You are on page 1of 10

The Decision to Parent or Not: Normative and Structural Components

Author(s): Marcia G. Ory


Source: Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 40, No. 3 (Aug., 1978), pp. 531-539
Published by: National Council on Family Relations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/350933
Accessed: 27-06-2016 21:41 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

National Council on Family Relations, Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Journal of Marriage and Family

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Decision to Parent or Not:
Normative and Structural Components*
MARCIA G. ORY**
The University of Minnesota

This study addresses antecedent factors associated with the decision to parent or not
to parent. Three interrelated aspects offertility were investigated: (1) the existence of
childbearing norms; (2) the strength of reinforcing social sanctions; and (3) the influ-
ence of social structural factors on the incorporation of such norms. Findings based
on questionnaire data from a purposive sample of 84 parent and nonparent couples
were examined to evaluate the utility of normative versus structural explanations of
fertility decisions. The findings indicate that the hypothesized normative and struc-
tural factors as well as two unanticipated mediating social psychological mechanisms
differentially influence fertility outcomes.

Fertility studies have, traditionally, investi- Americans depart from the above ideal by
gated quantitative differences between large having either too few or too many children
and small families, ignoring the voluntarily questions previous assumptions regarding the
childless family as a legitimate research existence and strength of childbearing norms.
area.' The current study attempts to counter- Two alternative explanations for deviations
act this selective research bias by focusing on from traditional childbearing preferences
normative and structural factors associated have been suggested by Ritchey and Strokes
with parenthood vis-a-vis nonparenthood. (1974): (1) norms prescribing children are
The theoretical framework underlying this re- not as universal as previously characterized;
search is the assumption that the widely docu- and (2) the childbearing norm is pervasive,
mented preference among American couples but various structural influences operate
for two-to-four child families (Blake, 1966, selectively to counter the prevailing mores
1972; Commission of Population Growth and and produce deviance. Thus, in the current
The American Future, 1972; U.S. Bureau research, two models for explaining differ-
of the Census, 1976) reflects a pervasive array ential fertility outcomes observed among 168
of interacting norms and sanctions that act to middle class adults are proposed: (1) the
regulate the number of children that one normative model which explains childbearing
ought to want and ought to have. That some decisions as the product of social norms and
sanctions; and (2) the structural model which
*Revised version of a paper presented at the 1976 explains family size outcomes as the result of
meetings of the National Council on Family Relations, traditional sociodemographic factors or psy-
New York City, 1976. This paper is based on the author's chosocial attitudes.
doctoral dissertation, Purdue University, 1976. Data
were collected in May-September, 1974. Acknowledge- METHODS
ment is extended to Dr. Richard Kerckhoff for his
support throughout this research. The author would also The Sample
like to thank Drs. JoAnne Earp, Reuben Hill and Ira
Reiss for their helpful suggestions and criticisms. The The data for this exploratory research were
author, however, bears full responsibility for the content derived from responses to 168 semistructured
of this paper. questionnaires distributed in late 1974 to a
**The Minnesota Family Study Center, University of purposive sample of young middle-class,
Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455. Midwestern adults. The sample was divided
into two ideal type subgroups: 27 voluntary
'Notable exceptions to this selective research bias childless couples and 54 voluntary parent
include Bram, 1974; Houseknecht, 1977; Ritchey and couples. A subsample of 10 couples also par-
Strokes, 1974; Thoen, 1977; and Veevers, 1973c. ticipated in in-depth interviews concerning

