores
rmin(120) 5 .26, rmax(120) 5 .43, p , .01. At 4 years of for each of these measures. Given the relative infre-
age, although there were significant correlations quency of children’s references to cognitions, desires,
between scores on the false belief and emotion under- and emotion, we adopted overall frequencies of
standing tasks, r(120) 5 .20, p , .05 and between the children’s mental-state references for our analyses.
scores on emotion understanding and deception In order to address the third study hypothesis
tasks, r(120) 5 .43, p , .01, false belief and deception (concerning whether the associations between
scores were not significantly correlated, r(120) 5 .06, mental-state references and social understanding are
ns. The aggregates showed modest internal consis- restricted to connected conversations), we used
tency at 2 years of age, Cronbach’s a 5 .63, and good Bespoke Software (Ensor, 2005) to marry the quality
internal consistency at 3 and 4 years of age, Cron- and content codes. This enabled us to calculate the
bach’s a 5 .73, for both. frequencies of mothers’ and children’s mental-state
Turning to mother – child talk, we first indexed the references within connected, initiatory, failed, and
quantity of mothers’ and children’s talk by calculating conflictual turns. Note that both mothers and children
the number and mean length of mother to child turns made too few references to desires, cognitions, and
and child to mother turns. Next, we indexed the emotions to calculate the frequency with which each

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics for Measures of Cognition and Talk (Proportions Are Shown in Parentheses)

Measure M SD Range

Mothers Number of turns 66.84 48.38 5 – 256


Mean length of turns 7.61 2.50 1.89 – 14.27
Desire reference 9.46 (.16) 7.71 (.12) 0 – 40 (.00 – .70)
Cognitive references 4.16 (.06) 4.49 (.06) 0 – 20 (.00 – .29)
Emotion references 1.43 (.02) 1.97 (.03) 0 – 12 (.00 – .12)
Connected turns 34.89 (.43) 33.39 (.20) 0 – 173 (.00 – .88)
Initiatory turns 8.70 (.14) 6.33 (.07) 0 – 31 (.00 – .50)
Failed turns 21.36 (.40) 13.63 (.25) 0 – 78 (.00 – 1.00)
Conflictual turns 3.43 (.09) 4.14 (.08) 0 – 23 (.00 – .40)
Children Number of turns 42.25 37.45 0 – 209
Mean length of turns 2.67 1.02 0 – 5.40
Desire references 2.79 (.08) 4.28 (.09) 0 – 22 (.00 – .46)
Cognitive references 0.29 (.01) 0.77 (.02) 0 – 5 (.00 – .10)
Emotion references 0.42 (.01) 0.91 (.03) 0 – 4 (.00 – .23)
Connected turns 27.31 (.60) 26.12 (.20) 0 – 135 (.00 – 1.00)
Initiatory turns 7.42 (.18) 8.47 (.12) 0 – 52 (.00 – .67)
Failed turns 3.32 (.12) 4.24 (.15) 0 – 29 (.00 – .80)
Conflictual turns .59 (.07) 1.12 (.10) 0 – 6 (.00 – .52)
Age 2 verbal comprehension 15.44 5.78 0 – 25
Age 2 deception 3.95 3.52 0 – 12
Age 2 false belief 0.66 0.77 0–2
Age 2 emotion understanding 10.74 10.12 0 – 26
Age 3 verbal comprehension 21.48 3.50 10 – 27
Age 3 deception 7.88 3.43 0 – 12
Age 3 false belief 1.66 1.43 0–4
Age 3 emotion understanding 30.69 10.12 9 – 47
Age 4 verbal comprehension 35.30 11.31 11 – 72
Age 4 deception 9.85 2.75 0 – 12
Age 4 false belief 1.65 1.42 0–4
Age 4 emotion understanding 40.85 7.08 4 – 48
208 Ensor and Hughes

