You are on page 1of 8

Perspectives

genetics as they emerge. The ultimate goal


opinion
is a diagnostic classification system for mental
disorders that is based not only on clinical
Can neuroscience be integrated symptoms but also on the aetiology and
pathophysiology of the disorders.
into the DSM‑V? Defining mental disorders
Mental disorders are a diverse group of brain
Steven E. Hyman disorders that primarily affect emotion,
higher cognition and executive function. The
Abstract | To date, the diagnosis of mental disorders has been based on clinical boundary between mental and neurological
observation, specifically: the identification of symptoms that tend to cluster disorders is arbitrary, reflecting the partly
together, the timing of the symptoms’ appearance, and their tendency to resolve, different and partly overlapping practice pat-
recur or become chronic. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders terns of the two medical specialities that treat
and the International Classification of Disease, the manuals that specify these disorders of the nervous system. Neurology
has tended to focus on disorders that have
diagnoses and the criteria for making them, are currently undergoing revision. It is
well-defined lesions, and on those that affect
thus timely to ask whether neuroscience has progressed to the point that the next sensation and movement. However, both
editions of these manuals can usefully incorporate information about brain neurologists and psychiatrists treat disorders
structure and function. of higher brain function, such as autism,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD), Tourette’s disorder and Alzheimer’s
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of mechanisms that underlie higher brain disease. In addition, as the neural circuits
Mental Disorders (DSM), fourth edition, function, the complexity of the genetic and that are involved in mental disorders are
text revision (TR)1 and the International developmental underpinnings of normal identified, treatments such as deep brain
Classification of Disease (ICD), tenth edition2 and abnormal behavioural variation, and stimulation, which were once reserved for
(chapter V) are the two diagnostic systems the unsatisfactory nature of current animal neurological disorders with focal patholo-
that list the currently recognized mental models of mental disorders. The existence gies (for example, Parkinson’s disease), are
disorders and the criteria for diagnosing them. of only a small number of well-validated now also beginning to be applied to mental
The American Psychiatric Association biomarkers and the early stage in which disorders, such as depression3. The term
produces the DSM, and the World Health our understanding of neurogenetics and ‘mental disorders’ is an unfortunate anach-
Organization produces the ICD. The ICD pathophysiology finds itself have, reason- ronism, one retained from a time when these
covers all medical diagnosis, with chapter V ably enough, impeded the incorporation disorders were not universally understood
being dedicated to mental and behavioural of neuroscience into psychiatric diagnosis to reflect abnormalities of brain structure,
disorders; the DSM is concerned only with to date. However, neuroscience provides connectivity or function. Although the
mental disorders. The DSM-IV and the much that is relevant and useful — even if it central role of the brain in these disorders is
ICD‑10 share their intellectual underpin- currently falls short of providing a basis for no longer in doubt, the identification of the
nings and are therefore similar, but they individual diagnoses. Indeed, I will argue precise neural abnormalities that underlie
differ in some important details. The DSM that neurobiological information can, along the different mental disorders has stubbornly
system is used far more widely in research, with clinical observations and family and defied investigative efforts.
and will thus be the focus of this Perspective, genetic studies, help to shape a reconsidera- The earliest attempts to standardize
but similar considerations are relevant to tion of the important aspects of the DSM medical diagnoses derived from the needs
the ICD. Both the DSM-IV and the ICD‑10 system. Moreover, the DSM‑V should be of policy makers to have statistical reporting
are currently being revised, in processes that structured to allow the incorporation of that would be meaningful across localities.
involve large numbers of researchers from well-replicated findings from neuroscience Historically, the first detailed classifications
around the world; it is thus an opportune and genetics as they emerge — without focused on causes of death, and only later
time to ask whether neuroscience is ready forcing us to wait a decade or more for the on the causes of morbidity and disability,
for the DSM‑V and the ICD‑11, and whether DSM-VI. In this Perspective, I begin with a including mental disorders. A count of
they in turn are ready for neuroscience. brief history of diagnosis and discuss some the institutionalized ‘insane’ was added
The substantial gaps in our knowledge of the shortcomings of the current diagnos- to the United States census in the mid‑
of the neurobiology that underlies mental tic systems. I then argue for experimentation nineteenth century as part of an attempt to
disorders derive in large part from the dif- within the DSM‑V that would facilitate the quantify individuals who were dependent
ficulty of characterizing the circuitry and incorporation of data from neuroscience and on the state (Timeline). Mental disorders

nature reviews | neuroscience volume 8 | september 2007 | 725


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group
Perspectives

Timeline | The development of a diagnostic classification system for mental disorders


The US Census Office, concerned with The International Classification of The DSM-III is produced, making use of field-
morbidity, mortality and dependence Causes of Death, the precursor to the tested, operationalized criteria to achieve
on the state, first records “the idiotic International Classification of Diseases improved reliability. In an attempt to attain
and insane” (as a single class) in the (ICD) is adopted in Paris. There is little universal acceptance, it contains no theories
decennial census. mention of mental disorders. of aetiology (including neural aetiology). The DSM-V is
expected. Ideally it
The US Census first attempts to classify The American Medico-Psychological Association (the will include
patients by type of mental disorder predecessor to the American Psychiatric Association) The DSM-IV is produced. experimental
(mania, melancholia, monomania, general issues the Statistical Manual for the Use of Institutions for It is a conservative criterion sets aimed
paralysis of the insane, dementia, the Insane, the first standardized psychiatric nosology (it revision of the DSM-III, at incorporating
dipsomania and epilepsy). The limitations had 22 categories, which largely referred to the somatic with explicitly high new genetic and
of the classification are widely recognized causes of behavioral disorders), to aid the Census thresholds for changing neurobiological
and a broad consensus is lacking. Bureau. It has little influence on clinical psychiatry. criteria. findings.

