You are on page 1of 9

419014

novic and SehicJournal of Learning Disabilities


LDXXXX10.1177/0022219411419014Dura

Regular Article
Journal of Learning Disabilities

The Speed of Articulatory Movements 46(3) 278­–286


© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2011
Reprints and permission:
Involved in Speech Production in Children sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0022219411419014

With Dyslexia
journaloflearningdisabilities.sagepub.com

Mirela Duranovic, PhD1 and Sabina Sehic, MA2

Abstract
A group of children with dyslexia (mean ages 9 and 14 years) was studied, together with group of children without dyslexia
matched for age. Participants were monolingual native speakers of the Bosnian language with transparent orthography. In
total, the diagnostic tests were performed with 41 children with dyslexia and 41 nondyslexic children. The participants
were asked to produce monosyllables, /pa/, /ta/, and /ka/, and the trisyllable /pataka/, as fast as possible. Analysis was
undertaken in four ways: (1) time of occlusion duration for plosives (duration of stop), (2) voice onset time for plosives,
(3) diadochokinetic rate—articulators rate measured by pronunciation of monosyllables and the trisyllable, and (4) time of
moving articulators from one gesture to another—time of interval length (from the explosion of one plosive to the start of
the explosion of another plosive). The results suggest that children with dyslexia have significant problems with the speed
of articulatory movements involved in speech production.

Keywords
dyslexia, articulation

Two current major hypotheses inform the etiology of the acquisition of the phonological system (De Bree, 2007).
developmental dyslexia. One of these assumes that the Use of speech coding in an articulatory loop is the same in
phonological processing with emphasis on the segmental children with and without dyslexia, but the system operates
level identifies the core deficit in developmental dyslexia less efficiently in children with dyslexia (Hulme &
and that it cannot be reduced to domain-general deficits of Roodenrys, as cited in Conway, 2003).
temporal information processing. The other hypothesis It is widely considered that speech production difficulties
implies that phonological processing deficits are symptom- in individuals with dyslexia are a reflection of their deficits
atic of domain-general dysfunction and that some dyslexia in phonological processing (Catts, 1986; De Bree, 2007;
subtypes are related to domain-general deficits of temporal Griffiths & Frith, 2002). The inability to associate the posi-
information processing for auditory and visual stimuli tion of their articulators with speech sounds may impair the
(Wolff, 2002). development of phonological awareness and the ability to
A developmental model of reading claims that a deficit convert graphemes to phonemes, which may be related to
in motor-articulatory feedback in developmental dyslexia is programming or feedback deficits (Heilman, Voeller, &
connected with the development of deficient phonological Alexander, 1996).
awareness and nonlexical reading. The articulatory repre- Several researchers investigated speech production and
sentation includes the oral-motor programs for the move- found that children with dyslexia exhibited residual speech
ment of the articulators to produce the pseudoword’s difficulties. Studies have found word-specific rather than
phonemes and consists of the somatosensory awareness of phoneme-specific production errors in children with dyslexia,
the distinctive features within these motor programs
(Conway, 2003). 1
Tuzla University, Tuzla, Bosnia and Herzegovina
Clinical observations and experimental reports provide 2
Rehabilitation of Hearing,Voice, and Speech, Health Center Tuzla, Tuzla,
evidence about speech problems in poor readers (Kamhi, Bosnia and Herzegovina
Catts, & Mauer, 1990). In the literature, articulatory disor-
Corresponding Author:
ders have been associated with dyslexia (Lalain, Joly- Mirela Duranovic, PhD, Tuzla University, Faculty of Education and
Pottuz, Nguyen, & Habib, 2003). Speech production skills Rehabilitation, Univerzitetska 1, 75000, Tuzla, Bosnia and Herzegovina.
include articulation difficulties as well as difficulties with Email: mirela.duranovic@bih.net.ba