occurred within connected, initiatory, failed, and two repeated measures ANCOVAs, each with two
conflictual turns. All measures of talk were log trans- within-subject factors: content (three levels: desire,
formed before conducting correlation and regression cognitive, and emotion references) and quality (four
analyses, in order to reduce any effects of the positive levels: connected, initiatory, failed, and conflictual
skew that were apparent in the raw data. turns). The number of mothers’ or children’s turns
was covaried in each respective analysis.
Main effects. Main effects of content of talk were
Preliminary Analyses
found for both children, F(2, 117) 5 50.01, p , .001,
Before describing associations between children’s and mothers, F(2, 117) 5 115.04, p , .001. Separate
social understanding aggregates (described previ- paired-samples t tests showed that children referred
ously) and children’s verbal comprehension, mater- to desires significantly more frequently than to either
nal education, and the number of mothers’ and cognitions, t(120) 5 6.39, p , .001, or emotions, t(120)
children’s turns, we summarize findings concerning 5 6.39, p , .001. Children referred with similar
developmental change, stability of individual differ- frequency to cognitions and emotions, t(120) 5 1.30,
ences, and sex differences in children’s social under- ns. Mothers also referred to desires significantly more
standing. As reported elsewhere (Hughes & Ensor, frequently than to either cognitions, t(120) 5 8.42,
2007), children improved significantly in their perfor- p , .001, or emotions, t(120) 5 11.46, p , .001. Mothers
mance on false belief tasks over the three time points; referred to cognitions significantly more frequently
in addition, individual differences in children’s false than to emotions, t(120) 5 5.70, p , .001.
belief task performance were significantly correlated. Main effects of quality of talk were found for both
Girls achieved higher social understanding scores children, F(3, 116) 5 46.01, p , .001, and mothers,
than boys at 2 years of age, t(120) 5 1.96, p 5 .05, F(3, 116) 5 56.99, p , .001. Children’s utterances were
and 3, t(120) 5 1.78, p , .10. However, there were no significantly more likely to be connected than initia-
sex differences in any other study measure, t(120) , tory, t(120) 5 10.65, p , .001; failed, t(120) 5 10.57,
1.54, ns, and girls and boys showed similar patterns of p , .001; or conflictual, t(120) 5 10.72, p , .001. In
correlations. To simplify analyses, we report findings addition, children’s utterances were significantly
for the whole sample. more likely to be initiations than failed, t(120) 5
The number of children’s turns was significantly 6.06, p , .001, or conflictual, t(120) 5 6.12, p , .001.
correlated with children’s age 2 social understanding, Children’s turns were equally likely to be failed or
r(120) 5 .18, p , .05, but not with social understanding conflictual, t(120) 5 .59, ns. Likewise, mothers’ utter-
at 3 or 4 years of age, r(120)  .07, ns. In addition, the ances were significantly more likely to be connected
number of mothers’ turns was not significantly cor- than initiatory, t(120) 5 9.77, p , .001; failed, t(120) 5
related with children’s social understanding at any 4.12, p , .001; or conflictual, t(120) 5 10.04, p , .001.
age, r(120)  .15, ns. Maternal education was signifi- Mothers’ utterances were also significantly more
cantly correlated with children’s social understand- likely to be initiations than failed, t(120) 5 11.23, p ,
ing, r(120)  .18, p , .05. Finally, children’s verbal .001, or conflictual, t(120) 5 11.50, p , .001. Mothers’
comprehension was significantly correlated with turns were significantly more likely to be failed than
social understanding, both concurrently, r(120)  conflictual, t(120) 5 12.75, p , .001.
.63, p , .01, and longitudinally, r(120)  .46, p , 01. Interaction effects. Most importantly, the interaction
Therefore, we controlled for the effects of age 2 social between quality and content was significant for both
understanding, age 4 verbal comprehension, and mothers, F(6, 113) 5 26.23, p , .001, and children,
maternal education in subsequent regression analy- F(6, 113) 5 25.09, p , .001. As shown in Table 3,
ses. In addition, to ensure comparability with prior mothers and children both appeared more likely to
studies that have used proportional measures of talk, refer to desires, cognitions, and emotions within
these analyses also controlled for any effects of the connected utterances than within initiatory, failed,
number of mother and child turns. or conflictual turns. To explore this interaction further,
we identified children and mothers who referred to
desires/cognitions/emotions preferentially within
Do Mental-State References Occur in Connected,
connected turns if the proportion of connected turns
Initiatory, Failed, or Conflictual Turns?
that included references to desires/cognitions/emo-
Our first hypothesis was that both mothers and tions was greater than the proportion of initiatory,
children would be especially likely to refer to mental failed, and conflictual turns that included references
states within connected (as opposed to failed or to desires/cognitions/emotions. The number of chil-
conflictual) turns. To assess this hypothesis, we used dren who showed such preferential use was 59 for
Mother – Child Talk and Early Social Understanding 209

Table 3 ences to desires and cognitions occurred more fre-


Mothers and Children Refer to Desires, Cognitions, and Emotions More quently within connected turns than within initiatory,
Frequently Within Connected Than Within Initiatory, Failed, or Conflic- failed, and conflictual utterances.
tual Turns

