You are on page 1of 9

European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 22, pp.

1775–1783, 2005 ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies

Fear learning induces persistent facilitation of amygdala


synaptic transmission

Bradley W. Schroeder and Patricia Shinnick-Gallagher


Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, Neuroscience Graduate Program, The University of Texas Medical Branch,
301 University Blvd, Galveston, TX 77555–1031, USA

Keywords: anxiety, fear conditioning, long-term potentiation (LTP), memory and learning, rat, synaptic plasticity

Abstract
In the maintenance phase of fear memory, synaptic transmission is potentiated and the stimulus requirements and signalling
mechanisms are altered for long-term potentiation (LTP) in the cortico-lateral amygdala (LA) pathway. These findings link amygdala
synaptic plasticity to the coding of fear memories. Behavioural experiments suggest that the amygdala serves to store long-term fear
memories. Here we provide electrophysiological evidence showing that synaptic alterations in rats induced by fear conditioning are
evident in vitro 10 days after fear conditioning. We show that synaptic transmission was facilitated and that high-frequency
stimulation dependent LTP (HFS–LTP) of the cortico-lateral amygdala pathway remained attenuated 10 days following fear
conditioning. Additionally, we found that the low-frequency stimulation dependent LTP (LFS–LTP) measured 24 h after fear
conditioning was absent 10 days post-training. The persistent facilitation of synaptic transmission and occlusion of HFS–LTP
suggests that, unlike hippocampal coding of contextual fear memory, the cortico-lateral amygdala synapse is involved in the storage
of long-term fear memories. However, the absence of LFS–LTP 10 days following fear conditioning suggests that amygdala
physiology 1 day following fear learning may reflect a dynamic state during memory stabilization that is inactive during the long-term
storage of fear memory. Results from these experiments have significant implications regarding the locus of storage for maladaptive
fear memories and the synaptic alterations induced by these memories.

Introduction
The amygdala is critically involved in the formation of emotional the lifetime of an adult rat also indicated a role for the amygdala in the
memories. The formation of fear memory is accompanied by increases permanent storage of fear memories (Gale et al., 2004). One
in synaptic strength (McKernan & Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997; Rogan complicating factor in behavioural studies may be the effects of
et al., 1997) and modifications in synaptic plasticity (Tsvetkov et al., experimental manipulations on the behavioural performance of the
2002; Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004) in afferent pathways to animal (Maren, 1999; Vazdarjanova et al., 2001; Wallace & Rosen,
the cortico-lateral amygdala (LA). Experimental manipulations that 2001). Here we examined whether storage of fear memories in the
block amygdala plasticity inhibit the formation and expression of fear amygdala is accompanied by transient or long-lasting alterations in
memory (Huang et al., 2000; Schafe et al., 2000; Fendt, 2001; Bauer synaptic mechanisms that are not dependent on recording of animal
et al., 2002; Rodrigues et al., 2002) suggesting that these alterations in performance.
synaptic physiology are necessary for acquisition of fear memories. To examine electrophysiological evidence of whether fear memories
More specifically, cued fear memories occlude long-term potentiation are stored long-term in the amygdala, we examined the effect of fear
(LTP; Tsvetkov et al., 2002) and alter the stimulus requirements and conditioning on amygdala synaptic efficacy and plasticity 10 days
signalling mechanisms for LTP in the cortico-LA pathway (Schroeder following fear learning. We show that synaptic transmission and
& Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004) suggesting fear memories encompass efficacy was enhanced in the amygdala 10 days following fear
overlapping and saturating mechanisms similar to those of underlying conditioning and that high-frequency stimulation dependent LTP
amygdala synaptic plasticity. (HFS–LTP) of the cortico-LA pathway remained attenuated, while
The role of the amygdala in the storage of long-term memory is low-frequency stimulation dependent LTP (LFS–LTP) recorded 24 h
somewhat controversial. Studies analysing inhibitory avoidance after fear conditioning was absent 10 days post-training. The persist-
suggest that the amygdala may play a time defined role in ent synaptic facilitation and occlusion of HFS–LTP suggested that the
consolidation of fear memory (Cahill et al., 1999; Vazdarjanova, cortico-LA synapse was involved in the long-term storage of fear
2000) whereas fear-conditioning studies showed little evidence for a memory.
temporal gradient regarding the involvement of the amygdala in the
expression of fear memories (Chapman et al., 1990; Cahill et al.,
1999; Maren, 2000; Vazdarjanova, 2000). Recent experiments lasting Materials and methods
Animals
Correspondence: Professor Patricia Shinnick-Gallagher, as above. Male albino Sprague–Dawley rats (virus free, Harlan, Houston, TX,
Email: psgallag@utmb.edu
USA) 4–7-weeks-old were used. All rats were acclimated in an
Received 24 February 2005, revised 25 July 2005, accepted 27 July 2005 isolated animal facility with food and water available ad libitum for a

doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04343.x
1776 B. W. Schroeder and P. Shinnick-Gallagher

minimum of five days. Animals were routinely handled prior to use in in separate chambers to control for effects of contextual conditioning.
electrophysiological or behavioural experiments. All experiments Amygdala slices were prepared from fear-conditioned (FC1) and
were performed with prior approval from the University of Texas unpaired (UP1) animals 24 h after the testing for use in the 1-day
Medical Branch Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. electrophysiological experiments. Animals used in the 10-day elec-
trophysiological experiments (FC10 and UP10) were returned to the
animal housing facility following testing where they remained
Fear conditioning undisturbed for 10 days.
Animals were fear-conditioned using a fear-potentiated startle para-
digm (Fig. 1A) adapted from (Campeau & Davis, 1995) as described
Amygdala slice preparation
previously (Shinnick-Gallagher et al., 2003; Zinebi et al., 2003). Prior
to training, rats were acclimated to the experimental chambers and Rats were decapitated, and brains rapidly removed and placed in ice-
subsequently trained and tested in enclosures attached to a stabilime- cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) solution bubbled continu-
ter ⁄ accelerometer device (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, ously with 95% O2 and 5% CO2 to maintain appropriate pH (7.4–
USA). Animal movement displaced an accelerometer and the 7.45). Anaesthetics were not used prior to this procedure because they
magnitude of the displacement was converted to a voltage and stored can mask changes in emotional learning. Brains were blocked,
on a desktop computer. The acoustic startle response was elicited mounted, and sliced into 500-lm serial coronal sections containing the
using a white noise burst of 95 db. Startle amplitude was defined as LA. Slices were placed in oxygenated ACSF and allowed to recover at
peak displacement within 200 ms after the onset of the startle room temperature for at least two hours. Slices were submerged in a
stimulus. A 72 db filtered, white noise tone of 2.7 s duration was used 0.8 mL tissue bath (Harvard Instruments, Holliston, MA, USA)
as the conditioned stimulus (CS) and paired ten times a day for 2 days continuously perfused with oxygenated ACSF (2–3 mL ⁄ min) and
with the unconditioned stimulus (US), a 0.5 mA co-terminating foot maintained at 33 ± 1 C. The composition of the ACSF was (in
shock of 0.5 s duration for the fear-conditioned group. Unpaired (UP) millimolar): NaCl, 117; KCl, 3.0; CaCl2, 2.5; MgCl2, 1.2; NaHCO2,
controls received the same number of CS and US presentations in 25; NaH2PO4, 1.2; and glucose, 11.5.
pseudorandom fashion. On the third day, the rats were tested using ten
startle stimuli to habituate the animals followed by ten startle stimuli
alone randomly interspersed with ten startle stimuli paired with the Extracellular field recording
CS. Fear-potentiated startle was measured as a change in startle Extracellular field potentials were recorded using a tungsten electrode
magnitude elicited by CS-startle stimulus pairing compared to that (2–5MW) and amplified using a Microelectrode AC Amplifier (A-M
elicited by the startle stimulus alone. Testing sessions were performed Systems, Carlsborg, WA, USA). Bipolar concentric stimulating