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
Duranovic and Sehic 279

especially with multisyllabic words. These difficulties reflect and phonological deficits within a unified account (Nicolson
the children’s poor phonological representations and pro- & Fawcett, 2005). If an infant has a cerebellar impairment,
cessing (De Bree, 2007). Gerrits and De Bree (2009) found this will first show up as a mild motor difficulty—the infant
that speech perception and production performance of chil- may be slower to sit up and to walk, and may have greater
dren at familial risk of dyslexia was poorer than that of con- problems with fine muscular control. If we consider that
trols. Speaking rate and the proportion of pausing time to our most complex motor skill is articulation, the infant
speaking time may provide an early indication of reading might be slower to start babbling and, later, talking. Speech
outcome in children at high risk for reading disability (Smith, and walking may be less fluent in infants with cerebellar
Roberts, Smith, Locke, & Bennett, 2006). impairment. If articulation is less fluent than normal, it
Kamhi and colleagues (1990) examined the speech pro- takes up more conscious resources, leaving fewer resources
duction abilities of young, poor readers. Their results to process sensory feedback. Processing the auditory, pho-
showed that poor readers made more speech errors than nemic structure of spoken words may be less complete
good readers when producing both words and phrases. (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2004).
The poor readers took significantly longer to produce Kasselimis, Margarity, and Vlachos (2008) carried out a
three of the words correctly than did their nondisabled study with aims (a) to assess the cerebellar deficit hypothesis
peers. The data indicated that encoding limitations, rather by examining children’s performance in cerebellar and cog-
than speech production limitations, were primarily respon- nitive tasks associated with dyslexia and (b) to investigate if
sible for the longer acquisition time. there is a differentiation in articulation speed in children with
Articulatory awareness was measured in adults with dys- dyslexia. Children with dyslexia showed significant impair-
lexia, on a task that is not confounded with orthography, and ment in one cerebellar test and performed significantly
the group with dyslexia showed deficits on this task in com- worse during the articulation speed test than the control
parison to the control group. Information about articulatory group. The present study supports the cerebellar deficit
movements for specific phonemes is less accessible to dyslex- hypothesis and the relationship between reading impairment
ics because of a deficient phonological processing system. An and speed of articulation.
articulatory awareness deficit can persist into adulthood, even There are three current theoretical issues that have not
when literacy skills are no longer seriously impaired, which yet been fully resolved in the literature. It is not clear (1)
suggests that this may be part of a cognitive deficit that under- whether the problems arise specifically in speech motor
lies dyslexia (Griffiths & Frith, 2002). planning or whether they also reflect slower speech produc-
In speech research, a metronome is often used to mea- tion, (2) whether such deficits are found only with more com-
sure timing precision. Wolff (2002) asked adolescents and plex stimuli, and (3) whether it is only the occurrence of
adults with dyslexia to repeat the bisyllabic consonant-vowel, errors in speeded repetition that results in deficient perfor-
or CV, sequences /pa-ta/ and /ta-ka/ and the trisyllabic/ mance (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2002).
pa-ta-ka/ correctly in time with a metronome beat. The There is growing interest in examining whether the man-
findings indicated that during a motor sequencing task, stu- ner in which a particular orthography accurately represents
dents with dyslexia anticipated the signal of an isochronic phonology relates to dyslexia (Goulandris, 2003). The aim of
pacing metronome by intervals that were two or three times the present study is to examine the articulatory movements in
as long as those of age-matched readers or adults without children with dyslexia who speak Bosnian, a language with
dyslexia. Children with dyslexia deviated more from the transparent orthography. Transparent orthographies are those
prescribed rate, repeated syllables too slowly at all metro- where the grapheme–phoneme correspondence is one to one.
nome speeds, and made more speech sequencing errors The mappings from letters to sound are much more consistent,
than nondyslexic children with other learning disabilities with very few irregular words (Gupta & Jamal, 2006).
(Wolff, Michel, & Ovrut, 1990). Consistency in sound–letter associations and primarily one-
A significant proportion of children with dyslexia show to-one phoneme–grapheme correspondence makes Bosnian a
a comorbidity with motor disorders, which—it has been transparent orthography.
hypothesized—reflects an automaticity deficit or a cerebellar There has been little systematic research on the articula-
dysfunction. The automaticity/cerebellar deficit framework tion of children with dyslexia in Bosnia and Herzegovina. In
researchers postulated that the cerebellum was unable to this article, we concentrate on articulatory movements in
regulate motor control. Therefore, articulation in speech children with dyslexia with the aim of investigating whether
would lead to deficient phonological representations and/or they have slower articulation of speech. Also in this article
to automate of overlearned tasks in reading and would we report research on whether children with dyslexia simply
affect the learning of grapheme and phoneme correspon- have slower articulation during pronunciation of a single
dences (Caylak, 2010). phoneme or whether the slower articulation problem also
The automaticity/cerebellar deficit framework provides occurs when moving articulators from one articulatory ges-
demonstration that it is possible to explain motor, speed, ture to another.

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
280 Journal of Learning Disabilities 46(3)

