You are on page 1of 19

| |

Received: 5 August 2020    Revised: 17 January 2021    Accepted: 13 February 2021

DOI: 10.1002/dev.22115

SPECIAL ISSUE

Theory of mind processing in expectant fathers: Associations


with prenatal oxytocin and parental attunement

Sofia I. Cardenas  | Sarah A. Stoycos | Pia Sellery |


Narcis Marshall | Hannah Khoddam | Jonas Kaplan | Diane Goldenberg |
Darby E. Saxbe

Department of Psychology, University of


Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA Abstract
Social cognition may facilitate fathers' sensitive caregiving behavior. We administered
Correspondence
Darby E. Saxbe, Department of the Why-­How Task, an fMRI task that elicits theory of mind processing, to expect-
Psychology, University of Southern
ant fathers (n = 39) who also visited the laboratory during their partner's pregnancy
California, Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA.
Email: dsaxbe@usc.edu and provided a plasma sample for oxytocin assay. Three months postpartum, fathers
reported their beliefs about parenting. When rating “Why” an action was being per-
Funding information
Robert Wood Johnson Foundation; formed versus “How” the action was being performed (Why > How contrast), partici-
National Science Foundation, Grant/
pants showed activation in regions theorized to support theory of mind, including the
Award Number: 1552452 and 1842487
dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and superior temporal sulcus. Fathers' prenatal oxy-
tocin levels predicted greater signal change during the Why > How contrast in the
inferior parietal lobule. Both prenatal oxytocin and attunement parenting beliefs were
associated with Why > How activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a theory
of mind region implicated in emotion regulation. Posterior parahippocampal gyrus and
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation during the Why > How contrast predicted
fathers' attunement parenting beliefs. In conclusion, fathers' neural activation when
engaging in a theory of mind task was associated with their prenatal oxytocin levels
and their postpartum attunement parenting beliefs. Results suggest biological and
cognitive components of fathering may track with the theory of mind processing.

KEYWORDS
fatherhood, neuroimaging, oxytocin, parental attunement, theory of mind

1  |  I NTRO D U C TI O N of mind—­may support fathers' ability to provide sensitive and at-


tuned caregiving, allowing fathers to infer what infants feel and need
Father caregiving behavior yields long-­
term benefits to children (Abraham et al., 2014; Frith & Frith, 2006). Preliminary research
in social, behavioral, and cognitive domains (Sarkadi et al., 2008). suggests that the neural regions that support the theory of mind
Extensive research examines the complex underpinnings of moth- (e.g., superior temporal sulcus) may also underlie fathering behavior
ers' relationship with infants, including biological, behavioral, and (Abraham et al., 2014). Fathers' theory of mind ability may be a key
psychological adaptations that serve to support the infants' survival building block underlying fathers' engagement in attuned caregiving
(Norholt, 2020). Infants form bonds with fathers (Cabrera et al., behavior.
2018), but less remains known about the neurocognitive mecha- The research literature on the fathering brain is scarce and
nisms that support the transition to fatherhood. Within the context preliminary. Most parenting-­
focused neuroimaging research has
of father–­infant relationships, social cognition—­specifically, theory measured parents' neural responses to infant stimuli, such as

Developmental Psychobiology. 2021;00:1–19. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/dev |


© 2021 Wiley Periodicals LLC     1
|
2      CARDENAS et al.

photographs, infant cry sounds, or video clips of the infant (e.g., Atzil in gray matter volume in regions involved in processing threat and
et al., 2012; Khoddam et al., 2020; Kuo et al., 2012). These stud- ambiguity (e.g., orbitofrontal cortex, and insula) and self-­awareness
ies have found associations between neural activity in brain regions (e.g., posterior cingulate cortex). Thus, preliminary studies suggest
broadly supporting social cognition, including theory of mind, and human maternal and paternal brains show structural changes across
the quality of fathering behaviors. However, these studies do not the transition to parenthood that may support caregiving behaviors.
specify the mental processes that fathers engage in when watching The human maternal brain recruits several subcortical regions
infant stimuli, leaving unanswered questions. Thus, although prior involved in motivation (Gregory et al., 2015) and cortical systems
studies provide insight into the neural correlates of engaging with involved in cognitive and emotional empathy (Rilling, 2013). One of
infant stimuli, there is a gap in understanding the neural correlates the cortical systems supports the ability to consider the thoughts
of theory of mind in parents, despite its importance for later par- and feelings of others (i.e., theory of mind). This neural “theory of
enting behavior. Furthermore, though several studies have linked mind” system is composed of a group of regions including temporo-
hormones with the neural systems implicated in fathering (Abraham parietal junction, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, ventromedial pre-
et al., 2014; Mascaro et al., 2014; Wittfoth-­Schardt et al., 2012), frontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus/superior temporal gyrus,
studies that use tasks meant to isolate specific socio-­emotional pro- and precuneus (Gallagher & Frith, 2003; Molenberghs et al., 2016).
cesses would allow for the identification of processes implicated in An emerging body of research applies neurobiological methods
father neurobiology. Emerging evidence suggests changes in men's for understanding paternal caregiving behavior. Atzil et al. (2012)
hormones before pregnancy may indicate preparation for caregiving measured mothers' and fathers' brain activity in response to their
(Khoddam et al., 2020; Saxbe et al., 2017). However, no studies to infants' videos. While mothers had greater activity in limbic areas,
our knowledge have looked specifically at prenatal levels of oxyto- such as the right amygdala, fathers had greater activity in social-­
cin, a neuropeptide hormone that has been associated with social cognitive cortical areas, such as the dorsal prefrontal cortex. A
affiliative behavior, within expectant fathers. second study by the same research group (Abraham et al., 2014)
This study addresses these gaps in the literature by investigat- presented first-­time mothers and fathers in an fMRI scanner with
ing the neural correlates of theory of mind within expectant fa- videos of themselves playing with their infants and a stranger play-
thers. We also examined whether paternal prenatal oxytocin levels ing with a strange infant. They compared three groups of first-­time
predict neural activation to theory of mind and whether prenatal parents with varying degrees of involvement in childrearing: primary
activation during theory of mind predicts subsequent postpartum caregiver mothers in a heterosexual relationship, secondary care-
self-­reported paternal beliefs about parenting. Fathers specifically giver fathers in a heterosexual relationship, and primary caregiver
reported on whether they believe infants benefit from parenting fathers in a homosexual relationship raising infants without mater-
that is more attuned—­that is, more sensitive and responsive to their nal involvement. When contrasting self-­infant interactions and un-
behaviors and signals. familiar parent–­infant interaction, the study found similar levels of
activation in brain regions supporting emotion and motivation (e.g.,
ventral anterior cingulate cortex, ventral tegmental area, anterior
1.1  |  Fathering behavior and theory of mind insula/inferior frontal gyrus) and mentalizing network (e.g., ventro-
medial prefrontal cortex, temporal poles, lateral frontopolar cortex).
The neurobiology of paternal caregiving is an emerging field of re- However, primary caregiver mothers and primary caregiver fathers
search that overlaps with existing research on the neurobiology of showed greater activation in the amygdala to self-­infant interactions
motherhood. Extensive literature evaluates the neural basis of par- versus unfamiliar parent–­infant interactions compared to secondary
enting behaviors in human maternal caregivers (Cárdenas et al., 2019; caregiver fathers. When viewing self-­infant interactions versus un-
Rilling & Mascaro, 2017). One seminal study found that the human familiar parent–­infant interactions, both groups of fathers exhibited
maternal brain undergoes structural changes in gray matter across greater activation in the superior temporal sulcus, an area widely
the prenatal and postpartum period, including regions involved in associated with the theory of mind. Findings from these two studies
theory of mind and language processing (e.g., superior temporal sul- suggest that the neurobiology of fatherhood has some overlap and
cus, middle and superior temporal gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus) some distinction from that of mothers and the degree of caregiving
and emotion, memory, and self-­awareness (e.g., precuneus, posterior behavior may play a role in how caregivers process infant-­related
cingulate cortex); these structural changes predicted parent-­related stimuli. Additionally, these studies provide preliminary evidence that
outcomes, such as mother-­to-­infant attachment, degree of hostility the theory of mind network may play a particularly important role in
toward the infant, and functional activity when looking at own ver- supporting fathering behavior.
sus other infants (Hoekzema et al., 2017). Another longitudinal study
looked at structural gray-­matter changes in fathers' brains during
the postpartum period (from 2–­4 weeks postpartum to 12–­16 weeks 1.2  |  Measuring neural correlates of theory of mind
postpartum; Kim et al., 2014); the study found that fathers had in-
creased gray matter volume in regions underlying motivation and Theory of mind is the consideration of the mental states and inten-
attachment (e.g., hypothalamus, amygdala, striatum) and decreases tions of people (Weimer et al., 2017). Theory of mind also allows
CARDENAS et al. |
      3

