You are on page 1of 13

Rudeness and Medical

Team Performance
Arieh Riskin, MD, MHA,a,b Amir Erez, PhD,c Trevor A. Foulk, BBA,c Kinneret S. Riskin-Geuz, BSc,d Amitai
Ziv, MD, MHA,d,e Rina Sela, CCRN, MA,e Liat Pessach-Gelblum, MBA,e Peter A. Bamberger, PhDa

OBJECTIVES: Rudeness is routinely experienced by medical teams. We sought to explore the abstract
impact of rudeness on medical teams’ performance and test interventions that might
mitigate its negative consequences.
METHODS: Thirty-nine NICU teams participated in a training workshop including simulations
of acute care of term and preterm newborns. In each workshop, 2 teams were randomly
assigned to either an exposure to rudeness (in which the comments of the patient’s
mother included rude statements completely unrelated to the teams’ performance) or
control (neutral comments) condition, and 2 additional teams were assigned to rudeness
with either a preventative (cognitive bias modification [CBM]) or therapeutic (narrative)
intervention. Simulation sessions were evaluated by 2 independent judges, blind to team
exposure, who used structured questionnaires to assess team performance.
RESULTS: Rudeness had adverse consequences not only on diagnostic and intervention
parameters (mean therapeutic score 3.81 ± 0.36 vs 4.31 ± 0.35 in controls, P < .01), but also
on team processes (such as information and workload sharing, helping and communication)
central to patient care (mean teamwork score 4.04 ± 0.34 vs 4.43 ± 0.37, P < .05). CBM
mitigated most of these adverse effects of rudeness, but the postexposure narrative
intervention had no significant effect.
CONCLUSIONS: Rudeness has robust, deleterious effects on the performance of medical teams.
Moreover, exposure to rudeness debilitated the very collaborative mechanisms recognized
as essential for patient care and safety. Interventions focusing on teaching medical
professionals to implicitly avoid cognitive distraction such as CBM may offer a means to
mitigate the adverse consequences of behaviors that, unfortunately, cannot be prevented.

aColler School of Management, and dSackler School of Medicine, University of Tel Aviv, Tel Aviv, Israel;
bNeonatology,
WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Rudeness is
Bnai Zion Medical Center, Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion, Israel Institute of Technology,
Haifa, Israel; cWarrington College of Business Administration, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida; and
routinely experienced by medical teams. Medical
eIsrael Center for Medical Simulation, Chaim Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer, Ramat Gan, Israel professionals exposed to rude behavior performed
poorly on diagnostic and procedural tasks related
Dr Riskin, conceptualized and designed the study, carried out the initial analyses, and drafted the to the medical treatment they provided. Reduced
initial manuscript; Dr Erez conceptualized and designed the study, coordinated data collection, information sharing and helping mediated the
carried out the initial analyses, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Mr Foulk
effects of rudeness on their performance.
carried out the initial analyses and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Mrs Riskin-Geuz
designed the data collection instruments, coordinated and supervised data collection, and WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Rudeness had adverse
critically reviewed the manuscript; Dr Ziv designed the study, coordinated data collection, and consequences not only on therapeutic components
critically reviewed the manuscript; Ms Sela designed the data collection instruments, coordinated of medical teams’ performance, but also on
and supervised data collection, and critically reviewed the manuscript; Ms Pessach-Gelblum collaborative team processes essential for such
designed the study, coordinated data collection, and critically reviewed the manuscript;
performance. Cognitive bias modification as a
Dr Bamberger conceptualized and designed the study and drafted the initial manuscript; and
all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
preventative intervention mitigated most of these
negative consequences of rudeness.
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-2305
Accepted for publication Nov 4, 2016
To cite: Riskin A, Erez A, Foulk TA, et al. Rudeness and Medical
Team Performance. Pediatrics. 2017;139(2):e20162305

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


PEDIATRICS Volume 139, number 2, February 2017:e20162305 ARTICLE
Rude and disrespectful behaviors, interactions as a risk factor for such team-level medical outcomes
prevalent in all organizations,1 iatrogenesis,17 this study raised as diagnostic and procedural
are increasingly widespread in 3 main questions that we seek to performance.
high-intensity, service-oriented address in the current investigation.
To address the third question, we
organizations, such as hospitals and
examined the potential mitigating
health care facilities.2–11 Doctors, First, although the preceding
effects of 1 potential preventative
nurses, and other health care findings suggest that collaborative
intervention and 1 potential
providers are regularly exposed to processes may be adversely affected
treatment intervention. In terms
rude behaviors from their superiors, by rudeness, we know little about
of the former, framing rudeness
peers, patients, and families of the team-level consequences of
as a threatening stimulus eliciting
patients. This is concerning because rudeness. Indeed, whereas laboratory
appraisal and interpretation
beyond the adverse effects such research using student participants
(processes drawing cognitive
behavior can have on their targets’ has consistently demonstrated the
resources from the task at hand),
well-being,12 a growing body of adverse impact of rudeness on the
we looked to interventions focused
research indicates that rudeness performance of individual victims
on cognitive bias modification
can also have devastating effects and witnesses,13–15 we are unaware
(CBM) targeting threat-related
on individual performance. For of research examining team-level
interpretation biases. CBM
example, studies have demonstrated effects. Accordingly, the first question
interventions involve brief,
that in comparison with controls, we address is the degree to which
computerized cognitive training
participants exposed to even mildly rudeness also affects team processes
modules designed to alter threat-
rude behavior (eg, insensitive and and, ultimately, team performance.
oriented biases in interpretation
unexpectedly disrespectful acts or Second, whereas the earlier study
by promoting a more positive/
utterances) performed poorly on demonstrated the adverse effects
benign rather than threat-based
cognitive tasks, exhibited reduced of colleague-based rudeness, the
interpretation of ambiguous
creativity and flexibility, and were implications of rudeness stemming
information or stimuli.18–21 Similar,
less helpful and prosocial. This effect from a patient or patient family
preperformance cognitive training
was observed regardless of whether member remain unknown. This is
modules have been demonstrated
they were the target of such behavior important because although medical
to enhance attention control and
or simply a witness.13–15 facilities may be able to control
are in place in a wide variety of
colleague-based rudeness, there is
performance domains, including
Building on such findings, in a recent little they can do to control patient or
surgery and flight control.22–24 CBM
study, we applied a simulation-based family rudeness. Hence, the second
may have similar potential in this
experimental design to examine the question we address is the degree to
regard because it has been shown
impact of colleague-based rudeness which findings regarding the impact
to increase people’s resiliency to
on a variety of individual-level of colleague-based rudeness are
attention-diverting stressors by
practitioner outcomes in a NICU generalizable to patient/family-based
training them to shift their attention
context, finding robust adverse rudeness. Finally, because rudeness
away from threat. Accordingly,
consequences with respect to can have such a devastating effect
we posited that to the extent that
accuracy of diagnosis and quality of on practitioner performance, we
practitioners can learn to interpret
care (ie, misspecification of medical questioned whether preventative
interpersonal emotional expression
orders and errors in fulfillment of and/or treatment interventions
as less hostile, they and their
medical orders).16 Moreover, we might mitigate these adverse effects.
teammates should be less affected
identified diminished information
by such expressions and be better
sharing and help seeking among the Accordingly, the first and second
positioned to apply their cognitive
NICU staff as key processes adversely aims of the current study were
resources to the tasks at hand and
affected by rudeness and mediating to see if we could replicate our
provide enhanced clinical care.
its impact on individual practitioner previous findings at the team level
performance.16 Overall, rudeness with rudeness stemming from The second, treatment intervention
explained 43% of the variance in an alternative source (ie, patient was informed by research by
practitioner performance; 20 points family). In this context, we examined Pennebaker on the treatment
more than that accounted for by all the extent to which patient-based of victims of sexual abuse.25–30
other commonly explored causes rudeness influenced such team- Pennebaker and others have
of iatrogenesis, such as chronic level processes as information demonstrated that recovery
sleep loss.16 Aside from highlighting sharing, workload sharing, helping among such victims is facilitated
the often overlooked role of social and communication, as well as by composing a narrative of the

