You are on page 1of 14

The Journal of Positive Psychology

Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice

ISSN: 1743-9760 (Print) 1743-9779 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpos20

Authenticity as a eudaimonic construct: The


relationships among authenticity, values, and
valence

Oscar Smallenbroek, John M. Zelenski & Deanna C. Whelan

To cite this article: Oscar Smallenbroek, John M. Zelenski & Deanna C. Whelan (2016):
Authenticity as a eudaimonic construct: The relationships among authenticity, values, and
valence, The Journal of Positive Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1187198

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1187198

Published online: 18 Jul 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 27

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rpos20

Download by: [Stephen F Austin State University] Date: 26 July 2016, At: 12:15
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1187198

Authenticity as a eudaimonic construct: The relationships among authenticity,


values, and valence
Oscar Smallenbroek1, John M. Zelenski  and Deanna C. Whelan
Department of Psychology, Carleton University, Ottawa, Canada

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


The construct of authenticity is thought of as an expression of individuality and eudaimonic well- Received 20 July 2015
being. Yet, previous research has related state authenticity more to positive affect and pleasant Accepted 31 March 2016
behavior. We examine the extent to which feeling authentic is a reflection of personally held
KEYWORDS
standards of worth (values) and authenticity’s relationship with affective states. We also examine
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

State authenticity; trait


whether feelings of authenticity are facilitated by dispositional authenticity. Study 1 had participants authenticity; values;
debate benevolent behavior (N  =  199). In study two (N  =  124) and three (N  =  146), participants eudaimonia; affect
described memories where they acted in concordance or against their values, in both pleasant and
unpleasant contexts. We found a relationship between acting in accordance with one’s values and
experiencing authenticity, thus demonstrating that authenticity is a form of eudaimonic well-being,
which is closely related to, but distinct from, affective states. We found less consistent associations
between dispositional authenticity and momentary authentic feelings.

The idea of authenticity has a long history in western Eudaimonic and hedonic well-being
thought (Harter, 2002) and is viewed as a healthy attribute
Eudaimonia refers to an ethical philosophy in which the,
in contemporary psychology. For example, self-determina-
‘… principle focus is on activity reflecting virtue, excel-
tion theory (SDT) (Kernis & Goldman, 2005) and notions of
lence, the best within us, and the full development of our
self-actualization (Maslow, 1971) and the fully functioning
potentials’ (Huta & Waterman, 2014, p. 1427). Eudaimonia is
person (Rogers, 1961) all include authentic functioning
often juxtaposed with a hedonic philosophy that stresses
as a central idea. Authenticity is a complex concept, but
presence of positive emotions and the absence of neg-
generally understood as acting in accordance with core
ative emotions as relevant criteria for evaluating one’s
aspects of the self. It is also seen as a hallmark of healthy
life. Positive psychologists draw on these philosophical
psychological functioning that contributes to well-being
traditions to conceptualize well-being in different ways
more broadly. Accordingly, research on the relationships
(Ryan & Deci, 2001). Psychologists who take a hedonic
among authenticity and various well-being indicators is
perspective focus on behavior that maximizes positive
extensive (see Schlegel & Hicks, 2011). However, the condi-
affect. They are therefore primarily concerned with the
tions that promote the momentary experience of authen-
outcome of feeling good, and assess people’s strategies
ticity remain understudied (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi,
with regard to efficacy in producing this outcome. The
2000). We present research that takes a step forward in
eudaimonic perspective involves a broader view that dis-
clarifying the experience of authenticity as a eudaimonic
tinguishes between healthy and unhealthy patterns of
form of well-being. We explore the idea that authenticity
psychological functioning. The goal is to foster virtuous
is experienced when behavior reflects internal standards
personal growth and development, which, in turn, creates
(values), taking care to distinguish this from mere positive
well-being. This introduces an objective element where
affect or pleasant behavior. We also distinguish between
certain behaviors and functioning are considered con-
authenticity as a momentary experience and authentic-
ducive to personal development; these are distinguished
ity as a more chronic personal characteristic, investigat-
from those intended to maximize pleasant feelings. In con-
ing how these temporally distinct conceptualizations of
temporary psychology, eudaimonia also has a subjective
authenticity are related.

CONTACT  John M. Zelenski  john_zelenski@carleton.ca


1
Social and Political Science Department, European University Institute, Fiesole, Italy
© 2016 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2    O. Smallenbroek et al.

component, or experiences that accompany the pursuit of draw on the psychological construct of values. Because
virtue, growth, etc. Authenticity is theorized to be a central they, by definition, define personally valued behaviors,
aspect of eudaimonic well-being, describing functioning values permit a more objective way to link aspects of the
that is consistent with core virtues (Huta & Waterman, self with behavior and experiences of authenticity.
2014). Authenticity can be experienced when behaving in Schwartz and his colleagues have defined human
accordance with internal standards and perceptions of the values as trans-situational goals that are desirable, vary
real or true self (Harter, 2002). in importance, and are used as guiding principles in life
(Schwartz et al., 2001). Schwartz and Bardi (2001) identified
ten value types which form a circumplex structure and
State authenticity and value fulfillment
which each represent different goals. This value structure
The contents of authentic experiences are difficult to is found across cultures and believed to include all basic
define because authentic states are the result of self-en- human values. The rank order or priority of each value is a
dorsed behavior that reflects an individual’s self view, reflection of one’s needs, culture, internalized social insti-
and are therefore idiosyncratic. Consequently, definitions tutions, and motivations. Schwartz and Bardi (2001) have
of authenticity often include expressing the ‘true self’ found that values relating to interpersonal relationships,
(Goldman & Kernis, 2002; Harter, 2002; Lenton, Bruder, competence, relatedness, and autonomy are endorsed
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