Method justify their reading impairments. None of the participants


reported a history of neurological diseases, psychiatric
Design disorders, or hearing problems. All children received an
The research presented here was designed to examine the evaluation of nonverbal IQ with Raven’s Progressive
speed of articulatory movement involved in speech produc- Matrices by a psychologist who verified the criteria of
tion. Articulation of voiceless plosives was measured. normal intelligence.
Plosives represent the basis for the pronunciation of some Children with dyslexia were tested by a speech lan-
other consonants, both in terms of behavior of speech guage therapist, an expert on dyslexia. All participants
organs during the articulation and in terms of acoustic were given tests of reading and writing. For the evaluation
structure, because their elements can be found in some of reading and writing abilities, a reading test for school
other consonants. They are very important for development children was used measuring reading time and reading
of the sound system in children. It is no wonder that they errors for real words and for nonwords. Writing was mea-
occur among the first sounds children make. They are sim- sured by 2-min spelling and 1- min writing. Tests titled
pler, with frequency slightly represented in speech (20.5%), One-Minute Reading Aloud (Furlan, 1965) and Word List
and more resilient than other sounds on a variety of patho- for Checking Reading Skills (Matanovic-Mamuzic, 1982)
logical deviations in articulation. The group of plosives were used. The 2-min spelling, 1-min writing, and non-
consists of six consonants of the Bosnian language: p, b, t, sense passage reading subtests were adapted for the
d, k, and g. The common feature of the plosive is airflow Bosnian language from the Dyslexia Screening Test–Junior
stop through partitions (occlusion), then a sudden release of (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2004).
the airstream (explosion). Partitions are created using dif- For all reading and writing measures, to qualify as having
ferent parts of speech organs. Lips are the most important dyslexia, participants had to score below two standard devia-
for pronunciation of sounds p and b, tongue tip is the most tions of the mean performance of the controls. In total, the
active for pronunciation of sounds t and d, and tongue back diagnostic tests were performed with 41 children with dyslexia
is the most engaged for pronunciation of sounds k and g and 41 children without dyslexia. (See Table 1 for participant
(Vladisavljevic, 1981). So, tasks in this study included lips, characteristics and group differences.)
tongue tip, and tongue back movements.
Analysis was undertaken in four ways: (1) time of
occlusion duration for plosives (duration of stop), (2) time Materials and Procedures
of voice onset time for plosives, (3) diadochokinetic rate— Speaker data collection. The participants were asked to
articulators rate measured by pronunciation of monosylla- produce monosyllables, /pa/, /ta/, and /ka/, and the trisyllable
bles and trisyllable, and (4) time of moving articulators /pataka/, as fast as possible. The children were instructed as
from one gesture to another—time of interval length (from follows: Take a deep breath and repeat pa-pa-pa/ta-ta-ta/ka-
the explosion of one plosive to the start of the explosion of ka-ka/pa-ta-ka, as fast as possible.
another plosive). Participants repeated the stimuli until instructed to
stop, after more than three repetitions, because three rep-
etitions were planned to be used for analysis. Three trials
Participants of each task were taken. The experimenter previously
A group of children with dyslexia (mean ages 9 and 14 years) demonstrated by example how to do that and gave instruc-
was studied, together with group of children without dyslexia, tions to pronounce it as fast as possible. The fastest and
matched for age. Participants were monolingual native speakers most accurate production among the three trials was
of Bosnian. The participants were enrolled in Grades 4 and 9 in selected for analysis.
two public schools in Tuzla Canton. The children with dyslexia Recording.  The recording was done in a quiet room.
were recruited from the same classes as the typically achieving The speech productions of the participants were
readers, and they did not attend additional remedial reading recorded using a Sony (MZ-R91) Portable Mini-Disc
classes. According to the teachers’ reports, none of these stu- Player/Recorder and a Sony (ECM-MS907) Electret
dents had any speech, language, or intellectual problems. None Condenser Microphone, with microphone-to-mouth
had ever been referred for speech or language treatment. All of distance of 30 cm.
them were children of upper-middle-class families. All were Editing.  Data was stored on a personal computer. Dr. Speech
first diagnosed with dyslexia during this research. software (Version 4: subprogram, Real Analysis: Tiger Elec-
The diagnosis followed the recommendations in the tronics, Seattle, WA) was used in the analysis. Real Analysis
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders– software is a real-time speech assessment and training system
Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, that helps to analyze, document, teach, reinforce, and report
1994), namely, normal level of general intelligence, and speech waveform in various applications. It is a powerful clini-
no neurological, sensory, or educational deficit that could cal tool that provides highly versatile information for speech

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
Duranovic and Sehic 281

Table 1. Test Scores and Ages for Children With and Without Dyslexia.

Children Children
Children With Without Children With Without
Dyslexia (9 years Dyslexia Dyslexia Dyslexia
old) (9 years old) (14 years old) (14 years old)  

  M SD M SD p M SD M SD p
Age in years and   9.26   1.07  9.38   .97   .710  14.16   .66 14.30   .71   .500
months
1-min readinga   26.10   7.51 67.10 12.15 <.001  65.30 14.53 99.67 11.81 <.001
Errors, real-word  25.87  11.30  2.6  1.99 <.001  10.05  2.19   .43   .75 <.001
readingb
Reading time, real- 238.07 115.28 86.13 17.46 <.001 106.00 24.44 38.43 10.78 <.001
word reading(s)b
2-min spellingc   7.33   2.19 13.22  2.39 <.001  14.85  2.74 22.33   2.11 <.001
1-min writingc   5.43   2.14  8.95  2.78 <.001  10.78  5.65 16.35   3.12 <.001
Nonsense passage  26.58  16.96 62.53   3.878 <.001  41.75 11.34 70.88   3.28 <.001
readingc
a
One-minute Reading Aloud (Furlan, 1965).
b
Word List for Checking Reading Skills (Matanovic-Mamuzic, 1982).
c
Subtests adapted for the Bosnian language from the Dyslexia Screening Test–Junior (Fawcett & Nicolson, 2004).