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


2 RISKIN et al
event. Underlying this approach Randomization teach debriefing skills, including
is the notion that “constructing reflection, analysis, and planning. We
Randomization was achieved by
stories is a natural human process also reviewed with participants the
using a system of randomly prepared
that helps individuals understand section of the consent form indicating
cards in sealed, nontransparent
their experiences and themselves,” that they were free to withdraw
envelopes containing the 4 condition
thus facilitating “a sense of from the study at any time. Upon
assignments. There were separate
resolution, which results in less completing the final instruments
envelopes for each simulation day.
rumination and eventually allows at the end of the simulation day,
At the start of each simulation day,
disturbing experiences to subside all participants were debriefed.
the research assistant drew the cards
gradually from conscious thought.”30 In this poststudy debriefing, we
from the envelope and assigned
Accordingly, we posited that by (1) again reviewed participants’
each of the preregistered teams
writing a narrative about a rude right to withdraw from the study
to 1 of the 4 conditions (control,
event just experienced, practitioners as well as their right to withdraw
rudeness, rudeness with narrative,
would more efficiently process their personal data from the study;
and rudeness with CBM) according
the experience and thus be better (2) reminded participants that
to the card pull for that day. On
positioned to focus their attention on although the “patients” and “family
those five simulation days on which
their team and subsequent cases they members” that they interacted with
a team scheduled to participate
were asked to manage. in different scenarios were just
failed to show up, the research
manikins and actors, respectively, it
assistant randomly excluded 1 of
was perfectly normal to be disturbed
the noncontrol condition cards from
METHODS by some of these experiences; and
the card draw to ensure that there
(3) informed participants that
was always a team in the control
Participants support was available for those
condition.
feeling particularly disturbed by their
Thirty-nine NICU teams, each experiences. None of the participants
Ethical Considerations
comprising 2 physicians and 2 withdrew their informed consent or
nurses, were recruited from among Each team member underwent a requested that their data be withheld
the various NICUs operating in prestudy, consenting process in or destroyed, and no one asked for
Israel’s hospitals. Teams were offered which they signed an informed the psychological assistance that we
the opportunity to join a full medical consent form (written per the offered to provide discretely.
simulation training day in The Israel specifications of the institutional
After reviewing the informed consent
Medical Simulation Center at Sheba review boards of the first author’s
forms, participants were briefed on
Medical Center at Tel Hashomer.31 university, and the medical center
the reflexivity training exercise that
The espoused purpose of the exercise with which Israel Center for Medical
would occur at the conclusion of each
was to train teams in the debriefing Simulation is affiliated). This
of the simulations during the day.
techniques described and examined form specified that the purpose
by Vashdi et al as a means to of the study was to examine Scenarios
facilitate team learning and enhance factors influencing medical team
performance.32 performance. As part of this process, Regardless of condition, the day
participants were specifically told comprised 5 emergency scenarios in
Procedure that in the course of the simulations, neonatal medicine:
their performance would be recorded
Neonate with severe jaundice
Four teams were recruited for and observed by others, and that
(pathologic hyperbilirubinemia)
each simulation day. Two teams they would be required to interface
because of glucose-6-phosphate-
were randomly assigned to either with actors playing the role of their
dehydrogenase deficiency. Because
a rudeness (in which the scripted patients' family members. They
intensive phototherapy did not
comments of the infant’s mother were also told that rather than
sufficiently reduce the bilirubin
early in the day included a rude giving them feedback during the
levels, double volume exchange
statement completely unrelated to course of their simulation work,
transfusion should be performed.
the teams’ performance) or control observers would do so after each
(neutral comments) condition, and simulation in the context of the Newborn in hypovolemic shock. The
2 additional teams were assigned to debriefing. We explained to them neonate was delivered via vacuum
rudeness with either a preventative that these postsimulation sessions extraction and developed a rapidly
(CBM) or therapeutic (narrative) would be used not only to provide expanding subgaleal hemorrhage
intervention. performance feedback but also to with consumption coagulopathy.

