You are on page 1of 14

Emotion

Adult Attachment and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives in Daily


Life
Tabea Springstein, Kyra K. Hamerling-Potts, Isidro Landa, and Tammy English
Online First Publication, September 15, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0001169

CITATION
Springstein, T., Hamerling-Potts, K. K., Landa, I., & English, T. (2022, September 15). Adult Attachment and Interpersonal
Emotion Regulation Motives in Daily Life. Emotion. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0001169
Emotion
© 2022 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 1528-3542 https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001169

Adult Attachment and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives in


Daily Life

Tabea Springstein, Kyra K. Hamerling-Potts, Isidro Landa, and Tammy English


Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis

Interpersonal goals and adult attachment have implications for how people interact with others as well
as for emotion experience and regulation. Literature on intrapersonal emotional processes has typically
not examined motivations underlying people’s engagement with others’ emotions and its connections to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

individual differences related to close relationships such as attachment. This study analyzed the relation-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ships between interpersonal emotion regulation motives, perceived social interaction outcomes, and
attachment. Undergraduates (N = 211) reported their trait attachment. Experience sampling was used to
examine the reasons why they wanted to regulate others’ emotions during daily interactions and per-
ceived changes in their own well-being and relationship quality with the target of regulation.
Attachment anxiety was associated with more self-focused prohedonic motives and impression manage-
ment motives, while attachment avoidance predicted less perceived increases in emotional and relational
well-being after interactions. People who tended to report more (self- and other-focused) prohedonic
and less impression management motives in daily life perceived more positive changes in their emo-
tional well-being and people who tended to report higher emotional similarity motives perceived more
positive changes in their relational well-being after interactions People also perceived more positive
emotional and relational interaction outcomes at times when they held more (self- and other-focused)
prohedonic, impression management, or relationship maintenance motives and less self-focused per-
formance and relationship distancing motives. Overall, these findings suggest that attachment anxiety
may guide why people engage with other people’s emotions and these extrinsic interpersonal emotion
regulation motives may play a role in socioemotional outcomes of daily interactions.

Keywords: emotion regulation, emotion, attachment, relationships, experience sampling

Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001169.supp

In our daily interactions, especially with those close to us, we often That is, interpersonal emotion regulation has underlying motives, just
attempt to regulate the emotions of others (Tamminen et al., 2019). like intrapersonal emotion regulation (Nozaki & Mikolajczak, 2020).
Emotion regulation broadly refers to the process of managing the Interpersonal emotion regulation encompasses both attempts to
expression and experience of emotion. Regulating another person’s regulate one’s own emotions with the help of others (i.e., intrinsic
emotions could be as simple as cheering them up or attempting to min- interpersonal emotion regulation) and attempts to regulate others’
imize the intensity of their feelings. These attempts are fueled by the emotions (i.e., extrinsic interpersonal emotion regulation; Zaki & Wil-
motivation to set and achieve contextually relevant goals. We might liams, 2013). We focus on motivations underlying extrinsic interper-
want to minimize the intensity of a person’s emotions because we sonal emotion regulation to get a better sense of why one regulates
would like to avoid feeling stressed ourselves, or we might try to regu- the emotions of another person. Theses motivations can vary between
late their emotions in order to increase closeness in the relationship. and within individuals. Between individuals, one possible individual

Preliminary results were presented at 2021 Annual Conference of the


Editor’s Note. Elise K Kalokerinos served as the action editor for this Society for Affective Science and the Washington University in St. Louis
article.—PRP Spring 2021 Undergraduate Research Symposium. The study presented in this
article was designed for Kyra K. Hamerling-Potts’s honors thesis.
Tabea Springstein https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4712-6543 The study’s design and parts of the analyses were preregistered on the
Kyra K. Hamerling-Potts https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0156-1817 Open Science Framework (OSF) and are available at https://osf.io/2nxv9.
Isidro Landa https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3139-6023 Materials and data reported in this article are available on the OSF (https://
Tammy English https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6666-4780 osf.io/2ms9w/).
Kyra K. Hamerling-Potts is now at the Mental Illness Research, Education Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Tammy
and Clinical Centers, James J. Peters Veterans Affairs Medical Center, The English, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington
Bronx, New York, United States. Isidro Landa is now at the Department of University in St. Louis, 1 Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO 63130, United
Psychology, University of Michigan. States. Email: tenglish@wustl.edu

1
2 SPRINGSTEIN, HAMERLING-POTTS, LANDA, AND ENGLISH

difference that could explain motivations behind regulatory behaviors interpersonal emotion regulation, we examined four subtypes in this
is attachment. Attachment first emerges through interactions with area: impression management, relationship maintenance, relation-
close others during infancy (Ainsworth & Bowlby, 1991). Attachment ship distancing, and emotional similarity.
has bearing on our habits when interacting with others and on what The hedonic motives underlying interpersonal emotion regula-
we wish to achieve from our social relationships, so attachment tion could be about the regulator’s own emotions (self-focused;
likely guides the motives that adults use to influence another per- i.e., making themselves feel better or worse) or someone else’s
emotions (other-focused; i.e., making someone else feel better or
son’s emotions and subsequent interpersonal emotion regulation
worse; Niven et al., 2012). Individuals are often motivated to
outcomes (e.g., relational, emotional well-being). Although peo-
engage in supporting others based on a desire to influence their
ple may generally report relying more on certain motives, their own emotions. For example, people may try to alleviate their per-
motives can also vary across contexts (i.e., certain situations sonal distress by assisting a friend going through a difficult event
may activate a given motive more than others, such as wanting (Eisenberg et al., 1989). Although less common, people can also
to make a good impression when meeting someone new). These be motivated to increase negative (e.g., anger) or decrease positive
intraindividual fluctuations in motives can also impact socioemo- emotion (e.g., happiness) in an interaction partner (contrahedonic;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tional outcomes. Netzer et al., 2015). Therefore, both self-related and other-related
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The present study aims to examine the role of adult attachment prohedonic and contrahedonic motives were included in the pres-
in preferences for interpersonal emotion regulation motives and in ent study.
perceived social interaction outcomes with romantic and nonro- As individuals are broadly motivated to preserve close rela-
mantic partners during daily life. Perceived changes in relationship tionships with others (Walton et al., 2012), the desire to main-
closeness and emotional well-being of the regulator were used as tain positive social bonds should also drive interpersonal emotion
indicators of interpersonal emotion regulation outcomes because regulation. Existing research on interpersonal support suggests
they are often superordinate goals of interpersonal emotion regula- that motives regarding the relationship with the target of regula-
tion and influence long-term well-being (Saphire-Bernstein & tion could be particularly relevant as instrumental motives for
Taylor, 2013; Tamir, 2016). interpersonal emotion regulation. Couples that receive more help
from their partners report higher relationship quality (Overall
et al., 2010) and provision of daily interpersonal support has been
Motivated Interpersonal Emotion Regulation associated with increases in relationship closeness (Gleason et al.,
2008). The social reasons underlying intrapersonal emotion regu-
Emotion regulation is a motivated process, as regulatory behav-
lation have often been delineated into those focused on making a
iors occur in the context of pursuing hedonic or instrumental
good impression on others (impression management) or foster
motives (Gross, 2015; Tamir, 2009). Emotion regulation motives
smooth interactions (relationship maintenance; Eldesouky & Eng-
are defined as the superordinate motives behind the emotions peo-
lish, 2019). Using certain types of interpersonal emotion regula-
ple would like to feel when they regulate their emotions (e.g.,
tion strategies is associated with the formation of new social ties
more excited, less sad; Tamir, 2016). People regulate their emo-
(Niven et al., 2015), suggesting that interpersonal emotion regula-
tions to reach certain emotional states because of how these emo-
tion can increase desire to affiliate with the regulator and that pro-
tions make them feel (i.e., hedonic motives), like wanting to feel
viding support can make positive impressions on others.
happier, or because of what emotions can do for them (i.e., instru-
However, other social motives may need to be considered to
mental motives), such as wanting to maintain anger to perform gain a deeper understanding of interpersonal emotion regulation.
well in a competition (Mauss & Tamir, 2014). While instrumental At times, for example, individuals may be motivated to manage
motives often reflect goals related to performance (Kalokerinos others’ emotions in order to create distance from them (relation-
et al., 2017), they are also commonly socially-oriented, such as ship distancing) or facilitate closeness through shared experience
trying to change a relationship or someone’s opinion of you (Eng- (emotion similarity; e.g., wanting your friend to feel happy when
lish et al., 2017). Individuals differ in the extent to which they you are feeling happy). When individuals feel threatened, they
want to influence their own emotions for social reasons, including may engage in self-protective behaviors to actively reduce close-
for the sake of others’ well-being or to be perceived favorably by ness to other people (Ainsworth & Bowlby, 1991). While relation-
others (Eldesouky & English, 2019). ship distancing may be harmful to social connections, promoting
While individuals generally prefer support that makes them feel similarity may be beneficial. Romantic partners and friends experi-
validated over support that helps them problem-solve (Liu et al., ence more cohesion and less conflict when sharing similar emo-
2021), the motives of interpersonal emotion regulation that are pre- tional states and these dyads frequently engage in coregulation, or
ferred by targets of regulation depends on the emotional and situa- the process of adapting to each other’s emotional state (Anderson
tional context (Pauw et al., 2019). In addition, how and why people et al., 2003).
regulate the emotions of others (e.g., to improve or worsen another To cover interpersonal emotion regulation motives more com-
person’s affect) varies across different relationships (Niven et al., prehensively, it may also be necessary to consider performance
2012). As with intrapersonal emotion regulation, there are several motives. Previous work has shown that the most frequently
types of interpersonal emotion regulation motives that underlie the endorsed intrapersonal instrumental emotion regulation motives
strategies that might be used depending on the relationship or con- center around performance-related concerns (Kalokerinos et al.,
text. We focus on hedonic motives (prohedonic, contrahedonic) and 2017). Much like hedonic motives, performance motives can be
the most common instrumental motives, social and performance. held regarding oneself (e.g., how emotions will impact one’s own
Given that social motives are theoretically most relevant for ability to complete a task) or the target of regulation (e.g., how
ATTACHMENT AND INTERPERSONAL REGULATION 3