August 1978 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY 531

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
their fertility attitudes and behaviors. desires or feels is ideal. In an attempt toward
Since the total population of nonparents is theoretical refinement, several dimensions of
relatively small (according to current census the normative structure of fertility attitudes
records [U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1973] and behaviors, the first major independent
only 5 percent of American married women variable, were measured. Respondents' opin-
report intentions to remain childless during ions of the number of children thought ideal
their lifetime), it was necessary to utilize both by most people serves as the criterion
formal and informal networks for identifying measure of dominant family norms. In order
nonparent couples. Two-thirds of the non- to better understand the nature and pattern
parents were located through their member- of fertility decisions, two related dimensions
ship in the National Organization for Non- of respondents' own family size norms were
parents. The remaining third were obtained measured: (1) the internalization of family-
from short term advertisements in local papers size decisions; and (2) the stability of family
and word-of-mouth referrals. size preferences over nine life stages from
A comparable group of parent couples, childhood to the present. The structuring of
matched in terms of marital status, general these family size preference variables pro-
age range, socioeconomic status and race, vides a simulated developmental orientation
were recruited from a population of parents that enhances an otherwise "static" cross-
whose children attended nursery school in sectional research design.3
one Midwestern university community of The presence of fertility-related social
64,000.2 sanctions, the second major independent
variable, was also decomposed into different
Measurenment of Variables
dimensions of stereotyping behaviors. The
The questionnaire included items measur- existence of social stereotyping was measured
ing: (1) adherence to fertility norms; (2) pres- from two different perspectives. Perceptions
ence of supporting social sanctions; and of other's stereotyping of couples with
(3) sociodemographic as well as psychosocial "deviant" family sizes were measured by
differentials between parents and nonpar- open-ended projective questions such as
ents. While operationalization of variables is "most people think couples who decide to
examined in greater detail elsewhere (Ory, remain childless are. . . ." Individual stereo-
1976), key independent and dependent vari- typing of nonparents vis-a-vis parents was
ables will be discussed briefly. The major operationalized by asking respondents to
dependent variable, parental status, is compare their expectations of couples
determined by each couple's current motiva- without children to couples with children in
tional as well as behavioral adherence to fer- terms of whether the childfree couples were
tility norms. For example, the nonparents in (on the average): more, less, or no different
the current study neither wanted nor had any than couples with children with respect to 12
children in their present households. Only different traits representing Veevers' (1973a)
couples with congruent parental desires were delineation of the traditional meanings of
included for study. parenthood. The strength of social sanctions
In other fertility studies, the existence of enforcing normative family sizes was mea-
underlying family-size norms are inferred sured by the extent and type of: (1) social
from certain behavioral and motivational stereotyping of couples with "deviant" family
indices such as the number of children in the sizes; (2) expected societal reactions to one's
average American family or the number of desired family size; and (3) perceptions of
children that the average American couple social pressures to conform to socially defined
ideal family size preferences. The structuring
2All respondents were currently-married Caucasians.
of the questionnaire items, as well as the
Most have (or are working toward) college degrees and, if selection of the sample population to include
working, are in professional or sales occupations. Ages
ranged from 22 to 36: parents are slightly older with a 3Due to the retrospective nature of items measuring
mean age of 29 compared to the nonparents' mean age of fertility preferences over the life cycle, a note of caution is
27. While nonparents were no more likely than parents in order. Participants' current reporting of the timing of
to be married at a later age, they were more likely, due to past fertility decisions should be considered as a rough
sampling procedures than parents to be married for indicator of timing subject to the biases of reconstructed
shorter periods of time (p < .001). history.

532 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY August 1978

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
both parents and nonparents, permits an ings pertinent to a normative explanation of
analysis and comparison of the following fertility outcomes; and (2) findings pertinent
self-other attitudes: (1) nonparents' views of to a structural explanation. The normative
parents' attitudes toward nonparents; structure will be discussed in terms of the sta-
(2) parents' perceptions of others' attitudes bility and change of family size ideals over the
toward nonparents; (3) parents' actual evalu- life cycle and the presence of social sanctions
ations of nonparents vis-a-vis parents; and regarding family size preferences. An exami-
(4) nonparents' evaluation of themselves vis- nation of the structural model includes a
a- vis parents. review of the association of sociodemographic
Finally, most of the sociodemographic and and psychosocial factors with parenthood
social psychological factors, constituting the versus nonparenthood.
third major independent variable, have been The Normative Model
operationalized by standard, closed-ended
questions and need not be described in detail. Fanilv size norms. Size norms are indicated
An emphasis was placed on variables which by the existence of commonly shared Ameri-
measure the respondent's, particularly the can family size ideals. That the majority of
female's, experience with and attitudes respondents (95.7 percent of parents and 94.5
toward extrafamilial roles. Identification with percent of nonparents) feel most people's
sex-role stereotypes was measured by a series family size preference would be two or three
of bipolar self-evaluative items adapted from children gives support to the position that
a sex stereotype scale developed by Rosen- American family size desires reflect a general
kratz et al. (1968). consensus about the desirability of having
In addition to being a measure of sex role children. Perceptions of the ideal American
identification, self-ratings on this scale reflect family size are not affected by traditional
two dimensions of self-concepts. Stereotypic sociodemographic factors, marital or family
male-valued traits such as being rational, composition variables.
independent, confident, not easily influenced For the majority of parents as well as non-
by others, and competitive, are labeled as a parents, the desire for children was internal-
"competency" cluster. On the other hand, ized at an early age. During childhood, no
stereotypic female-valued traits such as being significant qualitative differences in respon-
tactful, aware of the feelings of others, and dents' future parental desires were found
able to express tender feelings, are labeled as (p = .26). Only 4.4 percent of the parents
a "warmth-expressive" cluster. and 11.1 percent of the nonparents expressed
RESULTS an early preference for remaining childless.
Additional analyses showed that the nature
The presentation of the results will be and timing of fertility decisions was signifi-
organized around two main models: (1) find- cantly related to parental status. As

