You are on page 1of 8

Communication of Adult Rats by Ultrasonic Vocalization: Biological,

Sociobiological, and Neuroscience Approaches

Stefan M. Brudzynski

Abstract Whitney 1978) and vocal communication is a regular feature


of their social interactions. It is an intriguing phenomenon
Rats have developed antipredator defensive adaptations to that rats and other rodents have developed communication in
protect themselves from the large number of animals that the ultrasonic range of sound frequencies. Although re-
prey on them. One such adaptation is the ability to commu- searchers have known since the early 1960s, when bat detec-

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


nicate by ultrasonic vocalization, which decreases the likeli- tors became commercially available, that rats could emit and
hood of detection by a predator. Almost all rat vocalizations hear ultrasounds, studies of their ultrasonic communication
are inaudible to the human ear as well, so laboratory studies were hampered for at least a decade by technical difficulties
of ultrasonic vocalization require specialized electronic in recording and analyzing the ultrasonic signals.
equipment. The most popular of these is the “bat detector,” Like other mammals, rats produce sound through the lar-
which lowers ultrasonic frequency to a humanly audible fre- ynx. But unlike most other mammalian species, rodents can
quency. Adult rats emit two types of ultrasonic calls: alarm use their larynx in two modes of functioning. The first, com-
(22 kHz) calls, in aversive and dangerous situations, and mon to most other species, causes vibrations of vocal folds
appetitive (50 kHz) calls, in appetitive or “friendly” (i.e., and produces audible sound (squeals) of 2-4 kHz (Nitschke
nonaggressive) behavioral situations. These two types of 1982). These calls express both physical pain or discomfort—
calls differ in all acoustic parameters, and their initiation de- acute, approaching, or anticipated—and the affective dimen-
pends on activity in different ascending tegmental pathways sion of pain or anticipated pain (Borszcz 2006). Rats use
to the forebrain: 22 kHz calls require activity in the cholin- similar audible vocalizations, sometimes not distinguishable
ergic system, and 50 kHz calls in the mesolimbic dopamin- from pain calls, against predators. These signals not only in-
ergic system. The release of acetylcholine in the diencephalon dicate a potentially painful encounter but also serve to intimi-
and forebrain is associated with the emission of 22 kHz date or stop the advance of a predator, warning the approaching
vocalizations, and the release of dopamine in the nucleus animal that the rat is ready for an active defense (Litvin et al.
accumbens with 50 kHz calls. Thus the two calls are reliable 2007). The intensity of the call is dependent on the proximity
predictors of increased cholinergic or dopaminergic activity of the predator and independent of the proximity of other
in the brain. The calls serve as indices of the rat’s state, in- conspecifics (Litvin et al. 2007). Rats may also direct these
cluding its affective state, induced by activity of one or the vocalizations at humans in anticipation of, or when experi-
other neurochemical system. encing, physically unpleasant treatment or rough handling.
The second mode of larynx function in the rat is in the
Key Words: alarm calls; cholinergic system; defensive be- emission of ultrasounds. For these sounds, the larynx is sta-
havior; dopaminergic system; ultrasonic vocalization; warn- bilized and used as a whistle with a very small orifice cre-
ing calls; 22 kHz calls; 50 kHz calls ated by the vocal cords (Sanders et al. 2001), which are tightly
constricted and cannot vibrate (Nyby 2001). Animals build
up considerable abdominal pressure and push air through
Introduction the orifice. It is possible sometimes to observe this so-called
“pressure breathing” (Fokkema et al. 1986) with deep inha-

T
here is growing research interest in vocal communica- lations and prolonged exhalations associated with the pro-
tion in rats. Unlike many other rodent species, rats duction of long and loud alarm calls. The vibrating air
are highly social animals (Barnett 1967; Blanchard column produces the ultrasonic sound on the same principle
and Blanchard 1980; Lore and Flannelly 1977; Nyby and as a human whistle does, except that the small size of the
rat’s respiratory tract creates sound in the ultrasonic range
of frequencies. In this mode of vocalization, rats usually
Stefan M. Brudzynski, PhD, DSc, is Professor of Psychology and Neurosci- emit pure sounds of a constant or variable frequency with
ence in the Department of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines,
few or no overtones.
Ontario, Canada.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Stefan M. Brudzyn- Rats use the ultrasonic call in a variety of social situa-
ski, Department of Psychology, Brock University, 500 Glenridge Avenue, tions that include both aversive and appetitive encounters.
St. Catharines, ON, L2S 3A1 Canada or email sbrudzyn@brocku.ca. Ultrasonic acoustics and vocalizations are very natural for

Volume 50, Number 1 2009 43


rats and they emit these sounds from birth, although their appears once the animal has noticed the presence of a preda-
hearing develops at a slower rate (studies have reported the tor (or other localized source of danger) and acts to avoid or
onset of rat pup auditory function on postnatal day 12-14; eliminate the threat. This category includes many classic de-
Freeman et al. 1999a,b; Geal-Dor et al. 1993). Rat pups emit fensive behaviors such as immobility (freezing), avoidance,
early ultrasonic “isolation calls” when separated from their escape, threatening, or defensive attack (Edmunds 1974;
littermates or when they fall out and lose contact with the McFarland 1987).
nest. The isolation calls change only slightly in their acoustic The evolution of social life in most species created new
structure until the pups reach about 21 days of age, when a forms of defensive behavior. Animals living in groups do not
more abrupt transformation occurs (Brudzynski 2006) and need to be in a constant state of utmost vigilance and careful
the juvenile rats soon begin to emit calls similar to the 22 observation for the possible appearance of predators, since
kHz and 50 kHz types of adult ultrasonic vocalization. warning signals will likely arise from other group members
But why have rats, and rodents in general, developed vo- that spot the predator first (Edmunds 1974). Thus the social
calization in the ultrasonic range of frequencies? group provides some security. Rats developed well-organized
colonies with 22 kHz vocalizations that serve as alarm calls
(Blanchard et al. 1991, 1992) and thus form a higher-order
Biological Causes of the Development of defensive system (Brudzynski and Holland 2005). The

