You are on page 1of 10

Current Psychology

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00526-0

Materialism and adolescent problematic smartphone use:


The mediating role of fear of missing out and the moderating role
of narcissism
Jie Long 1 & Pengcheng Wang 1 & Shuoyu Liu 2 & Li Lei 1

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
Previous studies have shown that materialism, a series of values focused on wealth, possessions, image, and status, is an
important risk factor in the dependence of smartphone. However, little is known about the mediating and moderating mechanisms
behind this relationship. Based on the theory of self-determination, this study examined whether fear of missing out (FoMO)
would mediate the relationship between materialism and adolescent problematic smartphone use, and investigated the moderat-
ing role of narcissism in this mediating process. Our theoretical model was tested among 677 middle school students in China
(mean age = 16.79, standard deviation = 0.72). The participants provided self-reported data on demographic variables, material-
ism, FoMO, narcissism, and problematic smartphone use. The results indicated that materialism was positively related to
adolescent problematic smartphone use. Mediation analysis showed that FoMO mediated this relation. Moderated mediation
test further revealed that the mediated path was moderated by narcissism. This study highlights the mediating and moderating
mechanisms linking materialism to adolescent problematic smartphone use, which has important implications for the prevention
and intervention of problematic smartphone use among adolescents.

Keywords Adolescent . Materialism . Problematic smartphone use . Fear of missing out . Narcissism

Introduction and well-being (Jia et al. 2018; Kuss et al. 2014). Problematic
smartphone use is defined as Binability to regulate one’s use of
Nowadays, smartphones play a very important role in people’s the smartphone, which eventually involves negative conse-
lives because of the variety of their functions and their porta- quences in daily life (Long et al. 2016)^ . The negative influ-
bility, and there are more than 700 million smartphone users in ences of problematic smartphone use on teenagers have re-
China who spend an average of 27.6 h a week online. Instant ceived widespread attention, for example, prior researches
messaging, web music and short video apps account for most show problematic smartphone use is positively associated
of the online time of mobile netizens. (CNNIC 2018). with sleep disturbance, depression symptoms, chronic stress,
Adolescents are known to become addicted to smartphones academic performance (Augner and Hacker 2012; Dewi et al.
more likely than adults, which can be harmful to their health 2018; Elhai et al. 2017; Samaha and Hawi 2016), and even
increased suicidal ideation (Katsumata et al. 2008).
Jie Long and Pengcheng Wang are co-first authors who contribute equally Considering the negative impacts of problematic
to this work. smartphone use, it is important to explore the causes of prob-
lematic smartphone use. Previous studies have found that per-
* Pengcheng Wang sonality traits and offline interpersonal relationships are pre-
tianhai001@126.com
dictors for problematic smartphone use. For example, neurot-
* Li Lei icism, openness, conscientiousness and student-student rela-
dr.leili@qq.com
tionship predict the risk for smartphone addiction negatively
1 (Cocoradă et al. 2018; Roberts et al. 2015; Wang et al. 2017a,
Department of Psychology, The Center of Internet + Social
Psychology, Renmin University of China, No. 59 Zhongguancun b). In addition, maladaptive personality traits and dysfunction-
Street, Haidian District, Beijing 100872, China al parent-child relationships, which are also predictive of
2
School of Economics and Management, Ningxia University, No. 489 problematic smartphone use among adolescents (Trumello
Helanshan West Road, Xixia District, Yinchuan, Ningxia, China et al. 2018). However, few studies have explored the