Fig. 1. Diagram of preparation and time frame for experimental paradigm and fear-potentiated startle in the different animal groups. (A) Diagram of slice
preparation taken from Paxinos & Watson (1998) with stimulation and recording sites indicated. (B) In vivo fear training and testing occurred on similar days while
in vitro recordings were compared 1 and 10 days after fear testing. (C) Startle responses were significantly altered in fear-conditioned animals subsequently analysed
electrophysiologically in vitro 1 and 10 days post-testing. The mean ± SEM of the startle stimulus alone (startle alone), CS-startle stimulus pairing (tone + startle),
and the difference of the two values are plotted for fear-conditioned (FC1) and unpaired (UP1) animal groups 1 day (n ¼ 13 per group) and fear-conditioned (FC10)
and unpaired (UP10) groups 10 days (n ¼ 14 per group) post-testing. *P < 0.05 relative to startle in FC; ^P < 0.05 relative to difference in UP.

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783
Fear learning and persistent synaptic facilitation 1777

electrodes (50–75KW) were placed on the external capsule, and CS). Training also did not significantly affect startle response to the
orthodromic synaptic potentials were elicited with an Isolated Pulse startle stimulus alone in the 1 day populations (P > 0.05, between
Stimulator (A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA, USA). While the external FC1 and UP1 groups), but startle in the presence of the CS showed a
capsule also includes fibers originating but not terminating in the LA, significant group effect (P < 0.05, for subtracted values between FC1
the conventional term ‘cortico-LA pathway’ was used here to describe and UP1 groups).
dorsolateral LA field potentials elicited by external capsule stimula- Fear-potentiated startle amplitudes were determined in the animal
tion. Input–output (IO) relationships were assessed by stimulating groups whose brains were subsequently analysed in vitro 10 days after
afferents with increasing levels of intensity (100-ls biphasic pulses, testing (Fig. 1B). Responses to the startle stimulus in the presence and
typically ranging from 3 to 11 V) while recording the evoked field absence of the CS, and the difference in these two values were
excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) in the LA. Field potentials 118.3 ± 14.3, 191.7 ± 19, and 73.4 ± 7, respectively, for the fear-
were included for study if the maximum amplitude was above 0.8 mV conditioned group (FC10) and 123.6 ± 15.6, 131.2 ± 16.5, and
(without spikes). For analysis of synaptic plasticity, the stimulus 7.6 ± 4.8, respectively, for the unpaired group (UP10). Startle
intensity was adjusted to 33–50% of maximum fEPSP amplitude. amplitudes were potentiated 62% above baseline values (P < 0.05)
Experimenters were blinded to the animal groups and slices from in the FC10 group. There was no significant effect of the CS on startle
experimental and control animals were recorded simultaneously in the magnitude in the UP10 population (P > 0.05) and no significant
same chamber to control for day-to-day variation. Stimulation difference in baseline response to startle alone for the FC10 and UP10
protocols were generated and off-line analyses of fEPSP recordings groups (P > 0.05).
performed using pClamp 9.0 (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA, Analysis of startle values revealed no significant difference between
USA). Averaged traces of six successive sweeps were used for FC1 and FC10 groups for startle stimulus alone, conditioned tone plus
analysis of fEPSP amplitude and 10–90% slope. No significant startle stimulus, and the difference of the two values (P > 0.05).
differences were found between the two measurements. Additionally, there was no significant difference in startle values
between UP1 and UP10 groups. These data indicated that fear-
potentiated startle responses were not different in the 1 and 10 days
Statistical analysis post-training groups, which suggested that the degree of fear learning
One slice per treatment per animal was used for analyses such that n did not differ in the animal populations analysed electrophysiologi-
represents the number of animals. Data were presented as mean ± - cally at the two time points.