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


Ultrasonic Communication system depends on the fact that ultrasonic calls emitted
by one individual alarm and activate the whole colony, thus
An impressively large number of species—mammals, birds, the entire social group benefits from the behavior of one
and reptiles—feed on rodents, many of them almost exclu- individual.
sively. In addition, other vertebrate species may eat rodents But communication in the ultrasonic range of sound
occasionally or kill them during encounters without eating frequencies only increases the chances of survival, it does
them. This environmental pressure forced rats to develop, in not fully protect rats from predators. Indeed, the rat emit-
the process of natural selection, anatomical, physiological, ting alarm calls (usually the alpha or other dominant male;
and behavioral adaptations enabling their individual and Blanchard et al. 1991) may do so even when the calls could
species survival; such adaptations, which represent behav- attract the predator’s attention to the vocalizing rat. There-
ioral adjustments to minimize bodily harm and/or death, fore many other factors contribute to rodent survival,
are termed defensive responses (McFarland 1987). Taking such as the location of a particular rodent on its territory,
into consideration the position of rats in the food chain, duration of residence in a colony, and age, as was recently
researchers hypothesized that the rich repertoire of rat defen- shown for prairie dogs (Hoogland et al. 2006). Also, the
sive responses, including ultrasonic vocalization, developed animal’s involvement in a particular type of activity, or its
primarily as an antipredator behavior, and to a lesser degree preoccupation with important biological functions such
as a result of interactions between individuals within the spe- as sexual behavior or fighting in males, or pregnancy in
cies (Blanchard and Blanchard 1988, 1989, 1990; Blanchard females, may further influence its susceptibility to capture
et al. 1990). by predators despite early warning signals (Hoogland et al.
Communication in the ultrasonic range of frequencies 2006).
(above 20 kHz) has a significant advantage over communi-
cation by sonic signals (below 20 kHz): many predators,
although not all, cannot hear ultrasounds (e.g., birds of Detection of Ultrasounds
prey). Moreover, ultrasonic vocalizations are propagated
more directionally than audible sound, they are difficult to Equipment
detect and localize, and they dissipate easily in environ-
mental obstacles and weather conditions (e.g., wind, rain, Because ultrasounds are (by definition) beyond the human
fog). Finally, predators on the ground or in the air cannot hearing threshold, they require computerized electronic
hear these calls when they are emitted in underground equipment to transform the frequency by lowering it to the
burrows. human hearing range. The transformation is done by bat de-
Defensive behaviors may be primary or secondary in na- tectors, which were initially, as the term implies, designed
ture (Edmunds 1974) but all are specific to the individual— and used to detect the ultrasonic signals of bats (Pierce and
that is, only the animal demonstrating the behaviors benefits Griffin 1938). These instruments lower or divide the sound
from them. Primary defensive behaviors decrease the chance frequency by a given factor (usually 10 or 16 times); thus,
of being noticed or found by a predator, whether the predator for example, the bat detector would transform a frequency of
is nearby or not. They include thigmotaxy, when an animal 20,000 Hz to 2000 Hz or 1250 Hz, well within the range of
avoids open spaces and remains or moves in contact with human audibility.
objects, or anachoresis, when the animal remains for a pro- Acoustic analysis of detected ultrasonic vocalizations re-
longed period of time in burrows or other hiding spaces, usu- quires their storage, for which several options are available:
ally during the daytime (Edmunds 1974; McFarland 1987; the divided signal can be recorded on a classical tape
Treit and Fundytus 1988). A secondary defensive behavior recorder, or digitized and stored on the computer drive as

44 ILAR Journal
a divided frequency record, or the original signal can be
digitized by the analyzing system. The most modern acoustic
recording and analyzing systems do not require bat detectors,
instead recording, digitizing, and saving the signals directly
on the hard drive of a computer in the original, undivided
form. This method of recording is more accurate, eliminating
the distortions introduced by bat detectors during frequency
division (Holland and Brudzynski 2004), but it requires more
expensive equipment.
Acoustic analysis also requires the use of a sonograph
or sonographic computer program, which uses digitized
records to show the sound as it is emitted, displaying it in a
sonogram, a graph showing the sound frequency on the y-
axis and time on the x-axis, thus illustrating changes of
sound over time. Sonographs can further generate spectro-
grams (also called power spectra), which show all individual