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

relationship between personal values (e.g., materialism) and prevention strategies. The purpose of this study is to validate
adolescent problematic smartphone use. So there remains a a conceptual model in which, first, materialism will aggravate
need to inspect more meticulously the dynamics of potential the teen’s fear of missing out (FoMO), which in turn will
mechanisms that can explain problematic smartphone use. increase the likelihood of adolescent problematic smartphone
Materialism refers to a set of beliefs that emphasize the use; second, the associations between materialism and prob-
importance of possessions to one’s personal life (Richins lematic smartphone use through FoMO will be moderated by
and Dawson 1992). Recently, researchers have found that ma- narcissism.
terialism is related to smartphone addiction (Roberts and Pirog
III 2012). Recent theoretical developments suggest that mate- The mediating role of fear of missing out (FoMO)
rialism is symptomatic of an underlying feeling of psycholog-
ical insecurity, some people use materialism in order to at- FoMO is a psychological trait that is pervasive in the individ-
tempt to deal with their feelings of insecurity (Kasser 2016). uals, and people with high level of FoMO are worried that
Such negative life situations can motivate people to use the others are acquiring awarding experiences in their absence
Internet for reducing negative emotions. The model of com- (Beyens et al. 2016; Elhai et al. 2016; Przybylski et al.
pensatory internet use explained why some people keep 2013). Some theorists believe that FoMO can increase the
spending so much time online: people use the Internet to es- use of social media by teenagers to learn about the status of
cape real-life issues or alleviate dysphoric moods (Kardefelt- others (Przybylski et al. 2013; Vaidya et al. 2016). People with
Winther 2014). According to prior research, adolescents with high FoMO are more likely to use social media sites and are
elevated psychological insecurity are more likely to develop more likely to use their phones while driving or in class
problematic smartphone use (Jia et al. 2017). People with high (Przybylski et al. 2013). Further findings indicate that
levels of materialism tend to use the property or expensive FoMO is a predictor of problematic smartphone use, with
items to display their social image and highlight their status, higher levels of FoMO associated with more problematic
and as a common item that can be shown to others, a smartphone use (Elhai et al. 2016; Wolniewicz et al. 2018).
smartphone is often considered to be a representative that According to the self - determination theory (SDT), peo-
can show off wealth to others. At the same time, people with ple’s well-being depends on the satisfaction of their basic psy-
high levels of materialism tend to use valuable assets such as a chological needs (Deci and Ryan 1985; Ryan and Deci 2000).
smartphone to communicate, so the higher levels of material- People who pursue material as a priority have less satisfaction
ism may lead to a high degree of reliance on a smartphone with their demands for autonomy, ability, and intimacy. Since
(Lee et al. 2018). In addition, materialists can use the network they have excessive attention to external factors like money,
(such as through social network sites) to show off their own material wealth, and image, but ignore the need to chase those
wealth and status. That is to say, the smartphone can serve as a that might lead to more well-being. Thus, these low levels of
good platform to satisfy their needs in this regard. basic need satisfaction lead to low happiness for people with
From another point of view, it’s not difficult to find that high levels of materialism (Dittmar et al. 2014). Through the
materialism is an important risk factor for problematic SDT, the FoMO phenomenon can be understood as self-
smartphone use among teenagers. On one hand, materialism regulatory limbo arising from situational or chronic deficits
leads to lower life satisfaction and low self-esteem (Dittmar in psychological need satisfaction, and that is to say, people’s
et al. 2014), and smartphone as a kind of material wealth that low levels of basic need satisfaction tend to lead to higher
can show off, may bring a certain degree of material satisfac- FoMO.
tion to young people, thereby improving their life satisfaction For one thing, compared with the less materialistic, people
and self-esteem (Katz and Sugiyama 2005). On the other with high materialism often trapped in the vicious circle of
hand, some empirical studies have found that materialism is loneliness (Pieters 2013), have an anxious attachment style
positively related to smartphone addiction (Lee et al. 2017; (Norris et al. 2012), and find it more difficult to balance family
Lee et al. 2018; Roberts et al. 2015). In line with this notion, and work commitments (Promislo et al. 2010). For another,
a cross-sectional study indicates that materialism is positively materialism is also associated with lower quality interpersonal
associated with problematic smartphone use (Wang et al. relationships (Solberg et al. 2004). Studies conducted over
2018a, b, c). time have shown that the increase in psychopathology and
Nevertheless, there is little research on the relationship be- the decline in the well-being of American adolescents and
tween materialism and adolescent problematic smartphone Norwegian participants (Twenge et al. 2010; Hellevik 2003)
use, and the role of mediating and moderating variables that are associated with higher level of materialism. A meta-
may be involved in this relationship are still largely unex- analysis shows that materialism has a significant negative cor-
plored. Exploring the mediating and moderating mechanisms relation with personal well-being (Dittmar et al. 2014). What’s
is essential to improve our understanding of problematic more, personal well-being often affects people’s FoMO, and
smartphone use and to develop effective intervention and some studies examined the links between FoMO and