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


PEDIATRICS Volume 139, number 2, February 2017 3
Infant with severe respiratory discuss and develop a treatment plan were presented to the judges and
distress after meconium for each clinical scenario. Specifically, discussed by them to ensure that
aspiration syndrome. The neonate the team was required to identify the all had a common understanding of
developed persistent pulmonary acute deterioration in the newborn’s their meaning and application. For
hypertension, was intubated and condition and respond promptly each scenario, using a 5-point Likert
received inhaled nitric oxide. His by providing the appropriate scale (1 = failed; 5 = excellent), judges
respiratory status was further resuscitative treatments, while trying independently rated each team’s
complicated by a pneumothorax to diagnose the underlying medical performance along items relating to
necessitating insertion of a thorax condition. Based on conventional 9 parameters separated into 2 broad
drain. protocol, the main actions required aspects of team performance:
from the medical team were detailed
Newborn with severe neonatal Medical and therapeutic
for each scenario and distributed
asphyxia with hypoxic ischemic performance—(1) Diagnostic
to independent judges (senior
encephalopathy. Total body performance (ie, time taken to
neonatologists and veteran nurses)
cooling, also referred to as diagnose, accuracy of diagnosis),
to facilitate their monitoring and
hypothermia treatment, is the (2) quality of therapy plan (ie,
evaluation. The scenarios were
state-of-the-art management of appropriateness of plan given
designed such that a team’s failure
this condition. diagnosis; plan accounts for unique
to follow these specified actions
constraints), (3) intervention (ie
Imminent delivery of very premature would likely lead to further rapid
procedural or skill performance),
(23 weeks and 6 days gestation) deterioration and ultimately the
and (4) overall general assessment
extremely low birth weight infant infant’s demise within a short time.
of medical therapy (eg, errors
at the verge of viability. This The scenarios involved diagnostic
made in diagnosis, therapy
scenario required the team to and manual intervention skills and
plan or execution; adequacy
manage the initial delivery room required that members engage with
of performance given unique
resuscitation and stabilization. one another in making and executing
constraints).16
They then had to address the therapeutic decisions. Following
neonate’s deterioration secondary each simulation, the team entered a Teamwork or relational cooperative
to massive pulmonary hemorrhage separate room for a reflexivity-based aspects of performance within
and severe bilateral grade IV debriefing. Prior to the start of the the team—(5) Information
intraventricular hemorrhage. The reflexivity exercise, the judges were sharing, (6) workload sharing, (7)
scenario required the team to asked to complete a questionnaire in helping among team members,
manage family grieving as well as which they graded the participants’ (8) communication between team
ethical dilemmas. performance. members, and (9) overall general
assessment of teamwork.16,33–40
We arranged for the first scenario
(incorporating the rudeness event) Measures
Interventions
to occur concomitantly across all
Two independent NICU staff (1
4 conditions. The other scenarios Each NICU team was exposed to
senior doctor and 1 experienced
occurred in random order. comments or critiques by the infant’s
nurse) blinded to the experimental
parents, according to the control or
In each scenario, the participants intervention observed each
rudeness condition. These comments
were told that the NICU manikin lying team’s performance in each of
included neutral statements in
in the incubator was their patient and the simulation scenarios from an
the control condition or the same,
that the patient’s vitals would appear adjacent control room with 1-way
mildly rude statement (ie, “I knew
on the monitors immediately at the mirrors and multiple video monitors
we should have gone to a better
start of the simulation. Additionally, allowing for close-up observation and
hospital where they don’t practice
the participants were provided with the monitoring of the patient’s vital
Third World medicine!”) in all 3 of
neonate’s medical history. They signs. Before serving in that capacity,
the rudeness conditions (rudeness by
were also informed that they might all judges underwent a daylong
itself, CBM followed by rudeness, and
encounter professional actors playing training program emphasizing
rudeness followed by the narrative
parents of their patient and would the monitoring and assessment of
intervention).
be asked to interact and respond the team (rather than individual
to them as they would in real life. members) as the unit of analysis. Exposure to rude or neutral
Additionally, participants were asked To enhance interrater reliability, as comments from the mother of the
to work as a team and told that they part of this training, descriptors and infant occurred at the beginning of
would receive 20 to 25 minutes to examples of indicative behaviors the first scenario of the day, which

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


4 RISKIN et al
was the same for all groups (scenario trial began 100 to 400 milliseconds toward them, especially of the
1 of the infant with severe neonatal later. The order in which the various mother of the infant, as significantly
jaundice). faces were displayed was randomly more polite (ie, less rude) in the
determined. control condition (Mcontrol = 4.75,
Just before this first scenario,
SDcontrol = 0.45) than in the rude
teams in the preventative, CBM During this game, the computer
condition (Mrudeness = 3.75, SDrudeness =
intervention20 engaged in a program first determined the
0.77) (F1,38 = 16.31, P < .001).
20-minute computer game in participants threshold to threat (ie,
This effect remained consistent,
which they looked at a series of angry faces) and then gave them
although somewhat attenuated,
morphing faces,41 were asked to feedback designed to raise this
through the end of the day (Mcontrol =
move a cursor to indicate whether threshold and as such “immunize”
4.66, SDcontrol = 0.56 vs Mrudeness =
the emotion expressed was more of them from devoting substantial
4.01, SDrudeness = 0.61, F1,38 = 9.26,
anger or pleasure, and then received attention to minor threats.
P = .004), thus confirming that the
immediate feedback on their choice.
While teams in this condition were rudeness manipulation was effective.
engaged in the CBM computer
The Emotion Recognition Task of the Statistical Analysis
game, teams in the narrative
CBM Intervention
intervention28–30 worked on their
All analyses were conducted using
Stimuli for the task were generated first simulation and were exposed
SPSS (version 23, IBM, Armonk, NY)
using Morpheus Photo Morpher to the rudeness incident specified
unless otherwise indicated.
v3.16 (Morpheus Software, Grand earlier. Immediately after this
A power analysis based on data from
Rapids, MI). Four sequences of simulation, teams in this condition
a previous study16 and assuming
morphed faces were generated were directed to a debriefing room
a desired power of 80% with α
based on the happy and angry and asked to write 1 or 2 paragraphs
of .05 (2-sided test) indicated that
pictures of 2 males and 2 females about how they thought the mother
samples of at least 9 teams per
taken from the NimStim set30. The of the infant felt when it seemed to
condition would be required to
faces were selected to represent her that the team was unsuccessful in
capture moderate effects. Because
both genders and different racial treating her newborn.
each team’s performance in each
origins (Caucasian, African American,
In all, teams were randomly allocated of the scenarios was rated by 2
Hispanic, and Asian) and after pilot
to 1 of 4 conditions: (1) control, (2) judges, we assessed reliability (the
tests that demonstrated test–retest
rudeness, (3) rudeness with the relative consistency among raters),
validity for these morphed sequences
CBM intervention, and (4) rudeness by calculating intraclass correlation
in the emotion perception task.
with the narrative intervention. coefficients (ICC1; R version 2.15.0,
Each sequence consisted of 15 faces
Effects of rudeness exposure (vs The R Foundation for Statistical
equally spaced on a continuum
neutral control condition) and the Computing, Vienna, Austria). An
between the happy and angry end
moderating effect of the intervention ICC1 of 0.10 or higher indicated that
points. Each face from each morphed
(CBM or narrative) were assessed the item could be averaged across
sequence was presented 3 times,
repeatedly throughout day. judges.43 Comparisons of therapeutic
for a total of 180 trials (4 sequences
and teamwork performance scores
× 15 faces × 3 repetitions). Faces
Manipulation Check were done using multivariate ANOVA
were displayed in a random order.
(MANOVA). Comparisons of all
Each trial began with a fixation cross The primacy effect of the rudeness
variables included in therapeutic and
(800–1200 milliseconds), followed manipulation and its possible
teamwork performance scores in the
by a color morphed face picture degradation over time was checked
4 conditions (ie control, rudeness,
(90 mm in height and 70 mm in at 2 points during the simulation
rudeness with narrative intervention
width). The face was displayed for day, once at midday (after the third
and rudeness with CBM intervention)
200 milliseconds and then masked scenario) and once at day’s end
were analyzed using 1-way ANOVA.
by a scrambled face display for (after the fifth and final scenario).
Statistical significance was set at .05.
200 milliseconds. Then a question An analysis of variance (ANOVA)
mark appeared on the screen and with the rudeness condition as the
remained until a response was made. independent variable and perceived
RESULTS
Participants were instructed to press rudeness (assessed on the basis of a
1 of 2 designated buttons as fast as 4-item measure validated in previous To ensure that our randomization
they could to indicate whether the research13,14,42 and with α = .93) as process of assigning teams to
face was “angry” or “happy.” After the dependent variable indicated conditions was appropriate, we
the participant’s response, the next that participants rated the attitude first tested whether the cumulative