someone else’s emotions will impact their own performance (Net- in attachment anxiety have been shown to vary in the use of affect-
zer et al., 2015; Niven, 2016). Thus, the present study assessed worsening strategies across relationships, while people higher in
self- and other-focused performance motives in addition to the pre- attachment avoidance showed higher variability in affect-improving
viously noted hedonic and social motives. strategies across relationships (Niven et al., 2012). Affect-worsening
Prior research shows that people endorse various motives when and affect-improving strategies have underlying other-focused
regulating the emotions of others. However, it is unclear why peo- hedonic motives, as they involve behaviors or engagement with the
ple endorse these interpersonal emotion regulation motives or how target that are “intended to” worsen or improve the target’s emotional
they might be associated with immediate outcomes for the regula- state (Niven et al., 2012). Interpersonal emotion regulation motives
tor, such as emotional and relational functioning. Attachment is may be self-focused and include desires to maintain one’s own posi-
one promising candidate for helping to illuminate patterns of inter- tive emotional states and negative emotional states (i.e., prohedonic
personal emotion regulation motives and perceived changes in and contrahedonic motives). Avoidant individuals tend to avoid
social interaction outcomes related to one’s interpersonal emotion experiences of negative emotion and seek to enhance positive affect
regulation motivation. (Fraley & Shaver, 1997; Altan-Atalay, 2019). Individuals higher on
attachment anxiety, on the other hand, are more likely to have
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

learned to exaggerate threats and negative emotions to receive atten-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Attachment and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation


Motives tion from their attachment figure, and therefore may endorse higher
self-focused contrahedonic motives.
Attachment is inherently an interpersonal process, as its forma- Attachment is also associated with broader social-oriented
tion depends on interactions during infancy with a caregiver (Ains- motives in terms of how much closeness individuals desire and how
worth & Bowlby, 1991; Bartholomew, 1990). Attachment much closeness versus distance individuals would like in close rela-
insecurity, in the form of high attachment avoidance and attach- tionships. In addition to wanting more intimacy in their relation-
ment anxiety, develops in response to emotional or physical needs ships, individuals high in attachment anxiety are also less likely to
of an infant not being met. Attachment avoidance forms when the perceive intimacy, as compared with their less anxious peers. Con-
caregiver is dismissing, leading to tendencies to suppress one’s versely, people high in attachment avoidance not only want less inti-
needs and being overly independent. Attachment anxiety, results macy, but they are also more likely to perceive a relationship as
from inconsistent, unpredictable caretaking, leading to preoccupa- close, as compared with their less avoidant peers (Hudson & Fraley,
tion and clinginess in relationships. 2017). Based on these perceptual differences, more avoidant and
Attachment develops beyond infancy and remains relatively sta- more anxious people also differ from securely attached people in the
ble across adulthood (Bowlby, 1978). In adulthood, attachment goals they pursue in close relationships, including during interperso-
avoidance and attachment anxiety are associated with the quality nal emotion regulation. Avoidant attachment is associated with
of romantic relationships, with attachment security prospectively motives to avoid and not to approach closeness and submission.
predicting the quality of romantic relationships even after control- Anxious attachment is associated with focusing more on avoiding
ling for baseline levels of interpersonal functioning in the relation- distance in relationships (Locke, 2008).
ship (Holland & Roisman, 2010). In addition, adult attachment is Regarding emotional similarity, studies have investigated
associated with the quality of friendships in young adults (Safe- attachment and emotional coregulation, defined as the emotional
rstein et al., 2005). Specific behaviors that individuals engage in in synchronicity between partners. The hypersensitivity of attach-
their close relationships, like how they resolve conflicts with close ment anxiety predicts greater covariance in emotion, inferred to be
others, are also associated with their attachment style (Domingue due to emotion regulation between partners (Butner et al., 2007).
& Mollen, 2009). Covariance in emotion may result from emotional similarity
Adult attachment is predictive of individuals’ own emotion reg- motives, that is, wanting to make emotions be similar between two
ulation (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Attachment dimensions are people. Simply wanting to be on the same page as another person,
linked to the motivations that drive intrapersonal emotion regula- regardless of whether those emotions are positive or negative, is a
tion, namely the deactivation and hyperactivation of one’s emo- novel interpersonal emotion regulation motive that has not been
tional state. Attachment avoidance has been associated with explored in prior literature. Emotional similarity motives may
deactivation, which includes wanting to inhibit closeness, negative relate negatively to the emotionally distant nature of attachment
affect, and vulnerable emotional states. Attachment anxiety, on the avoidance and relate positively to the bonding and closeness seek-
other hand, is associated with hyperactivation, which includes ing of attachment anxiety. Relationship type will be examined to
desiring closeness, reassurance, and emotions that may elicit care test whether the associations between attachment and interpersonal
from their attachment figures (Girme et al., 2021; Özen et al., emotion regulation motives are stronger for romantic than nonro-
2011). This work on attachment and intrapersonal emotion regula- mantic targets. In adulthood, romantic partners tend to be the pri-
tion begins to suggest that interpersonal motives could also depend mary attachment figures for individuals (Doherty & Feeney,
on their attachment styles. 2004), so we expected that attachment related processes should be
Attachment has been linked to interpersonal emotion regulation in particularly salient in interactions with romantic partners.
ways that indirectly suggest links with both hedonic and instrumental
motives. Higher levels of attachment avoidance or anxiety may moti- Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives and
vate one to regulate others for one’s own sake. For example, one Well-Being
may influence another person’s emotions with the primary aim of
protecting oneself from threats associated with other’s negative emo- Successful implementation of intrapersonal emotion regulation
tions (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2009). Additionally, individuals higher strategies is indicated by individuals attaining their emotion goals
4 SPRINGSTEIN, HAMERLING-POTTS, LANDA, AND ENGLISH

(e.g., feeling less negative emotions; Gross, 2015). When individ- with lower emotional well-being and relationship satisfaction (Niven
uals hold instrumental emotion regulation motives, they do not et al., 2012; Özen et al., 2011). In addition, attachment anxiety and
regulate in order to feel more pleasurable immediately but in order avoidance have been linked to poorer emotion regulation skills
to achieve a goal that has long-term benefits (Tamir, 2016). For (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2019) and therefore individuals higher on
example, regulating your emotions so you can focus on studying these dimensions of attachment may also be worse at managing the
for a test may not immediately result in feeling good, but it can emotions of others. Accordingly, people higher in attachment avoid-
lead to positive feelings later as you are progressing toward your ance or attachment anxiety might be less likely to perceive positive
goal of studying. It has been suggested that affect serves as infor- outcomes from social interactions because these individuals are not
mation to the individual about progress toward meaningful goals. as good at implementing interpersonal emotion regulation in a way
If progress toward a goal is too slow then negative affect is output, that will help satisfy their motives. We also test if there is a stronger
whereas if progress toward the goal is fast then positive affect is association between attachment and interaction outcomes when the
the output (Carver & Scheier, 1990). When applied to emotion target of interpersonal emotion regulation is a romantic partner, who
regulation, this suggests that feeling more positive and less nega- may more strongly activating one’s attachment system (Doherty &
tive emotions could indicate progress toward a motive. Feeney, 2004).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