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

psychological well-being and found that FoMO is associated The present study
with negative experiences (Turkle 2017). Therefore, it is rea-
sonably assumed that there is an association between materi- To further disentangle the nature of this relationship between
alism and FoMO. So far, no research has directly studied the materialism and adolescent problematic smartphone use. The
mediating role of FoMO in the relationship between material- present study tested a conceptual model of the processes
ism and adolescent problematic smartphone use. which taking Chinese adolescents as the research object. In
this model, materialism is positively related to adolescent
problematic smartphone use. Based on previous research
The moderating role of narcissism and analysis, the following associations were hypothesized.
We hypothesized that (a) High materialism will be positively
Although materialism is considered to be a risk factor for related to FoMO, which in turn will be positively associated
adolescent problematic smartphone use through the interme- with problematic smartphone use. In other words, FoMO
diary role of FoMO, it may not always be the case due to would play a mediating role in the relationship between ma-
individual differences (Lycett and Dunbar 2000; Roberts and terialism and adolescent problematic smartphone use, and (b)
Pirog III 2012). Narcissism, for example, may serve as a mod- narcissism would moderate the direct and indirect relations
erator in the relationships between materialism, problematic between materialism and problematic smartphone use via
smartphone use, and FoMO. FoMO. Specifically, the direct and indirect relations between
Narcissism is defined as a personality trait reflecting a gran- materialism and problematic smartphone use would be much
diose and inflated self-concept (Buffardi and Campbell 2008), stronger for adolescents with higher Narcissism.
and narcissism is related to positive and inflated self-views of Figure 1 illustrates the proposed model.
agentic traits like intelligence, superiority and power, and be
admired by others (Buffardi and Campbell 2008; Pincus et al.
2009; YANG et al. 2017). A cross-temporal meta-analysis Method
found that narcissism levels have risen over the generations
(Twenge et al. 2008). Narcissists often have a strong desire for Participants
expression, seeking to achieve psychological satisfaction
through enhanced self-presentation and the establishment of This study conducted among a sample of adolescents from
a large number of interpersonal relationships (Campbell and two middle schools in Hebei Province, China. We acquired
Campbell 2009; Stoeber et al. 2015). informed consent from adolescents and schools. The final
According to the self-presentation theory, people often sample consisted of 677 participants (58.9% boys, age range
consciously express themselves according to certain patterns 15–19 years, Mage = 16.79, SDage = .72, 88% from urban
in order to make an impression they expect on others and to areas). For the parental education levels, 74% of the fathers
achieve a predetermined purpose (Goffman 2002). The nar- and 77% of the mothers had less than a high school education.
cissists tend to update their status (such as post attractive These students completed a survey designed to collect data
selfies on social networking sites or promote their own con- regarding demographic variables (gender, age, from urban or
tent) for self-presentation purposes more frequently than do rural areas, the educational qualifications and professions of
non-narcissists (Bergman et al. 2011). That is to say, People fathers and mothers), materialism, FoMO, Narcissism, and
with a high level of narcissism are keen to be perceived as problematic smartphone use.
more excellent, which is manifested by showing off the
material wealth of the smartphone itself and presenting a Measures
superior status on the Internet (particularly on SNSs)
(Andreassen et al. 2017). Thus, we believe that narcissism Materialism
will moderate the effects of high materialism on adolescent
smartphone addiction so that high narcissists will be more The Chinese version of the Material Values Scale (MVS) was
actively influenced by materialism than low narcissists. In administered to assess the level of materialism in participants
addition, narcissism may exacerbate the relationship between (Richins and Dawson 1990; Richins and Dawson 1992; Wang
FoMO and problematic smartphone use. A central feature of et al. 2018a, b, c; Wang et al. 2019a, b, c, d) and consisted of
narcissism is self-enhancement, and narcissists maintain pos- 18 items: 6 items for possession-defined success (i.e., BI don’t
itive self-esteem by demeaning others. Therefore, narcissists place much emphasis on the number of material objects peo-
often like to compete with competitive tasks (Morf et al. ple own as a sign of success^), and 7 items for acquisition
2000; Tanchotsrinon et al. 2007). Narcissism will make peo- centrality (i.e., BI try to keep my life simple, as far as posses-
ple with high FoMO more nervous, and thus more willing to sions are concerned^), and 5 items for pursuit of happiness
invest in the network. through acquisition (i.e., BIt sometimes bothers me quite a bit

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Fig. 1 illustrates the proposed FoMO


model

Materialism Problematic smartphone use

Narcissism

that I can’t afford to buy all the things I’d like^). Items were that ranges from B1 = not very true of me^ to B7 = very true of
scored using a 5-point scale that ranges from B1 = strongly me^. The Single-Item Narcissism Scale is related to both gran-
disagree^ to B5 = strongly agree^, with higher total scores diose and vulnerable aspects of narcissism, and previous stud-
indicating a higher level of materialism. Cronbach’s alpha in ies have examined its validity when assessing narcissism as an
the present sample was 0.71. overall construct (Konrath et al. 2014; Greenwood et al.
2013).
Problematic smartphone use

We measured problematic smartphone use with The Chinese


version of Smartphone Addiction Scale short-version (SAS- Results
SV; Kwon et al. 2013; Wang and Lei 2019; Wang et al. 2017a,
b; Wang et al. 2018a, b, c; Wang et al. 2019a, b, c, d). This Statistical analyses
scale is to evaluate the degree of excessive use of one’s
smartphone with interference in work, school or socially. Firstly, descriptive statistics and correlations among variables
The scale consists of 10 items, and a sample item is BHaving were inspected. After that, we tested the mediating role of
a hard time concentrating in class while doing assignments, or FoMO using Hayes (2017) PROCESS macro for SPSS
while working due to smartphone use^. Items were scored (Model 4). And then, we conducted multiple regressions using
using a 6-point scale that ranges from B1 = strongly disagree^ Hayes’ (Hayes 2017) SPSS Marco (Model 59) to analyze the
to B6 = strongly agree^. Cronbach’s alpha in the present sam- moderated mediation effect, which allows all possible direct
ple was 0.86. and indirect effects between materialism and problematic
smartphone use to be moderated by the moderator. We exam-
ined whether the factor narcissism moderated all pathways in
Fear of missing out the mediation model. We randomly sampled 1000 times from
the original data when doing the regressions, the 95% confi-
FoMO was measured using the Chinese version of Fear of dence intervals (CIs) without zero meant statistically signifi-
Missing Out scale (FoMOs; Przybylski et al. 2013; Wang cant. All continuous variables were standardized and interac-
et al. 2019a, b, c, d; Wang et al. 2018a, b, c; Yin et al. tion terms were computed from these standardized scores.
2019), which contains 10 items (e.g., BI get worried when I Additionally, before regression analyses, all variables were
find out my friends are having fun without me^). Items were mean-centred prior to analyses.
scored using a 5-point scale that ranges from B1 = not at all
true of me^ to B5=extremely true of me^, with higher total
scores indicating higher levels of FoMO. In the present study, Preliminary analyses
Cronbach’s alpha for the FoMOs was 0.73.
Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for all
Narcissism study variables are shown in Table 1. As the results showed,
smartphone addiction was positively associated with materi-
Narcissism was measured using the Single-Item Narcissism alism (r = .34, p < .001), FoMO (r = 0.38, p < .001) and nar-
Scale (Konrath et al. 2014), which contains only one item – cissism (r = .21, p < .001), respectively. Materialism was pos-
BTo what extent do you agree with this statement: I am a itively correlated with FoMO (r = .22, p < 0.001), narcissism
narcissist (Note: The word ‘narcissist’ means egotistical, (r = 0.23, p < .001), as well. And FoMO was positively asso-
self-focused, and vain)^. It was scored using a 5-point scale ciated with narcissism (r = .21, p < .001).