Slabu, & Sedikides, 2013; Schlegel & Hicks, 2011). In prac- across countries – there is more consensuses on the pri-
tice, defining the true self is usually left to the subject; ority of these values than disagreement. However, other
the reasoning is that authenticity is a state of self-expres- values like conformity, tradition, hedonism, and power
sion and thus will be recognized by individuals as such. show more individual and cultural differences. Crucial
However, by using subjects’ own implicit definitions of the for our research is that human values are standards by
true self, psychologists are unable to independently assess which individuals evaluate behavior and that they trigger
which behaviors are expressions of the true self. In other affective responses. Determining the degree of congru-
words, we have no independent means of assessing the ence between an individual’s behavior and values allows
authenticity of observable behavior. To further complicate us to assess whether self-rated authenticity is related to
matters, the assumption that a single true self exists and personally held standards of evaluation. Theoretically, val-
relates to a specific set of behaviors is suspect (Schlegel ue-congruent behavior is eudaimonic, and it is expected to
& Hicks, 2011). Thus, measuring these things objectively produce the experience of authenticity. This proposition,
presents a significant challenge. For current assessments, it however, remains untested in the psychological literature.
is more important that individuals perceive their behavior
as autonomous and personally endorsed, and consciously
Empirical research on the predictors of subjective
integrate external and internal influences (Harter, 2002;
authenticity
Kernis & Goldman, 2005; Wood, Linley, Maltby, Baliousis,
& Joseph, 2008). Although the eudaimonic and hedonic conceptions of
Although some degree of subjectivity is endemic to the well-being emphasize different standards and processes,
experience of authenticity, it also seems desirable to test they are not mutually exclusive. In fact Waterman (2008)
the links among self-concept, behavior, and authenticity noted that eudaimonic well-being is often accompanied
more directly. Such an approach will help verify that feel- by positive affect. Establishing a measure as eudaimonic
ings of authenticity are indeed an aspect of eudaimonic is therefore difficult, empirically, as hedonic and eudai-
living, and that authenticity is indeed a reflection of valued monic well-being tend to co-occur. Research on authen-
aspects of the self. To do this, we need to assess the cri- ticity has so far been unable to distinguish authenticity as
teria people use to decide that they are behaving as their eudaimonic, and only established its relation to positive
best selves. Previous research has asked participants to affect and pleasant behavior. For example, Lenton, Slabu,
identify behaviors that represent their true self and found Sedikides, and Power (2013) induced feelings of state
that these behaviors correlate positively with subjective authenticity using positive affect inductions (i.e. with
well-being, self-esteem, and meaning in life (Harter, 2002; non-self-relevant video clips). If merely happy people feel
Heppner et al., 2008; Reich, Kessel, & Bernieri, 2012). This state authenticity, it may be a hedonic rather than a eudai-
establishes an association between incidences of ‚true self’ monic manifestation of well-being.
behavior and well-being. This research is important and Sheldon, Ryan, Rawsthorne, and Ilardi (1997) consid-
establishes criterion validity, but leaves open the funda- ered personality trait consistency as an operationalization
mental question: whether or not self-reported authenticity of self-consistency and theorized that this consistency
is in fact a reflection of living in accordance with personally could also indicate authenticity. They had students rate
held beliefs of worth and fulfillment. To bridge this gap, we their big five traits and subjective authenticity ‘in general’,
The Journal of Positive Psychology   3

as well as separately for several roles (e.g. as a student or understood as the congruence between behavior, internal
employee). Personality trait consistency was determined processes, and representations of the self. Indeed, peo-
by correlating each role-trait rating with the correspond- ple commonly associate their true selves with internal
ing general-trait rating within participants; felt authentic- features (thoughts and feelings) more strongly than out-
ity was examined within roles, and as an average across ward expressions or behaviors (Schlegel & Hicks, 2011).
them. Results indicated that roles in which participants Trait authenticity should therefore reflect stable patterns
expressed conscientiousness, openness, agreeableness, of cognition that allow state authenticity to emerge.
and extraversion, regardless of general dispositions, felt In SDT, trait authenticity is conceptualized as the ability
(were rated as) more authentic. Employing experience to express authenticity when the situation permits (Kernis
sampling and more momentary assessments, Fleeson and & Goldman, 2006; Wood et al., 2008). SDT assumes that peo-
Wilt (2010) also found that conscientious, open, agreeable, ple have a natural tendency towards becoming integrated
and extraverted behaviors were perceived as authentic, parts of a community and towards healthy psychological
regardless of dispositions in these domains. In other words, functioning. This is motivated through three psychologi-
some behaviors seem to promote feelings of authenticity, cal needs: autonomy, relatedness, and competence. These
but it is not necessary to behave consistently with one’s needs are conceptually related to several human values
trait dispositions to experience authenticity. This is likely that are consistently prioritized (Kasser, 2002). SDT posits
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

because the big five domains are not strongly or widely that autonomous self-regulation will lead an individual to
valued in and of themselves. For example, lazy and intro- decisions that fulfill their needs, which, in turn, promote
verted people can better accomplish most valued goals psychological health and happiness (Ryan & Deci, 2011).
by behaving in ways that are more conscientious and Using this framework, Kernis and Goldman (2006) created
assertive than is their habit. Such findings are consistent a trait measure of authenticity (AI3) which has four com-
with theoretical notions of authenticity (if we assume that ponents: awareness of one’s motivations, feelings, desires,
one’s dispositions are not highly valued), yet further high- and self-relevant cognitions, unbiased processing, objectiv-
light a gap in the literature. Subjective authenticity has ity in assessing self-aspects, emotions, experiences, and
not been linked to behavior that comes from personally knowledge, behaving in accordance to one’s values, and an
valued aspects of the self. open and honest relational orientation. Trait authentic indi-
viduals are conscious of their internal states and outward
behavior in a non-defensive fashion. This enables them
Distinguishing ‘trait authenticity’ and its potential
to access self-aspects, such as values, and to assess their
role as a moderator
behavior according to these standards. Furthermore, trait
To this point, we have considered authenticity primarily as authenticity enables individuals to relate their affective
a short-term experience, a state that accompanies self-rel- experiences to their self-conceptions. We build on previ-
evant behaviors. Drawing on the commonly understood ous work by associating the trait authenticity question-
state-trait distinction, it is also possible to consider ‘trait naire with a more novel assessment of state authenticity.
authenticity’ as a more long-term personal characteris-
tic. To be clear, our use of the term ‘trait’ in this context
The present research
is not intended to imply the heritability, extreme stabil-
ity, or deterministic features sometimes associated with Past research has linked measures of authenticity with
temperamental dispositions (cf. McCrae & Costa, 1994). well-being and idiosyncratic notions of the true self.
Rather, trait authenticity refers to a relatively chronic fea- However, a gap remains in linking the experience of
ture of people that is more an individual difference than a authenticity to more verifiable or nomothetic aspects of
momentary state. One way to understand trait authenticity the self. Such links are needed to confirm authenticity as
would be as the frequent experience of state authentic- a eudaimonic (vs. purely hedonic) experience. To this end,
ity. That is, consistent with the density distribution model we conducted three studies that link values, mood, and
(Fleeson, 2001), trait authenticity is merely individual dif- state and trait authenticity. In the first study, participants
ferences in average experience over time. However, this were randomly assigned to debate for or against a widely
approach is agnostic about the processes that produce held value (benevolence) as a manipulation of value-con-
experiences of authenticity, and sidesteps more theoreti- gruent behavior. In the second and third studies, partici-
cally nuanced explanations. Frequent experiences of state pants were asked to recall a situation where they behaved
authenticity may describe trait authenticity, but are insuf- in accordance with a value (benevolence or achievement),
ficient as an explanation. either in a pleasant or an unpleasant atmosphere, and then
A deeper understanding of trait authenticity will draw rate their state authenticity. Participants were also given
on internal structures and processes. State authenticity is questionnaires to measure their value priorities and trait
4    O. Smallenbroek et al.