assessment, including among several features, real-time Voice onset time was estimated by the measured time
recording and playback (Huang & Lin, 1995). from the beginning of the realization of a consonant phoneme–
Analyses.  The total duration of the monosyllables and tri- plosive to the occurrence of first laryngeal wave of the
syllable were measured in all three repetitions, and the vowel that follows. Pronunciation of the trisyllable /pataka/
duration of the second syllable of the trisyllable in three rep- was used for this purpose.
etitions. The results obtained by producing monosyllables The speed of making rapid speech movements was also
were used to analyze the speed of movement of the lips, measured by analyzing the trisyllable /pataka/ in all three
tongue tip, and tongue back. Duration from the beginning of repetitions, and the duration of the second syllable in three
explosion of the phoneme /p/ to beginning of explosion the repetitions. Duration of the interval P-T (from beginning
next /p/ was used for analysis of the movement of the lips. of explosion of phoneme P to beginning of explosion of
The same procedure was used with the measurement of /t/ phoneme T) and duration of interval T-K (from beginning
and /k/ for getting information about the movement of the of explosion of phoneme T to beginning of explosion of
tongue tip and tongue back. phoneme K) were measured on the same trisyllable. The
Incomplete closures of stop consonants reflect undershoot- measurement was carried out on the middle syllable of the
ing of the articulatory movements (Esther, 2001). According trisyllable because it was the most suitable for acoustic
to Ackermann and Ziegler (1991; as quoted in Esther, 2001, measurements. (First and last phonemes were often not
p. 11) “incomplete closures resulting from reduced extension sufficiently visible.)
of the mandibular, labial, or lingual movements or from The measurement for all tasks was done in milliseconds.
reduced occlusive force may be expected to result in an The method used in the present study was a modification of
increase of sound pressure during stop realization.” the methods that were used in the research carried out by
Characteristics of plosives are to stop airflow through Hedjever (1996), Duranovic and Zecic (2001/2002), and
partition (occlusion), then suddenly release the airstream Duranovic (2003).
(explosion). The duration of stopping airflow through
partitions, or occlusion, was measured. Vladisavljevic
(1981) considered that from an acoustic aspect, there is Results
complete silence during occlusion for voices /p/, /t/, /k/. Mean for scores of children with dyslexia and scores pro-
Suspended airflow, after sudden opening of the partition, duced by the control group are shown in Figure 1. It can
appears as a crack of weaker or stronger intensity, which be seen that the performance of the children with dyslexia
extends almost throughout the frequency spectrum, creat- was consistently worse than that of the controls, for all
ing small, characteristic acoustic energy concentrates in tasks. The mean for all 14 tasks is higher for children with
certain frequency areas. dyslexia.

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
282 Journal of Learning Disabilities 46(3)

Table 2. Results of the Analyses of Variance to Show Effects by


Age and Group.
Children With Dyslexia
Control Group Age Group
3,500 Speech
3,000 Production Tasks F p F p
2,500 OCCLT F(1) = .53 NS F(1) = 68.52 <.001
2,000 OCCLK F(1) = .80 NS F(1) = 33.17 <.001
1,500 VOTT F(1) = .09 NS F(1) = 26.29 <.001
1,000 VOTK F(1) = .44 NS F(1) = 27.24 <.001
500
PAMIDDLE F(1) = .08 NS F(1) = 14.35 <.001
TAMIDDLE F(1) = .23 NS F(1) = 37.34 <.001
0
KAMIDDLE F(1) = .53 NS F(1) = 04.22 <.050
OCCLT
OCCLK
VOTT
VOTK

PATHREE F(1) = .34 NS F(1) = 19.11 <.001


PAMIDDLE
TAMIDDLE
KAMIDDLE

PATHREE
PTKMIDDL

TATHREE
KATHREE

TATHREE F(1) = .49 NS F(1) = 39.81 <.001


PTKTHREE
PTINTERV
TKINTERV
KATHREE F(1) = .02 NS F(1) = 16.27 <.001
PTKMIDDL F(1) = .44 NS F(1) = 00.40 NS
PTKTHREE F(1) = .39 NS F(1) = 06.21 <.050
Figure 1. Mean performance in milliseconds of articulatory PTINTERV F(1) = .35 NS F(1) = 49.75 <.001
movement involved in speech production tasks. TKINTERV F(1) = .07 NS F(1) = 44.68 <.001
Note: OCCLT = occlusion of voice /t/; OCCLK = occlusion of voice /k/;
VOTT = voice onset time of /t/; VOTK = voice onset time of /k/; PAMID- Note: OCCLT = occlusion of voice /t/; OCCLK = occlusion of voice /k/;
DLE = duration of second syllable /pa/ in three repetitions; TAMIDDLE = VOTT = voice onset time of /t/;VOTK = voice onset time of /k/; PAMID-
duration of second syllable /ta/ in three repetitions; KAMIDDLE = dura- DLE = duration of second syllable /pa/ in three repetitions; TAMIDDLE =
tion of second syllable /ka/ in three repetitions; PTKMIDDL = duration duration of second syllable /ta/ in three repetitions; KAMIDDLE = dura-
of second syllable /pa-ta-ka/ in three repetitions; PATHREE = duration tion of second syllable /ka/ in three repetitions; PTKMIDDL = duration
of monosyllable /pa/ in all three repetitions; TATHREE = duration of of second syllable /pa-ta-ka/ in three repetitions; PATHREE = duration of
monosyllable /ta/ in all three repetitions; KATHREE = duration of mono- monosyllable /pa/ in all three repetitions; TATHREE = duration of mono-
syllable /ka/ in all three repetitions; PTKTHREE = duration of trisyllable syllable /ta/ in all three repetitions; KATHREE = duration of monosyllable
/pa-ta-ka/ in all three repetitions; PTINTERV = duration of interval P-T /ka/ in all three repetitions; ; PTKTHREE = duration of trisyllable /pa-ta-
(from beginning of explosion of sound P to beginning of explosion of ka/ in all three repetitions; PTINTERV = duration of interval P-T (from
sound T); TKINTERV = duration of interval T-K (from beginning of explo- beginning of explosion of sound P to beginning of explosion of sound T);
sion of sound T to beginning of explosion of sound K). TKINTERV = duration of interval T-K (from beginning of explosion of
sound T to beginning of explosion of sound K); NS = nonsignificant.