TABLE 1. THE NATURE AND TIMING OF FERTILITY DECISIONS BY CURRENT PARENTAL STATUS

Percent Reporting First Awareness of Current


Parenthood Preference at Each Life Stage
Parents Nonparents
(N = 105a) (N = 54)
Life Cycle Stages Percent Number Percent Number
No active decisionb 25.7 27 5.6 3
Childhood 3.8 4 3.7 2
High School 4.8 5 5.6 3
Engagement 12.4 13 33.3 18
At marriage 9.5 10 0.0 0
Soon after marriage 9.5 10 9.3 5
A few years after marriage 22.9 24 40.7 22
Affected by accidental pregnancyb 11.4 12 1.9 1
p = .0002, C= .55
aNine parental responses were lost to don't know, no response categories.
bResponses to the category "no active decision" and "affected by accidental pregnancy" are classified as pas-
sive decisions.

August 1978 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY 533

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
indicated in Table 1, 92.5 percent of the non- ents' mean family size desires were larger
parents' versus 62.9 percent of the parents' than nonparents' at every life-cycle stage
fertility decisions were characterized as active (from X = 3.4 percent in childhood to X =
as opposed to passive. 2.2 percent at the present for parents and
The distribution of responses in Table 1 from X -= 2.6 percent to X = 0.0 percent for
also shows that nonparents tended to make nonparents); and (2) parents' mean family
later decisions. Whereas nonparents were less size preferences were always within the
likely than parents to make early decisions normative two to four child range while non-
(5.6 percent versus 25.7 percent at stage 1), parents' childbearing desires only fell within
they were more likely to make postmarital the normative range in childhood and high
decisions (50.0 percent versus 32.4 percent at school (X = 2.586 at childhood and X
stages 6 and 7). 2.073 at high school).
In addition to measuring the timing of cur- However, the relationship between par-
rent fertility decisions, respondents' mean ental status and fertility decisions must be
fertility ideals were graphed over nine life qualified by sex and age of the respondent. In
stages. As displayed in Figure 1, there are contrast to nondifferentiation among males
certain similarities between parents' and non- before engagement, parent and nonparent
parents' family size ideals: (1) in the early females' childbearing preferences are signifi-
stages, a majority of parents, as well as non- cantly different as early as childhood (p <
parents, were desirous of children; and .05). The differentiation between females but
(2) for both populations, there was a con- not males may be due to the effects of a more
tinuous decline, throughout the life cycle in intensive socialization toward parenthood for
the number of children thought ideal. females combined with more realistic oppor-
Although parents and nonparents have some tunities for females than males to weigh the
general developmental trends in common, the desirability and nondesirability of parent-
graph shows that there were always quantita- hood. That more than 40 percent of the males
tive differences between parents' and non- versus 25 percent of the females reported that
parents' ideals and that these differences they couldn't recall or didn't think about
widen at each progressive life cycle stage. their future childbearing plans during their
Two discernable patterns emerge: (1) par- own childhood suggests a greater saliency of