In interpersonal regulation contexts, successful implementation


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of interpersonal emotion regulation could be through observing Present Study


changes in momentary emotional and relational well-being. It is
unclear however whether the specific underlying motivations guid- This project aims to understand the role of attachment in predict-
ing interpersonal emotion regulation may be differentially associ- ing why people manage the emotions of others and the impact inter-
ated with better perceived social interaction outcomes. On one personal emotion regulation has on well-being. We examine
hand, given that helping others can be intrinsically rewarding (Ina- hedonic (prohedonic, contrahedonic) and instrumental (performance,
gaki & Orehek, 2017) and promote social bonding (Aknin et al., impression management, relationship maintenance, relationship dis-
2013), regulators may report improvements in their own emotional tancing, and emotional similarity) motivations behind interpersonal
well-being and their relationship with the target of their regulatory emotion regulation, considering self- and other-focused motives, as
efforts regardless of their interpersonal emotion regulation well as regulator-perceived outcomes of the social interactions
motives. It has also been shown that positive relationship interac- (changes in emotional well-being and relationship closeness). Ex-
tions can lead to more positive affect (Ramsey & Gentzler, 2015) ploratory aims of this study are to examine the associations between
so it is possible that a person regulating someone’s emotions for interpersonal emotion regulation motives and regulator-perceived
instrumental motives might ultimately reap hedonic benefits even social interaction outcomes as well as examine whether attachment
if their emotion regulation in the moment does not maximize impacts the relationship between interpersonal emotion regulation
hedonic benefits. However, on the other hand, holding certain motives and perceived social interaction outcomes.
interpersonal emotion regulation motives may be associated with We preregistered hypotheses regarding links of attachment to
better outcomes. For example, regulators may feel better after interpersonal emotion regulation motives and social interaction out-
interactions in which they strive to help their partner feel more comes.1 Attachment anxiety predicts rumination, fear of rejection,
positively or to promote a closer relationship with them, whereas and seeking continuous reassurance and closeness in relationships
they might feel worse after interactions where they strive to make (Girme et al., 2021; Özen et al., 2011), therefore, we predict that
their partner feel worse or create distance from them. One study attachment anxiety will be associated with greater self-focused con-
found that individuals who assisted others in regulating their emo- trahedonic, impression management, and relationship maintenance
tions generally showed increases in their own well-being (Doré motives, and conversely less relationship distancing motives (Hy-
et al., 2017), but the role of interpersonal emotion regulation pothesis 1). Attachment avoidance is associated with tendencies to
motives has not yet been explored. Associations between interper- avoid negative affect and minimize one’s emotional and attachment
sonal emotion regulation motives and interaction outcomes could needs (Girme et al., 2021; Özen et al., 2011). Avoidant individuals
appear both on the level of differences between people (e.g., some may further place emphasis on nonattachment motives and avoid
people might be more likely to hold prohedonic motives and to emotional distractions. We therefore hypothesize that attachment
also experience more positive changes in their interactions) and at avoidance will be associated with greater self-focused prohedonic,
the level of situations (e.g., regardless of how much people gener- self-focused performance, and relationship distancing motives, as
ally hold prohedonic motives, at times when they hold more pro- well as less relationship maintenance and emotional similarity
hedonic motives than usual they are more likely to experience motives (Hypothesis 2). We expect attachment anxiety (Hypothesis
positive changes in their interactions). Study designs that incorpo- 3) and avoidance (Hypothesis 4) to be associated with lower emo-
rate multiple assessments per person enable us to distinguish tional and relational interaction outcomes. The hypothesized rela-
between these two types of effects while exploring the role of tionships between attachment and interpersonal emotion regulation
interpersonal emotion regulation in daily interactions. (motives and social outcomes) are expected to be more pronounced
in romantic relationships (Hypothesis 5).
Attachment and Changes in Emotional and Relational We also explore two nonpreregistered research questions (RQs).
Well-Being First, we assess the links between interpersonal emotion regulation

Attachment is expected to predict how individuals perceive social 1


In the preregistration, the social interaction outcomes were labeled as
interactions to have impacted their emotional and relational well- efficacy indicators, however we have changed this framing based on
being. Attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety are associated helpful peer review comments.
ATTACHMENT AND INTERPERSONAL REGULATION 5

motives and social interaction outcomes to gather initial evidence their motives behind engaging in interpersonal emotion regulation
about whether how one is motivated to regulate others’ emotions during the interaction (if any), and the perceived emotional and rela-
relates to the perceived outcomes (exploratory RQ1). Second, we tional outcomes of this interaction. Social interactions were
assess whether attachment moderates the association between defined as being able to occur both in-person or virtually, if commu-
motives and interaction outcomes to get insight into potential nication was reciprocated (e.g., texts were sent back and forth), and
attachment-related differences in interpersonal emotion regulation there was no minimum length to be considered social interactions.
motive adaptiveness (exploratory RQ2). There was a reminder sent five minutes after the initial notification,
Experience sampling is used in the present study to assess inter- and the survey expired 15 min after the reminder. Participants were
personal emotion regulation in daily social interactions. This asked to try to complete as many of the surveys as possible.
approach increases reliability of the participants’ ratings of interper-
sonal emotion regulation and momentary well-being, as reporting Material
close to the time of the event should limit memory biases (Riediger,
2010). It also allows for the assessment of multiple contexts within- Attachment Measures
person, such as interactions with different partners. Previous work
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Adult attachment was assessed with the Experiences in Close


has shown that individuals vary in how they regulate the emotions
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Relationships–Revised (ECR-R), which is a questionnaire consisting


of different relationship partners (Niven et al., 2012) and that inter-
of 36 attachment-relevant statements that apply to general
personal emotion regulation is situationally dependent (Liu et al.,
(platonic and/or romantic) relationship tendencies, with 18 anxiety-
2021; Pauw et al., 2019). Therefore, assessing interpersonal emotion
related items (e.g., “I’m afraid that I will lose others’ love”; a = .91)
regulation motives and perceived changes in social interaction out-
and 18 avoidance-related items (e.g., “I prefer not to show others
comes repeatedly can provide more ecologically valid insight into
how I feel deep down”; a = .93). For each attachment item, partici-
interpersonal emotion regulation processes in daily life.
pants selected from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

Method Daily Measures


Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives. To capture the
Participants motivations behind engaging in interpersonal emotion regulation,
Participants (N = 211) at a midsized Midwestern university in participants reported on various motives they had for regulating
the United States (M age in years = 19.77, SD = 1.25) were their partners’ emotions during their most recent interaction (“To
recruited via Sona, from an online Psychology subject pool of what extent did you have any of following goals to manage your
undergraduates. Students who did not have a smartphone or were partner’s emotions during your most recent interaction, whether
traveling through different time zones for the duration of the study you acted on them or not?”). We focused on assessing hedonic
were ineligible to participate because a change in time zones motives, performance, and social motives because they are the
would prevent participants from gaining access to their daily sur- most commonly studied and reported reasons for managing emotion
veys. On average, participants completed 77% of surveys (SD = (English et al., 2017; Kalokerinos et al., 2017). Hedonic and per-
.22). No missing data was imputed as estimation in multilevel formance items2 were assessed in terms of both the regulator’s out-
models account for differing amounts of data per person. comes (self-focused) and the target’s outcomes (other-focused),
Most participants self-identified as female (73%), followed by whereas social motives were asked about in the context of the rela-
male (26%), nonbinary (.5%), and other (.5%). The sample was tionship between regulator and target and therefore not separated
stratified in terms of class year (23% first year, 30% second year, into self and other related components. Motives included the follow-
27% third year, 20% fourth year). Most participants identified as ing: prohedonic (self-focused/other-focused), contrahedonic (self-
White (54%), followed by Asian (33%), African American (11%), focused/other-focused), performance (self-focused/other-focused),
Latinx (11%), Middle Eastern (3%), and Native American (1%). impression management, relationship maintenance, relationship dis-
Participants were compensated with one course credit for an tancing, and emotional similarity. Participants rated their agreement
hour of participation in the study. The present study was approved with each motive item on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to
by the Institutional Review Board at Washington University in St. 7 (extremely). Prohedonic, contrahedonic, self-focused performance,
Louis. Sensitivity analyses for our main hypotheses are provided and emotional similarity motives were assessed with single items,
in Table S1 in the online supplemental material. while impression management, other-focused performance, relation-
ship maintenance, and relationship distancing motives were each
Procedure assessed with two items.3 For example, prohedonic interpersonal emo-
tion regulation motives were assessed with the items, “I wanted to
Following informed consent, participants were asked to download
the application Expiwell (2021) to their smartphone. Participants then 2
Other-focused performance motives were not initially examined due to
completed a 5-min background survey while on the phone, which our focus on socioemotional components in this study, but they were added
contained demographic questions, an attachment measure, and a per- during the review process for a more comprehensive assessment of
sonality trait measure and received an Ecological Momentary Assess- interpersonal emotion regulation motives.
3
ment (EMA) tutorial. The following day, participants began receiving Due to time constraints and participant burden, not all motives could
be assessed with two items. The decision about how many items to include
randomized two-minute surveys within four-hour intervals, three per construct was made based on breadth of the construct (with more
times a day, for seven days (within a 12-hr daily window of their straightforward, narrow motives, like the hedonic ones, being assessed with
choice). Each survey asked about the participant’s last interaction, a single item).
6 SPRINGSTEIN, HAMERLING-POTTS, LANDA, AND ENGLISH