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


sound frequencies (in kHz) and their relative power (in dB)
on a separate graph.
Figure 1 Exemplary sonogram of a pup isolation call with explana-
tion of the main acoustic parameters. The difference between the
maximal and the minimal sound frequency provides bandwidth,
Parameters while the peak frequency was found from a spectrogram, which
showed that the peak frequency of 44.48 kHz was the most abun-
The acoustic outcome from the bat detector is not how the dant frequency in this particular call, which is neither a 22 kHz nor
original call sounded because the instrument changes the 50 kHz call. The duration of the call is shown below in ms. The
frequency, but the output faithfully conveys other parame- graph (modified) is from Brudzynski SM, Kehoe P, Callahan M.
ters that are useful to researchers’ efforts to characterize 1999. Sonographic structure of isolation-induced ultrasonic calls of
the calls. These parameters usually include the duration of rat pups. Dev Psychobiol 34:195-204.
ultrasonic signals (in ms), bandwidth (i.e., the difference
between the call’s minimal and the maximal sound frequen-
eter describes the number of calls per series or the number of
cies, in kHz), frequency modulation, and the overall sono-
series and the interseries time interval.
graphic pattern of the call (e.g., sweep, trill, step). Use of
the spectrogram is necessary to calculate the original peak
frequency of an individual call (its strongest frequency Emission of 22 kHz Alarm Vocalizations
component, i.e., the one that had the highest amplitude and/
or was the most abundant) by, first, identifying the peak Circumstances
frequency and then multiplying it by the frequency division
of the bat detector. The basic acoustic parameters are illus- Rats emit the 22 kHz alarm call when facing danger, which
trated in Figure 1. includes any circumstance that causes significant anxiety
In addition, the call rate (number of calls per time unit) is in anticipation of an aversive, potentially harmful, or life-
a useful parameter to determine the magnitude of calling. A threatening situation (e.g., an aggressive opponent, predator,
series of individual calls is separated by breaks (intercall large mammal, loud noise). The animal may also emit such
intervals) of about 100 ms apiece (although they may be calls when it expects a known unpleasant stimulus without
much longer). The challenge is to determine how short these exact information about when it will happen (e.g., when an
breaks might be, a difficult measure as rats sometimes emit aversive stimulus appears in an unpredictable way or startles
fragmented calls. Fragmentation happens with long calls the animal). Classic examples are the presence of a predator
when the animal emits the call with a low air pressure and (e.g., a cat at a distance that allows the rat to escape), detec-
occasionally loses the sound, as a weak human whistle may tion of a predator’s odor (Blanchard et al. 2005), confronta-
be interrupted by too little airflow. The ability to recognize tion with a dominant and aggressive rat (Panksepp et al.
two sounds as either separate vocalizations or two fragments 2004; Thomas et al. 1983), an encounter with an unfamiliar
of one call comes only with experience. Call fragmentation human (Blanchard et al. 1986; Brudzynski and Ociepa
also poses a significant problem in studies that depend on the 1992), or even a light but unpredictable and sudden air puff
use of automated vocalization counting devices, which count (Brudzynski and Holland 2005; Knapp and Pohorecky 1995)
all sounds separated by a given time interval (including bro- or startling acoustic stimulus (Kaltwasser 1990, 1991).
ken calls) as two or more separate calls, thus inflating the Other circumstances in which rats emit 22 kHz vocaliza-
number of emitted calls. tions are dyadic encounters such as fights or sexual contact
Ultrasonic calls, and particularly the 22 kHz alarm calls, (Barfield and Geyer 1972; Barfield and Thomas 1986; Lore
are usually emitted in series. Thus, another acoustic param- et al. 1976; Thomas et al. 1983). In these situations, only one

Volume 50, Number 1 2009 45


rat in the pair—for example, the losing rat during a fight— Characteristics and Mechanisms
emits the calls (Panksepp et al. 2004; Thomas et al. 1983),
which not only signal an aversive and potentially dangerous Rats’ alarm calls are long—a single call may last from 100
situation for the loser but may also serve to appease the ag- ms to well over 3000 ms (Figure 2A)—and the peak fre-
gressor so that it reduces or stops the attacks, although stud- quency remains mostly within a narrow band of 20-23 kHz,
ies have not confirmed the latter (Thomas et al. 1983). In although some calls can reach 26 kHz or even 30 kHz (the
sexual contacts, males emit 22 kHz calls toward the end of alarm calls of females may be shorter and of a slightly higher
the series of copulations, probably signalling either a change sound frequency than those of males; Blanchard et al. 1992;
from an appetitive to an aversive affective state and expres- Haney and Miczek 1993). The vocalizing rats try to maintain
sion of the absolute refractory period (Barfield and Geyer a constant sound frequency, so frequency modulation is al-
1972) or cessation of contact with the female (Van der Poel most absent and 22 kHz calls appear on a sonogram as
and Miczek 1991). Males may also emit such vocalizations straight flat lines with a narrow bandwidth of 1-4 kHz. These
if they experience unsuccessful intromission while attempt- calls are emitted usually in short series of 2 to 7 calls. There
ing to copulate (McIntosh et al. 1984) or if a female shows a are also some short 22 kHz calls (less than 100 ms in dura-
low level of lordosis and a high number of agonistic re- tion), emitted almost exclusively after the long calls, but
sponses, making the sexual encounter difficult and aversive their communicative role is unknown (Brudzynski et al.