individuals to perceive the behavior as driven by unobservable in men and women suggest that oxytocin may causally support social
thoughts (Spunt & Lieberman, 2012). By age four, most typically de- cognitive processes, participants' sex must be taken into account.
veloping children achieve an understanding of the mind (Cutting & Many studies have investigated the neuroendocrine under-
Dunn, 1999). However, knowledge about mental states continues to pinnings of parenting (Gordon et al., 2010; Scatliffe et al., 2019).
increase after the age of four, including interpretive theory of mind Some studies have found associations between fathers' levels of
or the ability to understand that individuals interpret events differ- hormones, including oxytocin, testosterone, and vasopressin, and
ently (Carpendale & Chandler, 1996). Differences in theory of mind their caregiving behavior (Feldman & Bakermans-­Kranenburg, 2017;
abilities for children correlate with higher general social competence Gettler, 2014; Storey & Ziegler, 2016). Oxytocin has been associated
(e.g., end conversations appropriately, engage in cooperative play with social and affiliative behavior, including behavioral synchrony
behaviors, follow rules during games; Lalonde & Chandler, 1995). and responsiveness in mothers and fathers (Feldman et al., 2010;
Theory of mind abilities improve in typically developing adults be- Morris et al., under review; Scatliffe et al., 2019). In fathers, oxytocin
tween late adolescence and adulthood (Dumontheil et al., 2010). has been specifically implicated in stimulatory parenting behaviors
Thus, theory of mind has implications for social competence and can (Feldman et al., 2010; Scatliffe et al., 2019) supporting father–­infant
change across development. attachment (George et al., 2010). Furthermore, experimentally ad-
Extensive research seeks to develop accurate behavioral and ministered oxytocin levels elicit more stimulating play in fathers
neural measures that reflect the multidimensional, dynamic, and (i.e., interactions that promote child exploration, often described as
variable nature of the theory of mind construct (e.g., Keysar et al., “rough and tumble” play; Gordon et al., 2010; Naber et al., 2010;
2003). Neuroimaging studies using theory of mind-­related tasks Weisman et al., 2014). However, to our knowledge, no study has
reveal a consistent activation of brain regions including the medial assessed whether prenatal oxytocin levels in expectant fathers are
prefrontal cortex, temporal poles, temporoparietal junction, pre- linked with enhanced postpartum caregiving outcomes or attitudes.
cuneus, and posterior superior temporal sulcus (e.g., Frith & Frith, Furthermore, the prior literature does not reconcile whether men's
2003, 2006; Gallagher et al., 2000; Saxe et al., 2006). However, ability to engage in theory of mind is linked with prenatal oxyto-
activation in the theory of mind network varies across individual cin and subsequent postpartum father caregiving attitudes and
studies based on the mental state evaluated (belief vs. desire) and behaviors.
the stimuli used (verbal vs. non-­verbal; Spunt & Adolphs, 2014). Few studies have investigated associations between oxytocin,
This study used the Why-­H ow Task, which isolates the multiple brain activity, and father caregiving behavior (e.g., Abraham et al.,
cognitive processes underlying the day-­
to-­
day theory of mind 2014; Atzil et al., 2012). Atzil et al. (2012) had mothers and fathers in
that occurs when individuals perceive others' actions (Spunt & an fMRI scanner watched videos of own infant play and other infant
Lieberman, 2012). The task is one of several variants created by play. Mothers showed relatively greater activation in subcortical
a team of researchers studying social cognition (Spunt & Adolphs, regions linked with emotion processing (e.g., amygdala) and reward
2014; Spunt & Lieberman, 2012). The “Why” process is supported (e.g., caudate), whereas fathers had greater activation in social cog-
by brain regions associated with theory of mind, whereas the nitive networks that support social cognition and empathy (i.e., me-
“How” process is associated with areas related to action percep- dial PFC, precuneus, and inferior parietal gyri). Furthermore, fathers'
tion (Spunt & Lieberman, 2012). In the task, participants watch postpartum plasma oxytocin levels negatively correlated with activ-
sixteen blocks of eight photographs. Each of the blocks leads to ity in the left inferior and superior frontal gyri, left primary motor
a response question meant to elicit processes associated with ac- cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and left anterior cingulate cortex
tion perception (“Is the person looking sideways?”) or theory of (Atzil et al., 2012). Building upon this literature, Abraham et al. (2014)
mind (“Is the person helping?”; Spunt & Adolphs, 2014). assessed oxytocin, brain activity, and parent caregiving behavior.
All participants provided salivary oxytocin data, completed a func-
tional MRI task in which they watched videos of themselves playing
1.3  |  The role of oxytocin with their infant (i.e., self-­infant interaction) or an unfamiliar parent
playing with an unfamiliar infant (i.e., unfamiliar parent–­infant), and
Some research suggests that oxytocin may support social cognition took part in a father–­infant play task micro-­coded for parent–­infant
and pro-­sociality in humans (for review, see Bartz et al., 2011). One behavioral synchrony. Although oxytocin levels were similar across
study found that men in a within-­subject, double-­blind, placebo-­ mothers and fathers, fathers showed greater activity in a region of
controlled trial performed better in the Reading the Mind in the Eyes the brain involved in theory of mind, perspective-­t aking, and empa-
Test (RMET) after receiving the oxytocin administration compared thy (i.e., superior temporal sulcus). Fathers' superior temporal sulcus
to men who received placebo (Domes et al., 2007). In a double-­blind, (STS) activation while watching infant videos correlated positively
placebo-­controlled study, Yue et al. (2017) found that women who with parent–­infant synchrony as well as oxytocin levels. The rela-
inhaled oxytocin versus placebo displayed faster response times tionship between STS and synchrony was mediated by oxytocin.
when judging others' perspectives. This same effect was not found This study suggests that fathers' social cognition, oxytocin levels,
in males, indicating that oxytocin may differently impact men's and and fathering behavior may be interrelated constructs. However,
women's perspective-­t aking (Yue et al., 2017). Although some data as this study used a non-­standardized fMRI task, further research
|
4      CARDENAS et al.

should clarify how oxytocin interacts with specific social-­cognitive with fathering behavior (Cabrera et al., 2007; Malin et al., 2012). We
processes. Additionally, this study used salivary oxytocin assayed also controlled for partners' pregnancy stage.
without extraction. Although salivary oxytocin measures have been We tested three hypotheses:
utilized in the parenting literature, there is some doubt about the
reliability of these measures and uncertainty of the mechanism by (i) Consistent with Spunt and Adolphs (2014), we predicted that ex-
which oxytocin enters the saliva (for review, see MacLean et al., pectant fathers would show greater signal change during the
2019). Why versus How contrast in regions that have been associated
Surprisingly, no studies to our knowledge have assessed prena- with theory of mind specifically (e.g., dorsomedial prefrontal cor-
tal oxytocin levels in expectant fathers. Men and women have been tex; ventromedial prefrontal cortex; orbitofrontal cortex; tem-
found to show shifts in hormone levels from the prenatal to the poroparietal junction; posterior cingulate cortex; temporal pole;
postpartum period, and some evidence suggests that these shifts in anterior superior temporal sulcus).
hormones support parenting outcomes (Edelstein et al., 2017; Saxbe (ii) As discussed earlier, oxytocin may influence fathering behavior
et al., 2017). For instance, Storey et al. (2000) found that men and and may be associated with neural activity in regions linked to
women had similar stage-­specific shifts in cortisol, testosterone, theory of mind (Abraham et al., 2014). We hypothesized that
and prolactin across the prenatal and postpartum periods. However, fathers with higher prenatal oxytocin levels would show more
some research suggests that oxytocin does not shift similarly across signal change in brain regions associated with theory of mind
pregnancy. An older study by Leake et al. (1981) found that healthy processing (i.e., brain regions listed in hypothesis i) during the
men, non-­pregnant women, and pregnant women (15–­42 weeks Why > How contrast.
gestation) had similar levels of plasma oxytocin, suggesting that oxy- (iii) We hypothesized that activation during the Why > How contrast
tocin levels during pregnancy may not change. In contrast, Gordon in theory of mind regions would be associated with greater en-
et al. (2010) found intraindividual stability during the first 6 months dorsement of thoughts and beliefs associated with attuned care-
postpartum and interrelated plasma oxytocin levels in mother-­father giving behavior.
dyads at 1 and 6 months postpartum. Furthermore, paternal oxy-
tocin correlated with stimulatory parenting behavior. These find-
ings suggest that oxytocin levels may be consistent across time and
provide preliminary support for investigating the utility of prena- 2  |  M ATE R I A L S A N D M E TH O DS
tal oxytocin in men as a predictor of postpartum father caregiving
outcomes. 2.1  |  Participants
Theory of mind processing may inform paternal beliefs and cog-
nitions about parenting. Parents' tendency to consider the thoughts This study uses data from the larger longitudinal Hormones and
and needs of infants (i.e., engage in theory of mind) is related to timely Attachment across the Transition to Childrearing (HATCh) study,
and appropriate responsiveness to infants' needs (Ereky-­Stevens, which follows couples from the mothers' mid-­to-­late pregnancy
2008). Prior research suggests that parents' beliefs and principles through the first year postpartum. We recruited participants
about parenting are associated with their actual parenting behaviors through social media advertising (e.g., Facebook, online parenting
during early infancy (Winstanley & Gattis, 2013). However, to our groups), flyers, and word of mouth. This study used self-­reported
knowledge, no studies have assessed whether expectant fathers' data provided by fathers from prenatal laboratory visits, con-
theory of mind activation relates to any aspects of their postpartum ducted with both members of the couple when mothers were in
parenting, including their caregiving beliefs and cognitions. mid-­
to-­
late pregnancy, subsequent father-­
only MRI visits that
we scheduled within approximately 2 weeks of the prenatal in-­
laboratory visit, and postpartum questionnaires collected online
1.4  |  The current study approximately 3 months after the birth. Eligible couples were co-
habitating, first-­t ime parents of a singleton pregnancy, and did not
Although neural and behavioral research suggests that oxytocin and report any medications or conditions known to interfere with en-
theory of mind may support fathers in the transition to parenthood, docrine system hormones (e.g., steroid medicines, Cushing's dis-
our paper uniquely uses a longitudinal approach that combines a ease). We also excluded couples who reported psychiatric illness
standardized prenatal fMRI theory of mind task, prenatal oxytocin requiring medication or illegal drug use. Users of tobacco, mari-
levels, and postpartum beliefs and cognitions about parenting. This juana, and we allowed some psychiatric medications if able to ab-
study used the Why-­How Task (Spunt & Adolphs, 2014), an fMRI stain for 24 h before their study visit. Additional exclusion criteria
task designed to elicit theory of mind processing within a sample included contraindications for magnetic resonance imaging (MRI),
of expectant fathers. Moreover, we used immunoassay with extrac- such as left-­
handedness, neurological or movement disorders,
tion to measure oxytocin levels in plasma, which is recommended claustrophobia, history of brain injury, psychotropic medication,
for plasma oxytocin assay (Lefevre et al., 2017). We controlled for or severe learning disability. Additionally, participants needed
fathers' education, given evidence that education may be associated to have sufficient English language fluency to complete study
CARDENAS et al. |
      5

