You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222

www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Noise, psychosocial stress and their interaction in the workplace


Phil Leather*, Diane Beale, Lucy Sullivan
Institute of Work, Health and Organizations, University of Nottingham, Business School, Jubilee Campus, Wollaton Road, Nottingham NG8 1BB, UK
Received 6 September 2002; accepted 16 September 2002

Abstract

Occupational noise exposure has been linked with a range of negative health effects, with recent research emphasizing the need to
understand the full context of sound events in explaining these relationships. An emerging theme within environmental psychology
argues that physical features of the environment might work both directly on outcomes and interactively with either psychosocial
work elements. In the present study, the interaction of noise with psychosocial job stress was explored for 128 office workers
employed by a government agency in a city in the Midlands region of the UK. The results showed no direct effect of ambient noise
levels upon job satisfaction, well-being or organizational commitment. However, lower levels of ambient noise were found to buffer
the negative impact of psychosocial job stress upon these same three outcomes. Psychosocial job stress is, therefore, seen as a
valuable heuristic in operationazing the context of sound events at work.
r 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction two sources: telephones ringing and piped-in back-


ground music. In this same study, environmental
Exposure to occupational noise, that is, unwanted satisfaction, but not overall job satisfaction, was also
sound, has been linked with variety of adverse effects found to be inversely related to the amount of noise
upon well-being over and above its obvious relationship deriving from people talking on the telephone and from
with hearing loss (Kryter, 1970, 1994). In industrial typewriters.
settings, for example, noise exposure has been found to Importantly, many studies have also demonstrated
be associated with a range of indicators of physical that these various negative effects of exposure to
health, including cardiac problems (Jansen, 1961; occupational noise are often contingent upon either
Cuesdan et al., 1977); sickness-related absenteeism the characteristics of the task at hand, or features of the
(Cohen,1973); and self-reported fatigue (Carlestam, broader work context. Nurminen and Kurppa (1989),
Karlslon, & Levi, 1973). Similarly, McDonald (1989) for example, reported that pregnancy complications in
reported a dose-response relationship among blue collar women working under very noisy conditions were
workers between occupational noise exposure and exacerbated when they were also subjected to the
symptoms of psychological distress. That noise has a additional demands of shiftwork. Indeed, the interactive
negative impact upon job satisfaction is well documen- effect of occupational noise exposure and shiftwork
ted (Langdon, 1966; Nemecek & Grandjean, 1973; upon various health indices is a consistently reported
Boyce, 1974). Office workers, in particular, consistently theme in the literature (see, for example, Cesana et al.,
report ‘‘the ability to concentrate without noise and 1982; Ottman, Rutenfranz, Neidhart, & Boucsein,
other distractions’’ to be one of the most important (1987).
aspects of the work environment (Louis Harris & Carter and Beh (1989) and Mosskov and Ettema
Associates, 1978, 1980). In a survey of office renova- (1977) both report that the usual rapid habituation in
tions, Sundstrom, Town, Rice, Osborn, and Brill (1994) elevated cardiovascular responses to acute noise ex-
found declining job satisfaction amongst those office posure is blocked when people are simultaneously
workers who experienced an increase in noise from performing demanding cognitive tasks. A similar inter-
active effect of noise and task demands was reported by
*Corresponding author. Tel.: +44-115-84-66638. Welch (1979) who found, in a study of Russian
E-mail address: phil.leather@nottingham.ac.uk (P. Leather). manufacturing plant, elevated cardiovascular functioning

0272-4944/03/$ - see front matter r 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 2 7 2 - 4 9 4 4 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 0 8 2 - 8
214 P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222