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


(Brown 1979). 1993; Brudzynski and Holland 2005).
Social conditions and status may also influence the mag- All adult rats of different strains and ages know how to
nitude (i.e., number and/or duration) of 22 kHz calls. Indi- reach the “alarming” frequency. How? There are undoubt-
vidually reared rats emitted significantly fewer such calls in edly some inborn mechanisms that enable them to do so.
response to a mild aversive stimulus than rats reared in pairs Rats do not always begin their call at the right frequency,
(but tested individually) (Inagaki et al. 2005). Among rats so they “tune” the call down to the narrow band of 20-23
that remained in established pairs, the subordinate rats emit- kHz. Thus the very first alarm call, or the first in a series of
ted significantly more 22 kHz calls than the dominant ones calls, often has a mild downward frequency modulation (i.e.,
(Inagaki et al. 2005). decreasing from a slightly higher to lower sound frequency;
Although rats generally emit 22 kHz alarm calls in aver- the tuning is never upward). Once the call reaches the low
sive situations, the calls do not express direct pain. In fact, a band, the frequency remains constant. This observation sug-
vocalizing rat subjected to acute pain ceases to make ultra- gests that rats attempt to emit their alarm calls at as low an
sonic calls. One study reported the emission of similar au- ultrasonic frequency as possible, reaching a common frequency
dible calls as a result of direct painful stimuli, and noted that
the acoustic structure of the calls was dependent on the in-
tensity of the nociceptive stimulus (Jourdan et al. 1995). The
same group more recently demonstrated that the 22 kHz
calls do not represent affective pain (Jourdan et al. 2002): the
calls signal anxiety caused by potential danger, not an actual
painful or nociceptive event. For example, highly aversive
foot shock associated with more intensive anxiety (unavoid-
able shock) caused more 22 kHz calls than the same foot
shock associated with less anxiety (avoidable shock; Kikusui
et al. 2003). Thus, the magnitude of the anxiety and not the
physical nature of the aversive stimulus is the driving force
for expression of alarm calls.
The biological role of these calls may be further illustrated
by the example of a rat threatened by a cat. If the intensity of
the danger is high enough (e.g., the cat is within striking dis-
tance), the rat will either become silent or emit audible warn-
ing squeals (Blanchard et al. 1991; Litvin et al. 2007). If the
rat can escape, it will be silent until it reaches a safe place
(e.g., in or close to its burrow); then, when the immediate life-
threatening danger is over, it will emit the alarm calls. The 22 Figure 2 Sonograms of typical 22 kHz (A) and 50 kHz (B) calls.
During 2 s of recording, a rat emitted one 22 kHz call (A) and seven
kHz vocalizations signal a proximate danger to the calling rat,
50 kHz calls (B). The duration of the 22 kHz call is 1125 ms, while
and the rats who hear the alarm call receive a message that
the 50 kHz calls do not exceed 50 ms in duration. Average peak
the individual emitting it is only in potential danger and still frequency for the 22 kHz call is 23 kHz and peak frequencies of the
has a chance to escape. The rats in the colony may therefore 50 kHz calls are between 54.5 and 62.5 kHz. The sonogram in (B)
not respond to the distant 22 kHz alarm call—unless they hear (modified) is from Brudzynski SM, Pniak A. 2002. Social contacts
an abrupt stop to an ongoing call, when they respond with and production of 50 kHz short ultrasonic calls in adult rats. J
freezing and alert attention (Brudzynski and Chiu 1995). Comp Psychol 116:73-82.

46 ILAR Journal
band based on the physical features of their respiratory sys- in response to heterospecific play (“tickling”; Burgdorf and
tem (for details, Brudzynski and Holland 2005). Panksepp 2001). This result is the opposite of the effects of
Rats also have innate mechanisms associated with their social housing on the emission of 22 kHz alarm calls.
response to the alarm calls. In addition to the direct alarming
properties of the calls, data suggest that an innate mecha-
nism facilitates defensive responses to 22 kHz calls. Rats Characteristics
readily associate aversive and dangerous situations with 22
Appetitive 50 kHz calls are very brief, approximately a hun-
kHz alarm calls and retain this association in their memory
dredth of the duration of an alarm call, and their sound fre-
for a significantly longer time than they do for events associ-
quency is subjected to more modulation than alarm calls
ated with other types of calls or ultrasonic stimuli (Endres
(Figure 2B). The average duration of a 50 kHz call is 30-40
et al. 2007).
ms, with a peak frequency of 45-55 kHz, although it may
reach 70 kHz. The bandwidth of these calls is 5-7 kHz,
Emission of 50 kHz Appetitive broader than that of 22 kHz calls.
Vocalizations Researchers recently distinguished two major types of
50 kHz calls: constant frequency (flat) calls and frequency-
Circumstances modulated calls (step calls with trills; Burgdorf et al. 2007;