1840 1850 1880 1890 1893 1918 1948 1952 1968 1980 1994 2000 2011

The US Census enumerates The International Statistical The DSM-II is developed at The DSM-IVTR (text
the insane as a separate class. Classification of Diseases, approximately the same time as the revision) is produced.
Injuries and Causes of Death, ICD-8. It is an attempt to stabilize The text is revised,
1890s. Emil Kraepelin, studying thousands of sixth revision, is produced. It is diagnostic nomenclature in but the diagnostic
patients at his clinic in Heidelberg, Germany, the first to contain a section on textbooks and professional literature, criteria are not.
identified symptoms, signs and outcomes mental disorders, although this but it contains no major conceptual
common to patients with dementia praecox is widely viewed as perfunctory. differences to the DSM-I.
(schizophrenia) and manic-depressive
psychosis (bipolar disorder). His focus on the The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-I) is published by
general characteristics of the diseases rather the American Psychiatric Association. It is a manual of approved terms used for
than on individual life stories went into clinical diagnosis and for the statistical coding of psychiatric case records that
decline with the rise of psychoanalysis, but contains brief descriptive paragraphs for each disorder. It was strongly influenced
regained influence in the 1970s with the by psychoanalytical thinking, which saw psychopathology as a reaction to
return of medically-orientated diagnoses. developmental and recent experience rather than as something based in biology63.

were added to international classifications the mainstream medicine of the day. They contrary, there are striking cross-cultural
in the 1940s, partly as a result of the recogni- argued that a valid diagnosis of schizophrenia similarities in the symptoms of major disor-
tion of the toll of psychiatric casualties in and, by extension, of other mental disorders, ders such as autism, schizophrenia, bipolar
World War II, and partly as a result of the would result from a clinical description disorder, major depression and obsessive–
acknowledgement that mental disorders had that identifies clusters of symptoms that compulsive disorder (OCD) and, based on
a major impact on a person’s overall health occur together, from laboratory studies, family and twin studies, there is substantial
status. In the United States, these factors from a clear separation of one disorder from evidence for the heritability of these major
contributed to the publication of the DSM‑I another, from long-term follow-up studies mental disorders. Such observations suggest
by the American Psychiatric Association. to establish the stability of the diagnosis over that the current criteria, at least for the major
The DSM‑I and the DSM-II reflected the time, and from family studies. This approach disorders, pick out something real, even if
dominant psychoanalytical ideas of their provided the intellectual basis for the they do so imprecisely. Consistent with this
time, and so they emphasized the suggested DSM-III and its successor editions. The picture, current diagnoses can be used to
roles for experience in psychopathology and individual diagnoses were accompanied by select treatments, but the drugs do not respect
down-played the role of biology. The impetus diagnostic criteria, in order to make it possi- the boundaries of the disorders as defined
for the more detailed, medically orientated ble for different observers to make diagnoses in the DSM. Thus, antidepressants can treat
approaches to psychiatric diagnosis that with adequate reliability. many DSM-IVTR anxiety disorders, as well
characterize the DSM-III4 arguably arose as OCD and depression, and antipsychotic
following the birth of psychopharmacology. Validity and reliability of the DSM drugs can treat schizophrenia and bipolar
With the availability of a diverse range of The diagnostic criteria contained within disorder, as well as many other disorders.
efficacious treatments, including stimulants, the current DSM (DSM-IVTR) may The main stumbling block to the devel-
lithium, antipsychotic drugs, multiple classes increase the reliability of diagnoses com- opment of valid diagnoses is the complex-
of antidepressants, benzodiazepines and, pared with the situation before DSM-III6. ity of the disorders’ underlying biology.
later, cognitive–behavioural psychotherapies, However, gains in validity were far less However, the structure of the DSM clas-
it was clearly important to be able to make substantial7, owing to the lack of adequately sification itself may also have contributed to
diagnoses that would give precision to the replicated information regarding specific the problem. For example, a fairly arbitrary
selection of patients for clinical trials and, genetic or non-genetic risk factors, anatomi- decision was made to favour ‘splitting’ symp-
subsequently, to clinical treatment decisions. cal substrates and pathophysiology, or any toms over ‘lumping’ them, which resulted in
The seminal paper of Robins and Guze5 objective medical tests for mental disorders. the creation of a large number of disorders.
conceptualized the diagnosis of schizophre- This is not to say that the definitions of In addition, all disorders in the DSM system
nia as an exemplary mental disorder, in the major mental disorders as they appear were defined as categorical, that is, as states
terms of the approaches that characterized within the DSM-IVTR are arbitrary. On the that can be qualitatively separated from

726 | september 2007 | volume 8 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group
Perspectives