only amongst those workers exposed to both louder hypothesized between noise and a particular psychoso-
noise and higher levels of workload demands. Likewise, cial factor, i.e. job stress.
Cottington, Matthews, Talbott and Kuller (1983) Stress is operationalized in terms of Karasek’s model
reported a significant interaction between noise and of job strain (karasek, 1979; Karasek & Theorell, 1990),
job stress on diastolic blood pressure levels in a further wherein strain results from the interaction of high job
sample of manufacturing workers. Lercher, Hortnagl, demands and low decision latitude. If noise does interact
and Kofler (1993) found that a small positive association with job stress, then any negative impact should be
between noise exposure and diastolic blood pressure was greatest under conditions of both high stress and higher
amplified among those workers who also reported low noise levels. Put another way, lower levels of noise might
levels of social support on the job. serve to buffer any negative effect of job strain More-
As with its effects upon health outcomes, so the over, this pattern of results should hold across a range of
influence of occupational noise exposure upon perfor- negative outcomes commonly associated with occupa-
mance has been found to be contingent upon a number tional stress, such as reduced job satisfaction, impaired
of factors, including the nature of the noise and the type health and well-being, and increased psychological
of task involved. Essentially, two summary conclusions withdrawal (Cox, 1978, 1985; Cooper, 1985; Beehr,
can be drawn from the many laboratory investigations 1995).
of the negative effects of noise upon performance. First, As reviewed above, there are several lines of evidence
that the effects of unpredictable noise are more severe in the research literature that implicitly or explicitly
than are those of predictable noise. Second, that any support this noise  job stress interaction hypothesis.
negative effect of noise increases with task complexity. Laboratory studies, for example, have shown significant,
Thus, it is the interaction of unpredictable noise and increases in cardiovascular and neuroendocrine func-
high task complexity which has been found to result in tioning to occur when individuals are exposed to noise
increased errors in calculation, tracking and monitoring during demanding mental tasks (Tafalla, Evan, &
tasks, slower learning of new material and poorer recall Cohen, 1988). Shift workers are similarly at greater risk
and memorization (Sundstrom, 1986). of health complaints when working in noisy occupa-
The overall conclusion to be drawn from the research tional settings as compared to relatively quiet ambient
investigating the effects of occupational noise upon both conditions (Manninen, 1990). Matthews, Cottington,
well-being and performance is that the full context of the Talbott, Kuller, and Siegel (1987) found a parallel
situation needs to be taken into account when trying to interaction between noise and job satisfaction, such that
understand those effects. Further evidence for this noise heightened job dissatisfaction only amongst those
conclusion comes from the fact that technical noise workers who were also unhappy with their jobs.
measures explain only part of the inter-individual Leather, Pyrgas, Beale, and Lawrence (1998) explored
variance reported in noise annoyance (Kjellberg, Land- as similar interaction between windows in the workplace
strom, Tesarz, Soderberg, & Akerlund, 1996). As and job stress as that now being investigated between
Kjellberg et al. (1996) point out; nonphysical noise noise and job stress. Postulating multiple influencing
characteristics and other situational and individual mechanisms by which windows might impact upon work
characteristics are also of great importance in determin- outcomes, they found that sunlight penetration had a
ing subjective responses to noise. There is, they argue, a direct effect on job satisfaction, intention to quit and
fundamental need to understand the full context of general well-being. Access to a view of nature, on the
sound events when researching the possible effects of other hand, was found to buffer the negative impact of
noise. The potential for noise to interact with task job stress on intention to quit and to have a similar,
constraints and other situational variables represents a marginal, effect on general well-being.
major feature of this context. In light of the growing recognition of the importance
Evans and Lepore (1992) and Evans, Johansson, and of possible interactions between physical and psychoso-
Carrere (1994) offer a conceptual and methodological cial work elements, two specific hypotheses are tested in
framework that provides a valuable way of investigating this paper with respect to the impact of occupational
the context of sound events in the workplace. Any noise exposure:
feature of the physical environment, they argue, might
work both directly on outcomes and/or interactively Hypothesis 1. Higher noise levels will be associated with
with either (a) psychosocial work elements or (b) other lower job satisfaction, lower organizational commit-
physical elements. It is with the first of these possible ment and poorer well-being (main effects).
interactions that the present paper is concerned.
Specifically, the idea to be explored here is that the Hypothesis 2. Noise exposure and job stress will interact
effect of noise upon work outcomes might be either such that lower levels of noise will buffer any negative
direct or indirect in nature. In suggesting a possible effect of job strain, organizational commitment and
indirect influencing mechanism, an interaction is being well-being (moderator effect).
P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222 215

It should also be recognized that a direct effect of job complete the questionnaire within office hours and to
strain upon job satisfaction, organizational commitment post it, in the envelope provided, into sealed cardboard
and well-being is tested in this moderator model (Baron post-boxes located in each building.
& Kenny, 1986; Evans & Lepore, 1992; Evans et al., Physical noise measurements were taken at each
1994). However, since the focus of this paper is upon the employee’s workstation. To help ensure confidentiality,
impact of noise with job strain being used as a means of each subject was given a unique code number, known
operationalizing the importance of context, such direct only to the research team. This number was entered on
effects will only be commented upon in passing. each questionnaire and was used to match questionnaire
responses to noise measurements. Seating plans were
drawn for all work stations within the two buildings.
2. Method Each workstation was then allocated two numbers, one
for the desk number and the other for the coded
2.1. Location and subjects identification number for the person sitting there. This
allowed identification and compilation of the various
The study was carried out within the offices of a local objective and subjective measures on a subject by subject
government finance department situated in a city center basis.
location in the Midlands area of the UK. The employees The front cover of the questionnaire provided details
sampled worked in two separated buildings located on as to how it should be returned, descriptive information
either side of a narrow street. Both buildings were air- about the aims and purposes of the study, and an
conditioned and had windows that gave access entirely assurance of confidentiality. When the questionnaires
to similar urban views. were collected, subjects were debriefed at length about
All employees working in the two buildings were the exact hypotheses being tested in the study and what
given a questionnaire. A total of 143 questionnaires would happen to the data collected.
were distributed and 128 were returned, giving a
response rate 90%. Subjects varied in age from 21 to 2.3. Questionnaire
76 years, with a mean age of 35 years. Forty-four
percent of the respondents were male and 56% were In full, the questionnaire comprised eight sections that
female. The majority of the subjects (88%) were measured various features of the physical work envir-
employed in clerical work while the remainder (12%) onment, job design characteristics, attitudes to work and
were employed in a variety of leadership or supervisory the workplace, and self-reported health and well-being.
roles. The mean length of service with organization was Each section comprised either an established, or
7 years 8 months, with a mean tenure in the current job adapted, scale, or a bespoke series of questions tailored
of 4 years. No significant differences were found to the needs of the overall investigation. The present
between employees working in the two buildings with paper is concerned only with a specific subset of the
respect to the kind of work done (operationalized as derived variables, namely those measures relating to the
frequencies of job categories) (w2=1.14, n.s.), tenure impact of job strain and noise upon job satisfaction,
within the current department (t=1.48, df=122, n.s.), organizational commitment, and self-reported health
gender composition (w2=1.14, n.s.), or age composition and well-being.
(t=1.56, df=125, n.s.). The employees sampled in each
building could, therefore, be taken as equivalent in 2.4. Measures
terms of their demographic characteristics.
Subjective noise: Employees were asked to give a
2.2. Procedure subjective rating of how noisy they considered their
work environment to be, indicating this response on a 7-
A series of semi-structured interview was initially point Likert scale where 1=‘‘not at all noisy’’ and
conducted with a representative sample of employees in 7=‘‘extremely noisy’’. Five pre-determined sources of
each building, in order to reveal any particular issues of noise were each rated in terms of the frequency with
concern regarding the physical environment of their which they disturbed work concentration. These five
workplace. A draft questionnaire was then designed and sources were: air conditioning, telephones, office ma-
piloted amongst a small number of office employees, chines, people talking and noise from the street. In each
primarily with a view to establishing its face validity. case, a 7-point rating scale was used, where 1=‘‘never
Following several familiarization visits to each building, disturbs’’ and 7=‘‘continuously disturbs’’.
during which one of the investigators spent a short Objective noise: General levels of sound were mea-
amount of time explaining the purpose of the study to sured objectively using a portable Bruel . and Kjaer
each employee, the final questionnaire was distributed sound level meter (type 2205). Ambient noise readings
by hand to all 143 employees. They were asked to were taken at each workstation on four separate
216 P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222