The analysis was performed on the data from the produc- It may be seen that the children with dyslexia performed
ing monosyllables and the trisyllable to verify that the chil- significantly worse than the same-age controls on all tasks,
dren with dyslexia and a group of children without dyslexia except for one of them, which was previously mentioned.
differ with respect to the speed of simple articulatory ges- Analysis of variance showed that children with dyslexia
ture production and the speed of moving articulators from differ from the control group in 13 variables in the speed of
one gesture to another, and to verify effect of age on articu- articulatory movements. Discriminant analysis was employed
latory rates. First, we carried a one-way ANOVA to address to examine which of a number of variables best differentiates
effect of age. One-way ANOVA was undertaken separately the groups. The 14 components were used as predictors (all
for each task, involving the two groups. This analysis had variables of the speed of articulatory movements) in the dis-
one factor: chronological age (9 and 14 years). Age effects criminant analysis to distinguish among the groups (with
were not significant for any task (see Table 2). Because no dyslexia and control). One significant discriminant function
significant age effects had been found, difference by ages was evident, λ = .39, χ2(14, n = 82) = 69.75, p =.00. This
for the children with dyslexia and the control group was not function was significant and showed that there was a strong
considered in further analysis, and groups were divided association between groups and predictors. Based on the
only by the presence or absence of dyslexia. Next, ANOVA examination of the standardized discriminant function and
with the factor of group (dyslexia vs. control) revealed that structure coefficients (see Table 3), the variable with the
the region of significant difference between the children highest weight in defining the discriminant function was
with dyslexia and the control group was for all tasks, except occlusion of voice T (standardized coefficient = 0.61), fol-
for the task where we measured the duration of the second lowed by duration of interval P-T (standardized coefficient =
syllable of the trisyllable /pataka/ in three repetitions. It was 0.40) and duration of second syllable /ta/ in three repeti-
important to identify whether children with dyslexia per- tions (standardized coefficient = 0.37). All coefficients
form worse than children of the same age without dyslexia. were positive. Pronunciation of the second syllable of the

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
Duranovic and Sehic 283

Table 3. Discriminant Analysis: Standardized Discriminant during the movement of articulators from one articulatory
Function and Structure Coefficients. gesture to another.
Standardized To examine the feature of the production of stop conso-
Discriminant nants in children with dyslexia, the present study compared
Speech Function Structure voice onset time (VOT) in those children with dyslexia and
Production Tasks Coefficient Coefficient the control group. VOT is the time interval between the
OCCLT  .613 .732
burst that marks the release of the stop closure and the
OCCLK  .216 .509 onset of quasiperiodicity, which reflects laryngeal vibra-
VOTT  .018 .453 tion (Lisker & Abramson, 1967). The VOT is affected by
VOTK −.284 .461 place of articulation and voicing. Plosives exhibit distinc-
PAMIDDLE −.142 .335 tive acoustic events on a finer time scale. Typically, the
TAMIDDLE  .365 .540 closure interval ends in an abrupt increase in acoustic
KAMIDDLE −.069 .182 energy across the frequency range. The release interval is
PATHREE  .345 .386 measured from this burst onset to the start of periodicity or
TATHREE  .107 .558 to the onset of noise or silence. The duration of the release
KATHREE −.219 .357 interval is called VOT in cases when periodicity is present
PTKMIDDL −.065 .056 (Stouten & Van Hamme, 2009). On a spectrogram, this is
PTKTHREE −.492 .220 seen as the time between a sharp onset of broadband energy
PTINTERV  .396 .623 (the burst) and the onset of the formant transition (onset of
TKINTERV  .300 .591 vocal-fold vibration; Gleason & Ratner, 1998). VOT is a
temporal acoustic property that specifies voicing in stop
Note: OCCLT = occlusion of voice /t/; OCCLK = occlusion of voice /k/;
VOTT = voice onset time of /t/;VOTK = voice onset time of /k/; PAMID- consonants. Voiced stop consonants are associated with
DLE = duration of second syllable /pa/ in three repetitions; TAMIDDLE = relatively short VOT values, whereas voiceless stop conso-
duration of second syllable /ta/ in three repetitions; KAMIDDLE = dura- nants are associated with relatively long VOT values
tion of second syllable /ka/ in three repetitions; PTKMIDDL = duration
(Allen, Miller, & DeSteno, 2003). Voiceless stops such as
of second syllable /pa-ta-ka/ in three repetitions; PATHREE = duration of
monosyllable /pa/ in all three repetitions; TATHREE = duration of mono- (t) are characterized by VOTs that range between 40 and
syllable /ta/ in all three repetitions; KATHREE = duration of monosyllable 100 ms (Gleason & Ratner, 1998). In this research, the mean
/ka/ in all three repetitions; ; PTKTHREE = duration of trisyllable /pa-ta- of VOTs for children with dyslexia was 89 and 88.63 ms.
ka/ in all three repetitions; PTINTERV = duration of interval P-T (from
beginning of explosion of sound P to beginning of explosion of sound T);
If we consider previous data, we can conclude that they do
TKINTERV = duration of interval T-K (from beginning of explosion of not have longer VOT than it is expected, but it was signifi-
sound T to beginning of explosion of sound K). cantly longer than in the control group. The performance
of the children with dyslexia was inferior to that of their
controls on both tasks that were measured—VOT and
trisyllable /pataka/ in three repetitions is clearly not loaded occlusion. The occlusion of voice T is the function that
on the discriminant function. It is the weakest predictor, and most discriminates between children with dyslexia and
this fact suggests that it is not associated with dyslexia but, those without dyslexia. Post, Foorman, and Hiscock (1997)
rather, is a function of other unassessed factors. Furthermore, examined VOT during reading nonwords. They did not
functions at group centroids showed negative values for nor- find speech production difficulties of /t/ and /d/.
mally achieving children (−1.249) and positive values for Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, and Scanlon (2004) sum-
children with dyslexia (1.249). Therefore, normally achiev- marized some of the most important findings from research
ing children would more likely have low scores during the evaluating the hypothesized causes of dyslexia during the
evaluation of the articulatory speed. In contrast, children past four decades. They considered different researchers,
with dyslexia would more likely have high scores. including Elbro et al. (1998, as cited in Vellutino, Fletcher,
Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004, p. 23), who reported that chil-
dren without dyslexia in dyslexic families had deficits, rela-
Discussion tive to controls, on tests evaluating articulatory accuracy.
The aim of the present study was to compare the speed In the present study, the performance of the children
of articulatory movements of children with dyslexia in with dyslexia was inferior to that of their controls on all
transparent Bosnian orthography with a group of children tests except for the test in which we measured the duration
without dyslexia matched for age. The results showed of the second syllable of the trisyllable /pataka/ in three
that children with dyslexia have significantly slower repetitions. We can try to explain this result with the
articulatory movements than the control group. Children assumption that the speed of moving articulators was not
with dyslexia have problems in the speed of articulation, slower in children with dyslexia when it required a smaller
both during pronunciation of single phonemes and number of movements. In the case of three repetitions of