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


PEDIATRICS Volume 139, number 2, February 2017 5
TABLE 1 Team Performance Scores—Control Versus Rudeness
Control (n = 11) Rudeness (n = 10) F P η2
Mean SD Mean SD
Diagnostic score 4.27 0.41 3.89 0.49 3.80 .07 0.17
Therapy plan 4.23 0.34 3.81 0.38 7.27* .01 0.28
Intervention score 4.38 0.36 3.75 0.37 15.43** .001 0.45
General therapeutic score 4.37 0.40 3.80 0.34 12.02** .003 0.39
Information sharing 4.41 0.42 4.08 0.36 3.65 .07 0.16
Workload sharing 4.40 0.44 3.93 0.35 7.06* .02 0.27
Helping 4.50 0.37 4.08 0.37 6.56* .02 0.26
Communication 4.42 0.38 4.03 0.45 4.64* .04 0.20
General teamwork score 4.43 0.39 4.06 0.34 5.62* .03 0.23
Midday manipulation check 4.75 0.45 3.98 0.37 19.21** <.001 —
End day manipulation check 4.66 0.56 4.07 0.31 8.60** .009 —
—, not applicable.
* P < .05.
** P < .01.

experience of the team’s members 5 indicators of teamwork score followed by rudeness” condition
was distributed equally across the (ie, information sharing, workload versus control condition as the
conditions. We conducted an ANOVA sharing, helping, communication, and factor, and medical/therapeutic
with the conditions (eg, control, overall performance, with α = .96) performance and teamwork as the
rudeness, rudeness with narrative to form scales. Next we conducted a dependent variables. The overall
intervention, and rudeness with MANOVA with the rudeness versus model representing the influence
CBM intervention) as the factor control condition as the factor of the CBM intervention on the
and cumulative team experience and therapeutic and teamwork 2 dependent variables was not
(eg, number of years in a NICU) scores as the dependent variables. significant (multivariate P = .61).
as the dependent variable. The The overall model representing ANOVA results showed that after
results showed that there were no the influence of rudeness on the 2 employing the CBM inoculation,
significant differences between the dependent variables was significant rudeness did not affect the
conditions (P = .26) indicating that (multivariate P < .05, η2 = 0.36). therapeutic score (P = .48), nor did it
the randomization process had been ANOVA results showed that the affect the teamwork score (P = .47).
successful. Controlling for cumulative rudeness condition affected both Table 2 reports mean comparison
experience in all subsequent analyses the therapeutic score (P < .01, η2 = between the control and CBM groups
did not significantly change any of 0.39; Mrudeness = 3.80, SDrudeness = for all performance measures. As
the results. Therefore, we report 0.34, Mcontrol = 4.37, SDcontrol = 0.40) shown, the CBM intervention reduced
the results without controlling for and the teamwork score (P < .05, the effects of rudeness on all the
cumulative experience. η2 = 0.23; Mrudeness = 4.06, SDrudeness = team outcomes (eg, diagnostic score;
0.34, Mcontrol = 4.43, SDcontrol = 0.39). intervention score) and process (eg,
Because each team’s performance in Table 1 reports mean comparison information sharing, communication)
each of the scenarios was rated by 2 between the control and rudeness parameters. Thus, it seems that
judges, we first assessed reliability groups for all performance measures. the CBM intervention succeeded in
(the relative consistency among As shown, rudeness was associated “immunizing” participants from the
raters) by calculating ICC1. The with diminished team performance effects of rudeness.
resulting ICCs indicated moderate along all team outcomes (eg,
to high interrater reliability, thus Finally, we tested the effects of the
diagnostic score; intervention
supporting aggregation to the team narrative intervention on the 2
score) and process (eg, information
level (data not shown but available performance outcomes. Here again,
sharing, communication) parameters,
from the first author). we first conducted a MANOVA
although these differences were only
with the “rudeness followed by
marginally significant (P < .10) in
Next, we tested whether the rudeness narrative intervention” condition
the case of diagnostic accuracy and
manipulation harmed team’s versus control condition as the
information sharing).
performance. We first averaged the factor, and medical/ therapeutic
4 indicators of the therapeutic team Next, we tested the effects of the CBM performance and teamwork as the
scores (ie, diagnostic, therapy plan, intervention on the performance dependent variables. The overall
intervention, and overall therapeutic measures. We first conducted a model representing the influence of
performance, with α = .97) and the MANOVA with the “CBM inoculation the narrative intervention on the 2