FIGURE 1. MEAN NUMBER OF CHILDREN DESIRED AT EACH LIFE STAGE


Parents
4 3.410

3 2.568
S 2.586 *
2.4272.246
2246 2.847 2.729 2.472
2.073
2 o
1.435
o

S.935
= 1 o .673 Zo

o 0 0
.067
0
0 Nonparents o o o
Stagesa I II III IV V VI VII VIII IX

o 0 a)

534 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY August 1978

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
fertility norms among females than males. types: proscriptions of childlessness, and pre-
Furthermore, the finding that older scriptions toward having children. Again, no
respondents do not hold significantly differ- significant differences were found in reported
ent premarital family size preferences, while pressure by sex or years married.
young respondents tend to differentiate them- An examination of respondents' percep-
selves as prospective parents or nonparents tions of others' attitudes toward childless
prior to marriage suggests a change from the couples reveals a widespread application of
traditional patterns of becoming childless social sanctions proscribing childlessness as
(p <.05 for youngest; p = .50 for oldest). undesirable. As shown in Table 2, the major-
Older nonparents are more likely to reach ity of parents and nonparents alike feel that
their fertility decisions through a series of the dominant attitude toward childlessness is
postponements of children throughout the negative.
early years of marriage, whereas younger Although the majority of responses repre-
nonparent couples, who were married within sent negative characterizations (73 percent
the last five years, are more likely to enter for parents and 86 percent for nonparents),
marriage with the mutual understanding that some interesting between-group differences
neither partner wanted children. emerge. Aside from the primary characteriza-
Social sanctions. Nonparents are generally tions of childless couples as selfish, parents
aware of unfavorable reactions to their family focus on what such couples were missing or
size preferences: 83 percent expected un- that such couples were simply committed to
favorable reactions. Contrary to expectation, ideologies or goals incongruent with having
there was no significant difference in children. In contrast, nonparents feel that
anticipated negative reaction by sex or age of others would be most likely to question the
the respondent. More than two-thirds of the mental state of childless couples or label them
nonparents also feel pressure to conform to as immature.
the "socially acceptable" ideal of two to three In addition to their judgments of most
children. Pressures from family, friends, co- people's attitudes toward childless couples,
workers, and even strangers were of two respondents were also asked for their per-

TABLE 2. RESPONSES TO OPEN-ENDE I) ) I S I I()%\ -%1()'T PEOPLE THINK THAT COUPLES WHO DO
NOT PLAN TO HAVE ANY CHILDREN \I " IiY ' 1\ I I \ I \1, STATUS OF RESPONDENT
Parents Nonparents
Percent Over Percent Over Percent Over Percent Over
Total Number Total Number Total Number Total Number
of Respondentsa of Responsesb of Respondentsa of Responsesb
Responses (N = 112) (N = 146) (N = 47) (N = 91)
Negative Opinions
Selfish 42.8 32.9 68.1 35.2
Nonconforming 3.6 2.7 8.5 4.4
Immature 7.1 5.5 25.5 13.2
Sterile 8.9 6.8 12.8 6.6
Of a questionable mental state 12.5 9.6 29.8 15.4
People who dislike children 1.8 1.4 6.4 3.3
Missing out 16.1 12.3 8.5 4.4
Characterized by general negative qualities 1.8 1.4 8.5 4.4
Total Negative 94.6 72.6 168.1 86.9
Positive Opinions
Characterized by general positive qualities 5.4 4.1 14.9 7.6
Total Positive 5.4 4.1 14.9 7.6

Neutral-Nonjudgmental Attitudes
Committed to ideologies or goals
incongruent with having children 9.8 7.5 0.0 0.0
Characterized by nonjudgmental or
refusal to stereotype responses 20.5 15.8 10.6 5.5
Total Neutral 30.3 23.3 10.6 5.5

apercentage based on ratio of responses in each category over the total number of respondents.
bPercentage based on ratio of responses in each category over the total number of responses.