authenticity. This allowed us to compare the effects of online portal for a study titled ‘A Values Debate’. They
affect and behavior on state authenticity while taking into received course extra credit as compensation. Of 267 who
account value priorities and trait authenticity. Our specific gave informed consent (online), 234 completed most of
hypotheses were: the study. Primary analyses were further restricted to 199
Hypothesis 1: Because authenticity is a form of eudai- (164 female) participants who correctly omitted answers
monic well-being and theoretically related to value ful- to at least 2 of 3 requests to ‘leave this item blank’, and
fillment, value congruent behavior will promote feelings who appeared to generally follow instructions in their
of state authenticity. Furthermore, we expect authen- open-ended debate responses. Median age was 20, rang-
tic states to arise even in unpleasant situations if they
are value congruent. This follows from the assumption
ing from 17 to 55. Approximately 65% of participants
that authenticity reflects judgments of valued out- identified as Caucasian, with 5–9% selecting one of other
comes (eudaimonic) rather than merely affective states ethnicity descriptors (e.g. African, South Asian, East Asian,
(hedonic). This would establish state authenticity as dis- and Arabic).
tinct from hedonism.
Hypothesis 2: We also expect an interaction between Materials
affect and value congruent behavior because hedonic Values were assessed with the Portrait Value Questionnaire
happiness often results from eudaimonic functioning,
(PVQ) developed by Schwartz and Bardi (2001), a brief
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

and previous studies show that positive affect induces


state authenticity (Lenton, Slabu et al., 2013). Value con- version of the earlier Schwartz Value Survey. The PVQ
gruence may be more essential to experiencing authen- asks participants to rate how similar they are to 40 brief
ticity in unpleasant circumstances, whereas feeling good descriptions of hypothetical people using a 6-point scale
may signal authenticity on its own. (This hypothesis is that ranges from ‘very much like me’ to ‘not like me at all’.
relevant to studies 2 and 3 only.)
Although otherwise identical, items are phrased with
Hypothesis 3: We hypothesize that people who score gendered pronouns that match participants’ genders,
high on trait authenticity will generally experience more
for example ‘It’s very important to him to help the peo-
state authenticity.
ple around him. He wants to care for their well-being,’ for
Hypothesis 4: Qualifying hypothesis 3, we also hypothe-
male participants. The PVQ assesses 10 values, and scale
size an interaction between trait authenticity and exper-
imental conditions. People high in trait authenticity will scores are calculated as the mean of standardized item
be more sensitive to the link between their values and scores – the relative importance of an item in relation to
behavior. That is, they will be more likely to report high all other values within participant. In this study we were
state authenticity when behavior is congruent with val- primarily interested in the 4-item benevolence scale, yet
ues, and less authenticity for value-incongruent behav-
the internal consistency was low (α  =  0.41). Combining
ior. This is because trait authenticity is the capability of
people to act authentically, and their awareness of how benevolence items with six similar universalism items
their behavior compares to their true selves. to create a broader self-transcendence score improved
Hypothesis 5: The extent to which a value is endorsed is alpha somewhat (0.64), and yielded similar results with
hypothesized to predict state authenticity with an inter- regard to hypotheses. Given this pattern, and the fact that
active pattern. If a value is important to people, it follows low alphas do not imply poor validity with short scales
that their true selves will be closely tied to it, and they (Schmitt, 1996), we used the more theoretically focused
will also be more sensitive to a match or mismatch with benevolence scale in the analyses reported here.
relevant behaviors.
Trait authenticity was assessed with the Authenticity
Our hypotheses contrast with a perspective that views Inventory (AI3, Kernis & Goldman, 2006) developed by
state authenticity as merely an outcome of hedonic pro- drawing on SDT. It assesses people’s perceptions of their
cesses, a position highly plausible given extant research on capability to function authentically in contexts that allow
this new construct (e.g. Fleeson & Wilt, 2010; Lenton, Slabu it. There are 45 items rated on a scale from ‘1- strongly dis-
et al., 2013). If the experience of authenticity is merely the agree’ to ‘5- strongly agree’. Participants are encouraged
act of claiming a state high in positive affect and posi- to respond truthfully and told that there are no right or
tive behavior as one’s true self, then values should not be wrong answers. Although the measure can be scored with
related to authenticity, only positive affect would be. four subscales, we used a combined scoring (mean of all
items) to reflect an overall trait authenticity score across
Study 1 domains (α = 0.88).

Method State authenticity and effort.  Because no standard state


Participants authenticity scale existed, we used 10-items that were
Canadian students enrolled in first and second year psy- developed in our lab for another project, and which drew
chology courses were recruited via our department’s on items previously used by Fleeson and Wilt (2010). The
The Journal of Positive Psychology   5

items are face valid and ask how authentic participants with one final textbox and instructions to summarize and/
feel ‘in the moment’ using questions like ‘I felt like I was or add new, concluding arguments. Thus, participants
really being me during the last 20 minutes.’ Each item is were given a total of eight textboxes to complete.
rated on a scale from 1, ‘Strongly Disagree,’ to 7, ‘Strongly
Agree,’ and four are reverse coded. Internal consistency for Debate questionnaire.  We included eight ad hoc items
the authenticity scale was good (α = 0.93). Due in part to to assess participants’ perceptions of the debate task. They
beginnings with another project, our questionnaire has were rated on a 7-point scale of agreement and asked
the authenticity items interspersed with 5 items adapted about debate difficulty, effort, and whether participants’
from Gallagher, Fleeson, and Hoyle (2011) that assess randomly assigned positions matched their true beliefs.
effort, for example ‘It took a lot of effort to behave the We used three items in the latter category (e.g. ‘I agreed
way I did,’ (α = 0.81). Although the two scales correlated with all the arguments I made in the debate’) to create a
strongly and negatively here (r = −0.72), we maintained manipulation check scale such that high scores indicated
separate scoring due to their conceptual independence agreement with participants’ assigned position (α = 0.93).
(e.g. they may correlate positively in a context that
involves resisting temptation). Procedure
Participants followed a link to the study, ‘A Values Debate,’
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

Affect questionnaire.  Participants were asked to rate administered online via Qualtrics. After reading a brief
how much they felt 28 mood adjectives ‘while you were description of the study and participant rights, they pro-
arguing your assigned side of the debate,’ using a 1, ‘very vided informed consent, and then answered a few demo-
slightly or not at all,’ to 7, ‘extremely or a lot,’ scale. Affect graphic questions (age, gender, ethnicity). The gender
items were selected to assess the high arousal pleasant response was used to select a male or female version
and unpleasant affects (cf. the PANAS, Watson et al., 1988) of the PVQ, which was followed by the trait authenticity
as well as to represent other parts of the affect circumplex inventory. Next, participants received information about
(see Larsen & Diener, 1992) and some intuitively relevant the simulated values debate. All participants were told the
terms (e.g. embarrassed, self-conscious). As an ad hoc debate resolution, and that they were randomly assigned
assessment tool, various scorings are possible. For to argue either for or against this position. (This was true.)
consistency with the more established measure, we They were further informed that the simulated debate
report two 5-items scales of (aroused) positive affect would occur across seven rounds where they would type
(α  =  0.84) and negative affect (α  =  0.83). Alternative responses to arguments presented serially, with an oppor-
scorings revealed conceptually similar results. tunity to write a concluding statement to sum up their
position. Immediately following submission of their con-
Debate.  To create a situation where participants could cluding statement, participants were asked to complete
behave contrary to their values (while keeping the the state authenticity and effort measure, the affect ques-
potential for distress low), we developed a simulated tionnaire, and a few ad hoc items to assess perceptions
debate task. The debate focused on the value of of the debate which included the manipulation check. To
benevolence, and a web browser presented participants guard against any potential unpleasantness that might
with arguments for or against the resolution that, ‘It is have been caused by the debate task, participants were
good and right to help and support the people around given a brief positive mood induction (i.e. guided imagery
you.’ Participants were randomly assigned to the pro or about getting a dream job), and reported their affect once
con side of the debate, and saw only arguments opposing more before receiving debriefing information. The median
their assigned position. Because benevolence is generally time to complete the study was 38 min.
valued, we assumed that arguing against it would
produce counter-value behavior. The debate began with
Results
the resolution (above) and proceeded through six more
rounds. In each round a web page displayed an opposing Table 1 reports means and t-test comparisons of key out-
argument, a blank textbox for the participant’s refutation, comes across conditions. The experimental manipulation
and submission button. For example, participants (arguing for or against benevolence) appears to have
arguing for benevolence saw, ‘You should not help and functioned as we intended with participants indicating
support the people around you because it is arrogant much more agreement when randomly assigned to the
to assume that you can ever know what another person pro condition. Our primary hypothesis (H1) was that argu-
really needs.’ Participants were asked to provide a couple ing for benevolence would produce greater feelings of
sentences for each response, and were given unlimited momentary authenticity. This hypothesis received strong
time to complete their responses. The debate concluded support. In addition, participants arguing for benevolence
6    O. Smallenbroek et al.