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


6 RISKIN et al
TABLE 2 Team Performance Scores—Control Versus CBM intervention and process parameters
Control (n = 11) CBM (n = 9) F P including team information and
Mean SD Mean SD workload sharing. These findings not
only replicate earlier findings13,14
Diagnostic score 4.27 0.41 4.26 0.56 0.00 .97
Therapy plan 4.23 0.34 4.14 0.71 0.15 .71 demonstrating the deleterious effects
Intervention score 4.38 0.36 4.08 0.64 1.66 .21 of rudeness expressed by a senior
General therapeutic score 4.37 0.40 4.19 0.66 0.52 .48 colleague on individual medical
Information sharing 4.41 0.42 4.29 0.35 0.43 .52 performance16 but also extend
Workload sharing 4.40 0.44 4.22 0.50 0.69 .42
them by demonstrating that similar
Helping 4.50 0.37 4.26 0.34 2.16 .16
Communication 4.42 0.38 4.35 0.45 0.17 .68 effects are elicited by rudeness from
General teamwork score 4.43 0.39 4.29 0.46 0.54 .47 other sources and are manifested
Midday manipulation check 4.75 0.45 3.28 0.74 29.97** <.001 at the team level. Interestingly,
End day manipulation check 4.66 0.56 3.72 0.68 11.64** .003 however, the effects of rudeness on
** P < .01. diagnosis and information sharing
were only marginally significant.
dependent variables was significant 3 shows that, similar to the rudeness Although we can only speculate as
(multivariate P < .05, η2 = 0.31). without intervention condition, those to the relative weakness of these
ANOVA results showed that the in the CBM intervention condition particular effects, one possibility is
rudeness in the narrative condition viewed the mother in the rudeness that because these activities require
affected both the therapeutic score manipulation scenario as more rude greater conscious effort, they may
(P < .05, η2 = 0.25; Mnarrative = 3.89, than those in the control condition be less subject to any threat-based
SDnarrative = 0.48, Mcontrol = 4.37, both at midday (P < .01) and at the redirection of cognitive resources
SDcontrol = 0.40) and the teamwork end of the day (P < .01). In contrast, away from the task.
score (P < .05, η2 = 0.22; Mnarrative = at midday those in the narrative
4.04, SDnarrative = 0.39, Mcontrol = 4.43, condition did perceive the mother to Additionally, we demonstrated that
SDcontrol = 0.39). Table 3 reports mean be more rude than controls (P < .05), a preventative or “immunization”
comparison between the control and but by the end of the day, those in the CBM intervention, targeting negative
narrative groups for all performance narrative condition did not view the interpretations of emotional displays
measures. As shown, the narrative mother as more rude than controls and applied before the rudeness
intervention did not reduce the (P = .09). incident, largely mitigated these
effects of rudeness for any of the deleterious effects on team medical
team outcomes (eg, diagnostic score; outcomes and processes, whereas a
intervention score) and process (eg, DISCUSSION postincident, treatment intervention
information sharing, communication) Our findings indicate that NICU teams based on victim’s composition of a
parameters. Thus, the data indicate exposed to mild rudeness expressed narrative was largely ineffective in
that the narrative intervention failed. by a patient’s mother resulted in doing so. Still, it should be noted that
diminished team performance with although there was no statistically
Examination of the manipulation respect to outcome parameters significant difference between the
checks presented in Tables 1, 2, and relating to both diagnosis and performance scores of teams in the

TABLE 3 Team Performance Scores—Control Versus Narrative


Control (n = 11) Narrative (n = 9) F P η2
Mean SD Mean SD
Diagnostic score 4.27 0.41 3.88 0.52 3.64 .07 0.17
Therapy plan 4.23 0.34 3.81 0.57 4.30 .05 0.19
Intervention score 4.38 0.36 3.83 0.50 7.82* .01 0.30
General therapeutic score 4.37 0.40 3.89 0.48 5.85* .03 0.25
Information sharing 4.41 0.42 3.94 0.43 5.90* .03 0.25
Workload sharing 4.40 0.44 3.82 0.45 8.37* .01 0.32
Helping 4.50 0.37 4.17 0.22 5.66* .03 0.24
Communication 4.42 0.38 4.04 0.27 6.44* .02 0.26
General teamwork score 4.43 0.39 4.04 0.39 5.03* .04 0.22
Midday manipulation check 4.75 0.45 3.92 0.91 7.17* .01 —
End day manipulation check 4.66 0.56 4.17 0.67 3.25 .09 —
—, not applicable.
* P < .05.

