You are on page 1of 5

B~'mn & Development, 15 (1993) 173-177

03~7-7604/93/$06.00 ,~'? 1993 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. All rights reserved

BRADEV 00017

Original Articles

Long latency event-related potentials (P300) in gifted children

Florence Martin a, MD, Eric Delpont a, MD, Georges Suisse a, MD,


Christian Richelme b, MD and Claude Dolisi a, MD
aLaboratoire d'Explorations Fonctionnelles du Systdme Nerveux, Hdpital Pasteur, Nice, France
and bService de p{diatrie, Hdpital de Cimiez, Nice, France

Received 7 September 1992; accepted 8 December 1992

In children, P3 latency decreases with increasing age. This decrease could be linked with the maturation of cognitive processes. According to this
hypothesis, event-related potentials P3 were recording in gifted children to research an electrophysiological correlation with the mental precocity.
Auditory long latency event-related potentials were recorded in 10 gifted children (lQs over 140) and 23 control subjects. The part of variance
related to age was extracted by comparing deviations from regression line as a function of age. Stimulus-evoked NI component latency was not
statistically different in the two populations. Event-related P3 component latency was significantly shorter in the gifted children at Cz (P<0.05).
Inter-peak interval N1 P3 was significantly shorter at all three recording sites (P<0.01 at Fz and CZ, P<0.02 at Pz). These results suggest a
relationship between the P3 component and cognitive ability in children.

Key words: P3 (P300); Auditory event-related potential; Cognitive potential; Child development; Gifted child; Cognitive ability

INTRODUCTION MATERIALS AND M E T H O D S


The exact neuropsychological signification of long la- Subjects
tency event-related potentials has remained unclear
since Sutton's publication in 1965 [1]. Many authors Ten gifted children (7 boys aged 4.4-6.9 years and 3
have postulated that these potentials are related to cog- girls aged 5.4-7.7 years) all had intelligence quotients
nitive processes [2M]. (IQs) over 140, as measured by the Wechsler Intelli-
Numerous studies on children have revealed a de- gence Scale for Children (WISC) for subjects over 4
crease in the latency of the main positive component years of age, and by the Terman-Merrill Scale for sub-
(P3) with increasing age [5-10]. This could be related jects younger than 4 years.
to maturational phenomena concerning cognitive pro- The control children (8 boys aged 5.5-11.2 years and
cesses. Two studies demonstrating longer latencies in 15 girls aged 3.9-12.2 years) were recruited from chil-
mentally retarded children appear consistent with this dren whose performance was adequate in their school
hypothesis [11,12]. tasks.
This study was designed to investigate the possible
relationship between cognitive ability and P3 in gifted Recording procedures
children compared with a control group, rather than in
mentally retarded children. Long latency event-related potentials were obtained
by binaural auditory stimulation with an oddball para-
digm. The standard tone pip (80%) of 1,000 Hz and the
target tone (20%) of 2,000 Hz were presented through
earphones. The tones were presented in a random series
Correspondence address: Dr. Florence Martin, Laboratoire d'Ex- at a rate of 0.7/s. Tones were delivered at 60 dB above
plorations Fonctionnelles du Syst+me Nerveux, H6pital Pasteur, 30 each individual's sensation threshold, and had 10 ms
Voie Romaine, BP 069, 06002 Nice c6dex, France. rise/fall and 50 ms plateau time. Potentials were recor-

Brain & Development, Vol 15, No 3, 1993 173


at Fz, Cz and Pz (international 10/20 system) referred
NI N2 to linked mastoids (A1 A2) with an Evomatic 8000
system (Dantec, DK-2740 Skovlunde, Denmark). The
Fz impedance of all the electrodes was less than 5 kf2.
P2 P~
The bandpass was 0.5 50 Hz. Children were t:amiliar-
ized with the tone pips before recording was started.
They were asked to detect the rare tone pips and to
Cz keep a running mental count of them or to move a
P3 finger gently.
P2
Signals exceeding ÷80 /LV were rejected on-line to
NI N2 eliminate ocular and muscular artifacts. Potentials for
both target and non-target stimuli were averaged sepa-
P2 P3
rately. Event-related potentials for the target event were
IOUV [ , computed from the first 20 artifact-free epochs. Trials
lOOms were repeated to ensure reproductibility.
Fig l. Auditory event-related potentials (boy, 10-years old, control P3 (or P300) was defined as the maximum positive
group). Solid lines, target tone; dashed lines, standard tone. component between 250 and 700 ms after stimulus pre-
sentation on the curves elicited by target stimuli (Fig 1).
ded with silver chloride electrodes placed on the scalp P3 latency was measured at the peak. P3 amplitude was