August 1978 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY 535

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
ceptions of differences between childless and responsibility (p = .06); and (c) work his-
parent couples. Although asked to differenti- tories and attitudes of sample wives' mothers
ate couples on 12 traits representing the tra- (ns). Although first borns were overrepre-
ditional social meanings of parenthood sented in both populations (37.2 percent of the
(Veevers, 1973a), most respondents did not parents and 37.0 percent of the nonparents),
attribute characteristic differences to non- there tended to be more only children among
parent versus parent couples. As seen in nonparents (25.9 versus 16.8) and far fewer
Table 3, nonparents were, on the average, last borns (5.6 percent versus 21.1 percent).
more likely than parents to differentiate Additionally, mothers of nonparent females
couples on these variables: 41.6 percent of the tended to be more involved in, and more
nonparents versus 21.1 percent of the parents likely to set a model for, working outside of
reported some difference. However, when the home. Their children were also more
stereotypic differences were reported, parents likely to have some child-care responsibilities
were more likely than nonparents to define (excluding only children, 62.9 percent of
childlessness negatively (an average of 92 parents versus 24.1 percent of nonparents
percent of all "some difference" responses had no experience at all).
were negative). Parental status differences in family-of-
procreation variables were also found with
The Structural Model respect to religious attitudes and behaviors
Sociodemographic and psychosocial ffac- (p <.05). Nonparents were much more likely
tors. The sedond area of analysis focused on to: (1) consider themselves nonreligious (59.3
an investigation of any systematic differences percent versus 31.8 percent); (2) rarely attend
between parents' and nonparents' early traditional religious services (51.9 percent
and/or current life experiences which might versus 30.1 percent said they never attended);
facilitate or impede adherence to family size and (3) declare themselves as atheists or
norms. Instead of focusing solely on standard agnostics (33.4 percent versus 16.2 percent).
sociodemographic variables such as SES or This finding that nonparents hold signifi-
race, the current research will emphasize sex- cantly fewer traditional religious attitudes
role related variables such as respondents' and engage in fewer religious behaviors than
participation in or attitudes toward familial parents reflects a rejection by nonparents of
or extrafamilial roles. dominant American religions whose doctrines
Nonparents' early background experiences have been characterized as traditionally pro-
differed from parents' with respect to: natalist in nature.
(a) family position (p <.05); (b) childcare As predicted, nonparent wives were also

TABLE 3. STEREOTYPIC LABELING OF INTENTIONAL CHILDLESSNESS BY PARENTAL STATUS OF


THE LABELER

Percent Defining Childless Percent Defining Childless


Couples as "No Different Couples as More Positive
Than" Parent Couples Than Parent Couples
Traits Parentsa Nonparentsb Parents Nonparents
1. Selfishness 52.6 70.4 2.6 14.8
2. Religiosity* ** 71.1 63.0 0.0 3.7
3. Responsibility 75.4 40.7 5.3 55.6
4. Sexual Activity 81.6 49.1 13.2 50.9
5. Marital Happiness 80.7 29.6 7.9 70.4
6. Maturity 72.6 50.9 2.7 49.1
7. Community Activity* ** 61.4 45.3 25.4 41.5
8. Naturalness** 52.6 74.1 0.0 1.9
9. Femininity 80.5 64.2 8.8 35.8
10. Mental Problems for nonparent wives 73.5 47.2 12.4 50.9
11. Masculinity 91.2 71.7 2.6 26.4
12. Mental Problems for nonparent husbands 77.0 47.2 13.3 5.2.8
No difference - 78.9 58.4 Positive x 7.8 39
Some difference x 21.1 41.6 Negativex 92.2 61
aN = 114.
bN = 54 for categories 1, 2, 3, 5 and 8; N = 53 for categories 4, 6, 7, 9, 10, 11 and 12.
*Nonsignificant differences (p = > .05).
**Nonsignificant differences (p = > .05) with "no different than" responses eliminated.