Table 1. Agreement, authenticity, effort, and mood by condition (Study 1).


DV Argue for benevolence (n = 105) M (SD) Argue against benevolence (n = 94) M (SD) t d
Agreement check 5.93 (1.13) 2.57 (1.67) 16.80*** 2.36
Authenticity 5.34 (1.05) 3.09 (1.25) 13.76*** 1.95
Effort 2.84 (1.29) 4.25 (1.26) 7.75*** 1.11
Positive affect 4.61 (1.24) 3.96 (1.43) 3.44*** 0.49
Negative affect 2.31 (1.22) 3.13 (1.51) 4.26*** 0.60

***p < 0.001.

also reported significantly better moods and less effort. All trait authenticity score was positively associated with state
comparisons produced substantial differences, but state authenticity (b = 0.87, SE = 0.25, p = 0.001), whereas when
authenticity was the largest (baring the manipulation arguing against, trait authenticity scores were weakly asso-
check). ciated with state authenticity (b = 0.25, SE = 0.29, p = 0.38).
To further establish that the value manipulation influ- Although the full pattern is not statistically significant, trait
enced authenticity beyond its associations with mood, authenticity appears to predict state authenticity more
we regressed state authenticity on a condition dummy when behavior is indeed authentic with regard to the value
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

variable, positive affect, and negative affect (R2  =  0.61). of benevolence.


Each variable independently predicted authenticity (con-
dition b = 1.86, SE = 0.16, p < 0.01, positive affect b = 0.34,
Discussion
SE = 0.06, p < 0.01, negative affect b = −0.22, SE = 0.05,
p < 0.01). Behaving in a value congruent way seemed to This study supports our main hypothesis that state authen-
cause feelings of authenticity, beyond the effects of mood. ticity is experienced when behaving according to one’s
We also predicted that the effect of arguing for or values. Second, the link between affect and authenticity
against benevolence might further depend on individ- is in line with previous findings. State authenticity co-­
ual differences in the importance of that value (H5). To occurred with high positive affect and low negative affect.
test this we centered benevolence scores, created a cross This study also supports the conceptual link between trait
product (interaction) term, and entered these and a con- and state authenticity. Trait authenticity appears to enable
dition dummy variable in a linear regression predicting participants to feel authentic, especially when behaving
state authenticity (final R2 = 0.51). Condition had a signifi- in accordance with their values.
cant effect (b = 2.25, SE = 0.16, p < 0.01; cf. the t-test), but
benevolence did not (b = 0.10, SE = 0.13, p = 0.43) until
Study 2: Benevolence recall study
we added the interaction term, which was significant
(ΔR2 = 0.023, b = 0.76, SE = 0.25, p = 0.003). Simple slopes Study 2 extends our findings with regard to state
analysis revealed a pattern consistent with predictions. authenticity’s relationship with value priorities and trait
When arguing for benevolence, the value score was posi- authenticity by asking participants to recall instances of
tively associated with state authenticity (b = 0.43, SE = 0.16, value-­congruent or value-incongruent behavior (manip-
p  =  0.009), whereas when arguing against, high benev- ulated between subjects). It also addresses the covaria-
olence scores were (marginally significantly) negatively tion between state authenticity and positive affect by
associated with state authenticity (b = −0.33, SE = 0.19, dissociating pleasantness and value-congruence. That is,
p = 0.08). Thus, it appears that value-congruent behavior ­participants recalled two experiences, one pleasant and
predicts state authenticity when examined from both an one unpleasant, and reported on state authenticity and
individual differences perspective, as well as a more uni- mood for each. This tests our argument that state authen-
versal perspective (i.e. the condition difference). ticity is separate from positive affect, and therefore hedon-
We also predicted that people who report disposition- ism, by demonstrating that participants can feel state
ally high authenticity are likely to feel more authentic (H3) authentic while experiencing negative affect.
and are sensitive to whether or not their behavior is value
congruent (H4). We tested this with regression, includ-
Method
ing a condition x trait authenticity interaction term (final
R2 = 0.52). Both condition (b = 2.32, SE = 0.16, p = 0.001) Participants
and trait authenticity (b = 0.61, SE = 0.19, p < 0.001) had All participants were recruited online through the uni-
significant effects, but the interaction term was only mar- versity’s research website and rewarded with 0.5% extra
ginally significant (ΔR2 = 0.007, b = 0.62, SE = 0.38, p = 0.10). credit. The experiment required participants to describe
Nonetheless, simple slopes analyses were consistent with a situation from memory; some described inappropriate
predictions such that when arguing for benevolence, the situations or left the textbox blank. Of the 208 participants,
The Journal of Positive Psychology   7

Table 2. Mean (standard deviation) affect and authenticity by experimental condition (Study 2).
Experimental condition State authenticity Positive affect Negative affect
Congruent (n = 70) Pleasant atmosphere 5.52 (0.94) 5.15 (1.20) 2.36 (1.19)
Unpleasant atmosphere 5.08 (1.20) 3.83 (1.27) 3.82 (1.30)
Incongruent (n = 54) Pleasant atmosphere 4.34 (1.00) 3.22 (1.44) 3.35 (1.47)
Unpleasant atmosphere 4.33 (1.01) 2.95 (1.31) 4.00 (1.36)

53 did not fill in a state authenticity score, 12 participants context questions followed these questionnaires. Qualtrics
were excluded for otherwise not following directions, and randomly assigned participants to the congruent or incon-
19 participants spent less than 60 s on a questionnaire. gruent condition. The state authenticity questionnaire
This left 124 participants for analysis. The majority of par- and affect scales followed each context question. One
ticipants were female (99) and identified as Caucasian item was added to this questionnaire, ‘leave blank,’ to
(72.6%), between 0.8 and 7.3% of the sample identified safeguard against random responding. The study closed
with other ethnicities (e.g. African, South Asian, East Asian, with a mood induction to counter any adverse effects of
and Arabic). Age ranged between 18 and 48, the median recalling unpleasant situations.
age was 19.
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