measures and scanning procedures. The university Institutional Diabetes Research Institute (Armando Mendez, PI) for oxytocin
Review Board (IRB) approved all procedures, and all participants processing.
signed informed consent forms before participation.
Data for this study were available for 39 expectant fathers who
provided prenatal demographic and neuroimaging data. Of these 39 2.3.2  |  Oxytocin processing
fathers, 34 provided prenatal blood samples for oxytocin assay, and
we excluded one oxytocin sample due to laboratory processing is- We used enzyme-­linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kits (Arbor
sues. Of the 39 fathers with demographic and neuroimaging data, Assays; Ann Arbor, MI) to calculate oxytocin immunoreactivity.
37 fathers provided data on self-­reported parental attunement at In our sample, the lower limit of detection was 0.8 pg/ml. In line
3  months, and two fathers did not provide 3-­month questionnaire with prior work (Szeto et al., 2011), we extracted 2.2 ml of plasma.
data. Fathers were, on average, 31.56  years old (SD  =  4.25  years). Furthermore, we reconstituted in 220 µl assay buffer leading to a
The sample was ethnically and racially diverse, with fathers report- 10-­fold concentration compared to the starting plasma volume. We
ing European American (33.3%), Latino (30.8%), Black or African performed assays in line with manufacturer instructions. We as-
American (5.1%), Asian-­American (25.7%), and multiracial or other sessed samples in duplicate. The inter-­assay coefficient of variation
heritage (5.1%). The majority of expectant fathers (76.9%) reported was less than 10%. Of the MRI sample, nine fathers with oxytocin
a college degree or higher. (27% of the sample) had oxytocin levels below detection levels fol-
lowing extraction, similar to prior research analyzing extracted ox-
ytocin (e.g., Szeto et al., 2011; Tabak et al., 2011). Consistent with
2.2  |  Data availability statement prior work, we assigned a value of 0.4 pg/ml to samples below the
detection level (Saxbe et al., 2019).
The data that support the findings of this study are available from We analyzed the extracted oxytocin data for outliers and
the corresponding author upon reasonable request. normality using SPSS Statistics 26 (IBM) (M = 1.99; SD = 1.90
Skewness  =  1.87). Furthermore, we truncated the data by drop-
ping outliers greater than three standard deviations from the
2.3  |  Procedure sample (3%). We found that extracted oxytocin data remained
positively skewed after excluding outliers (M = 1.50; SD = 1.05;
During the prenatal in-­
laboratory visits, fathers and mothers Skewness  =  0.74). As a consequence, we natural log-­t ransformed
participated in three semi-­
s tructured discussion tasks: (a) 10-­ the data to satisfy normality assumptions (M  =  0.23; SD  =  0.88;
min conversation about hopes, fears, and expectations about Skewness = −0.03).
birth and their transition to parenthood, (b) 10-­min conversation
about projected division of childcare, and (c) 15-­min conversation
about areas of conflict within the relationship. In addition to the 2.3.3  |  Why-­how task
aforementioned discussion tasks, couples completed psychoso-
cial questionnaires, including the measures described below. At The Why-­H ow Localizer Task is a standardized task for investi-
the end of the laboratory visit, a licensed phlebotomist conducted gating social cognition (Spunt & Adolphs, 2014). The task uses
blood draws. Fathers returned to completed their scan session Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3 (PTB-­3) and MATLAB (The
within several weeks of their in-­laboratory visit; within this sam- MathWorks, Inc.). The task presents individuals with 16 blocks of
ple, the lag time between the prenatal laboratory visit and the items, with each block consisting of a question prompt and eight
scan ranged from 1 day to 5.71 weeks, with the majority of fathers photographs. The task consists of 42 pictures of common hand ac-
(32 out of 39 expectant fathers; 82.05%) completing MRI scan vis- tions and 42 pictures of familiar facial expressions. The question
its within 2 weeks of the in-­laboratory visit. During the MRI visit, prompts present as pre-­tested yes/no questions, with participants
fathers completed the Why-­H ow Task (Spunt & Adolphs, 2014) as given a limited amount of time for response. The task requires ap-
part of a larger MRI data collection protocol. proximately 6 min and 30 s to complete. Participants complete the
task in a suite for a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner.
Within the scanner, participants watch stimuli while lying down
2.3.1  |  Oxytocin collection in the scanner and can use a button box programmed to allow for
yes/no responses.
We collected blood for the plasma oxytocin assay into a sterile The Why-­How Task features a 2 (condition: Why, How) × 2 (be-
EDTA vacutainer tube. We added twenty microliters of a protease havioral category: face, hand) factorial design (see Figure 1). For
inhibitor (Amastatin; 10 µM final concentration) to the tube to the behavioral factor, participants watch a photograph with either
limit oxytocin degradation. Then, we centrifuged blood samples a face or a hand. For the stimulus factor, participants receive a
for 10  min to separate plasma. We stored aliquots at −80°C and stimulus prompt that asks either a Why question (i.e., the question
shipped on dry ice to the University of Miami School of Medicine elicits theory of mind) or a How question (i.e., the question evokes
|
6      CARDENAS et al.

FLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson & Smith, 2001) to regis-


ter high-­resolution structural and/or standard space images. For
pre-­
processing, we conducted motion correction with MCFLIRT
(Jenkinson et al., 2002), non-­brain removal using BET (Smith, 2002),
spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 5 mm, and
high-­pass temporal filtering with Gaussian-­weighted least-­squares
straight-­line fitting and a sigma = 50.0 s).
Next, we analyzed data using a General Linear Model (GLM)
with a multi-­level mixed-­effects design. In the GLM, the observed
hemodynamic response function (HRF) signal is predicted by one or
more explanatory variables that reflect a specific factor, also called
regressor or regressor of interest. The GLM is a method of model-
F I G U R E 1  Conditions of the Why-­How Task. The above diagram ing an observed neural signal in relation to one or more independent
displays the 2 × 2. factorial design of the Why-­How Task with type or explanatory variables, which are also referred to as regressors
of question (condition: Why, How) and stimulus (condition: face, (Jenkinson & Chappell, 2018). As such, we entered each component
hand). of the task as a regressor (Why-­Face trials, Why-­Hand trials, How-­
Face trials, Why-­Hand trials) and modeled by convolving the task
action-­perception). In this study, similar to Spunt and Adolphs (2014), design with a double-­gamma hemodynamic response function. We
we collapsed the behavioral category conditions (i.e., face, hand) and defined the task periods from the first photograph in each block to
focused on Why and How conditions. We contrasted Why > How the stimulus offset of the last photograph in each block. We also
to specifically examine the neural correlates of theory of mind pro- included in the model the temporal derivatives of the task regres-
cessing. We also scored the Why-­How task results behaviorally for sors and six motion parameters as nuisance regressors. We created
accuracy and response time and entered this information in SPSS for contrasts as linear combinations of the explanatory variables to look
further analysi. at overall task effects and condition effects (e.g., Why > How). At
the individual subject level, we generated statistical maps. We then
computed brain-­activity maps for each of the task effects and con-
2.3.4  |  fMRI data acquisition dition effects combining across all subjects using a higher-­
level
mixed-­effects design using FLAME to produce contrast-­level activity
We acquired whole-­brain images on a Siemens 3 Tesla MAGNETON maps. This study focused on the Why > How contrast. All higher-­
Prisma System scanner. We acquired high-­resolution, T1-­weighted level analyses included fathers' education level and weeks pregnant
images using a 3D Magnetization Prepared Rapid Acquisition as covariates. To account for multiple comparisons, we applied clus-
Gradient Echo (repetition time, 2530  ms; echo time, 3.13  ms; flip ter thresholding using Gaussian Random Field theory with a cluster
angle, 10°), with an isotropic voxel resolution of 1  mm3. We col- size probability threshold of p < 0.05. For the main effect (Why >
lected functional data using a T2* weighted echo-­planar imaging How contrast), we used a cluster-­forming z threshold of 3.1, a more
(EPI) with an interleaved sequence (194 2.5 mm transversal slices; stringent threshold. Given that we had a smaller sample for regressor
repetition time, 2000 ms; echo time, 25 ms; field of view, 192 mm2; analyses (prenatal oxytocin, postpartum attunement), we used a less
3.0 × 3.0 × 2.5 mm voxels, flip angle, 90 degrees). Functional scans stringent cluster-­forming z threshold of 2.3 to account for low power
employed Siemens' 3D PACE prospective motion correction (and and avoid type II error. All coordinates reported are in the Montreal
were then pre-­
processed for motion further offline, described Neurological Institute (MNI) standard brain space.
below; Thesen et al., 2000).
We presented stimulus presentation and response recording
using MATLAB (version 2012a; MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). 2.4  |  Measures
We used an LCD projector to show stimuli on a rear-­projection
screen. Participants made responses using their right index and mid- 2.4.1  |  Demographics
dle fingers on a button box.
At the prenatal visit, fathers self-­reported their educational attain-
ment with the following options: high school/GED, some college,
2.3.5  |  fMRI data analysis associate degree, bachelor's degree, master's degree, professional/
doctorate). Fathers also reported on age, and mothers reported
We analyzed neural responses to the Why > How contrast using their expected due date, from which we calculated their pregnancy
FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 6.00, part of FSL stage (number of days pregnant) at the time of their prenatal study
(FMRIB's Software Library, www.FMRIb.ox.ac.uk/fsl). We used visit.
CARDENAS et al. |
      7

TA B L E 1  Descriptive statistics of sample demographics and self-­


2.4.2  |  Parental attunement reported measures.