feel better or maintain my own positive feelings” (self-focused) and “I variables were grand mean-centered. Random slopes for within-
wanted to make them feel better or maintain their good feelings” person effects were included unless convergence issues occurred.
(other-focused). These items were adapted from the Emotion Regula- For exploratory RQ2, which focuses on attachment as a moderator
tion Goal Scale (Eldesouky & English, 2019). As preregistered, for of the motive-outcome links, we added attachment anxiety and
motives that had more than one item, scores were averaged when the avoidance as additional predictors along with consecutively test-
between and within-person omega were .6 or higher. See Table S2 in ing interactions between these dimensions of attachment and the
the online supplemental material for all items. within- person components of motives (controlling for other motives).
Perceived Changes in Social Interaction Outcomes. To cap- As preregistered, we performed two types of analyses building
ture perceived changes in social interaction outcomes, participants on the initial models. The first models added the interaction
were asked about the perceived change in their emotional well- between attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. The sec-
being (positive emotion and negative emotion) and relationship ond models tested robustness of the hypothesized effects by add-
well-being (relationship closeness and relationship satisfaction) in ing as covariates the Big Five personality traits previously linked
their most recent interaction. Participants indicated the extent to with attachment (i.e., extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism,
which there was change in well-being on a 7-point Likert scale, and contentiousness; Noftle & Shaver, 2006). Results are provided
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ranging from 1 (much less positive emotion than before) to 7


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in Tables S4 through S6 in the online supplemental material.


(much more positive emotion than before) for each of these four
items. As preregistered, because the between-person and within-
Results
person omegas for emotional well-being (xs = .85 and .80, respec-
tively) and relationship well-being (xs = .84 and .73, respectively)
suggested strong correlations between the perceived changes in Preanalyses
social interaction outcomes subscale items, scores were combined About 77% of the reported social interactions were within one
into emotional well-being and relationship well-being composites. hour of the prompt (42% occurred at the time of the prompt), mini-
Relationship Type. When asked about their most recent inter- mizing memory biases. The largest proportion of social interactions
action, participants responded by labeling the primary category of occurred between friends (53% college friends and 6% home
their relationship with the interaction partner from the following friends), followed by family members (17%) and romantic partners
options: romantic partner, college friend, home friend, family (11%). On average, interactions were with others whom participants
member, acquaintance, professor, stranger, or other. Participants
considered close (Mcloseness = 5.26 on a 7-point Likert scale). Attach-
were asked to label the primary person with whom they interacted.
ment avoidance was associated with less interaction with college
friends and more interaction with family, romantic partners, and pro-
Analysis Plan fessors. Attachment anxiety was associated with less interaction with
Given the data are nested (i.e., multiple daily measures for each college friends and romantic partners, and more interaction with fam-
person), this study utilized multilevel models using R (R Core ily and acquaintances (see Table S7 in the online supplemental mate-
Team, 2021), with the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015). As prereg- rial for correlations and descriptive statistics).
istered, the hypotheses (H1 through H4) were tested by constructing To assess how much variance in motives could be explained by
a series of models where interpersonal emotion regulation motives individual differences (e.g., attachment) we assessed between-person
and perceived changes in social interaction outcomes (Level 1 varia- and within-person variance in motives. Slightly more of the variance
bles) were simultaneously predicted by attachment anxiety and was at the within-person level for all motives (intraclass correlations
attachment avoidance (Level 2 variables), with participant gender [ICCs] = .38–.48), except for impression management (.53) and
included as a Level 2 covariate (coded as 1 = female and 0 = male). other-focused performance (.51). However, there was much more
Given the presence of time trends in the data (see Table S3 in the within-person variance than between-person variance for perceived
online supplemental material), we also included time (centered on changes in social interaction outcomes (emotional well-being = .14;
the first prompt for each participant) as a Level 1 covariate. relationship well-being = .16). This suggests that why people regulate
To test H5, separate models were run in which relationship type someone’s emotions and how they perceive the interaction depends
(coded as 1 = romantic partner and 0 = nonromantic interaction part- more on effects of the situation than on ways in which individuals
ner) and interaction terms between relationship type and each differ or what people are like on average. In terms of frequency of
dimension of attachment (i.e., Type 3 Anxiety and Type 3 Avoid- endorsing interpersonal emotion regulation motives, individuals most
ance) were included as addition predictors of each interpersonal strongly endorsed regulating others’ emotions to maintain the rela-
emotion regulation motive and social interaction outcome. The fre- tionship with them (M = 4.61), followed by prohedonic motives
quency of interacting with romantic partners was relatively low related to the target’s emotions (M = 4.09) and to one’s own emo-
(11% of prompts), so relationship type moderation analyses are tions (M = 3.82). See Table 1 for and descriptive statistics.
reported in the online supplemental materials.
For exploratory RQ1, which focuses on the link between interper- Preregistered Hypotheses: Does Attachment Predict
sonal emotion regulation motives and social interaction outcomes, Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives and
we constructed separate multilevel models with within-person and Perceived Social Interaction Outcomes?
between-person scores of all motives simultaneously predicting the
two types of outcomes. Within-person and between-person level See Table 1 for bivariate correlations between attachment and
variables were separated by person-mean centering motive variables predicted interpersonal emotion regulation outcomes. See Table 2
as recommended by Bolger and Laurenceau (2013). Between-person for unstandardized estimates of fixed effects of attachment on
ATTACHMENT AND INTERPERSONAL REGULATION 7

interpersonal emotion regulation motives and perceived change in

.06**

.10**
.16**
.29**

.10**
.25**
.14**
.17**
.25**

dicted outcome variable using residual and subject variance. ICCs represent between-person variability for each variable. Between-person correlations are below the diagonal. Within-person associa-
Note. Means and standard deviations were calculated averaging within-person mean scores and standard deviations across participants. Intraclass correlations (ICCs) were calculated for each pre-

tions for interpersonal emotion regulation measures are above the diagonal. WB = well-being. Items 3 and 4 refer to the indicators of perceived change in interaction outcomes while items 5 through
.01
14 social interaction outcomes. Attachment anxiety predicted more
self-focused prohedonic motives (c ¼ 0:22; p ¼ :020; R2b ¼ :02Þ,
and more impression management motives (c ¼ 0:32; p ¼ :001;
.15**
.06**

.39**
.16**

.09**
.34**
.10**
.07**

.73**
.00

.01
13

R2b ¼ :05Þ as hypothesized (H1). None of the hypothesized associa-


tions between attachment anxiety and contrahedonic motives, rela-
.33**
.29**
.30**
.07**

.39**
.49**
.14**
.20**

.19**
.47**
tionship maintenance motives, impression management motives, or
.04*
12

relationship distancing motives emerged. Attachment avoidance did


not significantly predict any of the hypothesized interpersonal emo-
tion regulation motives (H2).
.10**
.11**
.22**
.11**

.21**
.18**

.57**
.51**
.67**
.02

.03
11

Attachment anxiety did not predict perceived change in emo-


tional well-being (c ¼ 0:02; p ¼ :519; R2b ¼ :00) or relation-
ship well-being (c ¼ 0:00; p ¼ :914; R2b ¼ :00), contrary to
.32**
.26**

.21**
.36**
.05**
.15**

.61**
.31**
.79**
.80**
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.01
10
Between-Person and Within-Person Correlations for Attachment, Interaction Outcomes, and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives

our predictions (H3). In contrast, attachment avoidance predicted


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

both aspects of perceived social interaction outcomes (H4).