occasions during the 2 week data gathering period. expressing positive or negative commitment, to which
On each occasion, readings were taken over a 20 respondents are asked to rate their agreement on a
interval. For each 20 interval, the highest and lowest 7-point scale where 1=‘‘strongly disagree’’ and
reading were noted and the average calculated. Readings 7=‘‘strongly agree’’. Negative items are reverse scored
were taken in the morning (between 10.00 and 12.00) such that high scores indicate high commitment (range
and in the afternoon (between 14.15 and 16.15) on two 9–63).
separate days of similar weather conditions (cold and
overcast). On the second day, the order of taking the
readings was reversed. An overall average noise level per 3. Results
workstation was computed from the four readings
taken. 3.1. Reliability analyses
Job strain: Job strain was measured using Krasek’s
(1979) scales of job demand and decision latitude, which As Table 1 shows, all the measures displayed
utilize a 5-point response format. The 6-item scale of job satisfactory reliability, having Cronbach’s alpha relia-
demand (range: 6–30) focused on time pressure and bility coefficients in excess of the minimum recom-
workload, whereas the 7-item scale of job decision mended value of 0.7 (Nunally, 1978), with the exception
latitude (range: 7–35) comprised both control and of Karasek’s decision latitude scale. The decision
decision making at work. For each scale, a higher score latitude scale was, therefore, subjected to exploratory
denoted a greater amount of that characteristic. factor analysis, the results of which are detailed in Table
Following Karasek (1979) and Leather et al. (1998), 2. The 5-item single factor solution that resulted was
job strain was calculated as ‘‘relative excess’’ interaction used in the subsequent calculation of job strain, in
between mean job demand and mean decision latitude. preference to the original 7-item scale. With a coefficient
This basic formula, that is mean demand minus mean alpha of 0.72, this reduced length scale displayed
decision latitude, was then mathematically transformed adequate reliability.
to remove negative values (by squaring) and preserve
scale range (by taking the square root). The precise 3.2. Descriptive statistics
formula used to calculate job strain was therefore Job
strain=? (demand mean item score—decision latitude Descriptive statistics for the study sample as a whole
mean item score)2. In this way, job stress is represented are provided in Table 3 (objective sound levels and
as any imbalance between demands and decision subjective noise ratings) and Table 4 (psychometric
latitude, that is as either overload or underload (Cox, scales).
1978). From Table 3, it can be seen that a range of objective
Job satisfaction: Job satisfaction was assessed using noise levels were experienced by employees in the two
Warr, Cook, and Wall (1979) 15-item scale, each item of buildings, although all levels fell well below the UK and
which measures a different element of intrinsic or US legal limits of 90 decibels (Bell, Fisher, Baum, and
extrinsic satisfaction. Each item uses a 7-point response Greene, 1990; Occupational Safety & Health Adminis-
format (range: 15–105) with higher scores denoting tration, 1994; Health & Safety Executive, 1999). Inter-
greater job satisfaction. estingly, subjective appraisals of these ambient noise
Health and well-being: Subjects’ self-reported health levels varied widely, from ‘‘not at all noisy’’ to
and general well-being were measured using Ferguson’s ‘‘extremely noisy’’. However, the overall mean rating
(Ferguson, 1992; Ferguson & Cox, 1994) symptoms of 4.48 was broadly in accord with the mean objective
of infection diseases (SID) scale. This measure lists 17 sound level of 55.08 DB, which is neither quiet nor
common symptoms of sub-optimum health, with unduly noisy.
respondents being asked to rate the frequency From Table 4, it can be seen that, whereas the mean
with which they have experienced each symptom level of decision latitude was close to the scale midpoint,
‘‘over the last six months’’. Ratings are given on a the average level of job demand was almost six scale
6-point scale where 0=‘‘never’’ and 5=‘‘every day’’. points above the midpoint. Transformed to a mean item
Example items include: ‘‘Felt feverish’’, ‘‘Had aches and score, the ratio of job demand to decision latitude was
pains in my muscles and joints’’, ‘‘Had a sore throat’’, 3.94:3.11 (i.e. 1.27:1), from which it can plausibly be
‘‘Felt chilled and shivery’’ and ‘‘Had a reduced concluded that low to moderate levels of job strain
appetite’’. prevailed. Notwithstanding this likelihood, mean levels
Organizational commitment: Organizational commit- of job satisfaction and organizational commitment were
ment was measured using Cook and Wall’s (1980) both within three scale points of their respective
9-item scale. This measure taps the three core elements midpoints. Noticeably, however, the frequency with
of organizational commitment: identification, involve- which common ill-health symptoms were reported was
ment and loyalty. Each item is made up of a statement considerably above the scale mean.
P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222 217