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
284 Journal of Learning Disabilities 46(3)

the trisyllable /pataka/ and the speed of these movements, Pennington, Van Orden, Smith, Green, and Haith (1990).
differences between children with dyslexia and normally Smith and others (2006) found that children with RD
achieving children were significant, when more number of showed a significantly slower speaking rate. Articulation
movements were required. Wolff, Cohen, and Drake (1984) rate did not differ significantly across groups. Results in
found that adolescents with reading problems have greater our research are not the same as these other studies because
difficulty rapidly sequencing syllable strings than repeat- clear differences in articulation rate can be seen from the
ing single syllables. It was argued that the threshold at results of analysis of variance and discriminant analysis. A
which movement speed degrades timing precision for possible explanation for these differences in results is that
coordinated action best characterizes the motor impair- different techniques are used, and some of them are more
ment of individuals with reading problems. sensitive than others.
Results suggest that children with dyslexia have prob- Esther (2001) wrote about diadochokinesis (DDK) and
lems in the speed of articulatory movement involved in used Lundeen’s (1950) definition as “the maximum speed
speech production. The present findings are in line with ear- of movement with which a given reciprocating act can be
lier reports of a deficit in the repetition of articulatory ges- produced” (Esther, 2001, p. 3). In different literature, DDK
tures, problems in speeded articulation in both production analysis is considered as the measurement of rate and
and planning of gestures in children with dyslexia (see accuracy of placement. DDK rate is considered a maxi-
Fawcett & Nicolson, 2002). Our data extend these previous mum performance test. It can reflect diminished coordina-
findings by showing that VOT and time of occlusion are tion, range, or rate of articulators in clinical neurology.
effective acoustic parameters for articulation testing in chil- DDK rate could predict articulatory performance. It might
dren with dyslexia. Also, it has been shown that during anal- be because DDK rate is sensitive to oromotor deficits
ysis of the trisyllable /pataka/, duration of intervals of P-T (Esther, 2001). Measures of articulatory function are com-
and T-K should be taken into consideration because discrim- pleted via diadochokinetic tasks designed to measure pro-
inant analysis showed that these variables best differentiate duction in various contexts (Robarge, 2009).
groups. The results also showed that articulatory speed is Diadochokinetic rate is one means of assessing oral motor
important to distinguish between children with dyslexia and skills. DDK rate provides information about a person’s
control group for both languages with opaque orthography, ability to make rapid speech movements using different
and in languages w ​ ith transparent orthography. parts of his mouth. In this research, we can talk about diad-
Our finding about slower articulation rate in children ochokinesis for tasks that have measured the phonemes
with dyslexia is in line with a study by Fawcett & Nicolson /pa/, /ta/ and /ka/ where children used the front (the lips),
(1995) who found that for peg placing and articulation rate, middle (the tip of the tongue), and back (the back of the
the children with dyslexia were significantly slower than tongue). On all tasks, children with dyslexia were signifi-
their chronological age controls and suggested that children cantly worse than the control group.
with dyslexia have persistent, and unexpectedly severe, Differences between groups were obtained for the
problems in motor skill. Snowling (1981) and Catts (1989) interval P-T and interval K-T. On both tasks, children
also showed that older children with dyslexia are slower with dyslexia had significant worse results than the con-
and make more error on complex articulation tasks. This trol group, and discriminant analysis showed that these
was explained by difficulties in the planning stage of speech two tasks are two of the most important functions that
production. discriminate between children with dyslexia and children
Smith, Lambrecht Smith, Locke, and Bennett (2008) without dyslexia.
examined the development of timing characteristics in These results are in line with previous studies. Lalain
early spontaneous speech of children who were later and colleagues (2003) used aerodynamic/acoustic data to
identified as having a reading disability (RD). Early explore how children with dyslexia produce bilabial stops
speaking rate was significantly slower in the group with in French within a sentence, where they occurred in two
RD, with significantly different patterns of pausing com- positions and in three-vowel environments. A study with
pared with children without RD. Phonetic plans may be French dyslexics found that even though these children
shorter and/or less specified in children with RD, surfac- were able to produce a voicing contrast for /p/ and /b/,
ing as slow, short speaking turns with increased pausing their realizations showed different timing of articulatory
relative to articulation. movement than those of a reading-age and chronological-
As we previously mentioned, there is no consensus age matched group.
about articulation rate and differences between children Several studies connected articulatory and phonological
with dyslexia and their controls. In the research of Di problems in people with dyslexia. Mann and Foy (2007)
Filippo and colleagues (2006), children with reading dis- focused on speech production in relation to the development
abilities and control children did not differ in articulation of phonological awareness. Their results suggest that although
rate, and no clear deficits in articulatory speed were found the phoneme awareness measures were not apparently
in the adult group with dyslexia in the research by related to the articulatory measures, there were consistent