M Is Children’s Social Understanding Associated With Both


Mental-State and Connected Talk?
Content Quality Mothers Children
Our second study hypothesis was that mothers’
Desire references Connected 4.35 1.77
and children’s mental-state references and connected
Initiation 2.02 0.77
turns would each be significant and independ-
Failed 2.90 0.31
Conflict 0.27 0.04
ent predictors of children’s social understanding
Cognitive references Connected 2.07 0.21 at 4 years of age. Note that the patterns of
Initiation 0.44 0.04 correlations, shown in Table 4 were very similar
Failed 0.91 0.05 for false belief and emotion understanding when
Conflict 0.17 0.00 examined separately. To simplify analyses (and for
Emotion references Connected 0.63 0.31 reasons of space), we focus on the overall measures
Initiation 0.20 0.04 of social understanding.
Failed 0.47 0.04 Correlations: Mental-state references. Mothers’ cog-
Conflict 0.08 0.02 nitive references were significantly correlated with
children’s social understanding at 2, 3 and 4 years of
age, r(120)  .34, p , .01, even when children’s
concurrent verbal comprehension scores were par-
desire terms, 10 for cognitive terms, and 13 for tialed, r(120)  .27, p , .01. In contrast, mothers’ desire
emotion terms (i.e., 49%, 8%, and 11%). Correspond- and emotion references were not significantly corre-
ing numbers for mothers were 109, 76, and 56 (i.e., lated with children’s social understanding at any age,
91%, 63%, and 47%). Moreover, 75 mothers referred r(120)  .07, ns; note the significant contrasts in
preferentially to both cognitions and desires within strength of correlations between cognitive versus
connected turns (11 mothers did not refer preferen- desire/emotion references and social understanding,
tially to either cognitions or desires within connected z(120) . 2.17, p , .05. Children’s mental-state refer-
turns and 34 mothers referred preferentially to either ences (recall that children’s references to cognitive,
desires or cognitions within connected turns): McNe- desire, and emotion terms, because of their relative
mar (1, 119) 5 29.26, p , .001. In sum, the quality and infrequency, were combined) were significantly cor-
content of mothers’ talk co-occurred, such that refer- related with social understanding at 2, 3, and 4 years

Table 4
Correlations Between the Content and Quality of Talk and Children’s Social Understanding

Age 2 Age 3 Age 4

Social False belief Emotion Social False belief Emotion Social False belief Emotion

Mothers’ Desires references .07 .01 .07 .00 .02 .00 .06 .02 .07
Cognitive references .34** .30** .34** .34** .32** .33** .43** .33** .37**
Emotion references .06 .01 .06 .06 .02 .07 .04 .03 .04
Connected turns .33** .36** .33** .28** .27** .27** .39** .33** .32**
Initiatory turns .21* .24** .21* .16y .21* .14 .27** .27** .18*
Failed turns .09 .03 .09 .11 .14 .10 .10 .13 .04
Conflictual turns .02 .10 .02 .09 .04 .09 .12 .04 .12
Children’s Mental-state references .26** .31** .26** .25** .22** .25** .31** .19* .31**
Connected turns .16y .18* .16y .21* .23** .20* .26** .22** .21*
Initiatory turns .08 .05 .08 .07 .08 .07 .05 .05 .12
Failed turns .12 .06 .12 .01 .00 .01 .04 .06 .02
Conflictual turns .06 .12 .06 .05 .07 .05 .03 .01 .05

Note. Emotion 5 emotion understanding; False belief 5 false belief understanding; Social 5 social understanding.
y
p , .10. *p , .05. **p , .01.
210 Ensor and Hughes