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


Wöhr et al. 2008). Further experimental data indicate that rats
Rats emit 50 kHz calls, sometimes termed social calls, in emit one or the other type of call in dissimilar situations. Calls
many situations that appear neutral to the human observer with the flat sound frequency seem to play a social coordinat-
but that seem to be associated with a positive social function. ing function (Wöhr et al. 2008), to be associated with aggres-
The circumstances for such calls may be appetitive or neu- sive situations, or to indicate social ambivalence (Burgdorf
tral (i.e., nonaggressive, nonaversive, or “friendly”) behav- et al. 2008), whereas the frequency-modulated calls (step calls
ioral situations such as sexual contacts and the expectation or combined step-trill calls) are characteristic of reward situa-
of such contact, other rewarding situations (e.g., anticipation tions and the expression of positive affect (Burgdorf et al.
of play), encounters with an anesthetized (immobile and 2008). These calls may represent a vocal homologue or a pre-
therefore easily approachable) conspecific, meeting rats af- served-in-evolution counterpart of human laughter (Panksepp
ter a period of separation, or in the initial meeting of a resi- and Burgdorf 2000)—the calls themselves are not laughter in
dent-intruder pair (to signal nonaggression) (Bialy et al. the human sense, but rather are used by rats in a way that is
2000; Blanchard et al. 1993; Brudzynski and Pniak 2002; somewhat analogous to human laughter.
McGinnis and Vakulenko 2003; Takahashi et al. 1983). Juve-
nile rats emit the same type of call during homospecific
(rough-and-tumble) play or heterospecific play with a human Neurochemical Substrates for Emission of
hand (“tickling”) (Burgdorf and Panksepp 2001; Knutson 22 kHz and 50 kHz Calls
et al. 1998; Panksepp and Burgdorf 2000).1
Rats emit either 22 kHz or 50 kHz calls depending on the
Rats also emit large numbers of 50 kHz calls in anticipa-
situation, and both the circumstances and pattern of the
tion of other rewarding situations—for example, conditioned
emission of these calls seem to be mutually exclusive
place preference, a feeding session, rewarding brain stimula-
(Brudzynski 2007). While 22 kHz calls are associated with
tion, or receipt of a drug with rewarding properties (Burgdorf
aversive situations, behavioral inhibition, and breaking con-
et al. 2000; Knutson et al. 1999). They also emit many such
tacts among rats, 50 kHz calls are associated with appetitive
calls when they enter places frequently visited by other rats
situations, reward, behavioral activation, and approach
(even in the absence of those animals), demonstrating that the
(Brudzynski and Chiu 1995; Wöhr and Schwarting 2007).
emission of these calls is associated with expecting and/or
There are extremely rare, behaviorally ambiguous situations
seeking social contacts (Brudzynski and Pniak 2002). More
with brief bouts of emissions of both types of call, but rats
specifically, the number of calls is directly proportional to the
cannot acoustically mix the 22 kHz and 50 kHz calls.
number of rats that had previously visited the cage (Brudzynski
Furthermore, the acoustic parameters of the two calls
and Pniak 2002). However, calling associated with direct so-
are not overlapping and are often separated by a significant
cial contact may decrease in the constant presence of familiar
safety space that ensures other rats’ unambiguous recogni-
conspecifics; and social housing, as opposed to isolation, de-
tion of the calls (Brudzynski 2007). It is likely that each call
creased the number of 50 kHz calls that juvenile rats emitted
type signals a particular behavioral state and that the recipi-
ents of the calls recognize and understand them as conveying
information about the state of the caller. Such understanding
1It is worth mentioning that most juvenile rats readily learn to play with is crucial for the receiving rat to determine its own appropri-
humans as they play with their siblings—after just 2-3 repetitions most ate behavioral response (Brudzynski 2007).
juvenile rats will follow a human hand and emit many 50 kHz calls (Burgdorf
and Panksepp 2001; Burgdorf et al. 2005). Many rats in adulthood may still
But how does the rat brain initiate these two different and
emit such calls in contact with a familiar human (Schwarting et al. 2007), mutually exclusive types of ultrasonic vocalizations? Initia-
although these contacts do not have the features of juvenile play. tion is regulated by two ascending systems that originate in

Volume 50, Number 1 2009 47


the brain stem tegmentum: the cholinergic system and the me- behavioral activation—increased locomotor activity, explora-
solimbic dopaminergic system (Figure 3). In a broad sense, tion, and an increase in the number of 50 kHz vocalizations
these two systems are part of the ascending brainstem reticular (Brudzynski 2007; Burgdorf et al. 2001; Thompson et al.
system, which evolved as an ancient generalized arousal sys- 2006). Systemic or intra-accumbens application of amphet-
tem (Pribram 1971) and regulates the entire state of the organ- amine significantly increases the number of 50 kHz calls.
ism (for further discussion of the state of the organism, see Activity of the ascending cholinergic system induces a
McCarley 1980 and Brudzynski 2007). general negative state, defined as a response to “external or
Activation of the rat’s ascending cholinergic system, internal stimuli and/or situations (complex stimuli) that pres-
which originates from the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus ent threat or danger to the organism, cause or can cause physi-
and travels to the basal forebrain and limbic areas (Figure 3, cal damage or impairment, biological or sociobiological
filled arrows), induces defensive behavior and intensive 22 destabilization, and disruption of physiological functions and
kHz vocalization (Brudzynski and Barnabi 1996; Brudzyn- balances” (Brudzynski 2007, 262-263). The negative state of
ski 2001). Pharmacological activation of this system by the organism includes not only adaptive changes in the so-
cholinomimetics also induces 22 kHz alarm calls, while an- matic, autonomic, and endocrine systems but also a negative
tagonism of the system by anticholinergic agents blocks or affective state (for a full discussion, see Brudzynski 2007).
decreases the number of such calls (for details, see Brudzyn- Activity of the ascending dopaminergic system induces a

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


ski 2001). general positive state, defined as a response to “stimuli and/
On the other hand, activation of the rat’s ascending do- or situations that minimize threat or danger to the organism,
paminergic system, which originates from the ventral tegmen- enhance its security, preserve physical and social integrity,
tal area and terminates in the nucleus accumbens and other improve, or can improve biological or sociobiological
basal forebrain structures (Figure 3, blank arrows), induces stability, maintain and stabilize physiological functions and

Figure 3 Two ascending tegmental systems responsible for the initiation of vocalization. The system originating from the laterodorsal teg-
mental nucleus (LDT) is cholinergic (filled arrows). Release of acetylcholine in the medial cholinoceptive vocalization strip (stippled area)
induces 22 kHz vocalization. Main targets of this system are anterior hypothalamus (AH), preoptic area (PO), bed nucleus of stria terminalis
(BS), and lateral septum (LS). The system originating in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) is dopaminergic (blank arrows). Release of do-
pamine in the nucleus accumbens (NA) and neighboring areas (TU, olfactory tubercle) induces 50 kHz vocalization. The diagram (modified)
is from Brudzynski SM. 2007. Ultrasonic calls of rats as indicator variables of negative or positive states: Acetylcholine-dopamine interaction
and acoustic coding. Behav Brain Res 182:261-273.