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


PEDIATRICS Volume 139, number 2, February 2017 7
CBM intervention and those of the participants’ positive reappraisal precisely in these situations, the
teams in the control condition, the of her rude behavior, it failed to very collaborative processes that
scores for the former were nominally help them overcome the cognitive generally enable teams to outperform
lower, suggesting that for some disruption it caused. individuals may break down. To
groups under certain conditions, the extent that rudeness impedes
One possible explanation for this
the CBM might not always be team helping and workload sharing,
seemingly inconsistent finding is that
entirely effective. Nevertheless, we teams may not be able to deliver the
the cognitive processes involved in
demonstrated that these mitigation heightened level of patient care that
consciously reflecting on experienced
effects of the CBM intervention were, practitioners have come to expect
behavior and assessing its level of
on average, sustained over the entire from them. Thus, we believe that our
politeness (targeted by the narrative
day. findings offer the first real evidence
intervention and demanded by our
of the impact that rudeness has on
manipulation check for rudeness)
Interestingly, examination of the the performance of medical teams
are different from those involved in
manipulation checks presented in and not just individuals working in
more automatic determinations of
Tables 1, 2, and 3 shows that similar teams.
potential or actual threat (targeted
to the rudeness without intervention
by the CBM intervention).44 It Second, these findings are important
condition, those in the CBM
is known that these 2 types of because, consistent with our earlier
intervention condition viewed the
cognitive processes do not always findings at the individual level,16
mother in the rudeness manipulation
operate in tandem. In the CBM task, they suggest that relatively benign
scenario as more rude than those in
the judgment is fast and crude in but negative human interactions
the control condition. This suggests
the sense that it requires a quick, could underlie many of the iatrogenic
that the CBM intervention did not
dichotomous response, whereas the incidents commonly occurring in
distort participants’ views nor cause
politeness rating score is based on a medical care settings. On one hand, it
them to misperceive the mother’s
more thoughtful judgment process is highly disturbing because, as noted,
rude behavior. Instead, as designed,
(answering a 4-item questionnaire such interactions are prevalent
the CBM intervention “immunized”
rated on a Likert scale). Interestingly, and, particularly in high-intensity,
participants’ medical/therapeutic
this reflective and thoughtful process life-and-death contexts, unlikely
performance and teamwork by
may be more similar to the one in to be preventable. On the other
shifting their attention away from
the narrative condition and hand, the finding that (as shown in
the implicit threat posed by the
thus might have been more Table 1) rudeness explains 39% of
mother, thus likely preserving
influenced by it. the variance in team-level general
cognitive resources for the tasks at
therapeutic outcomes (a figure
hand. Results of these supplementary The findings of this study are
remarkably similar to the effect size
analyses indicated that the CBM significant in a number of respects:
found in our earlier individual-level
intervention operated not so first, our results reaffirmed how
analysis16) suggests that we may
much by mitigating the appraisal rudeness can debilitate intervention
have identified an important and
of rudeness (indeed, those in this acuity, thus resulting in poorer
potentially “treatable” iatrogenesis-
condition viewed the perpetrator medical treatment and, particularly
related risk factor.
as more rude than those in other in the intensive care context studied,
conditions), but by making team potentially catastrophic clinical Third, these findings are also
members more resilient to the outcomes. Moreover, we also important in that they suggest this
appraised rudeness; in particular, the demonstrated that these deleterious iatrogenesis-related risk factor
intervention mitigated the degree effects of rudeness are not restricted may indeed be “treatable” on the
to which such appraisals adversely to individuals as has been shown basis of “immunization” approaches
affected their ability to engage in previously16 but also to teams. structured around cognitive bias
team processes (eg, workload and This is important because, based modification. Indeed, our finding
information sharing) central to on the assumption that teams can that a 20-minute “computer game”
timely and accurate diagnosis and often overcome and compensate for generated a sustained (daylong)
error-free intervention. In contrast, individual performance limitations, mitigation effect suggests that
by the end of the day, those in the medical work is increasingly being contextual rudeness might be more
narrative condition did not view the structured around teams.45–48 Our amenable to intervention than are
mother as more rude than controls. findings question the generalizability many other iatrogenesis-related
These results suggest that although of this assumption in situations risk factors identified in previous
writing about the experience from in which rudeness is prevalent research (eg, patient overcrowding,
the mother’s perspective facilitated because they demonstrate that, physician workload, patient

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


8 RISKIN et al
morbidity characteristics, length In conclusion, the findings presented Medical Center, Jerusalem;
of hospitalization). Nevertheless, here suggest that the deleterious Dr Bernard Barzilay and Ms Sophy
because daily CBM treatments are effects of rudeness on medical Dombe from Assaf Haroefe Medical
unlikely to be feasible or efficacious, performance are no less severe on Center, Tzrifin, Rishon Lezion; Israel.
additional research is needed to teams than they are on individuals We are also grateful to the
estimate the robustness of the and that these effects are not participating teams from the neonatal
exhibited mitigation effects over specific to any particular type of intensive care units of the following
longer periods of time and source. Moreover, beyond its direct hospitals in Israel: Ziv Medical
identify alternative CBM effect on cognitive functions and on Center, Tzfat; Haemek Medical
approaches that might offer a performance of manual procedural Center, Afula; English Hospital,
more sustained impact. skills, we showed that exposure Nazareth; Bnai Zion Medical Center,
to rudeness debilitates the very Haifa; Rambam Medical Center,
Furthermore, although researchers
collaborative mechanisms, such as Haifa; Carmel Medical Center, Haifa;
have hinted that contextual
communication, workload sharing, Hillel Yaffe Medical Center, Hadera;
adversity may result in heightened
and helping, assumed to make Laniado Medical Center, Netanyah;
rates of iatrogenesis by affecting
team-based medical care more Meir Medical Center, Kfar Sava;
individual-level cognitive processing
effective and safe than that offered by Schneider Childrens’ Hospital, Petach
and team-level collaborative
individual care providers. However, Tiqua; Sheba Medical Center, Tel
processes,2,16,17,45,48 our findings
our findings also offer some basis Hashomer, Ramat Gan; Lis Maternity
identified and documented several
for optimism: although it may be Hospital, Tel Aviv; Wolfsson Medical
of the collaborative processes most
impossible to prevent patients and Center, Holon; Haddasah Medical
directly and adversely affected by
contextual rudeness. Indeed, an their families from being rude to Center, Jerusalem; Shhare Zedek
understanding of the collaborative care providers, we may be able to Medical Center, Jerusalem; Kaplan
mechanisms involved may facilitate “immunize” these same providers Medical Center, Rehovot; Barzilay
the development of proactive and against the adverse implications of Medical Center, Ashkelon; Soroka
reactive training and protocol such behavior on the performance Medical Center, Beer Sheva.
interventions designed either to of care providers and the teams to
We appreciate the assistance of the
strengthen these team processes or which they belong.
professional team at Israel Center for
to compensate for them when they Medical Simulation in the preparation
are weakened by the team’s exposure and operation of the simulation
to rudeness. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS scenarios. We thank Professor Yair
These study implications stand Bar-Haim and his PhD student
in stark contrast to the policy We are grateful to the judges who Dr. Keren Maoz from the laboratory
recommendations proposed by evaluated the teams after each for research on anxiety and trauma,
leading health care think tanks simulation and conducted the School of Psychological Sciences
with regard to patient safety, many reflexivity-based debriefing: Dr Omer and Sagol School of Neuroscience,
of which emphasize the need to Globus and Ms Sarah Meir from Tel Aviv University, Israel for their
enhance medical team engagement Sheba Medical Center, Tel Hashomer; insightful and helpful comments on
and patient focus.49 By highlighting Dr Ilan Segal from Barzilay Medical this manuscript. We thank Dr. Ellen
the impact that adverse social Center, Ashkelon; Dr Arye Simmonds Bamberger from the department of
contexts may have on team-level from Laniado Medical Center, Pediatrics and the Infection Control
coordinative processes, our Netanyah; Ms Limor Partom from unit at Bnai Zion Medical Center for
findings provide the foundation Bnai Zion Medical Center, Haifa; revising and professionally editing
for a wide range of interventions Dr Irit Berger and Ms Anna Shtamler our manuscript.
aimed at enhancing patient safety. from Lis Maternity Hospital,
Our results suggest that instituting Sorasky Medical Center, Tel Aviv;
protocols and procedures aimed at Dr Smadar Even-Tov Friedman, ABBREVIATIONS
bolstering the defenses of medical Dr Ofra Peleg, Ms Deena Schwartz and
ANOVA: analysis of variance
teams to the cognitive distraction Ms Olga Gorodetzky from Haddassah
CBM: cognitive bias modification
and drain elicited by rudeness Medical Center, Jerusalem; Dr Dan
ICC: intraclass correlation
exposure can help mitigate the Waisman and Ms Lina Khoury from
coefficient
devastating consequences of these Carmel Medical Center, Haifa;
MANOVA: multivariate analysis
events, even when they cannot be Dr Reuven Bromiker and Ms Adina
of variance
prevented. Gale Dorembus from Shaare Zedek