the state of being ‘well’. There is evidence, Overall, there is evidence that the cur- Despite the challenges that are detailed
however, that many mental disorders might rent diagnoses and their corresponding sets below, progress in neurogenetics, neuroim-
be better conceptualized as dimensional of criteria fall short of mapping nature22,23. aging and other areas of neuroscience is
traits. Dimensional or quantitative traits that Nonetheless, one should not be excessively beginning to yield significant insights into
are continuous with the ‘normal’ state are critical of the DSM system: throughout mental disorders.
consistent with the polygenic mode of inher- medicine, common, genetically complex
itance that is thought to characterize most diseases are being further categorized
mental disorders. The number or expression into new entities with different risk genes, The disparity between the
pattern of risk gene variants carried by ages of onset, outcomes and treatment
a given individual, and the interaction responses24. For example, in lung cancer, actual clinical presentations
of these variants with non-genetic factors of specific somatic mutations predict different of disorders and their criteria
varying strength, might produce more or treatment responses25. Similarly, in depres- in the DSM-IVTR is greatest
fewer symptoms of greater or lesser severity. sion, the response to a treatment may be
for children, but it also occurs
Mental disorders that might be captured predicted by a single nucleotide polymorphism
well by dimensional approaches include (SNP) in the FKBP5 gene, which encodes a frequently in adults.
depression8, schizophrenia9,10,11, autism12, chaperone that has a role in glucocorticoid
personality disorders13 and ADHD. receptor regulation26. From the point of
Considering the above points, it should view of revising diagnostic criteria in the Neurogenetics of mental disorders. Twin
not be surprising that in the clinic a large near future, however, there is a major dif- studies22,28,29 and, where they have been
percentage of patients do not fit the DSM- ference between our ability to apply such performed, adoption studies30–32, have
IVTR criteria with precision; the DSM-IVTR findings to cancer and our ability to apply demonstrated that genes exert a significant
copes with this problem by including, within them to depression. With lung cancer, there influence on the risk for many mental
groupings of similar disorders, a catch-all is a nosological framework based on the disorders, including autism, schizophrenia,
category termed ‘not otherwise specified’ direct pathological examination of human bipolar disorder, depression and addictive
(NOS). Among many families of disorders, tumours which increasingly includes disorders. However, specific risk genes
such as pervasive developmental disorders analysis of patterns of gene expression and have not yet been identified with adequate
(PDDs) and eating disorders14,15, the NOS of somatic mutations. Thus, for example, certainty to warrant their inclusion in the
diagnosis is often more commonly used than mutations in the epidermal growth fac- highly influential diagnostic manuals.
any of the specifically named disorders. The tor receptor have been sequenced in the The difficulty in identifying risk genes for
disparity between the actual clinical presen- tumours of multiple individuals with lung mental disorders results partly from the lack
tations of disorders and their criteria in the cancer and correlated with risk factors, cell of objective tests to narrow populations for
DSM-IVTR is greatest for children16,17, but it types and treatment responses. In depres- genetic study and, in large part, from the
also occurs frequently in adults. sion, the nosological framework is based on complexity of genetic risk33.
An additional problem is that a large clinical observation of symptoms, and the What we now call a single disorder might
fraction of patients with any DSM-IVTR significance for diagnostic subtyping of the result from the interaction of a large number
diagnosis qualifies for multiple diagnoses FKBP5 biomarker is difficult to establish. of common genetic variants, with no variant
— this situation is termed ‘co-morbidity’17,18. For the time being, I would propose that proving either necessary or sufficient for
Throughout medicine, there are situations in replicated findings that are potentially rel- developing the disorder34; this situation is
which one illness is a risk factor for others; evant to psychiatric diagnosis be evaluated thought to characterize the common forms
for example, diabetes mellitus is a risk by a committee, perhaps one appointed by of mental disorders. Alternatively, the dis-
factor for retinal disease, renal disease, the organizations that publish the diagnos- order might result from diverse individual
cardiovascular disease and neuropathy. This tic manuals. If this committee, by using mutations, and thus actually represent a
may also be the case for some mental disor- transparently enunciated criteria, finds such large family of rare Mendelian diseases with
ders; for example, bipolar disorder appears biomarkers to be adequately convincing, a similar pathophysiology, such as in retinitis
to be a risk factor for substance use disor- they can be posted as candidates for inclu- pigmentosa35. A more recent hypothesis is
ders19. However, co-morbidity might also sion in subsequent revisions of diagnostic that some disorders might result from new
reflect different patterns of symptoms that criteria. The goal would be to encourage germline mutations that may act against
result from shared genetic risk factors. Thus, relevant research in appropriate ill and well certain genetic backgrounds, as has been
DSM-IVTR-defined cases of major depres- populations. suggested for some cases of autism and
sion and generalized anxiety disorder may schizophrenia36. In addition, twin studies
co-occur at high rates because they represent What has neuroscience taught us? have demonstrated that heredity has a sig-
different faces of the same underlying risk Owing to the limited understanding of the nificant role in the major mental disorders,
genes20, and thus perhaps the same underly- biological underpinnings of mental disor- but that non-genetic factors also play a part.
ing disease processes. Co-morbidity could ders that neuroscience and genetics could Indeed, there is no common mental disorder
also be an artefact that arises from errors in provide at the time, the diagnoses within for which monozygotic twin pairs are 100%
the lumping and splitting of symptoms, so the DSM-III and the DSM-IV27 have neces- concordant. Environmental risk factors for
that a single pathophysiological process can sarily been based on clinical observation. mental illness have been difficult to establish
cause symptoms that meet the criteria for Despite the optimism of Robins and Guze, with certainty37, and where they have been
multiple DSM-IVTR entities. This appears laboratory tests for the major, common psy- established (as in the case of stress or nega-
to be the case for many personality disorder chiatric disorders have not yet materialized. tive life events), they may be risk factors for
diagnoses21. Yet excessive pessimism is not warranted. multiple disorders (a situation shared with