.32**
.28**
.32**
.06**
.05**
.38**

.20**
.44**
.81**

.35**

Attachment avoidance predicted worse social interaction out-


.08
9

comes both in terms of perceived change in emotional well-being


(c ¼ 0:12; p , :000; R2b ¼ :01) and in relationship well-being
(c ¼ 0:05; p ¼ :025; R2b ¼ :01Þ.
.21**
.25**
.29**

.07**

.76**
.38**
.51**
.83**
.29**
.55**
8

.02

Analyses pertaining to preregistered Hypothesis 5 (effect of


interacting with romantic relationship partner on associations
.20**
.14**
.07**
.14**

.50**
.37**
.74**
.61**
.47**
.50**
.82**

between attachment and motives) did not reveal any significant


7

associations. That is, there was no evidence that attachment was


more predictive of interpersonal emotion regulation motives or
.14**
.06**

.54**
.29**

.77**
.55**
.23**
.93**
.73**

perceived outcomes in interactions with romantic partners com-


.01

.12
6

pared with other types of social interactions. Detailed results are


provided in Table S8 in the online supplemental material.
.23**
.19**

.28**
.56**
.77**
.73**
.42**
.61**
.84**
.24**
.53**

In follow-up models, there was only a significant Attachment


5

Anxiety 3 Attachment Avoidance interaction for emotional simi-


larity (see Table S4 in the online supplemental material), such that
.59**

.40**

.27**
.40**
.41**

.37**
.44**

.25**
.14*
4

.13

.09

avoidance was only related to lower emotion similarity motives


when attachment anxiety was high. The hypothesized results were
.60**
.32**
.21**

.24**
.37**
.22**

.39**
.26**

largely robust to controlling for Big Five personality traits (see


.06

.06

.07
3

Table S6 in the online supplemental material).


.24**

Exploratory Research Questions: Do Interpersonal


.16*
.03
.02
.04
.03
.05
.05
.12
.07
.02
.02
2

Emotion Regulation Motives Predict Perceived Social


Interaction Outcomes?
.26**

.21**
.12
.04
.13
.07
.08

.07
.13
.10
.06
.03
.08

We assessed both within-person and between-person effects of


1

interpersonal emotion regulation motives predicting perceived


social interaction outcomes (exploratory RQ1). Within-person out-
ICC

14 refer to interpersonal emotion regulation motives.


.14
.16
.43
.39
.39
.53
.48
.44
.47
.38
.46
.51

comes here show how holding more or less of a motive (compared


with how much one endorses the respective motive on average)
1.01
1.08
1.02
0.62
1.32
0.82
1.32
1.19
1.12
0.89
1.18
1.43
0.69
0.87
SD

relates to perceived social interaction outcomes. Between-person


effects here show how individuals differ in their perceived social
3.58
3.33
4.56
4.38
3.82
1.76
2.74
3.71
4.61
2.04
2.75
4.09
1.68
2.33

interaction outcomes based on how much they endorse certain inter-


M

personal emotion regulation motives on average. Within-person and


between-person effects are reported in Table 3.
9. Relationship maintenance
8. Impression management

10. Relationship distancing

In terms of within-person effects, self- and other-focused prohe-


2. Attachment avoidance

13. Contrahedonic: Other


11. Emotional similarity

* p , .05. ** p , .01.
6. Contrahedonic: Self

14. Performance: Other


1. Attachment anxiety

donic motives were positively associated with emotional and rela-


12. Prohedonic: Other
7. Performance: Self
4. Relationship WB
5. Prohedonic: Self

tionship well-being; indicating that on occasions when people held


Variable

3. Emotional WB

prohedonic motives, they also reported more positive changes in


well-being due to the interaction. Similarly, when people held
Table 1

impression management and relationship maintenance motives, they


also reported more positive changes in their emotional and relation-
ship well-being. On the other hand, people reported lower emotional
8 SPRINGSTEIN, HAMERLING-POTTS, LANDA, AND ENGLISH

Table 2
Unstandardized Estimates and Semipartial Effect Sizes for Attachment Predicting Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives and
Interaction Outcomes
Attachment anxiety Attachment avoidance
Dependent variable Intercept c (SE) R2b 95% CI c (SE) R2b 95% CI
Motives
Prohedonic: Self 3.84 .22 (.09)* .02 [.04, .40] .17 (.09) .01 [.34, .00]
Contrahedonic: Self 1.89 .04 (.06) .00 [.08, .16] .03 (.06) .00 [.14, .08]
Performance: Self 2.91 .08 (.09) .00 [.09, .05] .06 (.08) .00 [.10, .22]
Impression management 3.62 .32 (.10)** .05 [.13, .52] .04 (.09) .00 [.23, .14]
Relationship distancing 2.11 .08 (.07) .01 [.05, .22] .05 (.06) .00 [.08, .17]
Relationship maintenance 4.80 .12 (.09) .01 [.05, .29] .07 (.08) .00 [.24, .08]
Emotional similarity 3.03 .17 (.09) .01 [.01, .35] .14 (.09) .01 [.31, .03]
Prohedonic: Other 4.13 .11 (.09) .01 [.06, .29] .11 (.09) .01 [.28, .06]
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Contrahedonic: Other 1.85 .03 (.06) .00 [.10, .15] .00 (.06) .00 [.12, .11]
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Performance: Other 2.43 .10 (.07) .00 [.04, .24] .01 (.07) .00 [.14, .12]
Interaction outcomes
Change in emotional well-being 4.68 .02 (.03) .00 [.09, .05] .12 (.03)** .01 [.18, .05]
Change in relationship well-being 4.44 .00 (.02) .00 [.05, .04] .05 (.02)* .01 [.10, .01]
Note. Values in square brackets indicate the 95% confidence interval for each estimate. Gender and time were included as covariates in each model.
Both attachment dimensions were grand-mean centered and included together as predictors of each motive and perceived change in interaction outcome.
Random intercepts were included.
* p , .05. ** p , .01.

well-being when they more strongly held contrahedonic and relation- interpersonal, motivated processes of ER, where one aims to reg-
ship distancing motives, as well as lower relationship well-being ulate another person’s emotions.
when they held relationship distancing motives. As a reminder, these Attachment anxiety was predictive of differences in interperso-
effects were shown controlling for other relevant interpersonal emo- nal emotion regulation motives in the context of social interac-
tion regulation motives and time, and they were present above and tions, with effect sizes generally indicating small associations, but
beyond the between-person effects of motives, or the tendency to set attachment avoidance was not. Consistent with our preregistered
interpersonal emotion regulation motives more generally. hypotheses, attachment anxiety predicted more impression man-
In terms of between-person effects, people that held more self- agement motives. Although attachment avoidance was not associ-
and other-focused prohedonic motives on average perceived more ated with differences in interpersonal emotion regulation motives
positive changes in emotional well-being. People that held more in general, there was an interaction between attachment avoidance
impression management motives on average perceived fewer posi- and attachment anxiety in predicting emotion similarity. For indi-
tive changes in emotional well-being, but no other between-person viduals high in attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance was
effects on relational well-being emerged. associated with less motivation to regulate their partners emotions
We also explored whether the relationship between interperso- in order to feel similar emotional states. Further, attachment avoid-
nal emotion regulation motives and perceived social interaction ance predicted worse social interaction outcomes, as expected,
outcomes was dependent on attachment (exploratory RQ2). How- both in terms of perceived change in emotional well-being and
ever, no significant interaction effects emerged (see Table S9 in relationship well-being. Attachment anxiety, however, was not
the online supplemental materials). predictive of social interaction outcomes.
There was some initial evidence that interpersonal emotion regula-
Discussion tion motives are differentially related to perceived social interaction
The present study aimed to examine whether attachment pre- outcomes. Evidence emerged both on the level of individual differen-
dicts why people regulate the emotions of others and how effec- ces (e.g., if someone reports prohedonic motives more frequently, are
tive they perceive this regulation to be for their relationship with they also more likely to report more positive changes in their interac-
the regulation target and for their own emotional well-being. A tion outcomes?) and at the situational level (e.g., regardless of the
secondary, exploratory aim of the study was to examine the asso- motives a person typically holds, do they report more positive
ciations between interpersonal emotion regulation motives and changes in their interactions when they endorse more prohedonic
perceived social interaction outcomes. Emotion regulation is motives?). People who tended to report more prohedonic, impression
closely tied to attachment, as attachment can manifest through management, or relationship maintenance motives and less contrahe-
emotion regulation habits that develop as a result of a relation- donic and relationship distancing motives in daily life also perceived
ship (or lack thereof) with a caregiver (Ainsworth & Bowlby, more positive changes in their emotional and relational well-being af-
1991; Bartholomew, 1990). However, prior work on attachment ter social interactions. People also perceived more positive emotional
primarily has focused on intrapersonal emotion regulation or cor- and relational interaction outcomes at times when they held more
egulation (Butler & Randall, 2013; Butner et al., 2007; Cassidy, prohedonic, impression management or relationship maintenance
1994; Girme et al., 2021). The current work extends these find- motives and less self-focused performance and relationship distanc-
ings by examining the role of attachment in the context of ing motives.
ATTACHMENT AND INTERPERSONAL REGULATION 9