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations of the main study (Model 59) of PROCESS macro (Hayes 2017). As revealed
variables
in Table 3, only the interaction effect of FoMO and narcissism
Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 on problematic smartphone use was significant (β = .07,
p < .05). According to the previous research, the average ef-
1.Smartphone addiction 3.41 .91 1 fect size in tests of moderation published in leading journals
2.Materialism 2.81 .41 .344*** 1 was only 0.094 (Aguinis et al. 2005), so the current effect is
3.FoMO 2.83 .63 .383*** .215*** 1 medium in size. As demonstrated in Fig. 3, to illustrate the
4.Narcissism 3.32 1.60 .212*** .232*** .209*** 1 interaction of FoMO×narcissism for problematic smartphone
use, we plotted the regression of FoMO on narcissism at high
SD standard deviation
and low levels of problematic smartphone use. The simple
N = 677
slope tests showed that the association between FoMO and
***p < .001
problematic smartphone use was stronger for high narcissism
(bsimple = .38, p < .001) than for low narcissism (bsimple = .23,
Testing for mediation
p < .001). Finally, the indirect effect of materialism on adoles-
cent problematic smartphone use through FoMO was also
In Hypothesis 1, we predicted that FoMO mediated the rela-
conditioned by narcissism.
tionship between materialism and smartphone addiction. So
As is illustrated in Table 4, although the conditional indi-
we used the Model 4 of the PROCESS macro (Hayes 2017) to
rect effect was always significant, it was strongest at high
test this hypothesis. As is shown in Table 2, it showed that
levels of narcissism (conditional indirect effect = .07), weaker
materialism negatively predicted FoMO, β = .76, p < .001 (see
at medium levels of narcissism (conditional indirect effect =
Model 1 of Table 2), which in turn affected smartphone ad-
.05), and weakest at low levels of narcissism (conditional
diction, β = .33, p < .001 (see Model 2 of Table 2). At the same
indirect effect = .04). Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was partially
time, it is noted that the residual direct effect was also signif-
supported.
icant, β = .47, p < .001 (see Model 3 of Table 2), which indi-
cates that FoMO partial mediated the link between material-
ism and adolescent smartphone addiction (indirect effect =
Discussion
.15, SE = .032, 95% CI = [.095, .228]. This model accounted
for 20% of the variance in smartphone addiction among ado-
The impact of materialism on problematic smartphone use has
lescents. And the mediation of FoMO was directly shown in
garnered considerable empirical support (Lee et al. 2018;
Fig. 2. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported.
Roberts and Pirog III 2012; Takao et al. 2009). However,
few studies have utilized a process-oriented approach to ex-
Testing for moderated mediation amine the mediating and moderating mechanisms underlying
the relationship between materialism and problematic
In Hypothesis 2, the present study assumed that narcissism smartphone use. Given that the accumulation of multiple risk
would moderate the indirect relation between materialism factors can have more adverse impacts on individual develop-
and problematic smartphone use. In order to test the moderat- ment than a single risk factor (Evans et al. 2013), a more
ing effect, we performed a moderated mediation analysis comprehensive understanding of the driving forces of adoles-
cent problematic smartphone use is necessary. The present
Table 2 Testing the mediation effect of materialism on problematic
smartphone use study formulated a moderated mediation model to examine
whether materialism would be indirectly related to adolescent
Predictors Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 problematic smartphone use through FoMO, and whether this
indirect association was moderated by narcissism. The key
Problematic FoMO Problematic
smartphone use smartphone use contribution of this study is to advance our understanding of
how and when materialism is associated with adolescent prob-
b t b t b t lematic smartphone use. Moreover, the pathways of our mod-
erated mediation model provide a broad perspective on the
Materialism .34 9.51*** .21 5.73*** .27 7.86***
prevention and intervention of problematic smartphone use
FoMO .32 9.29***
among adolescents.
R2 .12 .05 .21
F 90.48*** 32.87*** 94.11***
The mediating role of FoMO
N = 677. Each column is a regression model that predicts the criterion at
the top of the column As hypothesized, this study confirmed the moderating role of
***p < .001 FoMO in the association between materialism and adolescent

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Fig. 2 The mediation of FoMO.