SEM. A nonlinear regression analysis was used to compare the IO
relationships for control and fear-conditioned animals. Differences
between groups and treatment conditions were assessed using one- Fear memory formation facilitates cortico-LA synaptic
way and two-way anovas. When the overall F ratio was significant, transmission
Tukey posthoc tests were used to compare individual means. Synaptic strength was increased at the thalamo-LA pathway (McKernan
Statistical significance was defined as P < 0.05. & Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997) and LTP of the cortico-lateral amygdala
synapse was attenuated 24 h after fear conditioning (McKernan &
Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997; Tsvetkov et al., 2002; Schroeder & Shinnick-
Results Gallagher, 2004). These findings suggested that emotional learning
Fear potentiates startle facilitated synaptic transmission at the cortico-LA synapse through LTP-
like mechanisms, but the attenuation of LTP may be independent of a
Animals were fear-conditioned using a fear-potentiated startle para- change in synaptic strength. To analyse synaptic efficacy of cortico-LA
digm adapted from Campeau & Davis (1995). The fear-conditioned synapses, we evaluated IO relationships by recording fEPSP slope as a
group was exposed to ten unconditioned stimuli (US, 0.5 mA, 0.5 ms function of increasing stimulus intensity in slices from naı̈ve, UP1, and
foot shock) paired with ten conditioned stimuli (CS, 2.7 s tone) per FC1 animals (Fig. 2A). Non-linear regression analysis of IO relation-
day for 2 days and learned that CS presentation predicts subsequent ships revealed no significant differences between the naı̈ve and UP1
US presentation. The unpaired control groups received ten CS and ten groups (P > 0.05) whereas the IO relationship for FC1 animals was
US exposures per day for 2 days presented in pseudorandom fashion significantly different than that in the control groups (Fig. 2A,
and, consequently, the unpaired animals did not learn the CS–US P < 0.0001). These data suggested that fear memory strengthens
association. Fear potentiation of startle was measured on the third day synaptic transmission at the cortico-LA synapse.
by recording startle amplitude in response to the startle stimulus in the
presence and absence of the CS (Fig. 1). As a result of fear learning,
CS presentation in the absence of the foot shock elicited a potentiated
startle response. Startle values for the startle stimulus alone, the startle Persistent synaptic facilitation in the storage of fear memories
stimulus in the presence of the CS, and the subtracted value of those To determine the persistence of the increase in synaptic strength, we
measurements were significantly different in fear-conditioned and examined the IO relationship at the cortico-LA synapse 10 days after
unpaired groups in both 1 day and 10 day animal populations testing. Synaptic transmission was recorded in response to afferent
(Fig. 1B). Startle amplitudes for the three measures, startle stimulus stimulation at increasing stimulus intensities for naı̈ve, UP10, and
alone, startle stimulus plus CS, and the difference of the two values FC10 groups (Fig. 2B). Non-linear regression analysis of the IO
(142.2 ± 9.8, 237.8 ± 18.7, and 95.6 ± 11.5, respectively) for the relationships for naı̈ve and UP10 animals revealed no significant
1 day fear-conditioned group (FC1) showed a significant effect of CS differences in synaptic strength (Fig. 2B, P > 0.05). However, the IO
presentation on startle magnitude (67% potentiation of startle, relationship for the FC10 group showed a significant difference when
P < 0.05 between startle alone and startle plus CS). In 1 day unpaired compared to naı̈ve and UP10 groups (P < 0.0001). These data
group (UP1), startle values for the three measures (147.6 ± 21.6, indicated that synaptic strength remained facilitated 10 days after fear
151.2 ± 24.3, and 3.7 ± 5.9, respectively) showed no significant effect learning, suggesting that enhanced cortico-LA synaptic efficacy may
of CS presentation (P > 0.05 between startle alone and startle plus play a role in the long-term storage of fear memory.

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783
1778 B. W. Schroeder and P. Shinnick-Gallagher