Table 1 3.3. Sources and levels of noise


Scale reliabilities for job strain and outcome variables

Variable Coefficient a In order to identify the relative weighting of the


possible contributors to ambient noise levels, a multiple
Job strain variables
linear regression was performed with objective noise
Job demand 0.84
Decision latitude 0.39 levels as the dependent variable and the ratings of noise
Outcome variables disturbance from air conditioning, telephones, office
Job satisfaction 0.81 machines, people talking and street noise as the
Organizational commitment 0.79 predictors. Table 5 shows that, taken together, the
Symptoms of infectious diseases 0.78
predictors accounted for nearly 46% of the variance in
ambient noise level. However, only air conditioning
made a significant independent prediction (b=0.67,
t=9.42, po0.001), the size of the b weight indicating a
Table 2 moderate to strong relationship between ratings of the
Item-factor loadings and summary statistics for principal components noise disturbance it caused and objectively assessed
analysis of Karasek’s decision latitude scale (Varimax rotation with
Kaiser 1 rule of extraction)
ambient noise levels.
A second multiple linear regression analysis was
Item Factor loading performed utilizing the same predictors, but with the
Does your job require creativity? 0.804 single item rating of perceived office noise as the
Is a high level of skill required by your job? 0.741 dependent variable. Table 6 shows that, taken together,
Does your job involve nonrepetitive work?: 0.640 the predicators accounted for 38% of the variance in
Does your job require you to learn new things? 0.635
Do you have any say over what happens at work? 0.620
noise ratings. As with the prediction of objectively
measured ambient noise levels, the disturbance caused
Eigen value=2.39 by air conditioning made the greatest independent
Percentage of variance accounted for=47.87 contribution to noise ratings (b=0.52, t=6.88,
Note: Kaiser–Mayer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy=0.76; po0.001), although street noise now made an additional
Bartlett’s test of sphericity=116.714, N=128, po0.001; subject: significant contribution (b=0.18, t=2.41, po0.05).
variable ratio=18.29:1. Two Karasek items were discarded due to These various data demonstrate that the two build-
cross loading on initial factor solution (‘‘Do you have the freedom to ings housed similar employees (in terms of age, gender
work the way you want?’’ and ‘‘Does your job allow you to make a lot
of decisions?’’); final solution yielded a single factor so could not be
rotated.
Table 5
Multiple regression results using noise disturbance ratings as
predictors of objective ambient noise levels
Table 3 Predictors R R2 F b T
Minimum and maximum values, means and standard deviations for nnn
objectively and subjectively assessed ambient noise levels Air conditioning 0.68 0.46 19.95 0.67 9.42nnn
Telephones 0.09 1.21
Ambient noise levels Office machines 0.12 1.59
People talking 0.07 0.91
Variable Minimum Maximum Mean s.d. Street noise 0.01 0.11
Objective level of noise (dB) 45.75 62.50 55.08 4.36
n
Subjective noise rating 1 7 4.48 1.36 Po0.05.
nn
Po0.01.
nnn
Po0.001.

Table 6
Table 4 Multiple regression results using noise disturbance ratings as
Means, standard deviations and scale range for job strain and outcome predictors of subjective ambient noise ratings
variables
Predictors R R2 F b T
Variable Mean s.d. Scale range nnn
Air conditioning 0.61 0.38 14.27 0.52 6.88nnn
Job strain variables Telephones 0.01 0.06
Job demand 23.63 3.83 6–30 Office machines 0.07 0.86
Decision latitude 15.53 3.40 5–25 People talking 0.10 1.18
Outcome variables Street noise 0.18 2.41n
Job satisfaction 58.13 13.12 15–105 n
Po0.05.
Organizational commitment 39.08 9.5 9–63 nn
Po0.01.
Symptoms of infectious diseases 51.62 23.83 0–85 nnn
Po0.001.
218 P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222

composition and tenure) doing similar work, under a Table 7


range of objectively measured and subjectively perceived Analysis of variance results for job strain and ambient noise level upon
noise levels. However, ambient noise levels were outcome variables
significantly higher in one building than in the other. Source F value
This effect was consistent for both objectively measured
Job satisfaction Organizational Symptoms of
noise levels (t= 24.23, df=126, po0.001) and the df(1,124) commitment infectious
subjective ratings of noise given by their inhabitants df(1,124) diseases df(1,124)
(t= 4.86, df=126, po0.001). Despite this difference Job strain 26.03nnn 9.07nnn 8.50nn
between buildings in terms of mean noise levels, there Noise (dB) 0.42 1.48 2.16
Job strain 20.33nnn 5.69n 4.20n
was significant overlap of individually experienced noise
x noise
levels across the two buildings. Accordingly, the (dB)
manipulation of noise in the following moderation n
Po0.05.
analyses was done via a median split for the aggregated nn
Po0.01.
sample rather than according to building. nnn
Po0.001.