of age, r(120)  .25, p , .01, even when effects of verbal ers’ cognitive references and connected turns as
comprehension were partialed, r(120)  .18, p , .05. predictors of children’s age 4 social understanding.
Correlations: Connected, initiatory, failed, and Here, we entered children’s mental-state references,
conflictual turns. Mothers’ connected turns were signifi- mothers’ cognitive references, and mothers’ con-
cantly correlated with social understanding at 2, 3, and nected turns at Step 2 of the regression equation.
4 years of age, r(120)  .28, p , .01, even when children’s Children’s mental-state references and mothers’ con-
concurrent verbal comprehension scores were partialed, nected utterances (but not mothers’ cognitive refer-
r(120)  .26, p , .01. Children’s connected turns were ences) were significant predictors and together
marginally correlated with age 2 social understanding, accounted for an additional 5% of the variance in
r(120) 5 .16, p , .10, and significantly correlated with children’s age 4 social understanding, DR2 5 .05,
social understanding at 3 and 4 years of age, r(120)  .25, Finc(3, 112) 5 3.55, p , .02. The largest variance
p , .05; however, these latter correlations fell below inflation factor (VIF) was 6.23, indicating no problems
significance, when we partialled effects of verbal com- of multicollinearity (Bowerman & O’Connell, 1990),
prehension, r(120)  .11, ns. and the maximum Cook’s distance was .14, indicating
Mothers’ initiations were marginally correlated that no outliers were identified (Cook & Weisberg,
with social understanding at 3 years of age, r(120) 5 1982).
.16, p , .10, and significantly correlated with social We conducted the second analysis to assess the
understanding at 2 and 4 years of age, r(125)  .21, independence of mothers’ connected turns and cog-
p , .05; however, when we partialed effects of verbal nitive references as predictors of children’s age 4
comprehension, these correlations fell below signifi- social understanding. To minimize overlap between
cance, r(120)  .11, ns. Mothers’ failed and conflict constructs, we identified mothers’ cognitive referen-
turns were unrelated to social understanding at any ces that occurred outside connected utterances and
age, r(120)  .02, ns. Children’s initiatory, failed, and mothers’ connected turns that did not contain refer-
conflictual turns were not significantly correlated ences to cognitions. We entered children’s mental-
with social understanding at any age, r(120)  .12, ns. state references, mothers’ cognitive references that
Regression analyses: Mental-state references versus did not occur within connected turns, and mothers’
connected turns. As reported earlier, mothers’ refer- connected turns that did not contain references to
ences to cognitions and desires were particularly cognitions at Step 2 of the regression equation. Child-
likely to occur within connected turns. The correla- ren’s mental-state references and mothers’ connected
tions reported above demonstrate that mothers’ cog- turns that did not contain cognitive references (but
nitive references and connected turns (and child not mothers’ cognitive references that did not occur
mental-state references) were each associated with within connected turns) were independent predictors
higher social understanding in children. Together,
these findings indicate that mothers’ cognitive ref-
Table 5
erences and connected turns are likely to show Children’s (but Not Mothers’) Mental-State References and Mothers’
overlapping associations with children’s social Connected Turns Significantly Predict Children’s Age 4 Social Under-
understanding. To address this possibility, we con- standing
ducted two hierarchical regression analyses, in order
to assess whether mothers’ cognitive references and Variable B SE B b
connected turns are each significant and independent
Step 1 Maternal education .11 .05 .15*
predictors of children’s age 4 social understanding. In
No. of mother turns .00 .00 .23
both analyses, we entered the number of mother and No. of child turns .00 .00 .12
child turns, children’s age 2 social understanding and Age 2 social understanding .04 .01 .22**
age 4 verbal comprehension, and maternal education Age 4 verbal comprehension .03 .01 .47**
at Step 1 of the regression equation, R2 5 .44, F(5, 115) Step 2 Maternal education .11 .05 .14*
5 18.35, p , .001. The number of mother and child No. of mother turns .00 .00 .26
turns were included, as mentioned before, to ensure No. of child turns .00 .00 .21
comparability with prior studies that have used pro- Age 2 social understanding .03 .01 .19*
portional measures of talk. The other background Age 4 verbal comprehension .03 .01 .46**
measures were included because, as reported earlier, Child mental-state references .04 .01 .24*
Mother cognitive references .01 .02 .06
each was significantly correlated with age 4 social
Mother connected turns .03 .01 .26*
understanding.
Table 5 summarizes the results of a regression Note. R2 5 .44** for Step 1; DR2 5 .05* for Step 2.
analysis conducted to assess the significance of moth- *p , .05. **p , .01.
Mother – Child Talk and Early Social Understanding 211

and accounted for an additional 5% of the variance in within either failed or conflictual turns, z(120) . 2.89,
children’s age 4 social understanding, DR2 5 .05, p , .05; the one exception here was a marginally
Finc(3, 112) 5 3.59, p , .02. significant contrast in the strength of correlations
between mothers’ connected versus conflictual men-
tal-state references and age 2 social understanding,
Does Connected Talk Moderate Associations Between
z(120) . 1.85, p ,. 10. Children’s social understanding
Mental-State References and Social Understanding?
at all three time points was also significantly more
Our third hypothesis was that children’s social strongly correlated with the frequencies of their
understanding would be particularly strongly asso- mental-state references within connected turns than
ciated with mental-state references that occur within within failed turns, z(120) . 2.41, p , .05; in addition,
connected (as opposed to failed or conflictual) turns. age 4 social understanding was significantly more
Table 6 shows the correlations between children’s strongly correlated with children’s mental-state refer-
social understanding and both mothers’ and child- ences within connected versus conflictual turns,
ren’s mental-state references within each type of z(120) 5 2.05, p , .05.
speaker turn (i.e., connected, initiatory, failed, or Regression analysis. Table 7 summarizes the find-
conflictual). ings from a regression analysis that examined whether
Correlations, by type of turn. Within connected turns, mothers’ and children’s references to mental states
mothers’ and children’s mental-state references were within connected turns explained unique variance in
significantly correlated with social understanding at children’s age 4 social understanding. As before, at
ages 2, 3, and 4, r(120)  .25, p , .01. Within initiations, Step 1 of the regression equation, we entered the
mothers’ mental-state references were correlated with number of mother and child utterances, children’s
social understanding, marginally significantly at 3 age 2 social understanding and age 4 verbal compre-
years of age, r(120) 5 .17, p , .10, and significantly at 2 hension, and maternal education, R2 5 .44, F(5, 115) 5
and 4 years of age, r(120)  .24, p , .05. Children’s 18.35, p , .001. At Step 2 of the equation, mothers’ and
mental-state references within initiations were signifi- children’s connected mental-state references were each
cantly correlated with social understanding at 3 and 4 independent predictors and together accounted for an
years of age, r(120)  .24, p , .05, but not at 2 years of additional 6% of variance in children’s age 4 social
age, r(120)  .14, ns. (All of the preceding results understanding, DR2 5 .06, Finc (3, 112) 5 3.55, p , .02.
remained unchanged when we partialed effects of The largest VIF was 4.96, indicating no problems of
children’s concurrent verbal comprehension, r(120)  multicollinearity, and the maximum Cook’s distance
.18, p , .05.) Within failed or conflictual turns, however, was .12, indicating that no outliers were identified.
both mothers’ and children’s mental-state references
were not significantly correlated with social under-
Summary
standing at any age, r(125)  .14, ns.
Note that children’s social understanding showed Three main findings emerged from the present
significantly stronger correlations with mothers’ study. First, the majority of mothers referred to
mental-state references within connected turns than cognitions and desires preferentially within