Fathers' ratings of thoughts and behaviors completed a series Variable Subcategory Value/mean (%/SD)

of online questionnaires, including the Baby Care Questionnaire Education High school/GED 1 (2.6%)
(BCQ; Winstanley & Gattis, 2013). The BCQ is a 30-­item question- Some college 7 (17.9%)
naire that asks participants how strongly they agree or disagree Associate degree 1 (2.6%)
with a statement related to principles that guide feeding, holding, Bachelor's degree 12 (30.8%)
and soothing behaviors. Participants rate each item on a Likert
Master's degree 11 (28.2%)
Scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The BCQ
Professional/doctorate 7 (17.9%)
generates two subscales, “Structure” and “Attunement,” which
Race European American 13 (33.3%)
reflect parents' endorsement of more structured (reliant on rou-
Latino 12 (30.8%)
tines and schedules) versus more attuned (responsive to in-­the-­
Black or African 2 (5.1%)
moment infant cues) parenting style principles. We specifically
American
assessed the Attunement scale for this project. BCQ Attunement
Asian-­American 10 (25.7%)
subscale scores are generated by averaging scores on attunement
Multiracial or other 2 (5.1%)
items, such as “Babies benefit from physical contact with parents
Age at prenatal 31.56 (4.25), 23–­41
when they wake during the night,” Parent(s) should find a pattern
visit
of feeding/eating that suits their baby,” and “Responding quickly to
Extracted 0.23 (0.88),
a crying baby leads to less crying in the long run.” A prior validation
oxytocin −0.92–­1.92
study showed that the BCQ attunement subscale had acceptable
Weeks pregnant 27.84 (4.71),
internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha >0.70), acceptable test–­ 20.37–­38.52
retest reliability (>0.70), and external validity (i.e., endorsement
Weeks between 1.63 (1.15),
of attunement principle was correlated with higher bed-­sharing, laboratory 0.14–­5.71
breastfeeding, and holding; Winstanley & Gattis, 2013). We com- and MRI
puted internal consistency on our sample and found acceptable Parental 2.91 (0.31),
reliability in our sample's BCQ Attunement scores (Cronbach's attunement 2.42–­3.77
alpha = 0.70). Weeks pregnant is the self-­reported time fathers reported their female
partners were pregnant on the day of the laboratory visit.

3  |   R E S U LT S (37) = −8.38, p < 0.01, such that participants responded more slowly


during Why trials.
We present descriptive statistics in Table 1. Zero-­order correlations
between study variables are shown in Table 2. Fathers' education
and age were positively correlated (r (39) = 0.57, p < 0.01). Age cor- 3.2  |  fMRI results
related with accuracy during How trials (r (39)  =  0.35, p < 0.05).
Fathers' accuracy during Why trials correlated with fathers' accu- 3.2.1  |  Covariates: Associations between
racy during How trials (r (38)  =  0.78, p < 0.01). Fathers' response education and weeks pregnant during theory of mind
time during Why trials correlated with fathers' response time during
How trials (r (38) = 0.93, p < 0.01). We also found a trending negative Higher educational attainment predicted greater signal change in
correlation between extracted oxytocin and response time during theory of mind regions during the Why > How contrast. When we
How trials (r (32) = −0.35, p = 0.05). Of interest, we found a trending added education level as a regressor into the Why > How contrast
significance between prenatal oxytocin and postpartum attunement (see Table 3 and Figure 3), signal changes associated with this co-
(r (31) = 0.33, p = 0.069, see Figure 2). variate were found in four clusters, including the right precuneus,
bilateral precentral gyri, left postcentral gyrus, and right inferior
temporal gyrus. Given these findings, we included education as a
3.1  |  Behavioral results covariate in all analyses with cross-­subject regressors. The preg-
nancy stage was also included as a covariate in all analyses to control
Within this sample, we found behavioral results consistent with for potential differences resulting from men's partners' pregnancy
Spunt and Adolphs (2014). The percent accuracy on How (M = 94%, stage. When we added weeks pregnant as a regressor into the Why
SD  =  6.2%) trials differed from Why (M  =  91%, SD  =  7.3%) trials, > How contrast (see Table 4 and Figure 4), activation occurred in a
t (38)  =  4.09, p < 0.01, such that participants responded less ac- cluster near the cingulate, largely comprised of white matter. Thus,
curately to Why trials. Response time to How trials (M = 818 ms., we mean-­centered education level and pregnancy stage (i.e., weeks
SD = 134 ms.) differed from Why trials (M = 900 ms., SD = 155), t pregnant) and included as confound regressors in all analyses.
|
8      CARDENAS et al.

TA B L E 2  Bivariate correlation of main study variables.

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. Age
2. Education 0.57***
3. Weeks pregnant 0.22 0.29*
4. Prenatal oxytocin −0.05 −0.10 0.23
5. Parental attunement −0.10 −0.15 −0.10 0.33*
6. Why trials accuracy 0.15 0.13 0.02 −0.11 −0.18
7. How trials accuracy 0.35** 0.19 0.14 −0.09 −0.21 0.78***
8. Why trials RT 0.28* 0.12 −0.03 −0.28 −0.11 0.25 0.05
9. How trials RT 0.17 0.07 −0.08 −0.35** −0.16 0.28* 0.01 0.93***

Weeks pregnant is the self-­reported time fathers reported their female partners were pregnant including the time between the laboratory visit and
the MRI visit.
*p < 0.10; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

3.75
Postpartum self-reported parental attunement

3.50

3.25

3.00

2.75

2.50

-1.00 -.50 .00 .50 1.00 1.50 2.00

Prenatal plasma oxytocin

F I G U R E 2  Scatterplot of prenatal plasma oxytocin and postpartum self-­reported parental attunement.

3.2.2  |  Hypothesis 1: Neural activation during main largely replicate findings from the original Why-­How paper (Spunt
effect of task & Adolphs, 2014). However, during the Why > How contrast, this
study found additional signal change in several areas not found in
We partially replicated the key neural findings from the Spunt and the original task study: left amygdala, left dorsolateral prefrontal
Adolphs (2014) paper. The full list of brain regions activated by cortex, left caudate, left thalamus, and right posterior superior tem-
Why > How contrast is listed in Table 5. A visual representation poral gyrus.
of the brain regions activated by Why > How contrast is shown in
Figure 5. When contrasting activation during Why trials, compared
with How trials (i.e., Why > How contrast), we found signal change 3.2.3  |  Hypothesis 2: Associations between
in regions from the original group-­level results of the study: left oxytocin and BOLD signal during theory of mind
dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, left orbitofrontal cortex, right tem-
poroparietal junction (angular gyrus), bilateral posterior cingulate Prenatal oxytocin was added as a between-­subjects regressor in the
cortex, and right anterior superior temporal sulcus. These results group-­level analysis of the Why > How contrasts. Higher prenatal
CARDENAS et al.       9|
TA B L E 3  Neural regions positively
MNI coordinates
correlated with education during the Why
> How contrast. Region L/R k x y z Z

Precuneus R 2127 18 −62 42 4.55


R -­ 30 −40 46 4.38
R -­ 10 −66 60 4.05
R -­ 54 −52 50 3.93
R -­ 32 −50 56 3.87
R -­ 34 −56 64 3.85
Precentral gyrus R 824 10 −32 50 3.76
L -­ −22 −60 56 3.73
R -­ 2 −42 54 3.71
R -­ 4 −38 54 3.69
R -­ 8 −34 44 3.52
L -­ −16 −58 52 3.17
Postcentral gyrus L 632 −46 −28 52 4.02
L -­ −62 −38 44 3.91
L -­ −62 −42 44 3.8
L -­ −48 −26 44 3.52
L -­ −32 −26 42 3.45
L -­ −40 −18 60 3.23
Inferior temporal gyrus R 494 50 −58 −8 3.84
R -­ 38 −56 2 3.46
R -­ 30 −64 0 3.36
R -­ 18 −60 14 3.34
R -­ 24 −74 2 3.24
R -­ 28 −58 −2 3.24

All peaks survived a whole-­brain search thresholded at a voxel-­wise family-­wise error rate of 0.05
and a cluster corrected at z = 2.3. x, y, z = Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates in the
left-­right anterior-­posterior, and inferior-­superior dimensions, respectively (n = 39).

F I G U R E 3  Group-­level results of the Why > How contrast with education as a regressor. Sagittal, coronal, and axial view of whole-­brain
activation during the Why > How contrast. Analyses cluster corrected at z = 2.3 (n = 39).
|
10      CARDENAS et al.

TA B L E 4  Neural regions positively


MNI coordinates
correlated with weeks pregnant during
Region L/R k x y z Z Why > How contrast.