3.4. Moderation analyses


Second, objective noise levels showed no direct effect
Following Baron and Kenny (1986), the possible main upon any of the dependent measures. There was,
(direct) and interaction (indirect) effects of job strain however, a significant interaction between job strain
and noise on job satisfaction, organizational commit- and noise for job satisfaction (F(1,124)=20.33,
ment, and self-reported health and well-being were po0.001), organizational commitment (F(1,124)=5.68,
investigated by means of three two-way between groups po0.05) and symptoms of ill-health (F(1,124)=4.20,
analyses of variance. The independent variables, job po0.05).
strain and noise, were each converted into dichotomous Fig. 1 illustrates the nature of the interaction between
variables by means of a median split technique job strain and objective level of noise on job satisfaction,
(‘‘low’’=below the median, ‘‘high’’=above the median). and shows that lower noise levels helped to buffer the
Only the objectively measured noise levels were used in negative impact of job strain on job satisfaction.
these analyses, in order to avoid any common method Detailed exploration of this interaction effect using
variance bias that might have been introduced by the use Tukey’s HSD test showed that job satisfaction remained
of subjectively rated noise levels. Within an ANOVA relatively constant under conditions of low noise,
model, a moderator or buffering effect is indicated by irrespective of level of job strain. Job satisfaction fell
the presence of a significant statistical interaction (Baron significantly, however, under conditions of high strain
& Kenny, 1986; Evans & Lepore, 1992; Evans et al., and high noise compared to high strain and low noise
1994). Such an interaction indicates that the effect of (po0.05).
predictor variable (e.g. job strain) on an outcome Fig. 2 illustrates the nature of the interaction between
variable (e.g. job satisfaction) differ as a function of job strain and noise on organizational commitment and
the moderator variable (e.g. noise) (hypothesis 2). shows a similar pattern of results to that found for job
Although greater statistical power might have been satisfaction. Post hoc comparisons using Tukey’s HSD
achieved by maintaining the continuous nature of the test again showed that while organizational commitment
variables and subjecting them to hierarchichal regres- remained relatively stable under conditions of low noise,
sion analyses, a two-way between subjects ANOVA was it varied significantly under high noise conditions
preferred in order to facilitate graphical representation (po0.05). It should perhaps be noted, at this point,
of the interaction effect. Baron and Kenny (1986) clearly that while Fig. 1 and 2 suggest the possibility of there
state that either regression or ANOVA analyses are being a sizeable improvement in job satisfaction scores
suitable in illustrating moderation effects. under conditions of low job strain and high noise,
Table 7 summarizes the results of the two-way compared to the combination of low strain and low
between subjects analyses of variance for the three noise, Tukey’s HSD test revealed these differences to be
outcome measures: job satisfaction, organizational nonsignificant.
commitment and self-reported health. From Table 7, a Fig. 3 illustrates the two-way interaction between job
consistent pattern of results can be discerned. First, job strain and noise level on the symptoms of infectious
strain showed a significant negative relationship with diseases measure. In keeping with the two previous
each of the outcome measures, such that increased strain buffering effects, Fig. 3 shows how lower noise levels
was associated with decreased job satisfaction and moderate the negative impact of job strain upon the
(F(1,124)=26.03, po0.001), decreased organizational frequency of reported symptoms. Specifically, post hoc
commitment (F(1,124)=9.07, po0.01) and increased comparisons with Tukey’s HSD test showed the
symptoms of ill health (F(1,124) = 8.498, po0.01). frequency of reported symptoms to be significantly
P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222 219

different between the high strain, high noise condition


and both the low strain, low noise and low strain, high
noise conditions (po0.05). As with job satisfaction and
organizational commitment, the frequency of reported
ill-health symptoms remains largely constant when
ambient noise levels are low.

4. Discussion

With respect to the impact of noise, the pattern of


results reported in this paper is consistent across the
three dependent measures used. Specifically, ambient
noise level was found to have no direct effect upon job
satisfaction, organizational commitment or self-reported
Fig. 1. The effects of job strain and ambient noise on job satisfaction.
symptoms of ill-health. Hypothesis 1 is therefore not
supported by the data. Ambient noise level did,
however, interact with job strain to produce a significant
effect upon all three outcomes. Hypothesis 2 is therefore
supported by the data.
That noise did not have any direct effect upon job
satisfaction, organizational commitment or self-reported
ill-health symptoms runs counter to the many research
studies reporting precisely such negative effects of
exposure to occupational noise (e.g. Nemecek & Grand-
jean, 1973; Miller, 1974; Hedge, 1982; McDonal, 1989;
Sundstrom et al., 1994). A possible reason why noise
may not have had any direct effects in the present study
concerns the actual levels of ambient noise to which
employees were exposed. As reported in Table 3,
ambient noise levels varied between 45.75 and 62.5 dB,
with a mean level of 55.08dB. Such levels are well below
both the 90 dB level at which damage to hearing usually
occurs (Bell et al., 1990) and the 55–70 dB range
Fig. 2. The effects of job strain and ambient on organizational reported by Beranek (1956, 1957) as acceptable for office
commitment. work.
As reported in Table 7, and illustrated in Figs. 1–3,
the nature of the interaction between ambient noise and
job strain is such that lower levels of noise help to
moderate, or buffer, the significant negative impact of
job strain upon job satisfaction, organizational commit-
ment and self-reported symptoms of ill-health. In
passing, it can be noted that the significant main effect
reported for job strain on all three dependent measures
offers continuing support for the validity of Karasek’s
job strain model (Karasek, 1979; Karasek & Theorell,
1990), albeit with a modified version of his decision
latitude scale.
This moderating effect of lower noise is in keeping
with Ulrich’s (1991) theory of supportive design. The
basic argument underpinning this theory is that the
concept of stress provides a useful heuristic by which to
explain how characteristics of the built environment
can affect human health and well-being (Evans &
Fig. 3. The effects of job strain and ambient noise on symptoms of McCoy, 1998). Evans (2000), in a detailed exposition
infectious diseases. of this overall framework, suggests that the physical
220 P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222