Table 6
Correlations Between Mental-State References Within Connected, Initiatory, Failed, and Conflictual Turns and Children’s Social Understanding

Age 2 Age 3 Age 4


Quality
Speaker Content of turn of turn Social False belief Emotion Social False belief Emotion Social False belief Emotion

Mother Mental-state Connected .32** .31** .32** .42** .29** .41** .48** .34** .44**
references Initiation .24** .24** .24** .17y .16y .16y .24** .24** .16y
Failed .15 .15 .15 .05 .05 .05 .05 .05 .00
Conflict .09 .09 .09 .07 .07 .07 .10 .10 .08
Children Mental-state Connected .25** .29** .25** .31** .21* .31** .37** .26** .32**
references Initiation .14 .13 .14 .26** .16y .26** .24** .14 .25**
Failed .13 .06 .14 .13 .13 .12 .12 .12 .09
Conflict .06 .05 .05 .04 .13 .05 .10 .13 .07

Note. Emotion 5 emotion understanding; False belief 5 false belief understanding; Social 5 social understanding.
y
p , .10. *p , .05. **p , .01.
212 Ensor and Hughes

Table 7 present study. Our findings also demonstrate that


Mothers’ and Children’s Mental-State References Within Connected connectedness was both a strong predictor of child-
Turns Significantly Predict Children’s Age 4 Social Understanding ren’s social understanding and an important moder-
Variable B SE B b
ator: Children’s concepts of mind and emotion appear
to be most clearly reflected and/or promoted by
Step 1 Maternal education .11 .05 .15* mental-state references that occur within connected
No. of mother turns .00 .00 .23 conversations.
No. of child turns .00 .00 .12 Before discussing these findings, we should note
Age 2 social understanding .04 .01 .22** that others have, from a cognitive perspective, also
Age 4 verbal comprehension .03 .01 .47** argued that the mental-state terms and concepts
Step 2 Maternal education .11 .05 .15*
children encounter in conversations may not be
No. of mother turns .00 .00 .23
sufficient to promote their social understanding. For
No. of child turns .00 .00 .12
Age 2 social understanding .03 .01 .18*
example, de Villiers (2000) argues that specific syn-
Age 4 verbal comprehension .03 .01 .39** tactic structures (embedded complements) must be
Child connected .40 .17 .21* mastered before children can attribute different
mental-state references points of view. In brief, mental verbs typically occur
Mother connected .40 .19 .22* as main verbs in complex sentences with subordinate
mental-state references clauses—the sentential complement—as their gram-
matical object. Children use such embedded comple-
Note. R2 5 .44** for Step 1; DR2 5 .06* for Step 2.
*p , .05. **p , .01.
ments almost as soon as they start to produce mental
verbs, that is, around 2 years of age (Bartsch &
Wellman, 1995; Bretherton & Beeghly, 1982). Thus,
connected turns. Second, children’s mental-state by taking the frequency of children’s mental-state
references and mothers’ connected turns were each references into account, in the present study, we have,
significant and independent predictors of children’s by proxy, also controlled for children’s use of embed-
age 4 social understanding, even when the quantity of ded complements. However, a second wave of con-
maternal and child talk and children’s age 4 verbal versation excerpts is needed to examine child-driven
comprehension and age 2 social understanding were effects more completely. Children with precocious
all taken into account. Third, a moderation effect was social understanding may be especially skilled in
found such that predictive associations with child- eliciting connected talk; indeed, it is known that
ren’s age 4 social understanding were strongest for young children play a crucial role in initiating con-
mothers’ and children’s mental-state references versations (Clarke-Stewart & Hevey, 1981). Despite
within connected turns. this limitation, the present findings are valuable in
highlighting the potential importance of connected-
ness and in delineating the types of mental-state talk
that are particularly closely related to children’s social
Discussion
understanding.
The present study takes as its starting point the Children’s social understanding was more
premise that the act of engaging another person in strongly related to mothers’ references to cognitions
conversation implies an expectation to exchange or than to desires or emotions, even though both moth-
share points of view (de Rosnay & Hughes, 2006). ers and children referred to desires more frequently
Children’s experiences of such connectedness have than cognitions. Such lack of associations between
received surprisingly little research attention, even mothers’ desire and emotion references and child-
though it is known that children who are successful ren’s social understanding may reflect the consider-
on false belief tasks engage in frequent connected able variability within these categories of mental-state
conversations (Dunn & Brophy, 2005; Dunn & talk. For example, routine enquires (e.g., ‘‘Do you
Cutting, 1999; Slomkowski & Dunn, 1996). The pres- want this crayon?’’) are less likely to promote reflec-
ent findings demonstrate elevated frequencies of tion on mental states than are references to desires
references to cognitions, desires, and emotions within that occur within explanations of others’ actions (e.g.,
connected conversation compared with other forms ‘‘He chose that color because he wanted to finish his
of speech. This raises the likelihood that any associ- picture.’’). Nonetheless, our findings corroborate the
ation between mental-state talk and social under- findings of Jenkins et al. (2003) that exposure to family
standing may be explained by this overlap with talk about cognitions, rather than desires, was asso-
connectedness; this possibility was confirmed in the ciated with increases in children’s cognitive talk from
Mother – Child Talk and Early Social Understanding 213