FoMO
The link between materialism and
problematic smartphone use is .32(.03)***
mediated by FoMO. Path values .22(.03)***
are the path coefficients (standard
errors). Direct effect = .27,
SE = .03, 95% CI [.21, .34].
Indirect effect = .07, SE = .01, Materialism Problematic smartphone use
95% CI [.04, .10]. ***p < .001.
.27(.03)***

problematic smartphone use. Therefore, FoMO is an impor- The FoMO is seen as Bself-regulatory limbo arising from sit-
tant mechanism linking materialism and adolescent problem- uational or chronic deficits in psychological need satisfaction^
atic smartphone use. As far as we know, this study is the first (Przybylski et al. 2013). That is to say, FoMO played a medi-
to report such results in the literature. These findings illumi- ating role linking materialism to problematic smartphone use.
nated how modern social and cultural values are influencing Therefore, the substantial contribution is the examination of a
new media use like smartphones. We note that the indirect moderated mediation model which embraces the self-
effect explained by FoMO is not particularly large in this determination theory (Ryan and Deci 2000). This integrated
study. One possible reason is that the impact of FoMO might model is an important extension of traditional self-
have a long-lasting effect, which was not fully reflected in this determination theory. Secondly, the compensatory Internet
cross-sectional study. In fact, in a recent review, Davies et al. use theory (Kardefelt-Winther 2014) also provides an expla-
(2016) found that the proportion of significant mediational nation for the results of the current study. The compensatory
pathways identified in longitudinal studies were actually Internet use theory assumes that the unmet psychological
higher than the results from cross-sectional studies. needs can give rise to a motivation to go online to alleviate
Additionally, the relatively small size of the effect suggests negative feelings. Specifically, the second link in the media-
that other important mediators, such as adolescents’ peer rela- tion chain shown that adolescents with higher FoMO are more
tionships, should be examined in future studies. likely to get addicted to the smartphone, as they may try to
Firstly, according to the self-determination theory, the sat- compensate for their lack of security or dull their sense of
isfaction of basic psychological needs is significant to the insecurity through the Internet.
healthy development of individuals. In order to meet their
needs, people with a high level of materialism, who emphasize
the importance of possessions to their personal life, need to The moderating role of narcissism
make proactive behavioural regulation (Ryan and Deci 2000).
As another important aspect of the process-oriented research,
Table 3 Coefficients for the moderated mediation model hypothesis 2 assumed that narcissism would moderate the
coeff. SE LLCI ULCI 1

FoMO 0.8 Low narcissism


Materialism .18 ***
.04 .10 .25 0.6 High narcissism
Narcissism .17*** .04 .09 .24 0.4
Problematic smartphone use

Materialism × Narcissism .01 .04 −.06 .09


0.2
R2 = .27
F (3,673) =17.77*** 0
Low FoMO High FoMO
Problematic smartphone use -0.2
***
Materialism .26 .04 .19 .33
-0.4
FoMO .30*** .04 .23 .37
Narcissism .05* .04 .02 .15 -0.6
Materialism × Narcissism −.00 .03 −.06 .06 -0.8
FoMO× Narcissism .07* .03 .01 .15 -1
R2 = .48
Fig. 3 Problematic smartphone use among adolescents as a function of
F (5,671) =40.35*** FoMO and narcissism. Functions are graphed for two levels of positive
coping: 1 standard deviation above the mean and 1 standard deviation
N = 677. SE = standard error, LLCI = lower limit of the 95% confidence below the mean. Note that the graph is for descriptive purpose only. All
interval, ULCI = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval inferential analyses maintained the continuous values of FoMO and
*p < .05. ***p < .001 narcissism

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Table 4 Conditional indirect effect of materialism on problematic Despite these limitations, our findings have important practi-
smartphone use via FoMO at different levels of narcissism
cal implications. First, given that the current study indicates that
Level of narcissism Indirect effect Boot SE LLCI ULCI materialism is a risk factor for adolescent problematic
smartphone use, so the parents and teachers should help adoles-
Low (1 SD below mean) .04 .02 .01 .08 cents establish correct values and reduce their obsession with
Medium (mean) .05 .05 .03 .08 wealth. In addition, social media campaigns should be less about
High (1 SD above mean) .07 .02 .03 .12 instilling materialistic values in the adolescent. Second, we dem-
onstrated the mediating role of FoMO in the relationship be-
N = 677. SE = standard error, LLCI = lower limit of the 95% confidence
interval, ULCI = upper limit of the 95% confidence interval tween materialism and problematic smartphone use. This finding
may help practitioners understand pathways by which materialist
is related to adolescent problematic smartphone use. Therefore,
direct and/or indirect relationship between materialism and strengthening the friendship between adolescent and their peers
problematic smartphone use. The results showed that narcis- may help reduce adolescent problematic smartphone use. Third,
sism only moderated the path between FoMO and adolescent our study found that narcissism could moderate the link between
problematic smartphone use (second-stage moderation). materialism and problematic smartphone use, which suggests
Specifically, we found that the effects of FoMO on problem- that parents and teachers should help adolescent establish correct
atic smartphone use were stronger for adolescents with high self-perception. Especially, the therapists, whose youth clients
rather than low narcissism. But the previous study has found suffering from materialism and/or problematic smartphone use,
that narcissism turned out not to be directly related to fear of need be informed about mechanisms that influence individuals
missing out (Błachnio and Przepiórka 2018). to problematic smartphone use such as FoMO and narcissism.
Contrary to our expectation, narcissism did not moderate the
relationship between materialism and problematic smartphone
use. This result could be explained by the robust influence of Conclusions
mechanism on problematic smartphone use. Besides, we did
not find evidence for the moderating role of narcissism in the In sum, this study contributes to the literature by examining a
relationship between materialism and adolescent problematic moderated mediation model, which provides a unique per-
smartphone use, that is, the direct and/or indirect associations spective to understanding how and when materialism relates
between materialism and problematic smartphone use did not to adolescent problematic smartphone use. It provides evi-
vary as a function of narcissism. Nonetheless, it’s premature to dence that the link between materialism and adolescent prob-
discount the importance of narcissism, as more research is lematic smartphone use was partially mediated by FoMO. In
needed before we can draw any definitive conclusions about addition, the findings revealed that narcissism mitigated the
its role in altering the association between materialism and ad- adverse impact of FoMO on adolescent problematic
olescent problematic smartphone use. smartphone use. These findings advance the current under-
standing of the mechanisms linking materialism and adoles-
Limitations and practical implications cent problematic smartphone use.