Table 3
Unstandardized Within- and Between-Person Effects of Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives on Interaction Outcomes
Emotional well-being Relationship well-being
Predictor variable c (SE) R2b 95% CI RE (SD) c (SE) R2b 95% CI RE (SD)
Intercept 4.69 (.06) 4.43 (.04)
Within-person
Prohedonic: Self .10 (.01)** .01 [.07, .12] .01 (09) .04 (.01)** .01 [.03, .07] .00 (.07)
Contrahedonic: Self .06 (.02)** .00 [.11, .02] .02 (.14) .00 (.01) .00 [.03, .01] /
Performance: Self .07 (.01)** .01 [.10, .05] .00 (.06) .03 (.01)** .01 /
Impression management .04 (.01)** .01 [.01, .07] .07 (.01)** .01 [.06, .09] /
Relationship distancing .22 (.02)** .04 [.26, .18] .12 (.02)** .03 [.18, .13] .03 (.17)
Relationship maintenance .09 (.02)** .01 [.07, .13] .06 (.01)** .01 [.07, .11] /
Emotional similarity .01 (.01) .00 [.01, .04] .01 (.01)* .00 [.00, .04] /
Prohedonic: Other .11 (.01)** .02 [.01, .04] .05 (.01)* .01 [.03, .07] /
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Contrahedonic: Other .02 (.02) .00 [.01, .04] .02 (.02) .00 [.01, .06] .01 (.11)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Performance: Other .02 (.02) .00 [.01, .04] .01 (.01) .00 [.03, .02] /
Between-person
Prohedonic: Self .10 (.04)* .01 [.02, .19] .02 (.03) .00 [.03, .09]
Contrahedonic: Self .01 (.10) .00 [.20, .19] .02 (.07) .00 [.10, .15]
Performance: Self .09 (.05) .00 [.18, .01] .02 (.03) .00 [.10, .15]
Impression management .08 (.04)* .00 [.16, .00] .00 (.03) .00 [.01, .09]
Relationship distancing .10 (.07) .00 [.23, .03] .08 (.05) .00 [.16, .01]
Relationship maintenance .05 (.04) .00 [.03, .14] .01 (.03) .00 [.01, .10]
Emotional similarity .02 (.03) .00 [.09, .04] .06 (.02)* .01 [.01, .11]
Prohedonic: Other .15 (.06)** .01 [.05, .26] .07 (.04) .00 [.00, .13]
Contrahedonic: Other .13 (.11) .00 [.34, .08] .01 (.07) .00 [.15, .13]
Performance: Other 13 (.07) .00 [.01, .26] .01 (.05) .00 [.08, .10]
Note. Values in brackets indicate the 95% confidence interval for each estimate. Random slopes were included unless models showed convergence
issues. Gender and time were added as covariates in each model. Between-person effects were grand-mean centered. RE = random effect.
* p , .05. ** p , .01.

Attachment and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation et al., 2011). Preoccupation, fear of abandonment, and fear of
rejection associated with hyperactivation of the attachment system
Although we expected attachment avoidance to predict more pro- are characteristic of attachment anxiety and lead individuals to
hedonic motives and attachment anxiety to predict more contrahe- seek validation and avoid rejection.
donic motives, attachment anxiety was associated with more Attachment was unrelated to relationship maintenance and rela-
prohedonic motives while attachment avoidance was unrelated to ei- tionship distancing interpersonal emotion regulation motives.
ther type of hedonic motive. That is, increased attachment anxiety However, attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety interacted
was associated with the tendency to want to maintain a positive to predict emotional similarity motives, a largely unexplored rea-
emotional state in interpersonal emotion regulation interactions. son people may engage in interpersonal emotion regulation. We
Rather than consciously engaging in ruminative tendencies, perhaps expected attachment avoidance to predict decreased emotional
those high in attachment anxiety focus on self-comfort and soothing similarity motives, but avoidance was only linked to lower similar-
through interactions with others. Because anxiously attached indi- ity motives among those who were also high in attachment anxiety
viduals fear abandonment in relationships (Brennan et al., 1998), it (also known as “fearful attachment”). Avoidant attachment is char-
is plausible that they would try to regulate close others with the goal acterized by the tendency to preclude states of vulnerability. Avoi-
of relieving some of their own relationship-focused apprehension. In dant tendencies, when coupled with the intense fear of rejection
addition, more anxiously attached individuals have been shown to characteristic of anxiety, produces a deep distrust of social rela-
react to pain in others with greater personal distress (Britton & tionships that may lead to avoiding emotional linkage with another
Fuendeling, 2005; Monin et al., 2010). This suggests that when deal- person (Bartholomew, 1990). Future research should examine
ing with others’ emotions more anxiously attached individuals are whether intrapersonal emotional similarity motives (i.e., wanting
more emotionally reactive and experience a greater need to regulate to regulate your own emotion so you will feel the way your partner
their own emotions. feels) also are linked to attachment.
In terms of instrumental motives, our finding that attachment Our findings were mixed regarding perceived social interaction
anxiety is related to impression management in the context of outcomes. Both forms of insecure attachment were expected to pre-
interpersonal emotion regulation is consistent with prior literature dict worse perceived social interaction outcomes, as attachment
showing anxiously attached individuals desire approval from avoidance and attachment anxiety have been previously linked to
others to reduce the threat of abandonment (Brennan et al., 1998). lower emotional well-being and relationship satisfaction (Niven
Hypervigilance to threat means that anxious individuals are self- et al., 2012; Özen et al., 2011) as well as worse emotion regulation
focused in processing issues and will seek soothing and reassur- skills (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2019). However, only attachment avoid-
ance, particularly about the relationship (Girme et al., 2021; Özen ance was significantly associated with perceived social interaction
10 SPRINGSTEIN, HAMERLING-POTTS, LANDA, AND ENGLISH

outcomes in this context. The fact that attachment anxiety was linked In contrast, fewer effects of motives emerged on the between-
to interpersonal emotion regulation motives but not perceived inter- person level, indicating that how much people held motives in the
action outcomes, whereas attachment avoidance was linked to per- moment was more relevant to their well-being than why they typi-
ceived outcomes but not motives, suggests that maladaptive motives cally regulate the emotions of others. The effects that did emerge
may not be the cause of attachment-related interpersonal emotion suggest people who are more motivated to regulate the emotions
regulation difficulties. More broadly, these findings highlight the of others for prohedonic reasons (related to oneself or other per-
value of examining multiples aspects of the emotion regulation pro- son) and less motivated motived by the desire to make a good
cess, such as when, why, and how individuals try to manage partners’ impression showed higher perceived changes in their emotional
emotions, as well as whether they monitor the regulation process for well-being. In addition, people who were generally more moti-
indications of success or failure and switch strategies when needed vated to regulate others’ emotions to feel more similar showed
(Gross, 2015). more positive changes in relational well-being after interactions,
Apart from the findings discussed above, attachment did not aligning with research documenting the benefits of shared emo-
predict interpersonal emotion regulation motives and outcomes to tional states for relationship satisfaction (Anderson et al., 2003).
the extent that we predicted. Although null effects should be inter- Attachment did not influence associations between motives and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