Fz
Cz Pz

1lo
• • B

g 200
II B i ~ D

[]
100 '
z

• i • i i i
i i i - , , i
i - i , i - , ,

6oo
t m

400

• i • i • i - i , i

ul il

il B
400

~- aoo

2O0

• , , i , i - i * J • i • , - , • , • ,
4 6 8 10 4 6 8 10 4 6 8 10
AGE (years) AGE (years) AGE (years)

Fig 2. N1 and P3 latencies, and IPI N1 P3 as a function of age, in the two groups m: gifted children; []: controls. Regression lines are calculated
for the entire population. N1 latency at Fz, y 287.4123- 13.3135x, r - 0 . 5 6 ; Cz, y 294.0726- 14.3378x, r - 0 . 5 8 ; Pz, y = 301.2654- 15.1077x, r
-0.6~.P3latencyatFz`y-7~8.~853-33.~349x`r-~.7~;Cz~y-693.~62-3~.38~3x~r-~.67:Pz~y 704.4375-32.6909x, r 0.72.1P1 N1 P 3 a t
Fz, y - 4 2 0 , 7 7 3 - 19.8215x. r - 0 . 5 7 : Cz, ) ' - 3 9 9 . 0 3 3 6 - 1 7 . 0 4 2 5 x , r 0.52: Pz, y - 388.2352-16.0547x, r - 0 . 5 4 .

174 Brain & Development. Vol 15. No 3, 1993


measured peak-to-peak from the negative component Data analysis
just before P3 (N2).
In order to eliminate the possible influence of stimu- The relationships between age and latency, and be-
lus-linked evoked potential latency on P3 latency, N1 tween age and amplitude, were first assessed in the en-
latency and inter-peak interval (IPI) N1-P3 were stu- tire population (gifted and control children). Inter-
died. N1 was defined as the maximum negative peak group comparisons were then performed.
between 75 and 150 ms after stimulus presentation. When a correlation exists between age and latency or
amplitude, that part of variance related to age must be
extracted before two populations can be compared. For
this purpose, a theoretical value was calculated for each
child based on age, and the linear regression calculated
A. N1
for the entire population. The difference between the
measured value and the theoretical value was then cal-
culated for each child. Finally, mean values obtained
ms 0
for each population were compared. Differences be-
tween groups were analyzed with Student's t-test for
-20 independent samples.

-40
RESULTS

-60 Effect of age in the entire population

B. P3
Latencies
t"20 A significant negative correlation (P<0.01) was ob-
served between the variability of N1 and P3 latencies,
ms 0 and age in the entire population at all three recording
sites. Likewise, IPI N1-P3 decreased significantly with
-20 increasing age (P<0.01). Fig 2 shows the N1 latency,
P3 latency and IPI N l-P3 values, and the linear regres-
-40 sion at the three sites. There was no significant differ-
ence between girls and boys.
-60
P3 amplitude
C.N1 -P3 No significant correlation was found between P3 am-
+ 20 plitude and age at any of the three sites.

ms 0 " - - Comparison between groups (Fig 3)

-20
Latencies
Owing to the significant correlation between NI la-
tency, P3 latency, IPI N1-P3 and age, that part of var-
-40
iance related to age was extracted, as explained above,
by calculating the differences between the measured
-6O
latencies and the theoretical latencies, and comparing
Fig 3. Differences between measured data and theoretical values:
mean values for each population at the three recording sites. Control
the mean values for the two groups.
children: Fz I , Cz [], Pz []; gifted children, Fz [], Cz f-l, Pz I . A: NI latency. No significant difference was observed at
N1 latency. The mean deviations are positive in the gifted children any of the three sites. However, the mean deviations
and negative in the controls, without a significant difference. B: P3 were positive in the gifted children and negative in the
latency. The mean deviations are negative in the gifted children and
control group.
positive in the control, with a significant difference at Cz. C: IPI N1
P3. The mean deviations are negative in the gifted children and posi-
P3 Latency. The mean values were significantly dif-
tive in the controls. The mean values are significantly different at all ferent at Cz (P < 0.05). The mean deviations were nega-
three sites. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.02, "**P < 0.01. tive in the gifted children and positive in the control