White matter/cingulate gyrus R 400 26 −46 12 3.34


R -­ 36 −60 4 3.32
R -­ 32 −62 8 3.31
R -­ 16 −38 28 3.22
R -­ 20 −48 18 3.21
R -­ 34 −68 −2 3.16

All peaks survived a whole-­brain search thresholded at a voxel-­wise family-­wise error rate of 0.05
and a cluster corrected at z = 2.3. x, y, z = Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates in the
left-­right anterior-­posterior, and inferior-­superior dimensions, respectively (n = 39).

F I G U R E 4  Group-­level results of the Why > How contrast with weeks pregnant as a regressor. Sagittal, coronal, and axial view of
whole-­brain activation during the Why > How contrast. Analyses cluster corrected at z = 2.3 (n = 39).

plasma oxytocin predicted greater signal in theory of mind regions bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and right posterior parahip-
during the Why > How contrasts. Specifically, signal change emerged pocampal gyrus (see Table 7 and Figure 7).
in eight clusters, including bilateral temporoparietal junction (angular
gyrus), bilateral supramarginal gyrus, bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex, bilateral paracingulate gyrus, bilateral intracalcarine cortex, 3.2.5  |  Exploratory Analyses: Overlap between
bilateral posterior cingulate gyrus, and right superior temporal gyrus oxytocin and postpartum attunement activation
(see Table 6 and Figure 6). during theory of mind

To follow-­up on the above-­reported findings, we assessed whether


3.2.4  |  Hypothesis 3: Associations between there were areas of significant overlap in signal change associated with
postpartum attunement and activation both prenatal oxytocin and postpartum attunement during the Why
during theory of mind > How contrast. Specifically, we used fslmaths (Smith et al. 2004) to
mask the thresh_zstat image of the Why > How contrast with oxy-
We also explored whether signal change in theory of mind regions tocin as a regressor (i.e., hypothesis 2) with the thresh_zstat image of
during the Why > How contrast predicted greater endorsement of the Why > How contrast with postpartum attunement as a regressor
an attuned parenting philosophy at 3 months postpartum. We found (i.e., hypothesis 3). The results of this masking (Figure 8) revealed over-
that postpartum attunement was associated with signal change for lapping activation in a mostly right-­lateralized region in the prefrontal
the Why-­How contrast in two regions including portions of the cortex, mostly composed of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
CARDENAS et al. |
      11

TA B L E 5  Group-­level results of the


MNI coordinates
Why > How contrast.
Region L/R k x y z Z

Orbitofrontal cortex L 6372 −48 16 −22 6.45


L -­ −58 −8 −14 6.4
L -­ −42 28 −14 6.37
L -­ −56 4 −16 6.29
L -­ −52 24 12 6.22
L -­ −48 30 −6 6.21
Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex L 5759 −6 56 38 7.22
L -­ −8 64 26 7.2
L -­ −4 44 50 7.18
L -­ −10 60 32 7.18
L -­ −2 48 −16 7.16
L -­ −10 44 46 7
Posterior cingulate cortex R 1836 0 −50 30 6.27
L -­ −4 −60 34 6.08
L -­ −2 −60 30 5.93
L -­ −4 −50 22 5.64
L -­ −10 −52 4 4.31
R -­ 14 −48 6 3.64
Anterior superior temporal sulcus R 684 58 2 −22 5.31
R -­ 60 −6 −18 5.25
R -­ 62 −2 −22 5.03
R -­ 54 8 −22 4.98
R -­ 50 −8 −12 4.89
R -­ 48 −14 −10 4.8
Amygdala L 261 −26 −18 −18 5.01
L -­ −20 −10 −14 4.96
L -­ −28 −6 −22 4.19
L -­ −36 −16 −22 3.43
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex L 229 −42 14 54 4.22
L -­ −36 24 52 4.22
L -­ −34 18 50 4.17
Temporoparietal junction (angular gyrus) R 161 48 −58 24 4.24
R -­ 52 −62 28 4.02
R -­ 58 −56 26 3.64
Caudate L 159 −12 14 12 4.65
L -­ −10 −4 12 3.29
L -­ −14 14 0 3.25
Thalamus L 102 −4 −12 6 4.31
L -­ −2 −16 12 3.91
Posterior superior temporal gyrus R 100 64 −34 4 3.94
R -­ 54 −30 2 3.86
R -­ 46 −34 −2 3.78

All peaks survived a whole-­brain search thresholded at a voxel-­wise family-­wise error rate of 0.05
and a z-­threshold of 3.1. x, y, z = Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates in the left-­right
anterior-­posterior, and inferior-­superior dimensions, respectively (n = 39).
|
12      CARDENAS et al.

F I G U R E 5  Group-­level results of the Why > How contrast with education and weeks pregnant as covariates. Sagittal, coronal, and axial
view of whole-­brain activation during the Why > How contrast. Analyses cluster corrected at z = 3.1 (n = 39).

4  |  D I S C U S S I O N transition to parenthood, which may reflect parenting investment,


has been associated with increased caudate activation (Kuo et al.,
In this study, we partially replicated the results from the Spunt 2012). Although preliminary, our findings suggest that expectant
and Adolphs (2014) paper within a sample of expectant fathers. fathers are engaging both cortical and subcortical regions when
Consistent with our hypotheses, we found that theory of mind pro- completing the Why-­How theory of mind task; further research can
cessing during the Why > How contrast was associated with signal investigate whether these patterns of activation are dynamic in new
change in mentalizing network structures, including both anterior parents.
and posterior cortical midline structures and the right temporopa- Although not the focus of the study, fathers' educational attain-
rietal junction (angular gyrus). We also found activation to Why > ment was associated with Why > How activation in areas involved
How in a few additional regions, including the left dorsolateral pre- in vision and the processing of words (e.g., inferior temporal gyrus;
frontal cortex, right posterior superior temporal gyrus and subcor- Dien et al., 2013), the mirror neuron system (e.g., precentral gyrus,
tical regions (left caudate, left thalamus, left amygdala). Moreover, postcentral gyrus; Wu et al., 2017), and theory of mind (e.g., pre-
we found that prenatal oxytocin in expectant-­fathers during the cuneus; Saxe et al., 2006). In the parenting network, the mirror
Why > How contrast was associated with similar signal change as neuron system and the theory of mind network work together to
the Why > How contrast, including frontal and posterior cortical support a parents' ability to quickly resonate with infant behaviors
midline structures as well as the bilateral temporoparietal junction (i.e., mirror neuron system), consider intentions (i.e., theory of mind
(angular gyrus). Signal change in the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal network), and provide caregiving behavior that syncs with the child's
cortex and right posterior hippocampal gyrus during the Why > How needs (Feldman, 2015). Extensive research suggests that fathers
contrast predicted greater self-­reported beliefs and cognitions re- with higher education levels use more advanced vocabulary, which
garding attuned parenting styles approximately 6 months later when is linked with children's emotion regulation language development
infants were 3 months of age. (Cabrera et al., 2007; Pancsofar & Vernon-­Feagans, 2010). Thus, our
Although not originally found by Spunt and Adolphs (2014), we findings might reflect that more educated fathers may exhibit more
observed activation in the left amygdala and left caudate during comfort or engagement with the Why-­How Task, which requires
the Why > How contrast in expectant fathers. Interestingly, social participants to read questions quickly.
neuroscientists have posited that regions associated with process- Additionally, fathers' prenatal oxytocin levels predicted acti-
ing emotional salience (e.g., fearful faces) may foster higher order vation in areas of the brain that support embodied simulation and
empathic behavior by making individuals aware of the suffering of theory of mind. Of note, we collected prenatal oxytocin following a
others (Marsh, 2016). Within parenting, the amygdala may promote laboratory visit that included partner interaction and questions rel-
mothers' ability to detect biologically salient information in the care- evant to the transition to parenthood. Activation during the Why >
giving environment (Swain et al., 2014). The caudate is a reward re- How contrast was associated with prenatal oxytocin in a region that
gion and is theorized to support mothers' motivation to engage with included the bilateral temporoparietal junction (angular gyrus) and
their infants (Noriuchi et al., 2008). Men's testosterone across the bilateral supramarginal gyrus, which compose the inferior parietal
CARDENAS et al. |
      13

TA B L E 6  Neural regions positively correlated with prenatal TA B L E 6  (Continued)


oxytocin during Why > How contrast.
MNI coordinates
MNI coordinates
Region L/R k x y z Z
Region L/R k x y z Z
Superior R 340 48 −20 −4 4.04
TPJ/SMG L 3882 −48 −56 46 4.68 temporal R -­ 62 −20 2 3.21
gyrus
L -­ −58 −48 40 4.68 R -­ 62 −28 2 3.10
L -­ −50 −50 38 4.66 R -­ 70 −26 −4 3.88
L -­ −42 −52 54 4.54 R -­ 60 −12 −10 3.44
L -­ −52 −38 42 4.06 R -­ 60 −22 −18 3.28
L -­ −20 −74 42 4.05
All peaks survived a whole-­brain search thresholded at a voxel-­wise
TPJ/SMG R 1481 44 −56 50 4.17 family-­wise error rate of 0.05 and a z-­threshold of 2.3. x, y, z = Montreal
R -­ 46 −50 32 3.67 Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates in the left-­right anterior-­
posterior, and inferior-­superior dimensions, respectively (n = 33). TPJ/
R -­ 34 −80 32 3.94
SMG =Temporoparietal Junction (Angular Gyrus)/Supramarginal Gyrus.
R -­ 40 −60 46 3.93
R -­ 48 −44 56 3.87
R -­ 52 −46 40 3.63
lobule (IPL). The angular gyrus, which is part of the temporopari-
etal junction, has been implicated in theory of mind and mentalizing
Dorsolateral R 817 34 34 30 3.57
prefrontal tasks (for review, see Seghier, 2013). Concerning parenting, the IPL
R -­ 44 30 26 3.45
cortex is part of the embodied simulation network as well as the mirror neu-
R -­ 44 32 38 3.2
ron system (Feldman, 2015). The mirror neuron system supports an
R -­ 52 36 26 3.2
individual's ability to observe and mimic the emotions and behaviors
R -­ 36 18 36 3.16
of others (for review, see Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004). The embod-
R -­ 22 50 24 3.25 ied simulation network supports a parents' ability to mentally sim-
L 811 −36 −2 54 3.76 ulate the behavior of the infants and consider their intentions and
L -­ −38 20 50 3.60 needs (Feldman, 2015). The Why > How contrast was also related
L -­ −50 26 32 3.47 to oxytocin in areas that support social cognition, including the pos-
L -­ −36 26 44 3.47 terior cingulate cortex, a key region of the Default Mode Network
L -­ −38 32 28 2.97 (Li et al., 2014). Given the minimal research on oxytocin in expect-