536 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY August 1978

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
more likely (p <.05) than parent wives to be structural or psychological factors) will be
currently employed (92.6 percent of nonpar- summarized briefly. There are, of course,
ents and 57.9 percent of parents were em- limitations posed by the size and selectivity of
ployed). Furthermore, nonparents also the sample. The evidence cited and interpre-
tended (ns) to have had premarital work tation given must be seen as suggestive rather
experience, to be currently engaged at slightly than conclusive of the role played by
higher occupational levels and to be working normative and structural influences on
for primarily noneconomic, self-rewarding fertility attitudes and behavior.
motivations. The existence of a strong nega- First, the existence of family size norms, in
tive relationship between labor-force partici- a purely statistical sense, was clearly sup-
pation and fertility found in this study, as ported by a widespread consensus of two to
well as most other fertility studies, suggests three children as the ideal American family
that the work sphere provides women with size. The research findings further demon-
enriching life experiences and nonmaternal strate the widespread application and per-
sources of gratification which may compete ception of social sanctions prescribing the
with fertility desires.4 two-to-three child family as ideal and pro-
It was also hypothesized that nonparent scribing the childless, one-child or large
wives would be more likely than parent wives family as undesirable. Yet the presence of
to have less stereotyped gender role concepts. childbearing norms and reinforcing sanctions
However, incorporation of sex role-related were not sufficient to prevent these non-
self-concepts (Rosenkratz et al., 1968) was not parents from affirming their decision to
significantly related to parental status. Sur- remain childless. Why is this so?
prisingly, although female nonparents were Data from the current study are supportive
more likely than their parent counterparts to of a third alternative hypothesis for explain-
be employed or to aspire to advanced educa- ing trends in childlessness.5 Further examin-
tional degrees, they were no more likely than ation of the last two columns in Table 3
parent females to view themselves as more reveals that nonparents, when differentiating
"competency- oriented." couples on these 12 traits, are more likely
However, this lack of significant difference than parents to define childlessness in
between sex-role identification and parental positive terms. (An average of positive
status can also be seen as highly significant in responses over all 12 traits shows that 39
providing evidence to challenge the popular percent of the nonparents, compared to 7.8
folklore about pathological sex identification percent of the parents, characterized volun-
of nonparents. In the current study, non- tary childlessness positively.) That nonpar-
parent females were no more or less likely ents tend to define childlessness positively in
than parent females to identify with socially light of general societal proscriptions pro-
desirable warmth-expressive characteristics vides support for Veever's (1975) hypothesis
typically labeled "feminine." Similarly, non- that nonparents defend their variant belief
parent males were no more or less likely than system and insulate themselves from per-
parent males to incorporate socially valued ceived and real social pressures by "dis-
competency traits typically labeled mascu- crediting the discreditor" through a redefini-
line. tion of the social meanings of parenthood.
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS Another social psychological mechanism
may also be operating to nullify the effects of
General conclusions relating to the exist- prevalent social pressures to have children.
ence of, and interrelationships among, the Spontaneous comments generated by the
study's major fertility variables (family size study's nonparents support Houseknecht's
norms, social sanctions and other social (1977) conclusion that reference group
support seems to be an influential factor in
the decision to remain childless. Even though
Houseknecht studied unmarried female
4It is impossible, with the current data, to prove the
direction of the relationship between fertility choices and undergraduates who might be seen as more
extrafamilial experiences. That is, it is somewhat un-
clear if childless women work because they have no SSee Richey's and Stroke's (1974:1) two alternatives:
childcare responsibilities or if, because of work experi- (1) the weakening of norms and (2) the influence of
ences, no children were desired. social structural factors.