Results
Materials
This study used the PVQ (benevolence  =  0.62), AI3 We ran three repeated measure ANOVAs using the atmos-
(α = 0.76), affect (positive α = 0.71–0.82, negative α = 0.69– phere condition as within-subjects variable and congru-
0.74) and state authenticity questionnaires (α = 0.75–0.88) ence condition as between subjects variable. The outcome
used in study 1. Unlike study 1, we asked participants to variables for the ANOVAs were positive affect, negative
recall situations from their past and self-report on their affect, and state authenticity. Regression analysis was
experience. employed to determine if trait authenticity and value
Four recall questions were included in this study. (benevolence) endorsement predicted state authenticity.
Participants were randomly assigned (between subjects) The analysis of self-reported affect showed that the
to recall contexts where their behavior fulfilled their values congruence and atmosphere manipulations had an effect
(value congruent) or contexts where their behavior went on positive affect (see Table 2). The pleasant atmosphere
against their values (value incongruent). All participants condition was successful in its manipulation: participants
were asked to recall two instances, one that occurred in reported more positive affect in the pleasant atmosphere
a pleasant atmosphere, and another in an unpleasant (M = 4.31, SD = 1.36) than in the unpleasant atmosphere
atmosphere. (Valence is manipulated within-subject.) The condition (M = 3.44, SD = 1.36), F(1, 122) = 36.80, p < 0.001,
value used was benevolence, and the order of the pleasant η2 = 0.23. Furthermore, consistent with hints from previ-
versus unpleasant atmosphere was counterbalanced. The ous research (e.g. Lenton, Slabu et al., 2013), the value-­
two value congruent contexts were: ‘describe a time where congruent behavior conditions were higher in positive
you were able to care for a friend or family member in a affect (M = 4.49, SD = 1.04) than the incongruent conditions
pleasant atmosphere,’ and ‘describe a time where you were (M = 3.09, SD = 1.11), F(1, 122) = 54.13, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.31.
able to care for a friend or family member in an unpleas- A similar pattern was found for self-reported negative
ant atmosphere.’ The two incongruent questions were: affect. Participants reported feeling more negative affect
‘describe a time where you think you should have helped in the unpleasant atmosphere conditions (M  =  3.88,
a close other in a pleasant atmosphere, but did not,’ and SD  =  1.32) than the pleasant atmosphere conditions
‘describe a time where you think you should have helped (M = 2.78, SD = 1.40), F(1, 122) = 51.76, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.30.
a close other in an unpleasant atmosphere, but did not.’ Participants reported feeling more negative affect in the
Participants were asked to take three minutes to recall a incongruent conditions (M = 3.65, SD = 1.41) than the con-
relevant memory, and then to type a detailed description. gruent conditions (M = 3.09, SD = 1.24), F(1, 122) = 8.91,
An answer was coded as satisfactory when the participant p = 0.003, η2 = 0.07. Overall, the atmosphere manipula-
demonstrated that the person in need was close to them tions were effective in inducing the positive or negative
and that the atmosphere was as requested. affect, while the congruence manipulation also induced
the expected positive and negative affect.
Procedure To address our key hypothesis (H1), we tested the
Participants started with demographic questions (age, effects of atmosphere and behavior condition on the
sex, gender, nationality), followed by the PVQ (Schwartz state authenticity participants reported. The main effect
& Bardi, 2001) and the AI3 (Kernis & Goldman, 2006). The for value congruent versus incongruent behavior on state
8    O. Smallenbroek et al.

authenticity was significant, F(1, 122) = 38.03, p  <  0.001, increased feelings of state authenticity in the congruent
η2  =  0.24. As expected, participants in the congruent behavior condition (b = 0.48, p < 0.001), but had no signifi-
behavior condition had a higher state authenticity score cant effect in the incongruent behavior condition (b = −0.07,
(M = 5.30, SD = 0.87) than those in the incongruent behav- p = 0.63). In the pleasant atmosphere conditions, the trait
ior condition (M = 4.34, SD = 0.87). This finding supports authenticity score did not predict state authenticity signifi-
our central hypothesis that value congruent behavior cantly. The interaction between the behavior condition and
promotes feelings of state authenticity. There was also a trait authenticity was only marginally significant. In sum, the
significant, but much smaller, main effect for atmosphere hypothesis that trait authenticity enables state authenticity
condition, F(1, 122) = 4.22, p = 0.04, η2 = 0.03. Participants is supported (H3), but only in unpleasant circumstances. The
in the pleasant atmosphere (M = 5.00, SD = 1.13) felt more expected interaction effect between trait authenticity and
state authentic than those in the unpleasant atmosphere condition was found (H4), and indicates a significant link
(M = 4.76, SD = 1.18). The interaction between behavior only in the congruent behavior condition.
and atmosphere condition was also significant, F(1, 122) Finally, we hypothesized that, because authenticity is
= 3.78, p  =  0.05, η2 = 0.03. Participants who behaved in the fulfillment of one’s core self, individual differences in
benevolent ways felt more state authentic, but a pleasant value priorities would predict levels of state authenticity
atmosphere promoted state authenticity even further (H2). (H5). The regression table below suggests that values do
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

Nonetheless, the pattern still supports our hypothesis that play a role in state authenticity. In the pleasant atmosphere
participants would feel state authentic even in unpleasant, conditions, the benevolence scores and behavior condi-
but value congruent, situations. tion predict state authenticity. Higher benevolence scores
Regression analysis showed partial support for the and value congruent behavior made participants feel more
hypothesized relationship between trait authenticity and authentic. However, the interaction term with the behavior
state authenticity: that higher trait authenticity enables par- condition was not significant.
ticipants to feel state authentic (H3). Each atmosphere con- In the unpleasant atmospheres, only the behavior con-
dition was regressed separately because the atmosphere dition was a significant predictor; the benevolence scores
manipulation was within-subjects. In the unpleasant atmos- were not. However, the interaction was significant (see
phere condition, trait authenticity positively predicted Table 4). Simple slopes analysis showed different signed,
state authenticity; the interaction term with the behavior but not individually significant, slopes. The slopes are in
condition was also significant (see Table 3). Analyzing the the directions predicted; the more participants endorsed
interaction with simple slopes showed that trait authenticity benevolence the more state authentic they felt in the

Table 3.  Regression of behavior condition, trait authenticity and their interaction on state authenticity in pleasant and unpleasant
atmospheres (Study 2).
Pleasant atmosphere Unpleasant atmosphere
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Predictors B SE B SE B SE B SE
Intercept 5.22 1.15 4.66 1.56 2.10 1.31 −1.18 1.72
Behaviour condition (ref. value congruent) −1.18* 0.18 0.07 2.32 −0.79*** 0.20 6.41* 2.56
Trait authenticity 0.09 0.35 0.26 0.47 0.90* 0.39 1.89*** 0.52
Trait authenticity × behaviour −0.37† 0.70 −2.17*** 0.77
R2 0.27 0.27 0.14 0.19
N 124 124

p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.