2 to 4 years of age. That said, we also found that during reminiscing are typically embedded in an
mothers’ cognitive references did not explain unique explanatory narrative and so have been presented as
variance in children’s social understanding once critical for children’s theory of mind (Fivush &
effects of child verbal comprehension, age 2 social Nelson, 2006). In the present study, mothers did, in
understanding, the quantity of mother and child talk, some instances, explain their mental state during
children’s mental-state references, and, importantly, connected conversations; in the following extract,
mothers’ connected turns were controlled. In contrast, for example, a mother explains her thoughts about
mothers’ connectedness was not only a significant the insect she inspects with her daughter.
correlate but also an independent predictor of child-
ren’s social understanding at the age of 4 years. This
finding may carry implications for those of previous
studies, as apparently strong associations between Child 1, 2, 3, 4. (counting the insect’s legs)
Mother 5. I think he’s having a bad day.
mental-state talk and children’s social understanding
Child Because he’s, because he’s?
may reflect covarying effects of connectedness.
Mother Because he’s missing a leg. He should have six.
Why might mothers’ connectedness matter? Just as
a child’s acquisition of language is accelerated by
adults’ sensitivity in labeling objects within the
Mental states are not, however, always explained
child’s focus of attention (Tomasello & Barton, 1994;
in connected conversations; for example, in the fol-
Tomasello & Farrar, 1986; Tomasello & Todd, 1983), so
lowing extract, the references to mental states are not
too, a child’s social understanding appears to be
explained but are used to highlight the discrepancy
enhanced by adults’ comments that are related to
between the mother’s and child’s beliefs.
a child’s utterances. Such a shared conversational
focus makes salient the similarities or differences
between the child’s and interlocutor’s points of view.
In addition, connectedness enables conversational Child What’s the noise?
partners to coconstruct a shared perspective. Alter- Mother A neighbor talking?
natively, connected interactions may be characterized Child A lion.
by reduced negativity, which in turn is likely to be Mother I hope it’s not a lion.
beneficial for children’s understanding of mind and Child It is a lion
emotion (Ruffman, Perner, & Parkin, 1999). Support Mother It is a lion? I thought we
for the latter proposal comes from recent findings only had lions in zoos in this country.
demonstrating that mothers who tended to attribute
mental states to their 4- to 5-year-old children dis- Why then might references to mental states be
played reduced hostility and intrusiveness when especially meaningful when couched within con-
interacting with their children (Lok & McMahon, nected turns? Recall that connected turns are seman-
2006). However, connected conflictual utterances tically related to a child’s comment, and so, by
were included in our indices of connectedness. This definition, mental-state references that occur within
suggests that, for the 2-year-olds in our sample at a connected dialogue are relevant to the child’s
least, connectedness is not simply a proxy for the current focus of attention. Put another way, engage-
affective tone of an interaction. ment with, rather than exposure to, mental-state talk
It would be premature, however, to conclude that appears to be valuable in promoting children’s social
explicit labeling of mental states does not matter for understanding. This proposal converges with the
children’s social understanding. When we examined recent literature on maternal mind-mindedness that
the interplay between mental-state and connected highlights the particular importance of mothers’
talk, we found that children’s and mothers’ mental- ‘‘appropriate’’ comments on their infants’ mental
states’ references within connected (but not within states for their children’s later performance on false
failed or conflictual) turns did predict children’s belief tasks (Meins et al., 2002).
social understanding at the age of 4 years. One Talk about emotions may in particular comprise an
account of this finding is that mental states can only essential cornerstone in the development of young
be fully explained within connected conversations. In children’s psychological understanding (Fivush &
line with this proposal, researchers have emphasized Nelson, 2006; Lagattuta & Wellman, 2002, p. 557).
that explanations within family talk help children Early parent – child talk about past experiences, par-
understand emotions (Denham, Zoller, & Couchod, ticularly those involving negative emotions, contains
1994). Likewise, references to past mental states much discussion of both the external and internal
214 Ensor and Hughes