L -­ −50 32 24 2.96 ant fathers and the preliminary evidence we found of an association
between oxytocin and social cognition, we recommend that future
Paracingulate R 707 12 30 26 3.58
gyrus studies investigate oxytocin and measures of social cognition across
L -­ −10 42 30 3.29
the transition to fatherhood.
R -­ 2 14 56 3.05
Prenatal activation to the Why > How contrast in regions that
L -­ −6 28 44 3.14
support social cognition, behavioral organization, and emotion
R -­ 8 60 28 3.22
regulation predicted fathers' postpartum endorsement of a more
-­ 0 30 22 3.09 attuned parenting style, as assessed 3  months following the birth.
Intracalcarine R 561 2 −64 16 3.27 Specifically, attunement was associated with activation in two clus-
cortex L -­ −16 −62 0 3.23 ters, including the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and right
L -­ 12 −78 6 3.16 posterior parahippocampal gyrus. The dorsolateral prefrontal cor-
L -­ −12 −72 14 3.16 tex has been activated in studies assessing attention (Doricchi et al.,
L -­ −8 −68 10 3.04 2010; Shulman et al., 2010), executive control (Wagner et al., 2001),
L -­ −18 −72 2 3.03 and theory of mind (Kobayashi et al., 2007). The parahippocampal

Posterior R 429 2 −36 46 3.71


gyrus is part of the paralimbic region and underlies context and
cingulate memory processing (Bohbot et al., 2015). With regard to parenting,
R -­ 2 −44 50 3.57
gyrus the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is posited to be part of the pa-
L -­ −2 −30 44 3.29
rental emotion-­regulation/executive network (Feldman, 2015). The
L -­ −10 −24 42 3.25
emotion-­regulation/executive network is theorized to contribute to
L -­ −12 −20 40 3.22
parents' management of emotions, attention, multi-­t asking, and or-
L -­ −4 −22 46 2.92
ganization (Feldman, 2015; Swain et al., 2014). Furthermore, in a lon-
gitudinal study of cortical plasticity in new fathers, Kim et al. (2014)
found that fathers' show an increase in gray matter volume in the lat-
(Continues) eral prefrontal cortex across the first and fourth month postpartum.
|
14      CARDENAS et al.

F I G U R E 6  Group-­level results of the Why > How contrast with oxytocin as the main regressor and education and weeks pregnant as
covariates. Sagittal, coronal, and axial view of whole-­brain activation during the Why > How contrast. Analyses cluster corrected at z = 2.3
(n = 33).

TA B L E 7  Neural regions positively


MNI coordinates
correlated with postpartum attunement
Region L/R k x y z Z during Why > How contrast.

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex R 2120 32 34 28 3.98


L -­ −12 44 36 3.74
R -­ 30 8 40 3.62
R -­ 36 22 38 3.58
L -­ −8 56 24 3.55
R -­ 14 52 26 3.5
Posterior parahippocampal gyrus R 498 14 −34 −4 3.68
R -­ 10 −36 −4 3.66
R -­ 2 −42 0 3.57
R -­ 18 −34 −14 3.15
R -­ 0 −56 16 3.09
R -­ 16 −30 2 3.06

All peaks survived a whole-­brain search thresholded at a voxel-­wise family-­wise error rate of 0.05
and a z-­threshold of 2.3. x, y, z = Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates in the left-­right
anterior-­posterior, and inferior-­superior dimensions, respectively (n = 37).

Prior research suggests that the paralimbic region, which includes Both prenatal oxytocin and postpartum endorsement of attuned
the parahippocampal gyrus, is involved in processing own-­infant parenting styles showed similar activation in the dorsolateral pre-
stimuli (Swain, 2008). We found that fathers' neural activation in frontal cortex during the Why > How contrast. As mentioned, the
response to a theory of mind task during their partner's pregnancy dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is implicated in executive functioning
predicted their subsequent principles and cognitions about attuned and is posited to support parent attention and emotion regulation
parenting at 3 months after birth. For example, they endorsed be- (Feldman, 2015). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has not been
liefs that infants benefit from eating on demand and being soothed previously associated with oxytocin within the context of parenting.
to sleep rather than kept on strict schedules. More attuned par- However, research assessing the neural underpinnings of social sup-
enting may require more competent mentalizing abilities because port found that oxytocin enhanced the benefits of romantic partner
parents need to “read” infants' cues rather than follow pre-­ordered touch during shock, and oxytocin was associated with increased ac-
routines. It is intriguing that fathers' prenatal theory of mind activa- tivity in the middle frontal gyrus, which is part of the dorsolateral
tion predicted their subsequent beliefs about this style of parenting. prefrontal cortex (Kreuder et al., 2018). Relatedly, Weisman et al.
CARDENAS et al. |
      15

F I G U R E 7  Group-­level results of the Why > How contrast with the Baby Care Questionnaire (BCQ) attunement scale as the main
regressor and with education and weeks pregnant as covariates. Sagittal, coronal, and axial view of whole brain activation during the Why >
How contrast. Analyses cluster corrected at z = 2.3 (n = 37).

F I G U R E 8  Overlapping activity during the Why > How contrast with oxytocin and postpartum attunement as the main regressors.
Sagittal, coronal, and axial view of whole-­brain activation. Analyses cluster corrected at z = 2.3 and a p value of .05 (n = 33 for oxytocin,
n = 37 for BCQ attunement).

(2014) found a link between oxytocin administration and changes in principles. As a natural extension of our work, future studies can
testosterone levels linked with enhanced fathering behaviors (e.g., assess trajectories of oxytocin, theory of mind processing, and par-
vocal synchrony, positive affect, greater touch). Within this sample, enting across the transition to parenthood to assess longitudinal
we have found that paternal postpartum oxytocin levels are asso- trajectories.
ciated with more father–­infant touch during parenting interaction This study has a number of limitations. Our sample was ethnically
(Morris et al., under review). This study found associations in the diverse, but participants were largely from highly educated back-
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during theory of mind with prenatal grounds. Although larger than other samples in the neurobiology
oxytocin and fathers' greater endorsement of attunement as a post- of parenting literature, the sample size was small (39 fathers with
partum parenting principle. Given that all variables were measured MRI data; 33 with oxytocin data; 37 with postpartum attunement
once, we cannot infer directionality or causality between prenatal data). An additional limitation is that for several of our fathers, oxy-
oxytocin and theory of mind processing and postpartum attunement tocin levels were either missing (due to refusal or sample processing
|
16      CARDENAS et al.