nature reviews | neuroscience volume 8 | september 2007 | 727


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group
Perspectives

many disorders in general medicine, where, Thus, the COMT gene was thought to be a significance of genetic variants alone and in
for example, smoking and obesity may be candidate gene for schizophrenia, based on combination. This will be a long process, but
risk factors for many disorders). both biological and positional information. in the end it will contribute enormously to
Older genetic linkage methodologies A common variant within the COMT gene disease classification, predictors of outcome
that have been applied to mental disorders results in there being either a valine (Val) or and selection of treatments.
lack the power to find risk genes of small a methionine (Met) within the enzyme. The
effect33, although they have identified Val allele results in higher enzyme activity, Neuroimaging studies. Both structural and
positional candidate genes, such as the suggesting that individuals with this variant functional neuroimaging have provided
neuregulin 1 gene in schizophrenia38, would have lower levels of dopamine in important tools for the investigation of
that provide promising leads. In addition, their prefrontal cortex, and perhaps also mental disorders. Structural MRI has begun
‘biological’ candidate genes have been diminished cognitive performance. An to convincingly demonstrate patterns of
proposed based on findings from neuro- initial study reported an association of the grey-matter thinning in individuals with
science and pharmacology; thus, the gene Val allele with schizophrenia41; subsequent schizophrenia46, as well as longitudinal
that encodes the serotonin (5-HT) reuptake studies and a meta-analysis questioned patterns of grey-matter loss over time in
transporter (5HTT), which is the molecular this association42. Associations have also patients with childhood-onset schizophre-
target of many antidepressant drugs, has been reported and disconfirmed for bipolar nia47. This increasingly clear demonstration
been proposed to be a candidate gene for disorder43. Several association studies that of anatomical abnormalities in schizophre-
depression and for other conditions in examined prefrontal cortex-dependent cog- nia is likely to have significant implications
which selective serotonin reuptake inhibi- nitive performance have found poorer per- for studies of pathophysiology and, as will
tor antidepressants are efficacious39. SNPs formance in healthy subjects, schizophrenics be discussed below, for future diagnostic
and other forms of DNA sequence variation and unaffected siblings of schizophrenics classifications.
in both positional and biological candidate who have the Val allele. Other studies have Functional imaging studies have already
genes have been used to investigate possible failed to replicate such findings on some contributed to experimental therapies for
associations with disease phenotypes22,39. or all cognitive tests, or else have found individuals with treatment-resistant depres-
The combination of specific variants within complex relationships between the dosage sion. Studies that used positron emission
candidate genes (such as the 5HTT gene) of Val or Met variants and performance in tomography (PET) suggested that severely
and environmental factors (such as life both healthy and ill subjects43. Even if we depressed individuals exhibited excessive
stress) have also been tested as risks for look past the non-replications, complex activity in the subgenual cingulate cortex48.
depression40 and other disorders. questions remain as to what this variant With successful antidepressant treatment,
For many candidate genes there has been might mean for nosology. Is it a risk factor this activity returned towards normal49.
both replication and non-replication of for one or several disorders? Does it define Inducing sadness in normal subjects acti-
findings22,39. This is not surprising given the subgroups in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder vates this same region of the cingulate cor-
complexity of the genetic risk for mental dis- or schizoaffective disorder? Does it influence tex50. Based on this body of findings, patients
orders, but it should make the research com- cognition in all individuals, and thus modify with depression who did not improve with
munity conservative about using individual the symptom pattern when it also happens medication, psychotherapy or electrocon-
SNPs to revise official diagnoses. However, to occur in schizophrenic subjects? vulsive therapy had electrodes placed in the
there is a more subtle problem for the use Despite the genetic complexity of mental subgenual cingulate cortex and received
of individual genetic variants to define disorders, it is reasonable to hope that new deep brain stimulation. In the initial series,
diagnostic categories: single candidate genes technologies, such as high-density whole- four of six patients had sustained responses3.
provide only a narrow window on disorders genome association studies with very large Perhaps the most important implication of
that may prove to be polygenic. If genetic sample sizes, will provide increasingly com- this experimental treatment is that it is pos-
findings are to influence diagnostic catego- plete information about genetic risk factors. sible to identify the specific neural circuits
ries, there must be a more complete context This is already the case for other genetically that are involved in depression and use them
in which to place them. We must be able to complex disorders such as diabetes mellitus as a treatment target.
determine whether a specific genetic variant type II and inflammatory bowel disease44,45. Non-invasive neuroimaging cannot yield
contributes to the definition of a disorder, a In this context, genetic epidemiologists will information as precise as that derived from
subgroup within a disorder, a symptom that be better able to team-up with cognitive direct examination of a diseased tissue or
might be shared across multiple disorders, neuroscientists, clinical neuroscientists and from the culture of a disease-causing micro-
or if it is interpretable only in the context of pharmacologists to define the biological organism. Thus, findings from neuroimaging
other genetic findings. will probably prove most useful for diagnosis
This challenge is illustrated by studies when they are combined with other types of
of the gene that encodes catechol‑O- we should create information, including clinical data, genetic
methyltransferase (COMT), an enzyme circumstances in which new information and cognitive testing.
that is involved in the metabolism of cat-
echolamine neurotransmitters (including
information from genetics, How might neuroscience improve DSM‑V?
dopamine). This gene lies on the q11 region cognitive neuroscience, brain Despite the kinds of advances described
of chromosome 22, along with many other imaging, animal studies above, it is clear that our understanding of
genes. A microdeletion of this region pro- and so on can contribute mental disorders is still limited. Patients and
duces a complex syndrome (velocardiofacial families, clinical trials and epidemiological
syndrome) that manifests with schizo- to a reconsideration of the research would all benefit from an improved
phrenia-like symptoms (among others). boundaries of disorders. DSM that has firm diagnostic criteria, but

728 | september 2007 | volume 8 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group
Perspectives