social interaction outcomes, suggesting the adaptiveness of interper-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

preted cautiously, there are two potential reasons for the lack of
support for our hypotheses. First, interpersonal emotion regulation sonal emotion regulation motives might hold regardless of one’s
motives showed more within- than between-person variance, as attachment. Together, the findings regarding motive-outcome links
has been found for intrapersonal emotion regulation motives illustrate the importance of utilizing methods that measure interper-
(Kalokerinos et al., 2017), suggesting motives may depend on con- sonal emotion regulation motives in the moment (i.e., ESM, experi-
textual factors more so than on dispositional traits, such as attach- ments) and disentangling within- versus between-person effects.
ment. Second, we derived our hypotheses on interpersonal
emotion regulation and attachment based on the literature on intra- Limitations and Future Directions
personal emotion regulation and intrinsic interpersonal emotion One limitation of the present study is that it relied on self-
regulation due to the sparsity of research on attachment as it reports from the regulator. While emotion regulation strategies can
relates to regulating the emotions of others. However, it is possible involve observable behaviors, motivations behind emotion regula-
that attachment related processes that concern one’s own emotions tion involve internal mental processes that may be more difficult
do not operate in a similar way regarding others’ emotions. For to articulate. Moreover, perceived social interaction outcomes were
example, individuals higher in attachment avoidance might deacti- assessed from only one party and it is possible that although one
vate their attachment system regarding their own emotions (e.g., may perceive their relationship to be improved, their partner could
be less motivated to reach out for intrinsic interpersonal emotion have a different experience. For example, although individuals
regulation in order to increase closeness with someone else) but higher in attachment anxiety who engaged in interpersonal emo-
not apply this approach to how they engage with others’ emotions. tion regulation did not report lower relationship well-being, their
They may be equally motivated to regulate someone else’s emo- interaction partner might have reported more displeasure in the
tions in order to increase closeness as they do not have to be con- relationship. Future research should collect data from dyads to test
cerned about their own support needs not being met when they are if well-being consequences converge between both parties. This
the regulator. type of design could provide insight into how one’s interpersonal
emotion regulation efforts may influence the well-being of another
Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Motives and person and allow an examination of both the target’s perception of
Perceived Social Interaction Outcomes their own well-being and of the regulator’s well-being.
Second, the measures of interpersonal emotion regulation
Most interpersonal emotion regulation motives in the present motives and social interaction outcomes used in this study are
study predicted perceived social interaction outcomes. The pattern somewhat limited. To reduce participant burden and fatigue, some
of findings in the present study might suggest that, on average, motives were assessed using only one item which might have
people report achieving the goals of their interpersonal emotion increased measurement error. Future research is needed with mul-
regulation. Specifically, people reported more improvements in tiple, validated items for each interpersonal emotion regulation
emotional and relational well-being following social interactions motive. Further, although we captured a wide range of motives
where they held more self- or other-focused prohedonic, relation- that theoretically should be most relevant to interpersonal emotion
ship maintenance, or impression management motives, (and less regulation, there may be other motives worth exploring (Tamir,
self-focused performance motives and relationship distancing 2016). In addition, individuals were able to endorse multiple
motives), above and beyond the general tendency to set these motives and while hedonic and instrumental motives are theoreti-
interpersonal emotion regulation motives. Each of these motives cally distinct (i.e., individuals are thought to at times regulate emo-
were uniquely associated with well-being given that their effects tions based purely on their hedonic pleasure or purely based on
were simultaneously assessed. While previous work has shown their instrumental function), our hedonic and instrumental items
that individuals that support others (Inagaki & Orehek, 2017) and covaried in a way that indicates these motives often co-occurred.
help others regulate their emotions (Doré et al., 2017) generally Future work should try to assess regulation in a way that can
showed increases in their own well-being, our findings show that clearly capture instances of purely hedonic motivation.
these effects depend on the motives that people hold for engaging Further, our outcome measures were relatively broad in focus
in interpersonal emotion regulation. and did not differentiate between specific aspects of emotional and
ATTACHMENT AND INTERPERSONAL REGULATION 11

relational well-being. We did not include items that could capture regulate others’ emotions in order to minimize their own negative
proximal interpersonal emotion regulation outcomes for certain affect. Consistent with prior literature on the thought patterns of
motives, such as performance (e.g., perceived ability to focus on a anxious attachment, those higher in attachment anxiety also
task). In addition, our measures showed that participants often did engaged in interpersonal emotion regulation with the goal of
not perceive large amounts of changes in well-being related to the improving others’ opinions of them (Brennan et al., 1998). Attach-
interactions they reported on, which might have limited our ability ment avoidance, on the other hand, was linked to social interaction
to find effects. Event contingent or lab-based assessment could be outcomes with individuals higher in attachment avoidance
used to assess moment by moment changes in perceived social reported lower perceived emotional and relationship well-being af-
and emotional functioning in order to not require individuals to ter interpersonal interactions. Future work can build on these ini-
possess metaknowledge or insight into how their emotional and tial findings about implications of attachment for interpersonal
relational well-being changed over time. emotion regulation and the role of motive setting in interpersonal
As the attachment system is activated in emotionally intense sit- regulation. Doing so can shed light on the nature of psychological
uations (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2019), future research focusing on well-being in relational contexts and offer ways to improve inter-
emotionally evocative events might be able to paint a clearer pic- personal functioning.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ture of the role of attachment in interpersonal emotion regulation


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

motivation. While the attachment system is most likely activated


in close, romantic relationships, our sample included only a minor- References
ity of participants (N = 67) that were currently in a relationship
Ainsworth, M. S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to per-
and frequency of interacting with a romantic partner was generally
sonality development. American Psychologist, 46(4), 333–341. https://
low, which may have interfered with our ability to detect effects doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.4.333
pertaining to romantic relationships. Future studies focusing on Aknin, L. B., Dunn, E. W., Sandstrom, G. M., & Norton, M. I. (2013).
specific close relationships or employing dyadic designs could Does social connection turn good deeds into good feelings? On the value
overcome this limitation. of putting the ‘social’ in prosocial spending. International Journal of
Also, as in previous research (e.g., English et al., 2017) contra- Happiness and Development, 1(2), 155–171. https://doi.org/10.1504/
hedonic regulation was reported infrequently. Conflict interactions IJHD.2013.055643
with a close other (e.g., in laboratory settings or assessed through Altan-Atalay, A. (2019). Interpersonal emotion regulation: Associations
event-contingent experience sampling) that pose a threat to the with attachment and reinforcement sensitivity. Personality and Individ-
relationship might provide a promising context for examining con- ual Differences, 139, 290–294.
Anderson, C., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2003). Emotional convergence
trahedonic regulation of one’s own and someone else’s emotions,
between people over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
particularly with regard to more anxiously attached individuals ogy, 84(5), 1054–1068. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1054
who are more likely to perceive the upregulation of negative emo- Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspec-
tions as congruent with their goals to maintain closeness with their tive. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7(2), 147–178.
partner in a threatening situation (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2019). https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407590072001
As posited in the emotion regulation process model (Gross, Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear
2015), the activation of an emotion regulation motive or goal (e.g., mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1),
wanting to regulate someone’s emotions so the relationship 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
becomes closer) does not mean that individuals necessarily select Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods: An
a regulation strategy or successfully implement this strategy. Our introduction to diary and experience sampling research. Guilford Press.
research focused on the question to which extent people held cer- Bowlby, J. (1978). Attachment theory and its therapeutic implications. Ad-
olescent Psychiatry, 6, 5–33.
tain motives and participants were explicitly instructed to report
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measure-
on their motives whether they acted on them or not. Thus far there ment of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson &
has not been much research attention directed toward whether peo- W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–
ple act on their motives, which is an important aspect of intra- and 76). Guilford Press.
interpersonal emotion regulation or dysregulation. Future research Britton, P. C., & Fuendeling, J. M. (2005). The relations among varieties
will be necessary to assess why and how failure to act on one’s of adult attachment and the components of empathy. The Journal of
motives occurs and whether this is due to characteristics of the Social Psychology, 145(5), 519–530. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.145
regulator (e.g., lack of knowledge about strategies or inhibition) or .5.519-530
the regulation context (e.g., the conversation lacking opportunity Butler, E. A., & Randall, A. K. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close
to actively engage in regulation). relationships. Emotion Review, 5(2), 202–210. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1754073912451630
Butner, J., Diamond, L. M., & Hicks, A. M. (2007). Attachment style and
Conclusion two forms of affect coregulation between romantic partners. Personal
Relationships, 14(3), 431–455. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811
This study demonstrated that attachment predicts individual dif-
.2007.00164.x
ferences in certain interpersonal emotion regulation motives as
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive
well as the perceived emotional and relational impact of interper- and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review,
sonal emotion regulation. It also provided preliminary evidence of 97(1), 19–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19
links between interpersonal emotion regulation motives and per- Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relation-
ceived social interaction outcomes in daily social interactions. ships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Individuals higher in attachment anxiety may be more motived to 59(2–3), 228–249. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01287.x
12 SPRINGSTEIN, HAMERLING-POTTS, LANDA, AND ENGLISH