48 ILAR Journal
balances” (Brudzynski 2007, 263). The positive state of the Acknowledgments
organism includes the relevant changes in the somatic, auto-
nomic, and endocrine systems, as well as a positive affective Studies originating from our laboratory were supported by
state (for a full discussion, see Brudzynski 2007). the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of
These observations and numerous experimental results Canada.
led to the postulation that 22 kHz alarm calls and 50 kHz
appetitive calls can predict the state of an organism and thus
serve as reliable indices of the general (including the affec- References
tive) state of the rat organism2 (Brudzynski 2007; Knutson
Barfield RJ, Geyer LA. 1972. Sexual behavior: Ultrasonic postejaculatory
et al. 2002 had already postulated the usefulness of the ultra- song of the male rat. Science 176:1349-1350.
sonic vocalization in assessing an animal’s affective state). Barfield RJ, Thomas DA. 1986. The role of ultrasonic vocalizations in the
Based on the evidence presented in this review, one may fur- regulation of reproduction in rats. Ann N Y Acad Sci 474:33-43.
ther postulate that rats’ 22 kHz and the 50 kHz calls are also Barnett SA. 1967. Rats. Sci Amer 216:79-85.
reliable predictors of increased cholinergic or dopaminergic Bialy M, Rydz M, Kaczmarek L. 2000. Precontact 50 kHz vocalizations in
male rats during acquisition of sexual experience. Behav Neurosci
activity in the brain. 114:983-990.
Blanchard RJ, Agullana R, McGee L, Weiss S, Blanchard DC. 1992. Sex

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


differences in the incidence and sonographic characteristics of anti-
Conclusions predator ultrasonic cries in the laboratory rat (Rattus norvegicus). J
Comp Psychol 106:270-277.
Rats are highly social animals that can emit audible sounds Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. 1980. The colony model: Experience counts.
but communicate among themselves predominantly in the Behav Neural Biol 30:109-112.
Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. 1988. Ethoexperimental approaches to the
ultrasonic range of sound frequencies. Their ultrasonic vo-
biology of emotion. Ann Rev Psychol 39:43-68.
calization evolved as a defensive behavior and an antipreda- Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. 1989. Antipredator defensive behaviors in a
tor adaptation in particular. visible burrow system. J Comp Psychol 103:70-82.
Rats emit two basic types of ultrasonic calls: 22 kHz Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. 1990. Anti-predator defense as a model
(alarm) calls and 50 kHz (appetitive or social) calls. Behav- of animal fear and anxiety. In: Brain PF, Parmigiani S, Blanchard RJ,
Mainardi D, eds. Fear and Defence. Switzerland: Harwood Academic
ioral observations consistently indicate that a rat’s 22 kHz
Publishers GmbH. p 89-108.
vocalizations signal aversive situations and a negative state, Blanchard RJ, Flannelly KJ, Blanchard DC. 1986. Defensive behavior of
and 50 kHz calls signal appetitive situations and a positive laboratory and wild Rattus norvegicus.Comp Psychol 100:101-107.
state. The calls differ dramatically in their acoustic param- Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Agullana R, Weiss SM. 1991. Twenty-two
eters and are easily distinguishable: the alarm calls (22 kHz alarm cries to presentation of a predator, by laboratory rats living in
visible burrow systems.Physiol Behav 50:967-972.
kHz) are long (from 100 to 3000 or more ms) and have a
Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Rodgers J, Weiss SM. 1990. The character-
low sound frequency, whereas the appetitive calls (50 kHz) ization and modelling of antipredator defensive behavior. Neurosci
are short (30 to 40 ms) and have a twofold higher sound Biobehav Rev 14:463-472.
frequency. Blanchard RJ, Yudko EB, Blanchard DC, Taukulis HK. 1993. High-
The two call types are driven from the core limbic mid- frequency (35-70 kHz) ultrasonic vocalizations in rats confronted with
anesthetized conspecifics: Effects of gepirone, ethanol, and diazepam.
brain regions by two ascending projections: the activity of
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 44:313-319.
the ascending cholinergic system initiates alarm calls and Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ, Griebel G. 2005. Defensive responses to
the ascending dopaminergic system initiates appetitive calls. predator threat in the rat and mouse. Current Protoc Neurosci Chapter
These two systems work in a mutually exclusive way, thus 8:Unit 8.19.
animals normally emit only one or the other type of call. Borszcz GS. 2006. Contribution of the ventromedial hypothalamus to gen-
eration of the affective dimension of pain. Pain 123:155-168.
The underlying neurochemical mechanisms and consis-
Brown RE. 1979. The 22 kHz pre-ejaculatory vocalizations of the male rat.
tency of acoustic parameters of these two different calls Physiol Behav 22:483-489.
make them good indices of the rat’s general and affective Brudzynski SM. 2001. Pharmacological and behavioral characteristics of
state. The 22 kHz alarm calls inform about the rat’s anxiety 22 kHz alarm calls in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:611-617.
and negative state, and the 50 kHz calls about its appetitive Brudzynski SM. 2006. Ontogenetic development of ultrasonic alarm calls in
Long-Evans rats. 5th Forum of the Federation of European Neuroscience
and positive state. Although the calls may be useful in as-
Societies (FENS), Vienna, Austria. 8-12 July 2006. FENS Abstracts 3:572.
sessments of a rat’s affective state, ultrasonic vocalization Brudzynski SM. 2007. Ultrasonic calls of rats as indicator variables of nega-
does not provide information about a rat’s physical or affec- tive or positive states: Acetylcholine-dopamine interaction and acoustic
tive (the psychological dimension) pain. coding. Behav Brain Res 182:261-273.
Brudzynski SM, Barnabi F. 1996. Contribution of the ascending cholinergic
pathways in the production of ultrasonic vocalization in the rat. Behav
Brain Res 80:145-152.
2The general state of an organism is a “sum of measurable values obtained Brudzynski SM, Bihari F, Ociepa D, Fu X-W. 1993. Analysis of 22 kHz
from a (practically infinitive) number of variables describing functions of ultrasonic vocalization in laboratory rats: Long and short calls. Physiol
the whole organism” (Brudzynski 2007, 262, based on Rosen 1970). Thus Behav 54: 215-221.
the state of the organism includes both the psychological (affective) Brudzynski SM, Chiu EMC. 1995. Behavioral responses of laboratory
dimension as well as the physiological correlates from subcellular to rats to playback of 22 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations. Physiol Behav
cognitive parameters. 57:1039-1044.