environment might be conceptualized either as a source current study cannot establish cause and effect relation-
of stress or as a source of coping resources. Given that ships. Rather than job strain determining job satisfac-
physical and psychosocial elements frequently interact tion, well-being, and organizational commitment, for
in their effect upon well-being (Evans & Lepore, 1992; example, it may be that job strain is adversely affected
Evans et al., 1994; Leather et al., 1998), this opens up by being uncommitted, unwell and dissatisfied with the
the possibility that while the physical characteristics of job. The exact direction of the posited relationships can
an environment might not be stressful in themselves, only be determined through longitudinal designs. It is
they nevertheless might act to exacerbate or attenuate equally plausible, for example, that it is levels of job
the negative impact of some co-occurring psychosocial strain which moderate the negative impact of occupa-
stress. tional noise exposure. In effect, an additional cost of
Applied to the current findings, this is to suggest that coping with job stress could be reduced tolerance for
while the existing ambient noise levels might not have negative physical parameters in the work setting, e.g.
been loud enough to be stressful in themselves, they noise. To some degree, however, specifying which
could, nevertheless, constitute an important backdrop to variable, physical or psychosocial, is the independent
attempts to cope with simultaneously occurring job variable and which is the moderator, is less important
strain. Thus, where both noise and job strain are at their than recognizing that interactively they contribute to the
highest, so coping resources are likely to be most total stress equation. Moreover, while this paper has
severely taxed and depleted. Hence, it is here that the argued that low noise moderates the impact of job
negative impact on job satisfaction, organizational strain, conversely, the data could equally be interpreted
commitment and self-reported health is found to be as evidence of the amplification of job strain effects by
greatest. noise. Furthermore, a plausible hypothesis not empiri-
A noisy physical environment, in effect, affords little cally accounted for in this study, is that arousal may
by way of assistance in coping with any co-occurring job serve as the explanatory variable linking noise and job
strain. Conversely, where ambient noise levels are strain with negative well-being and work attitudes. It
markedly lower, the negative impact of job strain is might be speculated that it is the combination of high
minimized, perhaps because fewer coping resources are strain and high noise levels that leads to heightened
needed to adapt to the demands of the physical levels of arousal which are in turn responsible for
environment. A greater percentage of coping resources decreased well-being and negative work attitudes. The
are therefore left to address any stress which might be impact of arousal is a subject worthy of investigation in
present in the psychosocial work environment. future research.
This explanation is in keeping with the conclusion A second reason for caution in interpreting the results
drawn by Kjellberg et al. (1996); that the full context of of the current study derives from the absence, here, of
the situation must be taken into account when trying to any measure of individual differences in noise sensitiv-
understand the effects of occupational noise exposure. It ity. Research indicates substantial individual variation
also helps to integrate the current results with previously in both sensitivity to noise (Broadbent, 1979), and the
demonstrated contingent relationship between occupa- extent to which exposure leads to negative outcomes
tional noise exposure and shiftwork (Cesana et al., 1982; (Hiroto, 1974; Halpern 1995). Future studies should
Ottmann et al., 1987; Nurminen & Kurppa, 1989); therefore seek to include relevant individual difference
Workload (Welch, 1979) and task demands (Mosskov & variables in their research designs.
Ettema, 1977; Carter & Beh, 1989). It is only by paying A third potential limitation of the study is its reliance
due attention to important characteristics of both work upon a rather crude measure of noise exposure.
content and context that the true effects of occupational Taking the average of the highest and lowest reading
noise exposure can be determined. cannot adequately capture the overall amount of
It is perhaps the lack of attention to this context energy exposure. While daily or even hourly Leq
which most seriously weakens the validity of laboratory would provide a far more accurate estimate of noise
investigations of noise exposure (Sundstrom 1986). As exposure; The four 20 sampling windows utilized
Evans (2000) remarks, the removal of any potential in the study was the best that could be achieved
environmental stress or from its natural context is likely without unduly disrupting the ongoing activities
to distort the ecological validity of any stressor–health within the organization. other characteristics of noise
relationship found. In the present study, the concept of have also been found to be important; E.g. its frequency,
stress, operationalized as job strain, provides a valuable periodicity; duration, and predictability, And the extent
means of operationalizing a key feature of the occupa- to which subjects have personal control over its source
tional context in which noise occurs. It thereby helps to (Keighley, 1970; Glass & Singer 1972; Jones; 1983;
ensure the ecological validity of the results. Sundstrom 1986; Smith, 1991). Future studies might
This conclusion must, however, be tempered with usefully include a range of noise assessment procedures
caution. By virtue of its cross-sectional design, the in order to help determine which characteristics of the
P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222 221