Brain & Development, Vol 15. No 3, 1993 175


group. At Fz and Pz, the mean deviations were also served in the study which included the most such young
negative in the gifted children and positive in the con- children [10]. As far as amplitude was concerned,
trol group, but the differences were not statistically sig- Goodin et al. [5] reported an increase with increasing
nificant. age, although the correlation was not statistically sig-
IPI N1-P3. The mean values were significantly differ- nificant. The few investigators who have studied ma-
ent at all three recording sites (P<0.01 at Fz and Cz, turation of the stimulus-evoked N I component with
P < 0 . 0 2 at Pz). The mean deviations were negative in age [5, 9] failed to find a significant correlation.
the gifted children and positive in the control group. The present study demonstrated a decrease in audi-
tory-evoked P3 latency with increasing age in the en-
P3 amplitude tire population (control and gifted children), but the
The mean amplitudes were greater in the gifted chil- rate of decrease (37.7 ms/year at Pz) was greater than
dren at all three sites, but the differences were not sta- previously reported. We also found a significant de-
tistically significant. crease in N1 latency with increasing age. The greater
rate of decrease in P3 latency may be due to the fact
that our study included more young children ( < 1 0
DISCUSSION
years) than earlier studies, a possibility suggested by
Long latency event-related potentials ('cognitive' po- Pearce et al. [10]. Moreover, this study also included
tentials) have been related to mental processes implica- younger children (starting at 3.9-years old). The signifi-
ted in the cognition of sensory information. In particu- cant decrease in N I latency with age might also be due
lar, several studies have suggested that the main wave to these two facts. Our results concerning amplitude
(P300 or P3) may occur in a context of updating, a were similar to those of earlier studies. No significant
process of updating of expectancies, or a process of correlation was observed.
context closure [3,4]. In adults, Goodin et al. [13] and While the size of the gifted children population was
several other authors [14-17] reported longer P3 latency comparatively srnall, owing to difficulties in recruit-
in dementia. However, the sensitivity in these studies ment, the group was homogeneous. All the gifted chil-
was extremely variable, and many factors, such as or- dren were under 8 years of age and they had all been
ganic factors of varying etiologies, could have inter- tested before the age of 6.
fered with the results [18]. In children, Rothenberger The two populations were not age matched. That part
and Baschek [11] observed longer P3 latencies in a of variance related to age was therefore extracted by
group of mentally retarded children. The difference be- comparing the deviations between the measured laten-
tween this group and a group of normal children was cies and the theoretical latencies (calculated from the
statistically significant at the 2.5% level. A second age of each child and the linear regression for the en-
study in children [12] revealed a tendency for longer tire population). N1 latency deviations were not statis-
latencies with decreasing IQ, but latencies varied signif- tically different, whereas P3 latency deviations were
icantly with child IQ only at Pz and for non-targets. statistically different (P<0.05) at Cz. The mean devia-
Nevertheless, organic factors could have also influ- tions were negative in gifted children and positive in the
enced the results in these studies. control group, meaning that latencies were significantly
The purpose of the present study was to test the hy- shorter in the gifted children than in the control group
pothesis of a relationship between cognitive ability and at Cz. When rater-peak interval N I P3 was compared,
long latency event-related components, in gifted chil- deviations became statistically different at all three re-
dren rather than in mentally retarded subjects. Stimu- cording sites (P<0.01 at Fz and Cz, P < 0 . 0 2 at Pz).
lus-linked N I component latency was assessed in order The mean deviations were negative in gifted children
to exclude its possible influence on P3 latency. and positive m the control group, meaning that IPI
All studies to date on long latency event-related po- N1 P3 were significantly shorter in the gifted children
tentials in normal children [5 10] have revealed a sig- at all three sites. These results suggest that passage
nificant relationship between P3 latency and age. How- from N I to P3 might occur faster in gifted children.
ever, the results concerning the rate of decrease were Moreover, since IPI N1 P3 decreases with increasing
varied, ranging from 3.6 ms/year [8] to 19.3 ms/year age, mental precocity in the gifted children might re-
[10] at Pz. This variation may be due to differences in flect an electrophysiological precocity concerning the
task difficulty or in populations. For instance, the num- interval, NI P3.
ber of young children (under 10-years old) was often Although there was no significant difference in P3
rather small and the greatest rate of decrease was ob- amplitude, the mean values were higher in the gifted