they will be harmed if the new criteria rely Box 1 | Symptom clusters in schizophrenia Example disorders
on putatively objective tests that are not Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia is a serious and
validated to a high standard. Thus, it is nec- • Positive symptoms: psychological disabling disorder that generally begins in the
essary to state a caveat: it will be important phenomena that do not occur in healthy late teen years or in early adulthood; it runs
people, such as hallucinations and
to avoid premature inclusion of genetic or a chronic course that is typically punctuated
delusions
neurobiological findings in the DSM, no by episodes of severe psychotic symptoms
matter how interesting they are, if they are • Negative (deficit) symptoms: symptoms that such as delusions and hallucinations. A great
arise from deficits in normal functions.
not adequately replicated or if their relation- deal of research supports the division of
Examples include asocial behaviour,
ship to behavioural or disease phenotypes impoverished content of thought and the symptoms of schizophrenia into three
cannot be established with clarity. At the speech, blunting of emotional responses, clusters: positive, negative and cognitive
same time, a slavish adherence to the current and loss of motivation. symptoms that might reflect different aspects
classification system would impede progress • Cognitive symptoms: impairments in of the pathophysiology and possibly different
in research that is investigating the aetiology working memory and executive functions. genetic risk factors, and that respond differ-
of mental disorders and identifying new entially to current antipsychotic medications
treatments for them. If the current criteria (BOX 1). Like all disorders in the DSM-IVTR,
have not effectively “carved nature at the section entitled ‘criteria sets and axes pro- schizophrenia is defined as a category, and
joints”, then there is a risk that genetic, imag- vided for further study’. However, at present inclusion within this diagnostic class requires
ing and other disease-related studies will be this section has received little attention, as the presence of certain symptoms (the
confounded by the inclusion of heterogeneous evidenced by PubMed searches for studies DSM-IVTR does not require cognitive symp-
populations23,33,51. in which these sets and axes were applied. toms), a deterioration of functioning, and a
The term ‘endophenotype’ has become Thus, a special effort would have to be made rather arbitrary requirement for 6 months
popular for describing putatively simpler, or to encourage the exploration of such criteria of illness (this is supposed to establish that
at least objectively measurable phenotypes, sets by the research community, perhaps schizophrenia is a chronic illness).
such as neuropsychological measures that through supplements to existing research In their classic adoption studies of schizo-
might enhance diagnostic homogeneity. grants, especially in such areas as genetics, phrenia, Kety et al.30 found that the biological
I find this term less than ideal, because it and through meetings of key investigators families of individuals with schizophrenia
implies that the current diagnostic classifica- that are focused on the use of new diagnostic contained members who did not have
tion is basically correct, and that all that is groupings. psychotic symptoms, but who nevertheless
lacking is objective markers for these disor- Experimental approaches towards exhibited less dramatic schizophrenia-like
ders. If, however, the lumping and splitting a novel classification of mental disor- symptoms such as social isolation, suspi-
of symptoms that gave rise to the current ders could take three different forms, ciousness, eccentric beliefs and magical
classification was in error, then the search depending on the situation: dimensional thinking. When such symptoms are chronic
for biological correlates of these disorders approaches; the identification of clini- and impairing, the DSM-IVTR calls for the
will not prove fruitful. Instead, we should cally significant symptom clusters for diagnosis of schizotypal personality disorder.
create circumstances in which new informa- which there are compelling hypotheses Oddly, this is classified as a personality
tion from genetics, cognitive neuroscience, about the underlying neural circuits; and disorder, and is not grouped with schizophre-
brain imaging, animal studies and so on the abandonment of fine-scale splitting nia, even though subsequent studies have
can contribute to a reconsideration of the of disorders to yield larger ‘spectrum’ confirmed a genetic relationship between the
boundaries of disorders. disorders, the constituents of which are two10. More recently, multiple studies have
Considering the above, how might presumed to share pathophysiologi- found that ‘unaffected’ monozygotic twins
neuroscience help us to craft a better cal features52. The use of dimensional of patients with schizophrenia exhibit cogni-
DSM‑V? One way in which neuroscience approaches is consistent with the second tive abnormalities similar to those observed
could contribute to the creation of better and third of these options. In each of in individuals with schizophrenia. These
diagnostic criteria without prematurely these approaches, measures could be include deficits in spatial working memory
disrupting the current relatively reliable but incorporated that are based on findings and divided-attention tasks that demand
certainly imperfect (because of their limited from genetics, cognitive neuroscience, cognitive control53. Milder deficits can be
validity) clinical diagnoses is through the structural and functional neuroimaging observed in discordant dizygotic twin pairs
creation of experimental diagnostic criteria or other neurobiological studies. and in ‘unaffected’ siblings who are not co-
for research purposes that could shadow the The introduction of diagnostic experi- twins54,55. Structural MRI studies of co-twins
‘official’ criteria in the DSM‑V. Experimental ments runs some risk of creating confusion have also shown thinning of the dorsolateral
criteria need not be produced for all disor- that could interfere with the replication of prefrontal cortex, which is the brain region
ders or all groupings of disorders, but only research. This concern notwithstanding, the that is most important for working memory.
where there is enough evidence to warrant reification of the current diagnoses and cri- The thinning is more severe in monozygotic
them. Such experimental criteria could be teria may be the greater evil: the acceptance than in dizygotic co-twins, which may reflect
reconsidered at intervals and updated by of the current system has too often led grant the degree of DNA sharing with the affected
appropriately constituted committees as new review committees, journal referees and co-twin. The co-twins with schizophrenia
information emerges from neuroscience researchers to channel their efforts into the had additional grey matter deficits, the sever-
and genetics, without the need to wait for study of entities that may, in some cases, be ity of which generally correlated with the
an overall revision process that would lead blind alleys. The proposed approaches can severity of the symptoms46. In addition to the
to a new DSM volume. There is a precedent be illustrated briefly by reference to a few structural abnormalities of the dorsolateral
for such criteria: the DSM-IVTR contains a example disorders. prefrontal cortex, there are functional

nature reviews | neuroscience volume 8 | september 2007 | 729


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group
Perspectives

abnormalities in activation that can be functional neuroimaging, such an approach the cognitive functions that are abnormal
detected by PET or by functional MRI in would improve diagnostic reliability as well in schizophrenia will not, by itself, provide
response to working memory tasks. Abnorm­ as address concerns about validity. The use of a complete picture of a disorder that also
alities in activation generally correlate with quantitative scales would also give potential has symptoms such as hallucinations (a
impairments in task performance56,57. clinical status to symptoms that are now positive symptom) and avolition (a negative
In summary, findings from family and considered subsyndromal and therefore not symptom). Yet this type of ‘neural circuit’
genetic studies, cognitive neuroscience worthy of treatment, just as physicians might approach to mental disorders should have
and structural and functional imaging sug- intervene in mild hypertension before waiting substantial benefits for treatment develop-
gest that some important components of for the condition to become severe. ment. If the treatment target is categorical
schizophrenia form a continuum, with less A second possible approach is to ‘decon- DSM-IVTR schizophrenia, then research is
severe conditions observed in blood relatives. struct’ DSM-IVTR disorders such as schizo- hampered by the lack of animal models. If,
Moreover, the greater the genetic relatedness phrenia into related symptom clusters (or instead, treatment development is focused
to an affected individual, the more severe the quantitative dimensions). This would permit on the neural circuits that are involved in
deficits are in unaffected family members. In investigators to focus on those symptoms working memory and cognitive control,
addition, family and genetic studies suggest for which there are promising neurobio- then a great deal of relevant neurobiology
that many individuals who are diagnosed logical leads. In the case of schizophrenia, and pharmacology can be brought to bear59.
with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder or less it might be possible to make substantial Similar neural circuit approaches could
well-defined states such as schizoaffective progress in research on the cognitive be taken with aspects of other disorders.
disorder, may share vulnerability genes22. symptoms without having to focus on the Much is known about reward circuitry,
Based on observations of this kind, and neural underpinnings of delusions or hal- which is relevant not only to addiction and
given a probable polygenic mode of inherit- lucinations (about which little is known to other impulse control disorders, but also to
ance, it has been suggested that genetic and date). Relevant to cognitive symptoms, there depression, which is often characterized by
neurobiological studies of schizophrenia is much research on the neural circuitry that anhedonia. As will be described below, the
would be improved if study subjects could be underlies working memory and executive amygdala-based fear circuitry is increasingly
selected using a dimensional approach10,11,51, function, and this has informed functional well understood. Mapping the symptoms
rather than the categorical approach currently imaging research on schizophrenia56,57. of anxiety disorders to such circuits is
favoured by the DSM-IVTR. It has been Attempts are also being made to find asso- already an important area of research, and
proposed that dimensions could be defined ciations between structural (grey-matter it could have substantial implications for
using quantitative scales for positive, negative thinning) and cognitive (specific forms of treatment development. Perhaps more
and cognitive symptoms. In the future, it memory impairment) phenotypes with speculative is the mapping of the obsessive–
might be possible to develop a meaningful genetic markers in schizophrenia43,58. compulsive disorder spectrum (including
quantitative scale based on grey-matter An approach such as this, which focuses Tourette’s disorder and body dysmorphic
thickness, perhaps in some region of the on the neurobiologically tractable aspects of disorder52) to frontal-striatal-thalamic circuits.
prefrontal cortex. Insofar as dimensions could disorders, clearly has limitations for nosol- The third possible approach to the classi-
be measured objectively using cognitive tests ogy. A focus on the structural and functional fication of psychiatric disorders is to think in
in the laboratory or through structural and abnormalities in the circuitry that underlies terms of larger spectrum disorders. For the