Several limitations must be considered when interpreting the Acknowledgements This research was supported by fund for building
results of the present study. First, the cross-sectional design of world-class universities (disciplines) of Renmin University of China.
this study precluded examination of causality or directionality.
Although the current study suggests that materialism may lead Compliance with ethical standards
to adolescent problematic smartphone use, it is also possible
Conflict of interest On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author
that adolescent problematic smartphone use increases materi- states that there is no conflict of interest.
alism (Wang et al. 2018a, b, c). Future research can apply
experimental or longitudinal designs to better delineate direc-
tionality of these relationships. Second, this study relied pri-
References
marily on adolescents’ self-reports to collect data, future stud-
ies should collect data from multiple informants (e.g., adoles- Aguinis, H., Beaty, J. C., Boik, R. J., & Pierce, C. A. (2005). Effect size
cent, parent, teacher, or peer). Finally, the model in this study and power in assessing moderating effects of categorical variables
was examined among middle school students rather than clin- using multiple regression: A 30-year review. Journal of Applied
ical samples that met criteria for problematic smartphone use. Psychology, 90(1), 94–107.
Andreassen, C. S., Pallesen, S., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). The relation-
Therefore, these results should not be generalized to other ship between addictive use of social media, narcissism, and self-
groups. Future studies can test this model in other samples esteem: Findings from a large national survey. Addictive
such as the clinical samples. Behaviors, 64, 287–293.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Augner, C., & Hacker, G. W. (2012). Associations between problematic Hellevik, O. (2003). Economy, values and happiness in Norway. Journal
mobile phone use and psychological parameters in young adults. of Happiness Studies, 4(3), 243–283.
International Journal of Public Health, 57(2), 437–441. Jia, J., Li, D., Li, X., Zhou, Y., Wang, Y., & Sun, W. (2017). Psychological
Bergman, S. M., Fearrington, M. E., Davenport, S. W., & Bergman, J. Z. security and deviant peer affiliation as mediators between teacher-
(2011). Millennials, narcissism, and social networking: What narcis- student relationship and adolescent internet addiction. Computers in
sists do on social networking sites and why. Personality and Human Behavior, 73, 345–352.
Individual Differences, 50(5), 706–711. Jia, J., Li, D., Li, X., Zhou, Y., Wang, Y., Sun, W., & Zhao, L. (2018). Peer
Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). BI don’t want to miss a victimization and adolescent internet addiction: The mediating role
thing^: Adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to ado- of psychological security and the moderating role of teacher-student
lescents’ social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. relationships. Computers in Human Behavior, 85, 116–124.
Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 1–8. Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014). A conceptual and methodological critique
Błachnio, A., & Przepiórka, A. (2018). Facebook intrusion, fear of miss- of internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory
ing out, narcissism, and life satisfaction: A cross-sectional study. internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 351–354.
Psychiatry Research, 259, 514–519. Kasser, T. (2016). Materialistic values and goals. Annual Review of
Buffardi, L. E., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Narcissism and social net- Psychology, 67, 489–514.
working web sites. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Katsumata, Y., Matsumoto, T., Kitani, M., & Takeshima, T. (2008).
34(10), 1303–1314. Electronic media use and suicidal ideation in Japanese adolescents.
Campbell, W. K., & Campbell, S. M. (2009). On the self-regulatory Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 62(6), 744–746.
dynamics created by the peculiar benefits and costs of narcissism: Katz, J. E., & Sugiyama, S. (2005). Mobile Phones as Fashion
A contextual reinforcement model and examination of leadership. Statements: The Co-creation of Mobile Communication’s Public
Self and Identity, 8(2–3), 214–232. Meaning. In Computer Supported Cooperative Work. Mobile
CNNIC (2018) The 42nd China statistical report on Internet develop- Communications: Re-negotiation of the Social Sphere (pp. 63–81).
ment. Report, CNNIC, Beijing, China, March. Available at: Springer,London. https://doi.org/10.1007/1-84628-248-9_5
https://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/hlwtjbg/201808/ Konrath, S., Meier, B. P., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Development and
t20180820_70488.htm (accessed 23 May 2019). validation of the single item narcissism scale (SINS). PLoS One,
Cocoradă, E., Maican, C. I., Cazan, A.-M., & Maican, M. A. (2018). 9(8), e103469.
Assessing the smartphone addiction risk and its associations with Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., Karila, L., & Billieux, J. (2014). Internet
personality traits among adolescents. Children and Youth Services addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the
Review, 93, 345–354. last decade. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 4026–4052.
Davies, P. T., Martin, M. J., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2016). Emotional Kwon, M., Kim, D.-J., Cho, H., & Yang, S. (2013). The smartphone