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


PEDIATRICS Volume 139, number 2, February 2017 9
Address correspondence to Arieh Riskin, MD, MHA, Department of Neonatology, Bnai-Zion Medical Center, 47 Golomb St, POB 4940, Haifa 31048, Israel. E-mail: arik.
riskin@gmail.com
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2017 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: Professor Bamberger has support from the Israel Science Foundation research grant 1217/13, Israel Academy of Science and Humanities for this work.
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

REFERENCES
1. Porath CL, Pearson CM. The cost of bad 12. Lim S, Cortina LM, Magley VJ. Personal the establishment of novel treatment
behavior. Organ Dyn. 2010;39(1):64–71 and workgroup incivility: impact on for anxiety. Biol Psychiatry.
work and health outcomes. J Appl 2010;68(11):982–990
2. Flin R. Rudeness at work. BMJ.
Psychol. 2008;93(1):95–107
2010;340:c2480 22. Gopher D, Weil M, Bareket T. Transfer of
3. Blum LA, Hunt DW, Hanks J, et al. 13. Porath CL, Erez A. Does rudeness really skill from a computer game trainer to
Rude awakenings. Emerg Med Serv. matter? The effects of rudeness on flight. Hum Factors. 1994;36(3):387–405
1995;24(5):31–34, 73–75, 80–85 task performance and helpfulness.
Acad Manage J. 2007;50(5):1181–1197 23. Lynch J, Aughwane P, Hammond TM.
4. Rosenstein AH, O’Daniel M. Disruptive Video games and surgical ability:
behavior and clinical outcomes: 14. Porath CL, Erez A. Overlooked but not
untouched: how rudeness reduces a literature review. J Surg Educ.
perceptions of nurses and physicians. 2010;67(3):184–189
Am J Nurs. 2005;105(1):54–64, quiz onlookers’ performance on routine
64–65 and creative tasks. Organiz Behav Hum 24. Shebilske WL, Volz RA, Gildea KM,
Dec Proc. 2009;109(1):29–44 et al. Revised space fortress: a
5. Rosenstein AH, O’Daniel M. Impact and
implications of disruptive behavior 15. Rafaeli A, Erez A, Ravid S, Derfler- validation study. Behav Res Methods.
in the perioperative arena. J Am Coll Rozin R, Treister DE, Scheyer R. When 2005;37(4):591–601
Surg. 2006;203(1):96–105 customers exhibit verbal aggression,
employees pay cognitive costs. J Appl 25. Kearns MC, Edwards KM, Calhoun
6. Rosenstein AH, O’Daniel M. A survey Psychol. 2012;97(5):931–950 KS, Gidycz CA. Disclosure of
of the impact of disruptive behaviors sexual victimization: the effects of
and communication defects on patient 16. Riskin A, Erez A, Foulk TA, et al. The Pennebaker’s emotional disclosure
safety. Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf. impact of rudeness on medical team paradigm on physical and
2008;34(8):464–471 performance: a randomized trial. psychological distress. J Trauma
Pediatrics. 2015;136(3):487–495 Dissociation. 2010;11(2):193–209
7. Hutton S, Gates D. Workplace incivility
and productivity losses among direct 17. Suresh G, Horbar JD, Plsek P,
26. Meston CM, Lorenz TA, Stephenson
care staff. AAOHN J. 2008;56(4):168–175 et al. Voluntary anonymous
KR. Effects of expressive writing on
reporting of medical errors for
8. Anthony M, Yastik J. Nursing sexual dysfunction, depression, and
neonatal intensive care. Pediatrics.
students’ experiences with incivility PTSD in women with a history of
2004;113(6):1609–1618
in clinical education. J Nurs Educ. childhood sexual abuse: results from
2011;50(3):140–144 18. Cristea IA, Kok RN, Cuijpers P. Efficacy a randomized clinical trial. J Sex Med.
of cognitive bias modification 2013;10(9):2177–2189
9. Lewis PS, Malecha A. The impact
interventions in anxiety and
of workplace incivility on the 27. Brown EJ, Heimberg RG. Effects
depression: meta-analysis. Br J
work environment, manager skill, of writing about rape: evaluating
Psychiatry. 2015;206(1):7–16
and productivity. J Nurs Adm. Pennebaker’s paradigm with a
2011;41(1):41–47 19. Hallion LS, Ruscio AM. A meta- severe trauma. J Trauma Stress.
analysis of the effect of cognitive 2001;14(4):781–790
10. Holloway E, Kusy M. Systems
bias modification on anxiety
approach to address incivility and
and depression. Psychol Bull. 28. Pennebaker JW. Putting stress
disruptive behaviors in health-care
2011;137(6):940–958 into words: health, linguistic, and
organizations. Adv Health Care Manag.
2011;10:239–265 20. Bar-Haim Y. Research review: attention therapeutic implications. Behav Res
bias modification (ABM): a novel Ther. 1993;31(6):539–548
11. Spence Laschinger HK, Leiter M, Day
treatment for anxiety disorders. J Child
A, Gilin D. Workplace empowerment, 29. Pennebaker JW. The effects of
Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(8):859–870
incivility, and burnout: impact traumatic disclosure on physical and
on staff nurse recruitment and 21. Hakamata Y, Lissek S, Bar-Haim Y, mental health: the values of writing
retention outcomes. J Nurs Manag. et al. Attention bias modification and talking about upsetting events. Int
2009;17(3):302–311 treatment: a meta-analysis toward J Emerg Ment Health. 1999;1(1):9–18