sound itself are important (Sundstrom, 1986; Kjellberg Bell, P. A., Fisher, J. D., Baum, A., & Greene, T. E. (1990).
et al., 1996) Environmental psychology. Fort Worth, TA: Holt, Rineholt &
Conversely, however, the very fact that the data Winston.
Beranek, L. L. (1956). Criteria for office quieting based on
reported here demonstrate such a consistent moderating questionnaire rating studies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
effect for average noise intensity alone challenges certain America, 28, 833–850.
conclusions in the literature. It has been suggested, for Beranek, L. L. (1957). Revised criteria for noise in buildings. Noise
example, that since the noise made by office machines Control, 3, 19–26.
and ventilation equipment carries little information, so Boyce, P. R. (1974). User’s assessments of a landscaped office. Journal
of Architectural Research, 3, 44–62.
its impact upon well-being and performance is likely to
Broadbent, D. E. (1979). Human performance and noise. In C. H.
be minimal (Boyce, 1974). This conclusion is clearly Harris, (Ed.), Handbook of noise control (2nd ed.). New York:
refuted by the present data set. McGraw-Hill.
A final limitation to acknowledge in the present study Carlestam, G., Karlsson, C., & Levi, L. (1973). Stress and disease in
is the fact that all three outcome measures reply response to exposure to noise: a review. In W. Ward (Ed.),
exclusively upon self-report procedures. It should be Proceedings of the second international congress on noise as a public
health problem. Report 550/9-73-008 (pp. 479–486). Washington,
noted here, however, that objective noise readings, not DC: EPA.
subjective, were used in the moderation analyses Carlopio, J. R., & Gardner, D. (1992). Direct and interactive effects of
precisely to prevent common method variance inflating the physical work environment on attitudes. Environment &
any relationship between independent and dependent Behavior, 24(5), 579–601.
variables. Carter, N. L., & Beh, H. C. (1989). The effect of intermittent noise on
cardiovascular functioning during vigilance task performance.
Notwithstanding these and any other limitations
Psychophysiology, 26, 548–559.
which the study might have, the results have a number Cesana, G. C., Ferrario, M., Curti, R., Zanettini, R., Grieco, A., Sega,
of important implications both for the design of the R., Palermo, A., Mara, G., Libretti, A., & Alegri, S. (1982). Work
workplace and for occupational stress research. In terms stress and urinary catecholamines excretion in shift workers
of their practical relevance, they point to the potential exposed to noise I: Epinepherine (E) and notepinepherine (NE).
benefits of acoustical treatments and any other aspects La Medicana del Lavoro, 2, 99–109.
Cohen, A. (1973). Industrial noise and medical, absence, and
of office design which minimize levels of noise, even at accident record data on exposed workers. In W. Ward (Ed.),
relatively low ambient levels. Advances in noise abate- Proceedings of the second International Congress on noise as public
ment in the workplace are likely to yield continuing health problem, Report 550/9-73-008. (pp 451-454). Washington,
benefits to employee health and well-being (MacKenzie, DC: EPA.
1975). Cook, J., & Wall, T. D. (1980). New work attitude measures of trust,
organizational commitment and personal need non-fulfilment.
With respect to occupational stress research, the
Journal of Occupational Psychology, 53, 39–52.
data point to the need for a fuller integration of Cooper, C. L. (1985). The stress of work: an overview. Aviation, Space
physical elements of workplace design with the usual and Environmental Medicine, July, 627-632.
job and organization-related psychosocial factors Cottington, E. M., Matthews, K. A., Talbott, E., & Kuller, L. (1983).
studied e.g. workload; task demands; role relationships. Occupational stress and diastolic pressure in a blue-collar popula-
Perhaps most important of all, the present study adds tion: The Pittsburgh noise-hypertension project. Annual meeting for
the society for epidemiology, Winnipeg, Manitoba.
further weight both to the call for noise exposure Cox, T. (1978). Stress.. London: Macmillan.
research to be better contextualized (Kjellberg et al., Cox, T. (1985). The nature and management of stress. Ergonomics, 28,
1996) and to the need for more empirical research 1155–1163.
on the possible interactions between physical and Cuesdan, L., Teganeanu, S., Tutu, C., Raiciu, M., Carp, C., & Coatu,
psychosocial factors in the workplace (Carlopio & S. (1977). Study of cardiovascular and auditory pathophysiological
implications in a group of operatives working in noisy industrial
Gardner, 1992; Evans & Lepore, 1992; Evans et al.,
settings. Psychophysiologi, 14, 53–61.
1994; Leather et al., 1998). Without such research, Evans, G. W. (2000). Environmental stress and health. In A. Baum, T.
understanding of both the impact of the physical Revenson & J. E. Singer, (Eds.), Handbook of health psychology.
environment and of the stress process is likely to remain Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
partial and incomplete. Evans, G. W., Johansson, G., & Carrere, S. (1994). Psychosocial
factors and the physical environment: inter-relations in the
workplace. In: C. L. Cooper, I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International
review of industrial and organisational psychology, Vol. 9, (pp. 1-29).
Chichester, UK: Wiley.
References Evans, G. W., & Lepore, S. J. (1992). Conceptual and analytic issues in
crowding research. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 12,
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator 163–173.
variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, Evans, G. W., & McCoy, J. M. (1998). When buildings don’t work: the
strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality & role of architecture in human health. Journal of Environmental
Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. Psychology, 18, 85–94.
Beehr, T. A. (1995). Psychological stress in the workplace. London: Ferguson, E. (1992). Stress, personality and health. Unpublished Ph.D.
Routledge. thesis, University of Nottingham, United Kingdom.
222 P. Leather et al. / Journal of Environmental Psychology 23 (2003) 213–222