causes of feelings (Brown & Dunn, 1991; Lagattuta & Chandler, M., Fritz, A., & Hala, S. (1989). Small-scale
Wellman, 2002). Such discussions, however, did not deceit: Deception as a marker of two-, three-, and four-
occur with sufficient frequency to permit fine-grained year-olds’ early theories of mind. Child Development, 60,
analysis in the present study. Instead, what mattered 1263 – 1277.
for children’s social understanding in the current Clarke-Stewart, K. (1973). Interactions between mothers
and their young children: Characteristics and conse-
study was the ‘‘meeting of minds’’ within conversa-
quences. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
tion. That is, our findings indicated that connected- Development, 38, 1 – 108.
ness is also a crucial context for the mental-state Clarke-Stewart, K., & Hevey, C. (1981). Longitudinal
references that promotes children’s social under- relations in repeated observations of mother-child inter-
standing. Excerpts from children’s conversations at actions from 1 to 2 and a half years. Developmental
different ages are now needed to assess how child- Psychology, 17, 127 – 145.
ren’s awareness of mental states contributes to family Clements, W., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding
conversations. Further research is also necessary to of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377 – 397.
examine the importance of children’s exposure to Cook, R., & Weisberg, S. (1982). Residuals and influence in
conversations in which they are not directly involved. regression. New York: Chapman & Hall.
By documenting the variety of children’s experiences Denham, S. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior,
and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation.
of connectedness, we hope that future research will
Child Development, 57, 194 – 201.
further improve our understanding of how these Denham, S., Zoller, D., & Couchod, E. (1994). Socialisation
conversations enable children to venture beyond the of preschoolers’ emotion understanding. Developmental
here and now of human action to engage in a ‘‘meeting Psychology, 30, 928 – 936.
of minds.’’ de Rosnay, M., & Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and
theory of mind: Do children talk their way to socio-
References cognitive understanding? British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 24(1), 7 – 37.
Appleton, M., & Reddy, V. (1996). Teaching 3-year-olds to de Villiers, J. (2000). Language and theory of mind: What are
pass false belief tests: A conversational approach. Social the developmental relationships. In S. Baron-Cohen, H.
Development, 5, 275 – 291. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other
Balas, V. (2003). Drôles de plats! Paris: Nathan. minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. (1995). Children talk about the (pp. 83 – 123). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding.
Bloom, P., & German, T. (2000). Two reasons to abandon Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, Dunn, J. (1993). Young children’s close relationships: Beyond
77, B25 – B31. attachment (Vol. 4). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bowerman, B., & O’Connell, R. (1990). Linear statistical Dunn, J., & Brophy, M. (2005). Communication, relation-
models: An applied approach. Belmont, CA: Duxbury. ships, and individual differences in children’s under-
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal standing of mind. In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why
states: The acquisition of a theory of mind. Developmental language matters for theory of mind. New York: Oxford
Psychology, 18, 906 – 921. University Press.
Brophy, M., & Dunn, J. (2002). What did mummy say? Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1993). Early conversations about
Dyadic interactions between young ‘‘hard to manage’’ causality: Content, pragmatics and developmental
children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child change. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11,
Psychology, 30, 103 – 112. 107 – 123.
Brown, J., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about
about mental states? The significance of children’s feeling states and children’s later understanding of
conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child others’ emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27, 448 – 455.
Development, 67, 836 – 849. Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade,
Brown, J., & Dunn, J. (1991). ‘‘You can cry, mum’’: The L. (1991). Young children’s understanding of other
social and developmental implications of talk about people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and
internal states. Special Issue: Perspectives on the child’s their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 1352 – 1366.
theory of mind: II. British Journal of Developmental Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. (1999). Understanding others, and
Psychology, 9, 237 – 256. individual differences in friendship interactions in
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univer- young children. Social Development, 8, 201 – 219.
sity Press. Dunn, L. (1997). British Picture Vocabulary Scale – Revised.
Cassidy, K. (1998). Preschoolers’ use of desires to solve Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.
theory of mind problems in a pretense context. Develop- Elliott, C., Murray, D., & Pearson, L. (1983). British abilities
mental Psychology, 34, 503 – 511. scales. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.
Mother – Child Talk and Early Social Understanding 215