security theory and developmental psychopathology. addiction scale: Development and validation of a short version for
Developmental Psychopathology, 1–66. https://doi.org/10.1002/ adolescents. PLoS One, 8(12), e83558.
9781119125556.devpsy106 Lee, Y. K., Chang, C. T., Lin, Y., & Cheng, Z. H. (2017). Heads-down
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Cognitive evaluation theory. Intrinsic tribes across four Asian countries: Antecedents of smartphone ad-
Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior, (pp. 43– diction. International Journal of Mobile Communications, 15(4),
85). Springer, Boston, MA https://doi.org/10.2307/2070638. 414–436.
Dewi, R. K., Efendi, F., Has, E. M. M., & Gunawan, J. (2018). Lee, Y.-K., Chang, C.-T., Cheng, Z.-H., & Lin, Y. (2018). How social
Adolescents’ smartphone use at night, sleep disturbance and depres- anxiety and reduced self-efficacy induce smartphone addiction in
sive symptoms. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and materialistic people. Social Science Computer Review, 36(1), 36–56.
Health. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijamh-2018-0095 Long, J., Liu, T. Q., Liao, Y. H., Qi, C., He, H. Y., Chen, S. B., & Billieux,
Dittmar, H., Bond, R., Hurst, M., & Kasser, T. (2014). The relationship J. (2016). Prevalence and correlates of problematic smartphone use
between materialism and personal well-being: A meta-analysis. in a large random sample of Chinese undergraduates. BMC
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(5), 879. Psychiatry, 16(1), 408.
Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of Lycett, J. E., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2000). Mobile phones as lekking de-
missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to vices among human males. Human Nature, 11(1), 93–104.
problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, Morf, C. C., Weir, C., & Davidov, M. (2000). Narcissism and intrinsic
509–516. motivation: The role of goal congruence. Journal of Experimental
Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017). Social Psychology, 36(4), 424–438.
Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic Norris, J. I., Lambert, N. M., DeWall, C. N., & Fincham, F. D. (2012).
review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Can’t buy me love?: Anxious attachment and materialistic values.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251–259. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(5), 666–669.
Evans, G. W., Li, D., & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child Pieters, R. (2013). Bidirectional dynamics of materialism and loneliness:
development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 1342–1396. Not just a vicious cycle. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(4), 615–
Goffman, E., (2002). The presentation of self in everyday life. 1959. 631.
Garden City, NY, p.259. https://scholar.google.com/scholar?hl=zh- Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G.
CN&as_sdt=0%2C5&q=Goffman%2C+E.%2C+%282002%29.+ C., & Levy, K. N. (2009). Initial construction and validation of the
The+presentation+of+self+in+everyday+life.+1959.+Garden+ pathological narcissism inventory. Psychological Assessment, 21(3),
City%2C+NY%2C+p.259.&btnG= 365–379.
Greenwood, D., Long, C. R., & Dal Cin, S. (2013). Fame and the social Promislo, M. D., Deckop, J. R., Giacalone, R. A., & Jurkiewicz, C. L.
self: The need to belong, narcissism, and relatedness predict the (2010). Valuing money more than people: The effects of materialism
appeal of fame. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), on work–family conflict. Journal of Occupational and
490–495. Organizational Psychology, 83(4), 935–953.
Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi- Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013).
tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing
Publications. out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1841–1848.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Richins, M. L., & Dawson, S. (1990). "Measuring material values: A health. National Journal of Physiology, Pharmacy and
preliminary report of scale development", in ACR North American Pharmacology, 6(1), 97.
Advances Consumer Research.Volume 17, eds. Marvin E. Wang, P., & Lei, L. (2019). How does problematic smartphone use impair
Goldberg, Gerald Gorn, and Richard W. Pollay, Provo, UT : adolescent self-esteem? A moderated mediation analysis. Current
Association for Consumer Research, (pp. 169–175). Psychology, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00232-x.
Richins, M. L., & Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values orientation for Wang, P., Zhao, M., Wang, X., Xie, X., Wang, Y., & Lei, L. (2017a). Peer
materialism and its measurement: Scale development and validation. relationship and adolescent smartphone addiction: The mediating
Journal of Consumer Research, 19(3), 303–316. role of self-esteem and the moderating role of the need to belong.
Roberts, J. A., & Pirog III, S. F. (2012). A preliminary investigation of Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 708–717.
materialism and impulsiveness as predictors of technological addic- Wang, R., Liu, H., Jiang, J., & Song, Y. (2017b). Will materialism lead to
tions among young adults. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2(1), happiness? A longitudinal analysis of the mediating role of psycho-
56–62. logical needs satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences,
Roberts, J. A., Pullig, C., & Manolis, C. (2015). I need my smartphone: A 105, 312–317.
hierarchical model of personality and cell-phone addiction. Wang, P., Lei, L., Wang, X., Nie, J., Chu, X., & Jin, S. (2018a). The