Table 4. Regression of behavior condition, benevolence and their interaction on state authenticity in pleasant and unpleasant atmos-
pheres (Study 2).
Pleasant atmosphere Unpleasant atmosphere
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Predictors B SE B SE B SE B SE
Intercept 5.46 0.13 5.37 0.14 5.05 0.16 4.92 0.17
Behaviour condition (ref. value congruent) −1.17* 0.18 −0.96 0.22 −0.75*** 0.20 −0.44 0.25
Benevolence 0.11* 0.14 0.28* 0.17 0.07 0.16 0.33 0.20
Benevolence × behaviour −0.44 0.28 −0.66* 0.32
R2 0.27 0.29 0.10 0.13
N 124 124

*p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.
The Journal of Positive Psychology   9

Table 5. Mean (standard deviation) affect and authenticity by experimental condition (Study 3).
Experimental condition State authenticity Positive affect Negative affect
Congruent (n = 75) Pleasant atmosphere 5.67 (0.76) 5.73 (0.87) 2.15 (0.83)
Unpleasant atmosphere 4.47 (1.24) 4.01 (1.31) 4.19 (1.46)
Incongruent (n = 71) Pleasant atmosphere 4.64 (0.94) 4.15 (1.62) 3.88 (1.48)
Unpleasant atmosphere 4.21 (1.02) 3.15 (1.46) 4.67 (1.34)

Table 6.  Regression of behavior condition, trait authenticity and their interaction on state authenticity in pleasant and unpleasant
atmospheres (Study 3).
Pleasant atmosphere Unpleasant atmosphere
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Predictors B SE B SE B SE B SE
Intercept 3.83 0.67 2.67 0.99 3.58 0.92 2.57 1.35
Behaviour condition (ref. value congruent) −0.99*** 0.14 1.12 1.32 0.26 0.27 0.56 0.39
Trait authenticity 0.53** 0.19 0.88** 0.29 −0.24 0.19 1.60 1.82
Trait authenticity × behaviour condition −0.63 0.39 −0.54 0.53
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

R2 0.30 0.30 0.19 0.23


N 146 146

**p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Table 7. Regression of behavior condition, achievement value and their interaction on state authenticity in pleasant and unpleasant
atmospheres (Study 3).
Pleasant atmosphere Unpleasant atmosphere
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Predictors B SE B SE B SE B SE
Intercept 5.66 0.10 5.67 0.11 4.51 0.14 4.56 0.14
Behaviour condition (ref. value congruent) −1.02*** 0.14 −1.04*** 0.15 −0.27 0.19 −0.34† 0.20
Achievement 0.03 0.11 −0.04 0.16 −0.16 0.15 −0.34 0.21
Achievement × behaviour condition 0.07 0.22 0.35 0.29
R2 0.26 0.25 0.01 0.01
N 146 146

p <  0.10, ***p < 0.001.


congruent condition (b  =  0.21, p  =  0.11) and less state Study 3: Achievement recall study
authentic in the incongruent condition (b = −0.21, p = 19).
Study 3 extends our tests of these ideas to the value of
achievement. The methods and hypotheses were the same
Discussion as Study 2, excepting the value change.
This study sought to dissociate pleasant contexts from
value-congruent behaviors. Although state authenticity
Method
and pleasantness are expected to co-vary across con-
texts, it should be possible to experience relatively high Participants
levels of authenticity even in unpleasant contexts, if All 237 participants were recruited online through the
those situations also include behavior that is consistent university’s research website during the summer semester
with one’s values. The results of study 2 support this idea. and rewarded 0.5% extra credit. Fifty-six participants were
Moreover, we tested individual differences as moderators excluded for missing data. Thirty participants spent less
of these links, hypothesizing again that the link between than 60 s on a questionnaire, and five participants failed
value-­congruent behavior and state authenticity would to follow instructions. Of the remaining 146 participants,
be stronger for people who endorse the value more the majority were female (106), the median age was 19
strongly and those who score high in trait authenticity. and ranged from 18 to 44. Most identified as Caucasian
Although there was some variation across contexts and (63.7%) while other ethnicities (e.g. African, South Asian,
not all ­predicted interactions were statistically significant, East Asian, Arabic) comprised between 6.8 and 7.5% of
the ­pattern of results was generally consistent with this the sample.
thinking.
10    O. Smallenbroek et al.

Materials and procedure congruent conditions (M = 3.17, SD = 0.95) were lower in


The materials and procedure were identical to the benevo- negative affect than the incongruent conditions (M = 4.27,
lence recall study except that the context questions ask for SD = 1.21), F(1, 144) = 37.99, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.21. The inter-
an ‘achievement’ context. The alphas for the achievement action term between atmosphere and congruence condi-
value (α = 0.80) and the AI3 (α = 0.87) were satisfactory. The tion was significant. The difference in negative affect was
Cronbach’s alphas for the affect scales ranged from 0.78 to greater between the congruent and incongruent pleasant
0.90. The only alpha outside this range was the negative atmosphere conditions (1.73) than the difference between
affect scale in the value congruent pleasant atmosphere congruent and incongruent unpleasant atmosphere con-
condition (α = 0.57). The Cronbach’s alphas for the state ditions (0.46), F(1, 144) = 28.11, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.16.
authenticity scale ranged from 0.71 to 0.86. An ANOVA with state authenticity as the dependent
The value congruent context instructions now read, variable showed that the congruence and atmosphere
‘Describe a time when you demonstrated your abilities in manipulations had the expected effects. Participants
a context that generally is pleasant’ and ‘Describe a time in the congruent behavior condition felt more authen-
when you demonstrated your abilities in a context that tic (M  =  5.07, SD  =  0.76) than those in the incongruent
generally is unpleasant.’ The value incongruent contexts behavior condition (M = 4.43, SD = 0.79), F(1, 144) = 25.06,
were ‘Describe a time you did not meet your ambitions in p < 0.001, η2 = 0.15 (H1). Participants in the pleasant con-
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