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
Duranovic and Sehic 285

associations between rhyme awareness and articulation. Funding


This research can be extended with the aim of investigating The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
the connection of current problems in articulation rate with authorship, and/or publication of this article.
phonological skills in children with dyslexia.
There is evidence that children with dyslexia have prob- References
lems in the speed of articulatory movements involved in Allen, J. S., Miller, J. L., & DeSteno, D. (2003). Individual talker
speech production, including the speeded production of sin- differences in voice-onset time. Journal of the Acoustical
gle articulatory gestures and several articulatory gestures. Society of America, 113, 544–552.
There is no other visible cause for lower articulation speed American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
except the presence of dyslexia. The results of this study tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
confirm the previous claims about speech problems in chil- Author.
dren with dyslexia. This study provides useful information Catts, H. W. (1986). Speech production/phonological deficits in
about acoustic characteristics of speech, including voice reading-disordered children. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
onset time, time of occlusion, and evidence about slower 19, 504–508.
mobility of lips, tongue tip, and tongue back in children with Catts, H. W. (1989). Speech production deficits in developmen-
dyslexia. There is also evidence about slower mobility and tal dyslexia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54,
moving from one gesture to another in children with dys- 422–428.
lexia. These results can lead to a conclusion that there are Caylak, E. (2010). The studies about phonological deficit theory
problems in articulatory movements included in the speech in children with developmental dyslexia: Review. American
production in children with dyslexia. Journal of Neuroscience, 1(1), 1–12.
Slower speed of articulatory movements in children with Conway, T. W. (2003). Measuring phonological processing and
dyslexia indicates the need for organization in the training phonological working memory in adults with developmental
for improvement of these skills. Joly-Pottuz, Mercier, Leynaud, dyslexia: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
and Habib (2008) evaluated the benefit of training sensory- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Florida.
motor aspects of articulatory function in a group of children De Bree, E. H. (2007). Dyslexia and phonology: A study of the
with dyslexia. Auditory phonological training included daily phonological abilities of Dutch children at-risk of dyslexia.
listening series of exercises, mainly containing triplets of Utrecht, Netherlands: LOT.
words the dyslexics had to compare phonologically to dis- Di Filippo, G., Brizzolara, D., Chilosi, A., De Luca, M., Judica, A.,
close similarity between 2 of the 3 stimuli. Articulatory Pecini, C., . . . Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Naming speed and visual
awareness exercises put emphasis on phonetic oppositions search deficits in readers with disabilities: Evidence from an
between voiced and voiceless stop consonants. In the part of orthographically regular language (Italian). Developmental
the training on the phoneme production with computerized Neuropsychology, 30, 885–904.
visual feedback, children had to pronounce a chosen pho- Duranovic, M. (2003). Osobitosti glasa i govora osoba s Parkin-
neme through a microphone, while the program analyzed the sonovom bolescu i multiplom sklerozom [Characteristics of
acoustic quality of the production; the utterance was then voice and speech in persons with Parkinson disease and mul-
transformed into a visual index. The program was also used tiple sclerosis]. Magistarski rad, Edukacijsko-rehabilitacijski
for improvement in the accuracy of phoneme production; fakultet, Sveuciliste u Zagrebu.
development of skills in pronouncing a sequence of different Duranovic, M., & Zecic, S. (2001/2002). Brzina pokretljivosti
phonemes; and by using a specially designed 2- and 4-phoneme artikulatora kod djece sa cerebralnom paralizom [Speed
contrast to improve the accuracy in contrasting phonemes. of articulatory movement in children with cerebral palsy].
Results showed that this training is an efficient way for improv- Defektologija, 7, 95–100.
ing poor representation of phonemes. Esther, T. S. L. (2001). The relationship between diadochokinetic
It is important for speech therapists in Bosnia and rate and accuracy, reading rate, and sentence intelligibility in
Herzegovina, who provide support for children with dyslexia, Cantonese speakers with Parkinsonism. Unpublished manu-
to include in their work, among other interventions, remedia- script, University of Hong Kong.
tion tools for the improvement of articulatory function. Further Fawcett, A., & Nicolson, J. (2004). Dyslexia: The role of the
research is needed to explore the effectiveness and usefulness cerebellum. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational
of such training in work with children with dyslexia for Psychology, 2(2), 35–58.
Bosnian and other languages with transparent orthography. Fawcett, A., & Nicolson, R. (2004). Dyslexia Screening Test–
Junior (DST-J). London, England: Psychological Corporation.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (1995). Persistent deficits in
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect motor skill of children with dyslexia. Journal of Motor Behavior,
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. 27, 235–240.