Glossary
Anhedonia ICD‑10 (Chapter V) provide both classifications and Single nucleotide polymorphism
An inability to experience pleasure. diagnostic criteria. (SNP). The most common form of variation in human
DNA sequences. It occurs when a single nucleotide
Candidate gene Dimensional diagnosis (for example, thymine) replaces one of the other three
A gene implicated as one that confers an increased A diagnosis based on states that are defined as above- nucleotides (for example, cytosine).
phenotypic risk, and which is thus deserving of further threshold on one or more quantitative scales or
investigation (for example, in an association study). dimensions and that are continuous with the normal state. Spectrum disorders
Candidate genes can be identified based on biological For example, hypertension is defined in terms of two A group of disorders that are thought to be related
hypotheses, or as a result of their lying within a region of dimensions: systolic and diastolic blood pressure. through the sharing of risk genes or pathophysiological
interest identified by a linkage study or a chromosomal mechanisms.
break point (a so-called ‘positional candidate’). Disorder
A term generally used instead of the term ‘disease’ for Syndrome
Categorical diagnosis medical conditions in which the causative factors or A cluster of symptoms that can result from different
A disease state that is qualitatively separable from the pathophysiology remain unknown. disease processes. For example, cough and fever can
state of being ‘well’, for example, tuberculosis or leukaemia. result from bacterial, viral or fungal infections, or from
Nosology autoimmunity, with very different treatments and
Diagnostic classification The classification of diseases. outcomes.
A listing of diagnoses clustered by relatedness, for
example, cancers, metabolic diseases, infectious diseases Reliability Valid diagnosis
and unintentional injuries. The ICD was first developed to A diagnosis is reliable if the same conclusion is reached by A diagnosis that picks out a real entity based on aetiology
allow statistical reporting across countries, initially of two diagnosticians who examine the patient at or pathophysiology.
mortality and later of morbidity. approximately the same time (inter-rater reliability), or if a
patient receives the same diagnosis if examined more than Validity
Diagnostic criteria once within reasonably close time intervals (test-retest The extent to which a variable measures what it is
The rules for making diagnoses. The DSM-IV and the reliability). intended to measure.

730 | september 2007 | volume 8 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group
Perspectives