a context that generally is pleasant’ and ‘Describe a time ditions felt more authentic (M = 5.17, SD = 0.99) than those
you did not meet your ambitions in a context that gener- in the unpleasant conditions (M  =  4.35, SD  =  1.14), F(1,
ally is unpleasant.’ 144) = 58.27, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.29. There was also a signifi-
cant interaction effect, F(1, 144) = 13.05, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.08
(H2), the difference in state authenticity between congru-
Results
ent and incongruent conditions was larger in the pleasant
Given the similarity with study 2, our analytical approach atmosphere conditions (1.03) than the unpleasant atmos-
was the same, and this study conceptually replicated many phere conditions (0.26).
of the previous study’s results. The behavior and atmos- The regression analyses testing whether achievement
phere manipulations had the same effects on positive value (H5) or trait authenticity (H3) moderated state
affect, negative affect, and state authenticity as tested by authenticity were less supportive of our hypotheses;
ANOVAs (see means in Table 5). However, the trait authen- interactions were generally not significant (see Table 6 and
ticity and achievement value did not significantly moder- Table 7). Value endorsement did not predict state authen-
ate state authenticity felt in the recalled situations in the ticity in any condition. Trait authenticity and behavior con-
regression analyses. Moreover, attempts at interpreting the dition were both significant in the pleasant atmosphere.
non-significant interactions often did not reveal a cohesive Participants with higher trait authenticity experienced
or supportive pattern. more state authenticity, and those in the incongruent
Testing the congruence and atmosphere manipulations behavior condition felt less state authentic.
with an ANOVA showed they had an effect on positive
affect; the interaction was also significant. The pleasant
General discussion
atmosphere conditions reported more positive affect
(M  =  4.96, SD  =  1.52) than the unpleasant atmosphere In the three studies presented here, we test the idea that
(M = 3.60, SD = 1.44) conditions, F(1, 144) = 96.54, p < 0.001, state authenticity is a form of eudaimonic well-being, and
η2  =  0.40, and the congruent conditions were higher in results generally supported this view. When behaving in
positive affect (M  =  4.87, SD  =  0.81) than the incongru- accordance with one’s values, state authenticity was expe-
ent condition (M  =  3.65, SD  =  1.24), F(1, 144) = 50.22, rienced, thus validating theoretical links with eudaimo-
p < 0.001, η2 = 0.26. The difference in positive affect was nia in ways that previous research has not. Second, our
smaller between congruent and incongruent pleasant results suggest that state authenticity is distinct from, yet
atmosphere conditions (0.62) than the difference between closely related to, positive and negative affect. Third, we
congruent and incongruent unpleasant atmosphere con- also found evidence that trait authenticity, which can be
ditions (0.84), F(1, 144) = 6.91, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.05. interpreted as a form of self-insight, is associated with par-
Participants also reported significantly less nega- ticipants’ feelings of momentary authenticity.
tive affect in the pleasant atmosphere and congru- All three studies show that state authenticity is elicited
ent conditions. The pleasant atmosphere conditions when people behave in accordance with their values (H1).
reported less negative affect (M  =  2.99, SD  =  1.47) The comparisons across experimental conditions provide
than the unpleasant atmosphere conditions (M  =  4.42, consistent evidence for this hypothesis. Study 1 showed
SD = 1.42), F(1, 144) = 139.32, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.49, while the that arguing for benevolence increased state authenticity,
The Journal of Positive Psychology   11

and arguing against benevolence decreased state authen- which fulfil psychological needs (Goldman & Kernis, 2002;
ticity. Study 2 showed that participants, who were asked to Kernis & Goldman, 2006), however, some values reflect
recall a memory where they helped others, felt more state externally motivated behavior (Kasser, 2002). Acting
authentic. These results were conceptually replicated when benevolently fulfills the need to belong and – when inter-
participants recalled a situation where they accomplished nalized as a value – will induce state authenticity. In study
a goal in study 3. Interestingly, because these results rest 3 we asked participants to describe achievements. Some
on main effects, this part of the true self seems to be a participants described dealing with difficult customers at
shared feature across people. This is perfectly consistent their jobs. Although this behavior is an achievement, it
with the notion of values as constructs that are univer- may be motived by extrinsic rewards and therefore is not
sally positive (‘valued’), but it may contrast with intuitions self-determined behavior and does not fulfill psychological
that experiencing the true self is an idiosyncratic process needs. Second, the achievement value was ranked fourth
(Schlegel & Hicks, 2011). Most participants feel authentic or fifth in priority, on average in our samples, whereas
when expressing benevolence. State authentic behavior benevolence was ranked second in both samples. This indi-
does not need to be idiosyncratic, and finding it need not cates that participants do not identify as closely with the
rely on an individual differences approach (cf. Fleeson & achievement value as they do with benevolence. Although
Wilt, 2010). this might speak more to the weaker main effects in study
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

The relationship between affect and authenticity also 3, perhaps it also obscured interactions. Third, finding a
comes out clearly in these studies. The ANOVAs of stud- clear relationship between a trait and a single state can be
ies 2 and 3 show that participants who behaved value- difficult. Perhaps future research will find such an associa-
congruently reported more positive, and less negative, tion for achievement; aggregating across multiple relevant
affect. However affect also seems to influence levels of situations is likely to increase the chances of detecting the
state authenticity. The pleasant atmospheres promoted effect if it exists.
state authenticity, and unpleasant atmospheres reduced We found some evidence for an association between
state authenticity (H2), replicating Lenton, Slabu et al.’s trait authenticity and state authenticity. Because our exper-
(2013) findings. It seems clear that state authenticity has imental designs asked people to consider value-congruent
multiple causes; value-congruence, pleasant affect, and and value-incongruent behavior, the simple correlation (or
circumstances are all important. In daily life, authenticity regression coefficient) between state and trait authentic-
and positive affect often co-occur and interact. This may ity is less informative than the interaction between trait
explain some of the lay theories that emphasize mental authenticity and congruence condition. We reasoned that
states over behavior, or equate happiness with healthy liv- trait authenticity can boost state authenticity through
ing. Nonetheless, these two are separable as the significant self-insight, i.e. that people are performing as their true-
main effects of atmosphere and behavior condition show. selves (H3), and that this occurs with value-congruent
Thus, although affect induces authenticity, data support behavior more than value incongruent behavior (H4). Our
our hypothesis that authenticity is not only hedonism. ideas were partly supported by the data. In study 1 there
Value-congruent behavior induces state authenticity even was a main effect of trait authenticity and an interaction
in unpleasant situations. effect with the experimental condition (although this was
In our studies we also found some suggestive individual marginally significant). In study 2, trait authenticity inter-
differences, but the regression results were more ambig- acted with behavior condition this way to predict state
uous than the main effects. We found that participants authenticity in unpleasant atmosphere condition. In study
who endorsed benevolence as particularly important felt 3, trait authenticity predicted state authenticity in the
more authentic when arguing for benevolence (study pleasant atmosphere conditions but not the unpleasant
1) and when recalling benevolent behavior (in study 2, atmosphere conditions. Thus, we found limited evidence
but only significant in the unpleasant atmosphere). Said that trait authenticity increases state authenticity. A pos-
another way, feelings of state authenticity may not arise sible cause is a misspecification in our conceptual model.
for people who do not value benevolence strongly. Such Trait authenticity may promote state authenticity by pro-
findings are consistent with the idea that acting according viding accurate affective feedback to situations and behav-
to values promotes authenticity (H5). However results in ior instead of directly contributing to state authenticity.
study 3 failed to support this hypothesis in terms of indi- As with all research, our studies had some limitations
vidual differences in the achievement value. These results that must be considered when interpreting results. For
might be attributed to a few possibilities. First, it is possible example, we are not able to draw definitive conclusions
that not all value fulfilling behavior strongly induces state about valence and value-congruence in studies 2 and
authenticity. The behavior component of trait authenticity 3 because the data is retrospective and cross-sectional.
includes acting in accordance with intrinsic motivations Reports show how participants believe they experienced
12    O. Smallenbroek et al.