Ferguson, E., & Cox, T. (1994). Development and validity of the Manninen, O. (1990). Interaction studies on environmental factors:
symptoms of infectious disease (SID) scale. Proceedings of the recent findings. In B. Berglund & T. Lindvall, (Eds.), Proceedings of
British Psychological Society, 2, 23. the fifth international congress on noise as a public health problem,
Glass, D. C., & Singer, J. E. (1972). Urban stress.. New York: Vol. 5 (pp. 209–232). Stockholm: Swedish Council for Building
Academic Press. Research.
Halpern, D. (1995). Mental health and the built environment.. London: Matthews, K., Cottington, E., Talbott, E., Kuller, L., & Siegel, J.
Taylor & Francis. (1987). Stressful work conditions and diastolic blood pressure
Health & Safety Executive (1999). Introducing the noise at work among blue collar factory workers. American Journal of Epidemiol-
regulations. INDG75(rev) C150 11/99. Sudbury, Suffolk: HSE ogy, 126, 280–291.
Books. McDonald, N. (1989). Jobs and their environment: The psychological
Hedge, A. (1982). The open-plan office: A systematic investigation of impact of work in noise. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 10, 33–50.
employee reactions to their work environment. Environment & Miller, J. D. (1974). Effects of noise on people. Journal of the
Behavior, 14(5), 519–542. Acoustical Society of America, 56, 729–764.
Hiroto, D. (1974). Locus of control and learned helplessness. Journal Mosskov, J. I., & Ettema, J. H. (1977). II. Extra-auditory effects
of Experimental Psychology, 102, 187–193. in short-term exposure to aircraft and traffic noise.
Jansen, G. (1961). Adverse effects of noise on iron and steel workers. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health,
Stahl Eisen, 81, 217–220 (Cited in K. Kryter (1994). The handbook 40, 165–173.
of hearing and the effects of noise. New York: Academic press. Nemecek, J., & Grandjean, E. (1973). Results of an ergonomic
Jones, D. M. (1983). Noise. In G. R. J. Hockev, (Ed.), Stress and investigation of large-space offices. Human Factors, 15(2), 111–124.
Fatigue in human performance. Chichester: Wiley. Nunally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Karasek, R. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude and mental Nurminen, T., & Kurppa, K. (1989). Occupational noise exposure and
strain: Implications for job re-deign. Administrative Science course of pregnancy. Scandinavian Journal of Work and Environ-
Quarterly, 24, 285–308. mental Health, 15, 117–124.
Karasek, R., & Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy work: Stress, productivity Occupational safety & Health Administration (1994). OSHA summary
and the reconstruction of working life. New York: Basic Books. sheet—Noise/hearing conservation. Washington, DC: Department
Keighley, E. C. (1970). Acceptability criteria for noise in large offices. of Labor, OSHA.
Journal of Sound Vibration, 11(1), 83–93. Ottman, W., Rutenfranz, J., Neidhart, B., & Boucsein, W. (1987).
Kjellberg, A., Landstrom, U., Tesarz, M., Soderberg, L., & Akerlund, Combining effects of shiftwork and noise on catecholamine
E. (1996). The effects of nonphysical noise characteristics; ongoing excretion and electrodermal activity. In A. Oginski, J. Pokorski,
task and noise sensitivity on annoyance and distraction due to & J. Rutenfranz (Eds.), Contemporary advances in shiftwork
noise at work. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, 123–136. research (pp. 64–75). Krakow, Poland: Medical Academy.
Kryter, K. (1970). The effects of noise on man. New York: Academic Smith, A. (1991). A review of the non-auditory effects of noise on
Press. health. Work & Stress, 5, 49–62.
Kryter, K. (1994). The handbook of hearing and the effects of noise. Sundstrom, E. (1986). Work places: The psychology of the physical
New York: Academic Press. environment in offices and factories. New York: Cambridge
Langdon, F. J. (1966). Modern offices: A user survey. National University Press.
Building Research Paper No. 41. London: Her Majesty’s Station- Sundstrom, E., Town, J. P., Rice, R. W., Osborn, D. P., & Brill, M.
ery Office. (1994). Office noise, satisfaction and performance. Environment &
Leather, P., Pyrgas, M., Beale, D., & Lawrence, C. (1998). Windows in Behavior, 26(2), 195–222.
the workplace: Sunlight, view and occupational stress. Environment Tafalla, R., Evans, G. W., & Cohen, A. (1988). Noise and human
& Behavior, 30(6), 739–762. performance: the potential role of effort. In: B. Berglund, U.
Lercher, P., Hortnagl, J., & Kofler, W. W. (1993). Work, noise Bergland, J. Karlsson, T. Lindvall (Eds.), Proceedings of the fifth
annoyance and blood pressure: Combined effects with stressful international congress on noise as a public health problem, Vol. 3,
working conditions. International Archives of Occupational and (pp. 95–100). Stockholm: Swedish Council for Building Research.
Environmental Health, 65, 23–28. Ulrich, R. S. (1991). Effects of interior design on wellness: Theory and
Louis Harris & Associates, Inc. (1978). The steelcase national study of recent scientific research. Journal of Health Care Interior Design, 3,
office environments: Do they work? Grand Rapids, MI: Steelcase 97–109.
Inc. Warr, P. B., Cook, J., & Wall, T. D. (1979). Scales for the
Louis Harris & Associates, Inc. (1980). The steelcase national study of measurement of some work attitudes and aspects of psychological
office environments, no. 2. Grand Rapids, MI: Steelcase Inc. well-being. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 52, 129–148.
MacKenzie, S. T. (1975). Noise and office work: Employee and Welch, B. L. (1979). Extra-auditory health effects of industrial noise:
employer concerns. Ithaca, NY: New York State School of Survey of foreign literature. AHRL-TR-79-41. Wright Patterson
Industrial and Labor Relations, Cornell University. Air Force Base: Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory.

You might also like