] 76 Brain & Development, Vol 15, No 3, 1993


children. This could be due to the fact that amplitude 9. Martin L, Barajas J J, Fernandez R, Tortes E. Auditory event-
related potentials in well-characterized groups of children. Elec-
and latency are generally linked parameters: greater
troencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1988; 71: 375-81.
amplitudes correspond to shorter latencies [19]. 10. Pearce JW, Crowell DH, Tokioka A, Pachero GP. Childhood
The present study concerning gifted children seems to developmental changes in the auditory P300. J Child Neurol
support the hypothesis of a relationship between cogni- 1989: 4: 100-6.
tive ability and P3 latency in children. P3 latency might 11. Rothenberger A, Baschek V. Children with different cognitive
abilities. In: Rothenberger A, ed. Event-related potentials in
be related to mental maturation, but further studies with
children. Amsterdam: Elsevier 1982:317--24.
a greater number of subjects are required to identify any 12. Baumann S, Otto D, Robinson G, Schroeder S, Barton C. The
relationship between IQ and P3 latency in children. relationship of late positive ERPs, age, intelligence and lead ab-
sorption in socioeconomically disadvantaged children. In: John-
son Jr R, Rohrbaugh JW, Parasuraman R, eds. Current trends in
REFERENCES
ERP research (Suppl No 40 to Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophy-
1. Sutton S, Braben M, Zubin J, John ER. Evoked potentials corre- siol). Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1987:617 23.
lates of stimulus uncertainty. Science 1965; 150: 1187-8. 13. Goodin DS, Squires KC, Starr A. Long latency event-related
2. Donchin E. Surprise!... Surprise? Psychophysiology 1981; 18:493 components of the auditory evoked potentials in dementia.
513. Brain 1978; 101: 635~48.
3. Verleger R. Event-related potentials and cognition: a critique of 14. Squires KC, Chippendale TJ, Wrege KS, Goodin DS, Starr A.
the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation Electrophysiological assessment of mental function in aging and
of P3. Behav Brain Sci 1988; 11:343 56. dementia. In: Poon L, ed. Aging in the 1980s. Washington DC:
4. Donchin E, Coles MGH. Precommentary: is P300 component a American Psychological Association, 1980: 125-34.
manifestation of context updating? Behav Brain Sci 1988; i!: 15. Pfefferbaum A, Ford JM, Wenegrat BG, Roth WT, Kopell BS.
357 74. Clinical application of the P3 component of event-related poten-
5. Goodin DS, Squires KC, Henderson BH, Starr A. Age-related tials. II. Dementia, depression and schizophrenia. Electroence-
variations in evoked potentials to auditory stimuli in normal hu- phalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1984; 59: 104-24.
man subjects. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1978: 44: 16. Gordon E, Kraiuhin C, Harris A, Meares R, Howson A. The
447 58. differential diagnosis of dementia using P300 latency. Biol
6. Courchesne E. Neurophysiological correlates of cognitive devel- Psychiatry 1986; 21: 1123-32.
opment: changes in long-latency event-related potentials from 17. Patterson JV, Michalewski H J, Starr A. Latency variability of the
childhood to adulthood. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol components of auditory event-related potentials to infrequent
1978; 45:468 82. stimuli in aging, Alzheimer-type dementia, and depression. Elec-
7. Polich J, Howard L, Starr A. Effects of age on the P300 compo- troencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1988; 71: 45~60.
nent of the event-related potentials from auditory stimuli: peak 18. Pfefferbaum A, Ford JM, Kraemer HC. Clinical utility of long
definition, variation and measurement. J Gerontol 1985; 40:721 latency 'cognitive' event-related potentials (P3): the cons. Electro-
6. encephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990; 76: 6-12.
8. Finley WW, Faux SF, Hutcheson J, Amstutz. Long-latency 19. Polich J. Normal variation of P300 from auditory stimuli. Elec-
event-related potentials in evaluation of cognitive function in troencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1986; 65: 23640.
children. Neurology 1985; 35:323 7.

Brain & Development, Vol 15, No 3, 1993 177

You might also like