Doherty, N. A., & Feeney, J. A. (2004). The composition of attachment Mauss, I. B., & Tamir, M. (2014). Emotion goals: How their content, struc-
networks throughout the adult years. Personal Relationships, 11(4), ture, and operation shape emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Hand-
469–488. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00093.x book of emotion regulation (pp. 361–375). Guilford Press.
Domingue, R., & Mollen, D. (2009). Attachment and conflict communi- Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Adult attachment strategies and
cation in adult romantic relationships. Journal of Social and Personal the regulation of emotion. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion reg-
Relationships, 26(5), 678–696. https://doi.org/10.1177/02654075 ulation (pp. 446–465). Guilford Press.
09347932 Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2009). An attachment and behavioral sys-
Doré, B. P., Morris, R. R., Burr, D. A., Picard, R. W., & Ochsner, K. N. tems perspective on social support. Journal of Social and Personal Rela-
(2017). Helping others regulate emotion predicts increased regulation of tionships, 26(1), 7–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407509105518
one’s own emotions and decreased symptoms of depression. Personality Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2019). Attachment orientations and emo-
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(5), 729–739. https://doi.org/10 tion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 6–10. https://doi
.1177/0146167217695558 .org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.006
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, Monin, J. K., Schulz, R., Feeney, B. C., & Cook, T. B. (2010). Attachment
R. M., & Reno, R. R. (1989). Relation of sympathy and personal distress insecurity and perceived partner suffering as predictors of personal dis-
to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality and tress. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 1143–1147.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Social Psychology, 57(1), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.009
.1.55 Netzer, L., Van Kleef, G. A., & Tamir, M. (2015). Interpersonal instru-
Eldesouky, L., & English, T. (2019). Individual differences in emotion reg- mental emotion regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
ulation goals: Does personality predict the reasons why people regulate 58, 124–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.01.006
their emotions? Journal of Personality, 87(4), 750–766. https://doi.org/ Niven, K. (2016). Why do people engage in interpersonal emotion regula-
10.1111/jopy.12430 tion at work? Organizational Psychology Review, 6(4), 305–323. https://
English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regula- doi.org/10.1177/2041386615612544
tion strategy selection in daily life: The role of social context and goals. Niven, K., Garcia, D., van der Löwe, I., Holman, D., & Mansell, W.
Motivation and Emotion, 41(2), 230–242. https://doi.org/10.1007/ (2015). Becoming popular: Interpersonal emotion regulation predicts
s11031-016-9597-z
relationship formation in real life social networks. Frontiers in Psychol-
Expiwell. (2021). Expiwell (Version 2.0.6) [Mobile app]. Apple. https://
ogy, 6, 1452. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01452
apps.apple.com/us/app/expiwell/id1070733440
Niven, K., Macdonald, I., & Holman, D. (2012). You spin me right round:
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attachment and the sup-
Cross-relationship variability in interpersonal emotion regulation. Fron-
pression of unwanted thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social
tiers in Psychology, 3, 394. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00394
Psychology, 73(5), 1080–1091. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
Noftle, E. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2006). Attachment dimensions and the Big
.73.5.1080
Five personality traits: Associations and comparative ability to predict
Girme, Y. U., Jones, R. E., Fleck, C., Simpson, J. A., & Overall, N. C.
relationship quality. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(2), 179–
(2021). Infants’ attachment insecurity predicts attachment-relevant emo-
208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.11.003
tion regulation strategies in adulthood. Emotion, 21(2), 260–272. https://
Nozaki, Y., & Mikolajczak, M. (2020). Extrinsic emotion regulation. Emo-
doi.org/10.1037/emo0000721
tion, 20(1), 10–15. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000636
Gleason, M. E., Iida, M., Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2008). Receiving
Overall, N. C., Fletcher, G. J., & Simpson, J. A. (2010). Helping each other
support as a mixed blessing: Evidence for dual effects of support on psy-
grow: Romantic partner support, self-improvement, and relationship
chological outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
94(5), 824–838. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.824 quality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(11), 1496–1513.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future pros- https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210383045
pects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/10478 Özen, A., S€ umer, N., & Demir, M. (2011). Predicting friendship quality
40X.2014.940781 with rejection sensitivity and attachment security. Journal of Social and
Holland, A. S., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). Adult attachment security and Personal Relationships, 28(2), 163–181. https://doi.org/10.1177/02654
young adults’ dating relationships over time: Self-reported, observatio- 07510380607
nal, and physiological evidence. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), Pauw, L. S., Sauter, D. A., van Kleef, G. A., & Fischer, A. H. (2019). Stop
552–557. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018542 crying! The impact of situational demands on interpersonal emotion reg-
Hudson, N. W., & Fraley, R. C. (2017). Adult attachment and perceptions ulation. Cognition and Emotion, 33(8), 1587–1598. https://doi.org/10
of closeness. Personal Relationships, 24(1), 17–26. https://doi.org/10 .1080/02699931.2019.1585330
.1111/pere.12166 R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical
Inagaki, T. K., & Orehek, E. (2017). On the benefits of giving social sup- computing. https://www.R-project.org/
port: When, why, and how support providers gain by caring for others. Ramsey, M. A., & Gentzler, A. L. (2015). An upward spiral: Bidirectional
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(2), 109–113. https://doi associations between positive affect and positive aspects of close rela-
.org/10.1177/0963721416686212 tionships across the life span. Developmental Review, 36, 58–104.
Kalokerinos, E. K., Tamir, M., & Kuppens, P. (2017). Instrumental https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.003
motives in negative emotion regulation in daily life: Frequency, consis- Riediger, M. (2010). Experience sampling. In German Data Forum
tency, and predictors. Emotion, 17(4), 648–657. https://doi.org/10.1037/ (RatSWD) (Ed.), Building on progress: Expanding the research infra-
emo0000269 structure for the social, economic, and behavioral sciences (Vol. 1, pp.
Liu, D. Y., Strube, M. J., & Thompson, R. J. (2021). Interpersonal emotion 581–594). Budrich UniPress.
regulation: An experience sampling study. Affective Science, 2(3), 1–16. Saferstein, J. A., Neimeyer, G. J., & Hagans, C. L. (2005). Attachment as a
https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-021-00044-y predictor of friendship qualities in college youth. Social Behavior and
Locke, K. D. (2008). Attachment styles and interpersonal approach and Personality, 33(8), 767–776. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2005.33.8.767
avoidance goals in everyday couple interactions. Personal Relation- Saphire-Bernstein, S., & Taylor, S. E. (2013). Close relationships and happi-
ships, 15(3), 359–374. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2008 ness. In I. Boniwell & S. David (Eds.), Oxford handbook of happiness (pp.
.00203.x 821–833). https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199557257.013.0060
ATTACHMENT AND INTERPERSONAL REGULATION 13

Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why? Pleasure and util- Walton, G. M., Cohen, G. L., Cwir, D., & Spencer, S. J. (2012). Mere
ity in emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, belonging: The power of social connections. Journal of Personality
18(2), 101–105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01617.x and Social Psychology, 102(3), 513–532. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Tamir, M. (2016). Why do people regulate their emotions? A taxonomy of a0025731
motives in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Zaki, J., & Williams, W. C. (2013). Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emo-
Review, 20(3), 199–222. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315586325 tion, 13(5), 803–810. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033839
Tamminen, K. A., Page-Gould, E., Schellenberg, B., Palmateer, T., Thai, S.,
Sabiston, C. M., & Crocker, P. R. (2019). A daily diary study of interper-
sonal emotion regulation, the social environment, and team performance Received October 9, 2021
among university athletes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 45, 101566. Revision received July 5, 2022
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101566 Accepted July 27, 2022 n
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

You might also like