Personality and Individual Differences, 79, 13–19. exacerbating role of perceived social support and the Bbuffering^
Rose, P. (2007). Mediators of the association between narcissism and role of depression in the relation between sensation seeking and
compulsive buying: The roles of materialism and impulse control. adolescent smartphone addiction. Personality and Individual
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(4), 576. Differences, 130, 129–134.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the Wang, P., Nie, J., Wang, X., Wang, Y., Zhao, F., Xie, X., … Ouyang, M.
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well- (2018b). How are smartphones associated with adolescent material-
being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68. ism? Journal of Health Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Samaha, M., & Hawi, N. S. (2016). Relationships among smartphone 1359105318801069
addiction, stress, academic performance, and satisfaction with life. Wang, P., Xie, X., Wang, X., Wang, X., Zhao, F., Chu, X., et al. (2018c).
Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 321–325. The need to belong and adolescent authentic self-presentation on
Solberg, E. G., Diener, E., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why are materi- SNSs: A moderated mediation model involving FoMO and per-
alists less satisfied? In T. Kasser & A. D. Kanner (Eds.), Psychology ceived social support. Personality and Individual Differences, 128,
and consumer culture: The struggle for a good life in a materialistic 133–138.
world. American Psychological Association, (pp. 29–48). Wang, J., Wang, P., Yang, X., Zhang, G., Wang, X., Zhao, F., ... & Lei, L.
Washington, DC, US. https://doi.org/10.1037/10658-003. (2019a). Fear of Missing Out and Procrastination as Mediators
Stoeber, J., Sherry, S. B., & Nealis, L. J. (2015). Multidimensional per- Between Sensation Seeking and Adolescent Smartphone
fectionism and narcissism: Grandiose or vulnerable? Personality Addiction. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction,
and Individual Differences, 80, 85–90. 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00106-0
Takao, M., Takahashi, S., & Kitamura, M. (2009). Addictive personality Wang, P., Liu, S., Zhao, M., Yang, X., Zhang, G., Chu, X., et al. (2019b).
and problematic mobile phone use. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, How is problematic smartphone use related to adolescent depres-
12(5), 501–507. sion? A moderated mediation analysis. Children and Youth Services
Tanchotsrinon, P., Maneesri, K., & Campbell, W. K. (2007). Narcissism Review, 104, 104384.
and romantic attraction: Evidence from a collectivistic culture. Wang, P., Wang, X., & Lei, L. (2019c). Gender Differences Between
Journal of Research in Personality, 41(3), 723–730. Student–Student Relationship and Cyberbullying Perpetration: An
Trumello, C., Babore, A., Candelori, C., Morelli, M., & Bianchi, D. Evolutionary Perspective. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. https://
(2018). Relationship with parents, emotion regulation, and callous- doi.org/10.1177/0886260519865970.
unemotional traits in adolescents’ internet addiction. BioMed Wang, P., Wang, X., Nie, J., Zeng, P., Liu, K., Wang, J., et al. (2019d).
Research International, 2018, 7914261. Envy and problematic smartphone use: The mediating role of
Turkle, S. (2017). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology FOMO and the moderating role of student-student relationship.
and less from each other. Hachette UK. https://scholar.google.com/ Personality and Individual Differences, 146, 136–142.
scholar_lookup?title=Alone%20together%3A%20Why%20we% Wolniewicz, C. A., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., & Elhai, J. D. (2018).
20expect%20more%20from%20technology%20and%20less% Problematic smartphone use and relations with negative affect, fear
20from%20each%20other&publication_year=2012&author=S.% of missing out, and fear of negative and positive evaluation.
20Turkle Psychiatry Research, 262, 618–623.
Twenge, J. M., Konrath, S., Foster, J. D., Campbell, W. K., & Bushman, YANG, X., ZHOU, Z., LIU, Q., & NIU, G. (2017). Narcissism and social
B. J. (2008). Egos inflating over time: a cross-temporal meta-analy- networking sites use. Advances in Psychological Science, 25(9),
sis of the narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of Personality, 1552–1564.
76(4), 875–902. Yin, L., Wang, P., Nie, J., Guo, J., Feng, J., & Lei, L. (2019). Social
Twenge, J. M., Gentile, B., DeWall, C. N., Ma, D., Lacefield, K., & networking sites addiction and FoMO: The mediating role of envy
Schurtz, D. R. (2010). Birth cohort increases in psychopathology and the moderating role of need to belong. Current Psychology, 1–9.
among young Americans, 1938–2007: A cross-temporal meta-anal- https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00344-4
ysis of the MMPI. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 145–154.
Vaidya, N., Jaiganesh, S., & Krishnan, J. (2016). Prevalence of internet Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
addiction and its impact on the physiological balance of mental tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:

1. use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
2. use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
3. falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
4. use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
5. override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
6. share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at

onlineservice@springernature.com

You might also like