‘schizophrenia’ spectrum, this might include Much research in both animals and healthy 1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth
at least schizotypal personality disorder and humans has helped to elucidate the circuitry Edition, Text Revision (American Psychiatric
the non-affective psychoses in the DSM-IVTR that is involved in fear conditioning62. The Association, Washington D.C., 2000)
2. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural
(such as the rather questionable DSM-IVTR mapping of symptom dimensions onto an Disorders (World Health Organization, Geneva, 1992).
entity ‘schizophreniform disorder’). The amygdala-based fear circuitry, as is already 3. Mayberg, H. S. et al. Deep brain stimulation for
treatment-resistant depression. Neuron 45, 651–660
clear disadvantage for research that uses happening in some research programmes, (2005).
larger spectrum diagnoses is the greater could facilitate treatment development by 4. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third edition
heterogeneity of the study populations. The moving the focus from behaviourally defined (American Psychiatric Association, Washington D. C.,
advantage of such lumping is that it encour- syndromes to specific circuits, cells and syn- 1980).
5. Robins, E. & Guze, S. B. Establishment of diagnostic
ages inductive, bottom-up re-analysis of apses. Finally, the lumping of disorders that validity in psychiatric illness: its application to
phenotypes based on factors such as familial involve conditioned fear as a central symptom schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 126, 983–987
(1970).
aggregation of symptom clusters and the dimension (namely simple phobias, PTSD, 6. Pope, H. G. Jr & Lipinski, J. F. Jr. Diagnosis in
segregation of symptoms across generations. panic disorder and social anxiety disorder), schizophrenia and manic-depressive illness: a
reassessment of the specificity of ‘schizophrenic’
It could be asked how frequently cogni- followed by the type of re-analysis described symptoms in the light of current research. Arch. Gen.
tive symptoms (which would ideally be above for schziphrenia, might lead to a Psychiatry 35, 811–828 (1978).
7. Kendel, R. E. Clinical validity. Psychol. Med. 19,
measured dimensionally) co-segregate with redrawing of the boundaries of these disor- 45–55 (1989).
positive or negative symptoms, or with ders that is more consistent with genetic risk 8. Kendler, K. S. & Gardner, C. O. Jr. Boundaries of major
depression: an evaluation of DSM-IV criteria.
other symptoms such as depression or factors, neural circuits and pathophysiology. Am. J. Psychiatry 155, 172–177 (1998).
mania. It could also be asked how measures Although I have, in the interest of brevity, 9. Meehl, P. E. Schizotaxia revisited. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
46, 935–944 (1989).
of regional grey-matter thinning correlate provided heuristic examples from schizo- 10. Fanous, A., Gardner, C., Walsh, D. & Kendler, K. S.
with quantitative measures of symptoms’ phrenia and anxiety disorders, I could just Relationship between positive and negative symptoms
of schizophrenia and schizotypal symptoms in
dimensions. Symptoms, structural findings as well have discussed dimensional, neural nonpsychotic relatives. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
and functional neuroimaging findings could circuit and ‘spectrum re-analysis’ approaches 58, 669–673 (2001).
11. Tsuang, M. T., Stone, W. S., Tarbox, S. I. & Faraone, S. V.
be correlated with genetic risk alleles as they related to the OCD spectrum52, to impulse in Advancing DSM: Dilemmas in Psychiatric Diagnosis
emerge. The results will be quite different control disorders or to mood disorders. (eds Phillips, K. A., First, M. B. & Pincus, H. A.)
105–128 (American Psychiatric Association,
from those that would emerge from attempts Washington D. C., 2003).
to define subtypes of DSM-IVTR schizo- Conclusions and future directions 12. Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Verweij, C. J. &
Boomsma, D. I. Heritability of autistic traits in the
phrenia, because there is a high likelihood It is probably premature to bring neurobiol- general population. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 161,
that this category has artificial boundaries ogy into the formal classification of mental 372–377 (2007).
13. Widiger, T. A. & Costa, P. T. Jr. Personality and
with related disorders. disorders that will form the core of the personality disorders. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103,
DSM‑V. However, it is not too early to use 78–91 (1994).
14. Fairburn, C. G. & Bohn, K. Eating disorder NOS
Anxiety disorders. Anxiety disorders are neurobiology as a central tool to rethink the (EDNOS): an example of the troublesome “not
another type of disorder that could illustrate current approach to mental disorders, and to otherwise specified” (NOS) category in DSM-IV. Behav.
Res. Ther. 43, 691–701 (2005).
how new approaches to disorder classifica- begin some careful experiments that could 15. de Bruin, E. I., Ferdinand, R. F., Meester, S., de Nijs,
tion could be used. The DSM-IVTR category liberate science from the unintended con- P. F. & Verheij, F. High rates of psychiatric co-morbidity
in PDD-NOS. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 877–886
of anxiety disorders currently includes sequences of reifying the current diagnoses (2007).
generalized anxiety disorder, simple phobias, that probably do not mirror nature. Not all 16. Goldstein, S. & Schwebach, A. J. The comorbidity of
pervasive developmental disorder and attention
post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), disorders are equally susceptible to the kinds deficit hyperactivity disorder: results of a retrospective
panic disorder and social phobia as discrete of experiments that I have suggested. It is my chart review. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 329–339
(2004).
anxiety disorders. Yet co-morbidity among hope that the committees that are working 17. Reiersen, A. M., Constantino, J. N., Volk, H. E. & Todd,
the anxiety disorders and with depression is on the DSM‑V can develop and begin test- R. D. Autistic traits in a population-based ADHD twin
sample. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 464–472
common. In addition, all of these disorders ing dimensional criteria for several groups (2007).
respond to antidepressants. Moreover, neu- of disorders, and that they can define ‘larger 18. Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K. R.
& Walters, E. E. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity
roimaging research suggests that hyperactive groupings’ or ‘spectra’ for a smaller group of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National
responses within the fear circuitry, as evi- of disorders based on existing knowledge of Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
62, 617–627 (2005).
denced by hyperactive amygdala responses symptom clustering, familial transmission, 19. Weiss, R. D. et al. Substance use and perceived
to fearful stimuli, may be a general charac- genetics and pathophysiology. The goal of symptom improvement among patients with bipolar
disorder and substance dependence. J. Affect. Disord.
teristic of some of these anxiety disorders60,61. creating larger groupings is to encourage 79, 279–283 (2004).
How might the use of dimensions and re-analysis in the way I described above for 20. Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C.
& Eaves, L. J. Major depression and generalized
the deconstruction and lumping of disorders the schizophrenia spectrum. I hope in addi- anxiety disorder. Same genes, (partly) different
apply to research on anxiety disorders? An tion that mechanisms can be created to post environments? Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 49, 716–722
(1992).
attempt to define dimensions seems war- and evaluate relevant new findings, be they 21. Skodol, A. E. et al. The borderline diagnosis I:
ranted because the thresholds for anxiety genetic markers or neuroimaging results, psychopathology, comorbidity, and personality
structure. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 936–950 (2002).
disorder diagnoses in the DSM-IVTR seem and that there can be interim processes to 22. Craddock, N., O’Donovan, M. C. & Owen, M. J.
arbitrary; moreover, there is much symptom adjust the experimental criteria long before The genetics of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder:
dissecting psychosis. J. Med. Genet. 42, 193–204
overlap between disorders. It might facilitate it is time to start thinking about DSM-VI. (2005).
research to define quantitative dimensions, 23. Happe, F., Ronald, A. & Plomin, R. Time to give up on
Steven E. Hyman is Provost at Harvard University, a single explanation for autism. Nature Neurosci. 9,
such as responsiveness to fear-inducing Massachusetts Hall, Cambridge, Massachusetts 1218–1220 (2006).
stimuli (or some other measure of learned or 02138, USA. 24. Florez, J. C., Hirschhorn, J. & Altshuler, D. The
inherited basis of diabetes mellitus: implications for
conditioned fear), and measures of anticipa- e‑mail: seh@harvard.edu
the genetic analysis of complex traits. Annu. Rev.
tory anxiety, and of distress and dysphoria. doi:10.1038/nrn2218 Genomics Hum. Genet. 4, 257–291 (2003).