issues) or undetectable (due to overly low levels). Furthermore, Conceptualization (equal); Funding acquisition (lead); Writing—­
we note that this study only involved one neuroimaging timepoint Review & Editing (supporting).
during the transition to parenthood. The extant literature supports
that neural regions involved in social cognition can develop and ORCID
change over time (Blakemore, 2008; Dumontheil et al., 2010). Future Sofia I. Cardenas  https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7873-1215
research that incorporates multiple scans throughout the transition Darby E. Saxbe  https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8951-4216
to parenthood can better characterize the fathering brain. As the
first study to examine the theory of mind processing, oxytocin, and REFERENCES
postpartum parenting attitudes together in one investigation, this Abraham, E., Hendler, T., Shapira-­Lichter, I., Kanat-­Maymon, Y., Zagoory-­
work would benefit from replication and extension prior to further Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2014). Father's brain is sensitive to child-
care experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
interpretation.
of the United States of America, 111(27), 9792–­9797. https://doi.
Despite the aforementioned limitations, this study makes sev- org/10.1073/pnas.14025​69111
eral important contributions to research on parenting and child Atzil, S., Hendler, T., Zagoory-­Sharon, O., Winetraub, Y., & Feldman, R.
development. This study extends prior work on the neurobiology (2012). Synchrony and specificity in the maternal and the paternal
brain: Relations to oxytocin and vasopressin. Journal of the American
of fatherhood by using a standardized task to measure the theory
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(8), 798–­811. https://
of mind in expectant fathers. This is also the first study examining doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.06.008
prenatal oxytocin levels in tandem with brain activation in expect- Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Social ef-
ant fathers. Our sample of expectant fathers was racially and eth- fects of oxytocin in humans: Context and person matter. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7):301–­3 09. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
nically diverse and contributed longitudinal, multimodal data. Our
tics.2011.05.002
results suggest that oxytocin may support social cognition process- Blakemore, S. J. (2008). Development of the social brain during adoles-
ing during the prenatal period. Additionally, fathers who showed cence. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 40–­49.
greater prenatal neural theory of mind activation subsequently https://doi.org/10.1080/17470​21070​1508715
endorsed more attuned parenting styles at 3 months postpartum. Bohbot, V. D., Allen, J. J. B., Dagher, A., Dumoulin, S. O., Evans, A. C.,
Petrides, M., Kalina, M., Stepankova, K., & Nadel, L. (2015). Role of
These findings further the current understanding of neurobiolog-
the parahippocampal cortex in memory for the configuration but
ical factors that support fathers' preparation for parenthood and not the identity of objects: Converging evidence from patients with
highlight future directions for the parenting brain research. selective thermal lesions and fMRI. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
9, 431. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00431
Cabrera, N. J., Shannon, J. D., & Tamis-­LeMonda, C. (2007). Fathers' in-
AC K N OW L E D G M E N T S
fluence on their Children's cognitive and emotional development:
This work was supported by National Science Foundation (NSF) Career From toddlers to Pre-­K . Applied Developmental Science, 11(4), 208–­
Award (1552452; Saxbe), National Science Foundation Grant/Award 213. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888​69070​1762100
(DGE-­1842487; Cárdenas), and a Health Policy Research Scholars Cabrera, N. J., Volling, B. L., & Barr, R. (2018). Fathers are parents, too!
Widening the lens on parenting for children's development. Child
Award funded by the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (Cárdenas).
Development Perspectives, 12(3), 152–­157. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Portions of this work were presented at the International Society of cdep.12275
Developmental Psychobiology (ISDP) Conference; we thank ISDP and Cárdenas, E. F., Kujawa, A., & Humphreys, K. L. (2019). Neurobiological
their sponsors (i.e., NICHD; Columbia University's Nurture Science changes during the peripartum period: Implications for health and
behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(10), 1097–­
Project) for granting a Travel Award to Sofia Cárdenas.
1110. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsz091
Carpendale, J. I., & Chandler, M. J. (1996). On the distinction between
C O N FL I C T S O F I N T E R E S T false belief understanding and subscribing to an interpretive the-
The authors have no conflict of interests to report. ory of mind. Child Development, 67(4), 1686–­1706. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1467-­8624.1996.tb018​21.x
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understand-
AU T H O R C O N T R I B U T I O N S ing, language, and family background: Individual differences and
Sofia I. Cardenas Conceptualization (equal); Formal Analysis interrelations. Child Development, 70(4), 853–­865. https://doi.
(equal); Investigation(supporting); Writing—­
O riginal Draft org/10.1111/1467-­8624.00061
Preparation (lead); Sarah A. Stoycos: Investigation (equal)); Project Dien, J., Brian, E. S., Molfese, D. L., & Gold, B. T. (2013). Combined ERP/
fMRI evidence for early word recognition effects in the posterior
Administration (leading); Writing—­Review & Editing (supporting);
inferior temporal gyrus. Cortex, 49(9), 2307–­2321. https://doi.
Pia Sellery: Investigation (supporting); Project Administration org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.03.008
(supporting); Writing—­
Review & Editing (supporting); Narcis Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Michel, A., Berger, C., & Herpertz, S. C. (2007).
Marshall: Investigation (supporting); Writing—­
Review & Editing Oxytocin improves “mind-­reading” in humans. Biological Psychiatry,
61(6), 731–­733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops​ych.2006.07.015
(supporting); Hannah Khoddam: Investigation (supporting);
Doricchi, F., Macci, E., Silvetti, M., & Macaluso, E. (2010). Neural cor-
Project Administration (supporting); Writing—­
Review & Editing relates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous
(supporting); Jonas Kaplan: Formal Analysis (equal); Writing—­ and stimulus-­ driven orienting of attention in the Posner task.
Review & Editing (supporting); Diane Goldenberg: Investigation Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1574–­1585. https://doi.org/10.1093/cerco​
r/bhp215
(equal); Project Administration (supporting); Darby E. Saxbe:
CARDENAS et al. |
      17

Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Online usage Jenkinson, M., & Smith, S. (2001). A global optimization method for ro-
of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. bust affine registration of brain images. Medical Image Analysis, 5(2),
Developmental Science, 13(2), 331–­338. https://doi.org/10.1111/ 143–­156. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1361​-­8 415(01)00036​-­6
j.1467-­7687.​2009.00888.x Keysar, B., Lin, S., & Barr, D. J. (2003). Limits on theory of mind use in
Edelstein, R., Chopik, W. J., Saxbe, D. E., Wardecker, B. M., Moors, A. adults. Cognition, 89(1), 25–­ 41. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010​
C., & LaBelle, O. P. (2017). Prospective and dyadic associations -­0277(03)00064​-­7
between expectant parents' prenatal hormone changes and post- Khoddam, H., Goldenberg, D., Stoycos, S. A., Horton, K. T., Marshall,
partum parenting outcomes. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(1), N., Cárdenas, S. I., Kaplan, J., & Saxbe, D. (2020). How do expect-
77–­90. ant fathers respond to infant cry? Examining brain and behav-
Ereky-­Stevens, K. (2008). Associations between mothers' sensitivity to ioral responses and the moderating role of testosterone. Social
their infants' internal states and children's later understanding of Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(4), 437–­4 46. https://doi.
mind and emotion. Infant and Child Development, 17(5), 527–­543. org/10.1093/scan/nsaa051
https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.572 Kim, P., Rigo, P., Mayes, L. C., Feldman, R., Leckman, J. F., & Swain, J. E. (2014).
Feldman, R. (2015). The adaptive human parental brain: Implications for Neural plasticity in fathers of human infants. Social Neuroscience, 9(5),
children's social development. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(6), 387–­ 522–­535. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470​919.2014.933713
399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.004 Kobayashi, C., Glover, G. H., & Temple, E. (2007). Children's and
Feldman, R., & Bakermans-­Kranenburg, M. J. (2017). Oxytocin: A parent- adults' neural bases of verbal and nonverbal “theory of mind”.
ing hormone. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 13–­18. https://doi. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1522–­1532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.011 neuro​psych​ologia.2006.11.017
Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Schneiderman, I., Weisman, O., & Zagoory-­ Kreuder, A. K., Wassermann, L., Wollseifer, M., Ditzen, B., Eckstein, M.,
Sharon, O. (2010). Natural variations in maternal and paternal Stoffel-­Wagner, B., Hennig, J., Hurlemann, R., & Scheele, D. (2018).
care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following Oxytocin enhances the pain-­relieving effects of social support in
parent-­infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(8), 1133–­1141. romantic couples. Human Brain Mapping, 40(1), 242–­251. https://
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2010.01.013 doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24368
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, Kuo, P. X., Carp, J., Light, K. C., & Grewen, K. M. (2012). Neural re-
50(4), 531–­534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001 sponses to infants linked with behavioral interactions and testos-
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology terone in fathers. Biological Psychology, 91(2), 302–­3 06. https://doi.
of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: org/10.1016/j.biops​ycho.2012.08.002
Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459–­473. https://doi.org/10.1098/ Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J. (1995). False belief understanding goes
rstb.2002.1218 to school: On the social-­emotional consequences of coming early
Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2–­3), 167–­
of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 77–­83. https://doi. 185. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699​93950​8 409007
org/10.1016/S1364​-­6613(02)00025​-­6 Leake, R. D., Weitzman, R. E., Glatz, T. H., & Fisher, D. A. (1981). Plasma
Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & oxytocin concentrations in men, nonpregnant women, and preg-
Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: nant women before and during spontaneous labor. The Journal of
An fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 53(4), 730–­733. https://doi.
Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 11–­ 21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028​ org/10.1210/jcem-­53-­4-­730
-­3932(99)00053​-­6 Lefevre, A., Mottolese, R., Dirheimer, M., Mottolese, C., Duhamel, J.-­R .,
George, M. R. W., Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2010). Positive as- & Sirigu, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to measure central
pects of fathering and mothering, and children's attachment in and peripheral oxytocin concentrations in human and non-­human
kindergarten. Early Child Development and Care, 180(1–­2), 107–­119. primates. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 17222. https://doi.org/10.1038/
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004​43090​3 414752 s4159​8-­017-­17674​-­7
Gettler, L. T. (2014). Applying socioendocrinology to evolutionary mod- Li, W., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2014). The default mode network and social
els: Fatherhood and physiology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 23(4), understanding of others: What do brain connectivity studies tell
146–­160. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21412 us? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 74. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Gordon, I., Zagoory-­Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2010). fnhum.2014.00074
Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biological MacLean, E. L., Wilson, S. R., Martin, W. L., Davis, J. M., Nazarloo, H. P.,
Psychiatry, 68(4), 377–­382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biops​ & Carter, C. S. (2019). Challenges for measuring oxytocin: The blind
ych.2010.02.005 men and the elephant? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 107, 225–­231.
Gregory, R., Cheng, H., Rupp, H. A., Sengelaub, D. R., & Heiman, J. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2019.05.018
R. (2015). Oxytocin increases VTA activation to infant and sex- Malin, J. L., Karberg, E., Cabrera, N. J., Rowe, M., Cristaforo, T., &
ual stimuli in nulliparous and postpartum women. Hormones Tamis-­ LeMonda, C. S. (2012). Father–­ toddler communication
and Behavior, 69, 82–­8 8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.​ in low-­income families: The role of paternal education and de-
12.009 pressive symptoms. Family Science, 3(3–­4), 155–­163. https://doi.
Hoekzema, E., Barba-­ Müller, E., Pozzobon, C., Picado, M., Lucco, F., org/10.1080/19424​620.2012.779423
García-­García, D., Soliva, J. C., Tobeña, A., Desco, M., Crone, E. Marsh, A. A. (2016). Understanding amygdala responsiveness to fearful
A., Ballesteros, A., Carmona, S., & Vilarroya, O. (2017). Pregnancy expressions through the lens of psychopathy and altruism. Journal
leads to long-­ lasting changes in human brain structure. Nature of Neuroscience Research, 94(6), 513–­525. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Neuroscience, 20(2), 287–­296. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4458 jnr.23668
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2002). Improved opti- Mascaro, J. S., Hackett, P. D., & Rilling, J. K. (2014). Differential neural
mization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion responses to child and sexual stimuli in human fathers and non-­
correction of brain images. NeuroImage, 17(2), 825–­8 41. https:// fathers and their hormonal correlates. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
doi.org/10.1016/s1053​-­8119(02)91132​-­8 46, 153–­163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2014.04.014
Jenkinson, M., & Chappell, M. (Eds.), (2018). Introduction to neuroimaging Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J. D., & Mattingley, J. B. (2016).
analysis, (1st ed.) New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Understanding the minds of others: A neuroimaging meta-­analysis.
|
18      CARDENAS et al.