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015
286 Journal of Learning Disabilities 46(3)

Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (2002). Children with dyslexia Matanovic-Mamuzic, M. (1982). Lista sa serijama rijeci za ispi-
are slow to articulate a single speech gesture. Dyslexia, 8, tivanje citanja [Word list for checking reading skills]. In:
189–203. Teškoce u citanju i pisanju [Reading and writing difficulties].
Furlan, I. (1965). Jednominutni ispit glasnog citanja [One-minute Zagreb, Croatia: Skolska knjiga.
reading aloud]. Zagreb, Croatia: Skolska knjiga. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (2005). Developmental dyslexia,
Gerrits, E., & De Bree, E. (2009). Early language development of learning and the cerebellum. In W. W. Fleishhacker & D. J.
children at familial risk of dyslexia: Speech perception and pro- Brooks (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 19–36).
duction. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42, 180–194. New York, NY: SpringerWien.
Gleason, J. B., & Ratner, N. B. (1998). Psycholinguistics. Belmont, Pennington, B. F., Van Orden, G. C., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., &
CA: Thomson Learning, Inc. Haith, M. M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and defi-
Goulandris, N. (2003). Dyslexia in different languages: Cross- cits in adult dyslexics. Child Development, 61, 1753–1778.
linguistic comparisons. London, England: Whurr. Post, Y. V., Foorman, B. R., & Hiscock, M. (1997). Speech perception
Griffiths, S., & Frith, U. (2002). Evidence for an articulatory and speech production as indicators of reading difficulty. Annals
awareness deficit in adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 8, 14–21. of Dyslexia, 47, 1–27.
Gupta, A., & Jamal, G. (2006). An analysis of reading errors of Robarge, K. (2009). Communication management in amyotrophic
dyslexic readers in Hindi and English. Asia Pacific Disability lateral sclerosis: The role of the speech-language pathologist
Rehabilitation Journal, 17(1), 73–86. during disease progression. The Internet Journal of Allied
Hedjever, M. (1996). Akusticka analiza vremenskih segmenata nor- Health Sciences and Practice, 7(2), 1–6.
malnog i poremecenog govora [Acoustic analysis of time seg- Smith, A. B., Lambrecht Smith, S., Locke, J. L., & Bennett, J.
ments of normal and disordered speech]. Doktorska disertacija, (2008) A longitudinal study of speech timing in young chil-
Edukacijsko-rehabilitacijski fakultet, Zagreb, Croatia. dren later found to have reading disability. Journal of Speech,
Heilman, K. M., Voeller, K., & Alexander, A. W. (1996). Develop- Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1300–1314.
mental dyslexia: A motor-articulatory feedback hypothesis. Smith, A. B., Roberts, J., Smith, S. L., Locke, J. L., & Bennett, J.
Annals of Neurology, 39, 407–412. (2006). Reduced speaking rate as an early predictor of reading
Huang, D. Z., & Lin, S. (1995). Dr. Speech for Windows (Version 4): disability. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
User’s guide. Seattle, WA: Tiger Electronics. 15, 289–297.
Joly-Pottuz, B., Mercier, M., Leynaud, A., & Habib, M. (2008). Snowling, M. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dys-
Combined auditory and articulatory training improves phono- lexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219–234.
logical deficit in children with dyslexia. Neuropsychological Stouten, V., & Van Hamme, H. (2009). Automatic voice onset time
Rehabilitation, 18, 402–429. estimation from reassignment spectra. Speech Communica-
Kamhi, A. G., Catts, H. W., & Mauer, D. (1990). Explaining tion, 51, 1194–1205.
speech production deficits in poor readers. Journal of Learn- Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M.
ing Disabilities, 23, 631–636. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we
Kasselimis, D. S., Margarity, M., & Vlachos, F. (2008). Cerebellar learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology
function, dyslexia and articulation speed. Child Neuropsychology, and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.
14, 303–313. Vladisavljevic, S. (1981). Poremecaji izgovora [Articulation dis-
Lalain, M., Joly-Pottuz, B., Nguyen, N., & Habib, M. (2003). orders]. Beograd, Serbia: Privredni pregled.
Dyslexia: The articulatory hypothesis revisited. Brain and Wolff, P. H. (2002). Timing precision and rhythm in developmen-
Cognition, 53, 253–256. tal dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 15, 179–206.
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1967). Some effects of context on Wolff, P. H., Cohen, C., & Drake, C. (1984). Impaired motor tim-
voice onset time in English stops. Language and Speech, 10, ing control in specific reading retardation. Neuropsychologia,
1–28. 22, 587–600.
Mann, V. A., & Foy, J. G. (2007). Speech development patterns Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. F., & Ovrut, M. (1990). The timing of syl-
and phonological awareness in preschool children. Annals of lable repetitions in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech
Dyslexia, 57, 51–74. and Hearing Research, 33, 281–289.

Downloaded from ldx.sagepub.com at NORTH DAKOTA STATE UNIV LIB on June 30, 2015

You might also like