Volume 50, Number 1 2009 49


Brudzynski SM, Holland G. 2005. Acoustic characteristics of air puff- Knutson B, Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. 1998. Anticipation of play elicits high-
induced 22 kHz alarm calls in direct recordings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev frequency ultrasonic vocalizations in young rats. J Comp Psychol
29:1169-1180. 112:65-73.
Brudzynski SM, Kehoe P, Callahan M. 1999. Sonographic structure of iso- Knutson B, Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. 1999. High-frequency ultrasonic vocal-
lation-induced ultrasonic calls of rat pups. Dev Psychobiol 34:195-204. izations index conditioned pharmacological reward in rats. Physiol Be-
Brudzynski SM, Ociepa D. 1992. Ultrasonic vocalization of laboratory rats hav 66:639-643.
in response to handling and touch. Physiol Behav 52:655-660. Knutson B, Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. 2002. Ultrasonic vocalizations as indi-
Brudzynski SM, Pniak A. 2002. Social contacts and production of 50 kHz ces of affective states in rats. Psych Bull 128:961-977.
short ultrasonic calls in adult rats.J Comp Psychol 116:73-82. Litvin Y, Blanchard CD, Blanchard RJ. 2007. Rat 22 kHz ultrasonic vocali-
Burgdorf J, Knutson B, Panksepp J. 2000. Anticipation of rewarding electri- zations as alarm cries. Behav Brain Res 182:166-172.
cal brain stimulation evoked ultrasonic vocalization in rats. Behav Neu- Lore R, Flannelly K. 1977. Rat societies. Sci Amer 236:106-116.
rosci 114:320-327. Lore R, Flannelly K, Farina P. 1976. Ultrasounds produced by rats accom-
Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. 2001. Tickling induces reward in adolescent rats. pany intraspecific fighting. Aggress Behav 2:175-181.
Physiol Behav 72:167-173. McCarley RW. 1980. Mechanisms and models of behavioral state control.
Burgdorf J, Knutson B, Panksepp J, Ikemoto S. 2001. Nucleus accumbens Chairman’s overview of part V. In: Hobson AJA, Brazier MAB, eds. The
amphetamine microinjections unconditionally elicit 50 kHz ultrasonic reticular formation revisited: Specifying function for a nonspecific sys-
vocalizations in rats. Behav Neurosci 115:940-944. tem. International Brain Research Organization Monograph Series, Vol.
Burgdorf J, Kroes RA, Moskal JR, Pfaus JG, Brudzynski SM, Panksepp J. 6. New York: Raven Press. p 375-403.
2008. Ultrasonic vocalization of rats during mating, play, and aggres- McFarland D. 1987. The Oxford Companion to Animal Behaviour. Oxford

Downloaded from http://ilarjournal.oxfordjournals.org/ at Istanbul University on April 22, 2014