nature reviews | neuroscience volume 8 | september 2007 | 731


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group
Perspectives

25. Paez, J. G. et al. EGFR mutations in lung cancer: 41. Egan, M. F. et al. Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met 56. Tan, H. Y. et al. Dysfunctional prefrontal regional
correlation with clinical response to gefitinib therapy. genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for specialization and compensation in schizophrenia.
Science 304, 1497–1500 (2004). schizophrenia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 1969–1977 (2006).
26. Binder, E. B. et al. Polymorphisms in FKBP5 are 6917–6922 (2001). 57. Barch, D. M. et al. Selective deficits in prefrontal
associated with increased recurrence of depressive 42. Munafo, M. R., Bowes, L., Clark, T.G. & Flint, J. cortex function in medication-naive patients with
episodes and rapid response to antidepressant Lack of association of the COMT (Val158/108 Met) schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58, 280–288
treatment. Nature Genet. 36, 1319–1325 (2004). gene and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of case- (2001).
27. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and control studies Mol. Psychiatry 10, 765–770 58. Cannon, T. D. et al. Association of DISC1/TRAX
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth edition (2005). haplotypes with schizophrenia, reduced prefrontal
(American Psychiatric Association, Washington D.C., 43. Craddock, N., Owen, M. J. & O’Donovan, M. C. gray matter, and impaired short- and long-term
1994). The catechol‑O‑methyl transferase (COMT) gene as a memory. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 1205–1213
28. Kendler, K. S., Gatz, M., Gardner, C. O. & candidate for psychiatric phenotypes: evidence and (2005).
Pedersen, N. L. A Swedish national twin study of lessons. Mol. Psychiatry 11, 446–458 (2006). 59. Hyman, S. E. & Fenton, W. S. Medicine. What are the
lifetime major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 44. Cardon, L. R. Genetics. Delivering new disease genes. right targets for psychopharmacology? Science 299,
109–114 (2006). Science 314, 1403–1405 (2006). 350–351 (2003).
29. Freitag, C. M. The genetics of autistic disorders and 45. Sladek, R. et al. A genome-wide association study 60. Etkin, A. et al. Individual differences in trait anxiety
its clinical relevance: a review of the literature. identifies novel risk loci for type 2 diabetes. Nature predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to
Mol. Psychiatry 12, 2–22 (2007). 445, 881–885 (2007). unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron 44,
30. Kety, S. S., Rosenthal, D., Wender, P. H. & Schulsinger, F. 46. Cannon, T. D. et al. Cortex mapping reveals 1043–1055 (2004).
Mental illness in the biological and adoptive families regionally specific patterns of genetic and disease- 61. Shin, L. M. et al. A functional magnetic resonance
of adopted schizophrenics. Am. J. Psychiatry 128, specific gray-matter deficits in twins discordant for imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal
302–306 (1971). schizophrenia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in
31. Kendler, K. S., Gruenberg, A. M. & Strauss, J. S. 3228–3233 (2002). posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
An independent analysis of the Copenhagen sample 47. Thompson, P. M. et al. Mapping adolescent brain 62, 273–281 (2005).
of the Danish Adoption Study of schizophrenia. III. The change reveals dynamic wave of accelerated gray 62. Phelps, E. A. & LeDoux, J. E. Contributions of the
relationship between paranoid psychosis (delusional matter loss in very early-onset schizophrenia. Proc. amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models
disorder) and the schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 11650–11655 (2001). to human behavior. Neuron 48, 175–178 (2005).
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 38, 985–987 (1981). 48. Drevets, W. C. Functional neuroimaging studies of 63. Grob, G. N. Origins of DSM-I: a study in appearance
32. Sigvardsson, S., Bohman, M. & Cloninger, C. R. depression: the anatomy of melancholia. Annu. Rev. and reality. Am. J. Psychiatry 148, 421–431 (1991).
Replication of the Stockholm Adoption Study of Med. 49, 341–361 (1998).
alcoholism. Confirmatory cross-fostering analysis. 49. Mayberg, H. S. et al. Cingulate function in depression: Acknowledgements
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 53, 681–687 (1996). a potential predictor of treatment response. I gratefully acknowledge the probing discussions of my col‑
33. Merikangas, K. R. & Risch, N. Will the genomics Neuroreport 8, 1057–1061 (1997). leagues, whose ideas on this Opinion have tested and shaped
revolution revolutionize psychiatry? Am. J. Psychiatry 50. Mayberg, H. S. et al. Reciprocal limbic-cortical my own thoughts over the past year. I would like to assert
160, 625–635 (2003). function and negative mood: converging PET findings that this work represents my personal viewpoint, not an offi‑
34. Botstein, D. & Risch, N. Discovering genotypes in depression and normal sadness. Am. J. Psychiatry cial position of the DSM‑V Task Force, of which I am a mem‑
underlying human phenotypes: past successes for 156, 675–682 (1999). ber, or of the International Advisory Group to the World
mendelian disease, future approaches for complex 51. Faraone, S. V. et al. Diagnostic accuracy and linkage Health Organization for the Revision of ICD‑10, Chapter V
disease. Nature Genet. 33 (Suppl.), 228–237 analysis: how useful are schizophrenia spectrum (Mental and Behavioral Disorders), of which I am the Chair.
(2003). phenotypes? Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 1286–1290
35. Daiger, S. P., Bowne, S. J. & Sullivan, L. S. Perspective (1995). Competing interests statement
on genes and mutations causing retinitis pigmentosa. 52. Phillips, K. A., Price, L. H., Greenberg, B. D. & The author declares competing financial interests:
Arch. Ophthalmol. 125, 151–158 (2007). Rasmussen, S. A. in Advancing DSM: Dilemmas in see web version for details.
36. McClellan, J. M., Susser, E. & King, M. C. Maternal Psychiatric Diagnosis (eds Phillips, K. A., First, M. B.
famine, de novo mutations, and schizophrenia. JAMA & Pincus, H. A.) 57–84. (American Psychiatric
296, 582–584 (2006). Association, Washington D. C., 2003). DATABASES
37. Kendler, K. S., Davis, C. G. & Kessler, R. C. The 53. Cannon, T. D. et al. The inheritance of Entrez Gene:
familial aggregation of common psychiatric and neuropsychological dysfunction in twins discordant for http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=gene
substance use disorders in the National Comorbidity schizophrenia. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 67, 369–382 5HTT | COMT | FKBP5 | neuregulin‑1
Survey: a family history study. Br. J. Psychiatry 170, (2000). OMIM: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.
541–548 (1997). 54. Brahmbhatt, S. B., Haut, K., Csernansky, J. G. & fcgi?db=OMIM
38. Stefansson, H. et al. Neuregulin 1 and susceptibility to Barch, D. M. Neural correlates of verbal and Alzheimer’s disease | attention deficit hyperactivity disorder |
schizophrenia. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 71, 877–892 nonverbal working memory deficits in individuals with autism | bipolar disorder | obsessive–compulsive disorder |
(2002). schizophrenia and their high-risk siblings. Schizophr. Parkinson’s disease | schizophrenia | Tourette’s disorder
39. Levinson, D. F. The genetics of depression: a review. Res. 87, 191–204 (2006).
Biol. Psychiatry 60, 84–92 (2006). 55. Niendam, T. A. et al. A prospective study of childhood FURTHER INFORMATION
40. Caspi, A. et al. Influence of life stress on depression: neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenic patients Steven Hyman’s homepage: www.provost.harvard.edu
moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. and their siblings. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, ALL LINKS ARE ACTIVE IN THE ONLINE PDF.
Science 301, 386–389 (2003). 2060–2062 (2003).

732 | september 2007 | volume 8 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2007 Nature Publishing Group

You might also like