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


10 RISKIN et al
30. Pennebaker JW, Seagal JD. Forming a 37. Beer M. Silent killers of team Organizations. San Francisco, CA:
story: the health benefits of narrative. performance: How honest, collective, Jossey-Bass; 2000:349–381
J Clin Psychol. 1999;55(10):1243–1254 public conversations can overcome 44. Kahneman D. Thinking, Fast and Slow.
31. Ziv A, Erez D, Munz Y, et al. The Israel them. In: Salas E, Tannenbaum SI, New York, NY: Farrar, Straus & Giroux;
Center for Medical Simulation: Cohen D, Latham G, eds. Developing 2011
a paradigm for cultural change and Enhancing Teamwork in
Organizations—Evidence-Based Best 45. Burtscher MJ, Manser T, Kolbe M,
in medical education. Acad Med. et al. Adaptation in anaesthesia
2006;81(12):1091–1097 Practice and Guidelines. San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass, Wiley; 2013:27–47 team coordination in response
32. Vashdi DR, Bamberger PA, Erez M, to a simulated critical event and
Weiss-Meilik A. Briefing-debriefing: 38. Chiocchio F. Project team performance: its relationship to clinical
using a reflexive organizational a study of electronic task and performance. Br J Anaesth.
learning model from the military coordination communication. Proj 2011;106(6):801–806
to enhance the performance of Manage J. 2007;38(1):97–109
46. Cohen SG, Bailey DE. What makes
surgical teams. Hum Resour Manage. 39. Marks MA, Mathieu JE, Zaccaro SJ. teams work: group effectiveness
2007;46(1):115–142 A temporally based framework and research from the shop floor to
33. Bunderson JS, Sutcliffe KM. Comparing taxonomy of team processes. Acad the executive suite. J Manage.
alternative conceptualizations of Manage Rev. 2001;26(3):356–376 1997;23(3):239–290
functional diversity in management 40. van Ginkel WP, van Knippenberg D. 47. Espinosa JA, Lerch FJ, Kraut RE.
teams: Process and performance Knowledge about the distribution Explicit vs. implicit coordination
effects. Acad Manage J. of information and group decision mechanisms and task dependencies:
2002;45(5):875–893 making: when and why does it work? One size does not fit all. In: Salas
34. Geller D, Bamberger PA. The impact Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. E, Fiore SM, eds. Team Cognition:
of help seeking on individual task 2009;108(2):218–229 Understanding the Factors That
performance: the moderating effect of 41. Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, et al. Drive Process and Performance.
help seekers’ logics of action. J Appl The NimStim set of facial expressions: Washington, DC: APA Books;
Psychol. 2012;97(2):487–497 judgments from untrained research 2004:107–129
35. Nahum-Shani I, Bamberger PA. participants. Psychiatry Res. 48. Riethmüller M, Fernandez Castelao E,
Explaining the variable effects of social 2009;168(3):242–249 Eberhardt I, Timmermann A, Boos M.
support on work-based stressor-strain 42. Colquitt JA, Conlon DE, Wesson Adaptive coordination development
relations: the role of perceived pattern MJ, Porter CO, Ng KY. Justice at in student anaesthesia teams: a
of support exchange. Organ Behav the millennium: a meta-analytic longitudinal study. Ergonomics.
Hum Decis Process. 2011;114(1):49–63 review of 25 years of organizational 2012;55(1):55–68
36. Nahum-Shani I, Bamberger PA, justice research. J Appl Psychol. 49. Leape L, Berwick D, Clancy C,
Bacharach SB. Social support and 2001;86(3):425–445 et al; Lucian Leape Institute at the
employee well-being: the conditioning 43. Bliese PD. Within-group agreement, National Patient Safety Foundation.
effect of perceived patterns of non-independence, and reliability. In: Transforming healthcare: a safety
supportive exchange. J Health Soc Klein K, Kozlowski S, eds. Multi-Level imperative. Qual Saf Health Care.
Behav. 2011;52(1):123–139 Theory, Research, and Methods in 2009;18(6):424–428

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


PEDIATRICS Volume 139, number 2, February 2017 11
Rudeness and Medical Team Performance
Arieh Riskin, Amir Erez, Trevor A. Foulk, Kinneret S. Riskin-Geuz, Amitai Ziv, Rina
Sela, Liat Pessach-Gelblum and Peter A. Bamberger
Pediatrics 2017;139;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-2305 originally published online January 10, 2017;

Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/139/2/e20162305
References This article cites 45 articles, 8 of which you can access for free at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/139/2/e20162305#BIBL
Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the
following collection(s):
Administration/Practice Management
http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/administration:practice
_management_sub
Risk Management
http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/risk_management_sub
Safety
http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/safety_sub
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or
in its entirety can be found online at:
http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020


Rudeness and Medical Team Performance
Arieh Riskin, Amir Erez, Trevor A. Foulk, Kinneret S. Riskin-Geuz, Amitai Ziv, Rina
Sela, Liat Pessach-Gelblum and Peter A. Bamberger
Pediatrics 2017;139;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-2305 originally published online January 10, 2017;

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/139/2/e20162305

Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it
has been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by
the American Academy of Pediatrics, 345 Park Avenue, Itasca, Illinois, 60143. Copyright © 2017
by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1073-0397.

Downloaded from www.aappublications.org/news at Unicamp on November 3, 2020

You might also like