sion: Behavioral concomitants, relationship to reward, and self-admin- UK: Oxford University Press.
istration of playback. J Comp Psychol 122:357-367. McGinnis MY, Vakulenko M. 2003. Characterization of 50 kHz ultrasonic
Burgdorf J, Panksepp J, Brudzynski SM, Kroes R, Moskal JR. 2005. Breed- vocalizations in male and female rats. Physiol Behav 80:81-88.
ing for 50 kHz positive affective vocalization in rats. Behav Genet McIntosh TK, Barfield RJ, Thomas D. 1984. Electrophysiological and ul-
35:67-72. trasonic correlates of reproductive behavior of the male rat. Behav Neu-
Burgdorf J, Wood PL, Kroes R, Moskal JR, Panksepp J. 2007. Neurobiol- rosci 98:1100-1103.
ogy of 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in rats: Electrode mapping, le- Nitschke W. 1982. Acoustic Behavior in the Rat: Research, Theory, and Ap-
sion, and pharmacology studies. Behav Brain Res 182:274-283. plications. New York: Praeger Publishers.
Edmunds M. 1974. Defence in Animals: A Survey of Anti-Predator De- Nyby J. 2001. Auditory communication in adults, Chapter 1. In: James F.
fences. New York: Longman Group. Willott, ed. Handbook of Mouse Auditory Research: From Behavior to
Endres T, Widmann K, Fendt M. 2007. Are rats predisposed to learn 22 kHz Molecular Biology. Boca Raton: CRC Press. p 3-18.
calls as danger-predicting signals? Behav Brain Res 185:69-75. Nyby J, Whitney G. 1978. Ultrasonic communication of adult myomorph
Fokkema DS, Koolhaas JM, Van der Meulen J, Schoemaker R. 1986. Social rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2:1-14.
stress induced pressure breathing and consequent blood pressure oscil- Panksepp J, Burgdorf J. 2000. 50 kHz chirping (laughter?) in response to
lation. Life Sci 38:569-575. conditioned and unconditioned tickle-induced reward in rats: Effects of
Freeman S, Geal-Dor M, Sohmer H. 1999a. Development of inner ear (co- social housing and genetic variables.Behav Brain Res 115:25-38.
chlear and vestibular) function in the fetus-neonate. J Basic Clin Physiol Panksepp J, Burgdorf J, Beinfeld MC, Kroes RA, Moskal JR. 2004. Re-
Pharmacol 10:173-189. gional brain cholecystokinin changes as a function of friendly and ag-
Freeman S, Plotnik M, Elidan J, Sohmer H. 1999b. Development of short gressive social interactions in rats. Brain Res 1025:75-84.
latency vestibular evoked potentials in the neonatal rat. Hear Res Pribram KH. 1971. Languages of the Brain: Experimental Paradoxes and
137:51-58. Principles in Neuropsychology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Geal-Dor M, Freeman S, Li G, Sohmer H. 1993. Development of hearing in Pierce GW, Griffin DR. 1938. Experimental determination of supersonic
neonatal rats: Air and bone conducted ABR thresholds. Hear Res notes emitted by bats. J Mammal 19:454-455.
69:236-242. Rosen R. 1970. Dynamical System Theory in Biology, Vol 1: Stability The-
Haney M, Miczek K. 1993. Ultrasounds during agonistic interactions ory and Its Applications. New York: Wiley.
between female rats (Rattus norvegicus).J Comp Psychol 107:373-379. Sanders I, Weisz DJ, Yang BY, Fung K, Amirali A. 2001. The mechanism of
Holland G, Brudzynski SM. 2004. Comparison of recordings of 22 kHz rat ultrasonic vocalization in the rat. Soc Neurosci Abstr 27:88.19.
calls by direct digitisation and via bat detectors. 13th International Schwarting RK, Jegan N, Wöhr M. 2007. Situational factors, conditions and
Behavioral Neuroscience Society Conference, Key West, Florida, June individual variables, which can determine ultrasonic vocalizations in
16-20. Int Behav Neurosci Abstr 13:39. male adult Wistar rats. Behav Brain Res 182:208-222.
Hoogland JL, Cannon KE, DeBarbieri LM, Manno TG. 2006. Selective pre- Takahashi LK, Thomas DA, Barfield RJ. 1983. Analysis of ultrasonic vocal-
dation on Utah prairie dogs. Am Nat 168:546-552. izations emitted by residents during aggressive encounters among rats
Inagaki H, Kuwahara M, Kikusui T, Tsubone H. 2005. The influence of (Rattus norvegicus). J Comp Psychol 97:207-212.
social environmental condition on the production of stress-induced 22 Thomas DA, Takahashi LK, Barfield RJ. 1983. Analysis of ultrasonic vocal-
kHz calls in adult male Wistar rats.Physiol Behav 84:17-22. izations emitted by intruders during aggressive encounters among rats
Jourdan D, Ardid D, Chapuy E, Eschalier A, Le Bars D. 1995. Audible and (Rattus norvegicus). J Comp Psychol 97:201-206.
ultrasonic vocalization elicited by single electrical nociceptive stimuli Thompson B, Leonard KC, Brudzynski SM. 2006. Amphetamine-induced
to the tail in the rat. Pain 63:237-249. 50 kHz calls from rat nucleus accumbens: A quantitative mapping study
Jourdan D, Ardid D, Eschalier A. 2002. Analysis of ultrasonic vocalisation and acoustic analysis. Behav Brain Res 168:64-73.
does not allow chronic pain to be evaluated in rats. Pain 95:165-173. Treit D, Fundytus M. 1988. Thigmotaxis as a test for anxiolytic activity in
Kaltwasser MT. 1990. Startle-inducing acoustic stimuli evoke ultrasonic rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 31:959-962.
vocalization in the rat.Physiol Behav 48:13-17. Van der Poel AM, Miczek KA. 1991. Long ultrasonic calls in male rats fol-
Kaltwasser MT. 1991. Acoustic startle induced ultrasonic vocalization in lowing mating, defeat and aversive stimulation: Frequency modulation
the rat: A novel animal model of anxiety?Behav Brain Res 43:133-137. and bout structure. Behaviour 119:127-142.
Kikusui T, Nishizawa D, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. 2003. Conditioned fear-related Wöhr M, Houx B, Schwarting RK, Spruijt B. 2008. Effects of experience and
ultrasonic vocalizations are emitted as an emotional response.J Vet Med context on 50 kHz vocalizations in rats. Physiol Behav 93:766-776.
Sci 65:1299-1305. Wöhr M, Schwarting RK. 2007. Ultrasonic communication in rats: Can
Knapp DJ, Pohorecky LA. 1995. An air-puff stimulus method for elicitation playback of 50 kHz calls induce approach behavior? PLoS ONE
of ultrasonic vocalizations in rats.J Neurosci Methods 62:1-5. 2:e1365.

50 ILAR Journal

You might also like