the situations, and this may differ from what they actually The current research reduces this ambiguity by showing
experienced. Fleeson and Wilt (2010), showed there is a that authenticity can arise from meeting trans-situational
discrepancy between retrospective and experience sam- and personally meaningful goals. Nevertheless, the precise
pling data when measuring the relationship between big relationship between positive affect and state authentic-
five traits and authenticity. Study 1 partially addresses this ity should be explored further, especially in relation to
issue by asking participants to participate in a debate and trait authenticity and its cognitive mechanisms. This will
immediately report their states. Results were similar across broaden our understanding of the true self and its psy-
all three studies, supporting the hypothesis that there is chological functions.
a relationship between authenticity and value congruent
behavior. This indicates that participants are able to judge
Note
how their values and behavior influence state authentic-
ity, but future work might consider crossing momentarily 1. 
No significant differences were found between behavior
value-congruent behavior with pleasant versus unpleasant conditions in trait authenticity or value priorities.
circumstances.
The second limitation of these studies is the sam- Acknowledgements
ples. The university participants are not fully developed.
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

We thank Renée Pelletier for assistance in creating the value


During the ages of 18–25 identity formation is ongoing. debate and data collection, and Zack van Allen for coding and
Participants are struggling with ideological and normative compiling data in study 1. All research was carried out by Oscar
issues which can create ambiguous attachments to their Smallenbroek and Renée Pelletier in partial fulfillment of the
value priorities. This does not negate the validity of the requirements for the B.A. Honours program in Psychology at
results as much as warrant a cautious note that the pat- Carleton University.
terns of association may be different in older populations.
With a stable identity and value priorities, the relationship Disclosure statement
between value priorities and authenticity may strengthen.
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
A third limitation of the studies is the large number of
participants who were excluded. In studies 2 and 3, par-
ticipants often did not write down their memories despite Funding
completing the questionnaires. These participants were
This work was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities
excluded because we could not verify they recalled an Research Council of Canada [grant number 410-2009-1004].
appropriate situation. Second, these studies seemed to
require more time and concentration than many partic-
ipants were willing to devote online. Several participants ORCID
logged into the study two or three times, or stopped after John M. Zelenski   http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3668-5764
completing one recall prompt. This may be attributable
to the nature of the study. Introspection and deciding on
value priorities are potentially difficult and draining activi- References
ties. There is also a difference in the dropout rates between Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure-and process-integrated
the behavior conditions in studies 2 and 3. Participants view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states.
seemed to struggle recalling incongruent behavior. This Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027.
Fleeson, W., & Wilt, J. (2010). The relevance of Big Five trait
may indicate a third variable problem1 or reflect selective content in behavior to subjective authenticity: Do high levels
memory when it comes to remembering ‘inauthentic’ of within-person behavioral variability undermine or enable
behavior. authenticity achievement? Journal of Personality, 78, 1353–
Overall, we found encouraging results suggesting that 1382.
state authenticity is more than endorsing positive affec- Gallagher, P., Fleeson, W., & Hoyle, R. H. (2011). A self-regulatory
mechanism for personality trait stability: Contra-trait effort.
tive states as the true self. On the other hand, it is clear
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 335–342. doi:
that state authenticity and positive affect co-occur and 10.1177/1948550610390701
interact with each other. For participants, these conflicting Goldman, B. M., & Kernis, M. H. (2002). The role of authenticity in
feelings may lead them to report varying levels of state healthy psychological functioning and subjective well-being.
authenticity in ambiguous situations, such as in unpleas- Annals of the American Psychotherapy Association, 5, 18–20.
ant atmospheres. This also makes it difficult to gauge the Harter, S. (2002). Authenticity. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.),
Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 382–394). New York, NY:
potential for authenticity to distinctly promote well-­being Oxford University Press.
because its relationship to experiencing meaning and Heppner, W. L., Kernis, M. H., Nezlek, J. B., Foster, J., Lakey, C.
well-being is confounded with contextual affective states. E., & Goldman, B. M. (2008). Within-person relationships
The Journal of Positive Psychology   13

among daily self-esteem, need satisfaction, and authenticity. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human
Psychological Science, 19, 1140–1145. potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic
Huta, V., & Waterman, A. S. (2014). Eudaimonia and its distinction well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166.
from hedonia: Developing a classification and terminology Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2011). A self-determination theory
for understanding conceptual and operational definitions. perspective on social, institutional, cultural, and economic
Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 1425–1456. supports for autonomy and their importance for well-
Kasser, Tim (2002). Sketches for a self-determination theory being. In V. I. Chirkov, R. M. Ryan, & K. M. Sheldon (Eds.),
of values. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self- Human autonomy in cross-cultural context, cross-cultural
determination theory (pp. 123–140). Rochester, NY: University advancements in positive psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 45–64).
of Rochester Press. London: Springer.
Kernis, M. H., & Goldman, B. M. (2005). From thought and Schlegel, R. J., & Hicks, J. A. (2011). The true self and psychological
experience to behavior and interpersonal relationships: A health: Emerging evidence and future directions. Social
multicomponent conceptualization of authenticity. In A. and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 989–1003. doi:
Tesser, J. V. Wood, & D. A. Stapel (Eds.), On building, defending 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00401.x
and regulating the self: A psychological perspective (pp. 31–52). Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha.
New York: Psychology Press. Psychological Assessment, 8, 350–353.
Kernis, M. H., & Goldman, B. M. (2006). A multicomponent Schwartz, S. H., & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across
conceptualization of authenticity: Theory and research. In cultures: Taking a similarities perspective. Journal of Cross-
Downloaded by [Stephen F Austin State University] at 12:15 26 July 2016

M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Cultural Psychology, 32, 268–290.
(pp. 284–357). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. Schwartz, S. H., Melech, G., Lehmann, A., Burgess, S., Harris, M.,
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the & Owens, V. (2001). Extending the cross-cultural validity of
circumplex model of emotion. In M. S. Clark (Eds.), Emotion: the theory of basic human values with a different method
Review of personality and social psychology no. 13 (25–29). of measurement. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32,
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 519–542.
Lenton, A. P., Bruder, M., Slabu, L., & Sedikides, C. (2013). How Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive
does “being real” feel? The experience of state authenticity. psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5–14.
Journal of Personality, 81, 276–289. doi:10.1111/j.1467- Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Rawsthorne, L., & Ilardi, B. (1997).
6494.2012.00805.x Trait self and true self: Cross-role variation in the Big-
Lenton, A. P., Slabu, L., Sedikides, C., & Power, K. (2013). I feel Five personality traits and its relations with psychological
good, therefore I am real: Testing the causal influence of authenticity and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality
mood on state authenticity. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 1202– and Social Psychology, 73, 1380–1393.
1224. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.778818 Waterman, A. S. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: A eudai-
Maslow, A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. New monist’s perspective. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3,
York, NY: Viking. 234–252.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1994). The stability of personality: Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
Observations and evaluations. Current Directions in validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect:
Psychological Science, 3, 173–175. The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Reich, W. A., Kessel, E. M., & Bernieri, F. J. (2012). Life satisfaction 54, 1063–1070.
and the self: Structure, content, and function. Journal of Wood, A. M., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., & Joseph, S.
Happiness Studies, 14, 293–308. (2008). The authentic personality: A theoretical and empirical
Rogers, C. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of conceptualization, and the development of the Authenticity
psychotherapy. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55, 385–399.

You might also like