Ensor, T. (2005). Quality and content of talk: A calculation tool. psychological understanding. Developmental Psychology,
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 38, 564 – 580.
Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2006). Parent-child reminiscing Leslie, A., & Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the
locates the self in the past. British Journal of Developmental false belief task: Two conjectures. Developmental Science,
Psychology, 24, 235 – 251. 1, 247 – 254.
Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language
communication, self, and culture. Chicago: University of in the development of false belief understanding: A
Chicago Press. training study. Child Development, 74, 1130 – 1144.
Garnham, W., & Ruffman, T. (2001). Doesn’t see, doesn’t Lok, S. M., & McMahon, C. A. (2006). Mothers’ thoughts
know: Is anticipatory looking really related to under- about their children: Links between mind-mindedness
standing of belief? Developmental Science, 4, 94 – 100. and emotional availability. British Journal of Developmen-
Gottman, J. (1983). How children become friends. Mono- tal Psychology, 24, 477 – 488.
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M.,
48(Serial No. 201). Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mind-
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. Syntax and edness and attachment security as predictors of theory
Semantics, 3, 26 – 40. of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715 –
Hala, S., & Chandler, M. (1996). The role of strategic 1726.
planning in accessing false belief understanding. Child Meltzoff, A., Gopnik, A., & Repacholi, B. (1999). Toddlers’
Development, 67, 2948 – 2966. understanding of intentions, desires and emotions: Ex-
Hala, S., Chandler, M., & Fritz, A. (1991). Fledgling plorations of the dark ages. In P. Zelazo, J. Astington, &
theories of mind: Deception as a marker of three-year- D. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social
olds’ understanding of false belief. Child Development, 62, understanding and self-control (pp. 17 – 41). Mahwah, NJ:
83 – 97. Erlbaum.
Harrist, A., & Waugh, R. (2002). Dyadic synchrony: Its Moerbeek, K. (1994). Can’t sleep. London: Western Publi-
structure and function in children’s development. Devel- shing.
opmental Review, 22, 555 – 592. Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways into the community
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1992). American parenting of of minds. In J. Astington & J. Baird (Eds.), Why language
language-learning children: Persisting differences in matters for theory of mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
family-child interactions observed in natural home Press.
environments. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1096 – 1105. Newton, P., Reddy, V., & Bull, R. (2000). Children’s
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday deception and performance on false belief
everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18,
Brookes. 297 – 317.
Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Olson, D. R. (1988). On the origins of belief and other
Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British intentional states in children. In J. W. Astington, P. L.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233 – 253. Harris, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of mind
Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and (pp. 414 – 426). New York: Cambridge University Press.
emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state Peterson, C. (2002). Drawing insight from pictures: The
talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, development of concepts of false drawing and false
34, 1026 – 1037. belief in children with deafness, normal hearing, and
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and autism. Child Development, 73, 1442 – 1459.
theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Peterson, C. (2004). Theory-of-mind development in oral
Developmental Psychology, 43, 1447 – 1459. deaf children with cochlear implants or conventional
Hughes, C., Jaffee, S., Happé, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & hearing aids. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Moffitt, T. (2005). Origins of individual differences in 45, 1096 – 1106.
theory of mind: From nature to nurture? Child Develop- Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation
ment, 76, 356 – 370. and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Psychiatry, 36, 459 – 474.
Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (2000). Insights into theory of
language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, mind from deafness and autism. In M. Coltheart, & M.
27, 236 – 248. Davies (Eds.), Pathologies of belief (pp. 123 – 144). Malden,
Jenkins, J., Turrell, S., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S., & Ross, H. MA: Blackwell.
(2003). A longitudinal investigation of the dynamics Repacholi, B., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about
of mental state talk in families. Child Development, 74, desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month olds. Develop-
905 – 920. mental Psychology, 34, 1017 – 1025.
Lagattuta, K., & Wellman, H. (2002). Differences in early Repacholi, B., & Slaughter, V. (Eds.). (2003). Individual
parent-child conversations about negative versus posi- differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and
tive emotions: Implications for the development of atypical development. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
216 Ensor and Hughes

Ruffman, T., Perner, J., & Parkin, L. (1999). How parenting Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and
style affects false belief understanding. Social Develop- early language. Child Development, 57, 1454 – 1463.
ment, 8, 395 – 411. Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation acquisition style. First Language, 4, 197 – 211.
between children’s and mothers’ mental state language Wellman, H., & Banerjee, M. (1991). Mind and emotion:
and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, Children’s understanding of the emotional consequen-
73, 734 – 751. ces of beliefs and desires. British Journal of Developmental
Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy Psychology, 9, 191 – 214.
of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. Wellman, H., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-
Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of analysis of theory of mind development: The truth
communication skills: Modifications in the speech of about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655 – 684.
young children as a function of listener. Monographs Wellman, H., Phillips, A., & Rodriguez, T. (2000). Young
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38, children’s understanding of perception, desire, and
1 – 37. emotion. Child Development, 71, 895 – 912.
Siegal, M., & Beattie, K. (1991). Where to look first for Wilson, A., Smith, M., & Ross, H. (2003). The nature and ef-
children’s knowledge of false beliefs. Cognition, 38, 1 – 12. fects of young children’s lies. Social Development, 12, 21 – 45.
Slomkowski, C., & Dunn, J. (1996). Young children’s Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs:
understanding of other people’s beliefs and feelings representation and constraining function of wrong be-
and their connected communication with friends. Devel- liefs in young children’s understanding of deception.
opmental Psychology, 32, 442 – 447. Cognition, 13, 103 – 128.
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in Woolfe, T., Want, S., & Siegal, M. (2002). Signposts to
nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child
639 – 650. Development, 73, 768 – 778.

You might also like