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 276–­291. https://doi. Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens,
org/10.1016/j.neubi​orev.2016.03.020 T. E. J., Johansen-­Berg, H., Bannister, P. R., De Luca, M., Drobnjak,
Morris, A. R., Turner, A., Gilbertson, C. H., Corner, G., Mendez, A. J., & I., Flitney, D. E., Niazy, R. K., Saunders, J., Vickers, J., Zhang, Y.,
Saxbe, D. E. (under review). Physical touch during father-­infant De Stefano, N., Brady, J. M., & Matthews, P. M. (2004). Advances
interactions is associated with paternal oxytocin levels. Infant in functional and structural MR image analysis and implemen-
Behavior and Development. tation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23(Suppl 1), S208–­ S219. https://doi.
Naber, F., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Deschamps, P., van Engeland, H., & org/10.1016/j.neuro​image.2004.07.051
Bakermans-­Kranenburg, M. J. (2010). Intranasal oxytocin increases Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2014). Validating the why/how contrast for
fathers' observed responsiveness during play with their children: A functional MRI studies of theory of mind. NeuroImage, 99, 301–­311.
double-­blind within-­subject experiment. Psychoneuroendocrinology, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​image.2014.05.023
35(10), 1583–­1586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyne​ Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Dissociating modality-­specific and
uen.2010.04.007 supramodal neural systems for action understanding. The Journal of
Norholt, H. (2020). Revisiting the roots of attachment: A review of the bi- Neuroscience, 32(10), 3575–­3583. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUR​
ological and psychological effects of maternal skin-­to-­skin contact OSCI.5715-­11.2012
and carrying of full-­term infants. Infant Behavior and Development, Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., & Wynne-­Edwards, K. E. (2000).
60, 101441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101441 Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and expect-
Noriuchi, M., Kikuchi, Y., & Senoo, A. (2008). The functional neuro- ant fathers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(2), 79–­95. https://doi.
anatomy of maternal love: Mother's response to infant's attach- org/10.1016/S1090​-­5138(99)00042​-­2
ment behaviors. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 415–­423. https://doi. Storey, A. E., & Ziegler, T. E. (2016). Primate paternal care: Interactions
org/10.1016/j.biops​ych.2007.05.018 between biology and social experience. Hormones and Behavior, 77,
Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-­Feagans, L. (2010). Fathers' early contributions 260–­271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.07.024
to children's language development in families from low-­income Swain, J. E. (2008). Baby stimuli and the parent brain: Functional neu-
rural communities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(4), 450–­ roimaging of the neural substrates of parent-­infant attachment.
463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.001 Psychiatry, 5(8), 28–­36.
Rilling, J. K. (2013). The neural and hormonal bases of human parental Swain, J. E., Kim, P., Spicer, J., Ho, S. S., Dayton, C. J., Elmadih, A., & Abel,
care. Neuropsychologia, 51(4), 731–­747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. K. M. (2014). Approaching the biology of human parental attach-
neuro​psych​ologia.2012.12.017 ment: Brain imaging, oxytocin and coordinated assessments of
Rilling, J. K., & Mascaro, J. S. (2017). The neurobiology of fatherhood. mothers and fathers. Brain Research, 1580, 78–­101. https://doi.
Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 26–­32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. org/10.1016/j.brain​res.2014.03.007
copsyc.2017.02.013 Szeto, A., McCabe, P. M., Nation, D. A., Tabak, B. A., Rossetti, M. A.,
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-­neuron system. Annual McCullough, M. E., Schneiderman, N., & Mendez, A. J. (2011).
Review of Neuroscience, 27(1), 169–­192. https://doi.org/10.1146/ Evaluation of enzyme immunoassay and radioimmunoassay
annur​ev.neuro.27.070203.144230 methods for the measurement of plasma oxytocin. Psychosomatic
Sarkadi, A., Kristiansson, R., Oberklaid, F., & Bremberg, S. (2008). Fathers' Medicine, 73(5), 393–­4 00. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013​
involvement and children's developmental outcomes: A systematic e3182​1df0c2
review of longitudinal studies. Acta Paediatrica, 97(2), 153–­158. Tabak, B. A., McCullough, M. E., Szeto, A., Mendez, A. J., & McCabe, P.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-­2227.2007.00572.x M. (2011). Oxytocin Indexes relational distress following interper-
Saxbe, D. E., Edelstein, R. S., Lyden, H. A., Wardecker, B. M., Chopik, W. sonal harms in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(1), 115–­122.
J., & Moors, A. C. (2017). Fathers' prenatal testosterone decline and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyne​uen.2010.07.004
synchrony with partner testosterone predicts greater postpartum Thesen, S., Heid, O., Mueller, E., & Schad, L. R. (2000). Prospective ac-
relationship investment. Hormones & Behavior, 90, 39–­47. quisition correction for head motion with image-­based tracking for
Saxbe, D., Khaled, M., Horton, K. T., & Mendez, A. J. (2019). Maternal real-­time fMRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 44(3), 457–­465.
prenatal plasma oxytocin is positively associated with prenatal https://doi.org/10.1002/1522-­2594(20000​9)44:3<457:aid-­mrm17​
psychological symptoms, but method of immunoassay extraction >3.0.co;2-­r
may affect results. Biological Psychology, 147, 107718. https://doi. Wagner, A. D., Maril, A., Bjork, R. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2001). Prefrontal
org/10.1016/j.biops​ycho.2019.107718 contributions to executive control: fMRI evidence for functional dis-
Saxe, R., Moran, J. M., Scholz, J., & Gabrieli, J. (2006). Overlapping and tinctions within lateral prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage, 14, 1337–­1347.
non-­overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection Weimer, A. A., Parault Dowds, S. J., Fabricius, W. V., Schwanenflugel, P. J.,
in individual subjects. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, & Suh, G. W. (2017). Development of constructivist theory of mind
1(3), 229–­234. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl034 from middle childhood to early adulthood and its relation to social
Scatliffe, N., Casavant, S., Vittner, D., & Cong, X. (2019). Oxytocin and cognition and behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
early parent-­infant interactions: A systematic review. International 154, 28–­45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.002
Journal of Nursing Sciences, 6(4), 445–­453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Weisman, O., Zagoory-­Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2014). Oxytocin ad-
ijnss.2019.09.009 ministration, salivary testosterone, and father–­infant social behav-
Seghier, M. L. (2013). The angular gyrus: Multiple functions and mul- ior. Progress in Neuro-­Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry,
tiple subdivisions. The Neuroscientist: A Review Journal Bringing 49, 47–­52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.11.006
Neurobiology, Neurology and Psychiatry, 19(1), 43–­61. https://doi. Winstanley, A., & Gattis, M. (2013). The Baby Care Questionnaire: A
org/10.1177/10738​58412​4 40596 measure of parenting principles and practices during infancy. Infant
Shulman, G. L., Pope, D. L. W., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., Snyder, Behavior & Development, 36(4), 762–­775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
A. Z., & Corbetta, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dominance during infbeh.2013.08.004
spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside Wittfoth-­Schardt, D., Gründing, J., Wittfoth, M., Lanfermann, H.,
the dorsal frontoparietal network. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(10), Heinrichs, M., Domes, G., Buchheim, A., Gündel, H., & Waller, C.
3640–­3651. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUR​OSCI.4085-­09.2010 (2012). Oxytocin modulates neural reactivity to children's faces
Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain as a function of social salience. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(8),
Mapping, 17(3), 143–­155. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062 1799–­1807. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.47
CARDENAS et al. |
      19

Wu, H., Tang, H., Ge, Y., Yang, S., Mai, X., Luo, Y. J., & Liu, C. (2017).
Object words modulate the activity of the mirror neuron system How to cite this article: Cardenas SI, Stoycos SA, Sellery P,
during action imitation. Brain and Behavior, 7(11), e00840. https:// et al. Theory of mind processing in expectant fathers:
doi.org/10.1002/brb3.840
Associations with prenatal oxytocin and parental
Yue, T., Jiang, Y., Yue, C., & Huang, X. (2017). Differential effects of oxy-
tocin on visual perspective taking for men and women. Frontiers attunement. Dev Psychobiol. 2021;00:1–19. https://doi.
in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 228. https://doi.org/10.3389/ org/10.1002/dev.22115
fnbeh.2017.00228

You might also like