August 1978 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY 537

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
prone to seek outside peer support, her con- ability to balance work and family life. We
clusions also hold for older nonparent thus hypothesize that whereas the normative
couples. In the current study, many model is most appropriate for explaining
nonparent couples indicate that they coun- general childbearing expectations, the struc-
tered pronatalistic pressures by minimizing tural decision-making model seems best
social contacts with parents and friends, suited for explaining specific fertility out-
seeking new friends among the childfree, or comes within a normatively prescribed range.
joining organizations (such as the National Similarly, nonparents' fertility decisions
Organization for Nonparents) that provided cannot be classified as strictly structural
support and acceptance for the decision to versus normative. While nonparents' defi-
remain childless. ance of dominant childbearing norms might
The purpose of the section on social imply a decision-making model in which the
demographic and social psychological fac- pros and cons of having a child are based on
tors, in conjunction with the previous ones on situationally specific experiences, nonpar-
family size norms and sanctions, was to test ents' isolation from other parents and close
the creditability of Ritchey's and Strokes' association with other nonparents, as well as
(1974) two alternative proposals for ex- their redefinition of the social meanings of
plaining childlessness: (1) childbearing parenthood seems to indicate that nonpar-
norms are not so universal as previously ents' attitudes and behaviors are not
thought; and (2) childbearing norms are anormative but may be motivated by adher-
pervasive, but various structural influences ence to variant subcultural norms.
operate selectively to counter the prevailing Thus, the author argues the importance of
mores and produce deviance. identifying and further refining both norma-
Since the first part of this analysis pro- tive and structural components of fertility
vided an indication of pervasive pronatalist decisions. In addition to these two factors,
norms and reinforcing sanctions, the effects the influence of two social psychological
of sociodemographic and psychosocial factors factors mediating the impact of the norma-
on fertility outcome were examined to evalu- tive structure should also be recognized in
ate the validity of Ritchey's and Strokes' future fertility studies: (1) respondent's defi-
second proposal. That some significant nition of the situation; and (2) his selective
differences between young middle-class par- referent group participation.
ents and nonparents were found in terms of
early childhood and current life experiences
gives partial support to Ritchey's and REFERENCES
Strokes' second explanation.
Although the data presented in the current Blake, J.
study do not afford a direct test of the 1966 "Ideal family size among white Americans: A
influence of normative versus structural ex- quarter of a century's evidence." Demography
3 (1):154-173.
planations, there are some clues to the
manner in which normative and structural 1972 "Coercive pronatalism and American popula-
tion policy." Research paper prepared for the
models explain fertility outcomes in each Commission on Population and the American
population. The observations related to the Future. Berkeley:International Population and
timing and nature of fertility decisions Urban Research, University of California.
Bram, S.
showing that, for the majority of parents, the
1974 "To have or have not: A social psychological
desire for children was present early in life,
study of voluntarily childless couples, parents-
are in accord with a normative explanation of to-be, and parents." Unpublished doctoral dis-
parenthood. sertation, University of Michigan at Ann
However, parents' reasons for their fluctu- Arbor.

ation of family size desires within the two-to- Commission of Population Growth and the American
Future
four child range indicate that quantitative
1972 Population and the American Future. Wash-
fertility decisions were often determined by
ington, D.C.:U.S. Government Printing
situationally specific concerns such as the Office.
cost of an additional child, consideration for Goode, W.
overpopulation problems or concern for the 1970 Quoted in B. Rollins, "Motherhood: who needs
impact of additional children on the parent's it." Look 34:17.

538 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY August 1978

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Houseknecht, S. ports, Series P-20, No. 288. Washington, D.C.:
1977 "Reference group support for voluntary child- U.S. Government Printing Office.
lessness: Evidence for conformity:" Journal of Thoen, G.
Marriage and the Family 39 (May):285-292. 1977 "Commitment among voluntary child free
Ory, M. couples to a variant lifestyle." Unpublished
1976 "The decision to parent or not: Normative and doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota.
structural components." Unpublished doctoral Veevers, J.
dissertation, Purdue University. 1972 "The violation of fertility mores: Voluntary
Ritchey, P., and C. Strokes childlessness as deviant behavior." Pp. 571-592
1974 "Correlates of childlessness and expectations to in C. Boydell (Ed.), Deviant Behavior and
remain childless: United States, 1967." Social Societal Reaction in Canada. Toronto: Holt,
Forces 52 (March):349-356. Rinehart and Winston.
Rosencratz, P., S. Vogel, H. Bee, I. Broverman, and D. 1973a "Social meaning of parenthood." Psychiatry:
Broverman Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes
1968 "Sex role stereotypes and self-concepts in col- 36 (August):291-310.
lege students." Journal of Consulting and 1973b "Voluntarily childless wives: An exploratory
Clinical Psychology 32 (June):287-295. study." Sociology and Social Research 57
U.S. Bureau of the Census (April):365-366.
1973 "Birth expectations of American wives: June 1973c "Voluntary childlessness: A neglected area of
1973." Current Population Reports, Series family study." The Family Coordinator 22:
P-20. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government (April): 199-205.
Printing Office. 1975 "The moral careers of voluntarily childless
1976 "Fertility history and prospects of American wives: Notes on the defense of a variant world
women: June 1975." Current Population Re- view." The Family Coordinator 24 (October):
473-487.

August 1978 JOURNAL OF MARRIAGE AND THE FAMILY 539

This content downloaded from 128.210.126.199 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 21:41:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like