Fig. 2. Fear learning facilitated synaptic transmission in the cortico-LA pathway 1 day post-testing and this potentiation persisted 10 days after fear conditioning.
(A) Traces of fEPSPs were recorded at increasing stimulus intensities in slices from naı̈ve control rats and from unpaired and fear-conditioned animals one day post-
testing. (B) fEPSPs were measured in slices from naı̈ve control and from unpaired and fear-conditioned animals 10 days post-testing. (C) Synaptic strength (fEPSP
slope) is plotted as a function of increasing afferent stimulation intensity in slices from naı̈ve control (d), unpaired (UP1, s) control, and fear-conditioned (FC1, .)
animals. The input–output (IO) relationship for synaptic transmission was shifted upward in FC1 animals, P < 0.0001 vs. control, nonlinear regression analysis. (D)
The upward shift in the IO relationship for synaptic transmission remained in slices from fear-conditioned animals analysed 10 days post-testing. Synaptic strength
(fEPSP slope) is plotted as a function of increasing afferent stimulation intensity for naı̈ve (d), unpaired (UP10, s), and fear-conditioned (FC10, .) animals,
P < 0.0001 vs. control, linear regression analysis.

Presynaptic involvement in cortico-LA synaptic facilitation Enhanced probability of transmitter release in fear memory
Paired-pulse ratio (PPR) is a measure of short-term plasticity widely To examine whether the change in PPR recorded 1 day after fear
used to probe for changes in presynaptic function. In this paradigm, conditioning is persistent, we recorded cortico-LA synapses 10 days
pairs of fEPSPs are elicited by two temporally close stimuli. The effect after fear conditioning (Fig. 3). Analysis of PPRs revealed no
of the first stimulus on the second can be used to infer changes in significant differences between naı̈ve and UP10 control groups, while
probability of transmitter release from the presynaptic terminal. To PPRs from the FC10 group were significantly depressed at 25, 35, and
examine whether the increased synaptic strength recorded 1 day after 50 ms interpulse intervals (naı̈ve, n ¼ 8; UP10, n ¼ 8 and FC10,
fear conditioning is mediated by a presynaptic component, we n ¼ 11, P < 0.05, Fig. 3B). These data suggested that the modulation
analysed PPRs for naı̈ve, UP1, and FC1 groups (Fig. 3). Ratios of of presynaptic function induced by fear memory formation persisted in
the slope of the second fEPSP to the slope of the first fEPSP were the long-term storage of fear memory.
examined at varying interpulse intervals (Fig. 3A) in slices from
control and fear-conditioned animals. No significant differences were
measured between naı̈ve and UP1 controls, however, analysis of PPRs
revealed a significant depression in the FC1 group at 25, 35, and HFS-LTP remains attenuated 10 days after fear conditioning
50 ms intervals (naı̈ve, n ¼ 8; UP1, n ¼ 9 and FC1, n ¼ 12 At cortico-LA synapses HFS-induced LTP is attenuated 1 day after
P < 0.05). The PPR depression measured in the FC1 group suggested fear conditioning (Fig. 4A; Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004).
that the synaptic facilitation generated by fear learning is mediated at To investigate whether fear-induced alterations in synaptic plasticity at
least in part by an increase in presynaptic release probability. the cortico-LA synapse were enduring, we examined the effects of

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783
Fear learning and persistent synaptic facilitation 1779

Fig. 3. Alterations in PPR in fear-conditioned animals that were recorded 1 day post-testing persisted 10 days post-testing. (A) Pairs of fEPSPs were recorded at
25 ms intervals in slices from naive control and from unpaired and fear-conditioned groups 1 day after fear testing. (B) fEPSP pairs were measured at 25 ms
intervals in naı̈ve, unpaired and fear-conditioned groups 10 days post-testing. (C) PPRs (fEPSP2 ⁄ fEPSP1) are plotted as a function of varying interpulse intervals in
naı̈ve (d), unpaired (UP1, s), and fear-conditioned (FC1, .) animals (n ¼ 8 per group for naı̈ve and FC1, n ¼ 7 for UP1). *P < 0.05 for FC1 relative to control
groups. (D) PPRs (fEPSP2 ⁄ fEPSP1) are plotted as a function of varying interpulse intervals in naı̈ve (d), unpaired (UP10, s), and fear-conditioned (FC10, .)
animals (n ¼ 8 per group for naı̈ve and FC10, n ¼ 7 for UP10). *P < 0.05 for FC10 compared to control groups.

HFS (100 Hz for 1 s, repeated five times at 3 min intervals) on weight and presynaptic enhancement recorded 1 day after fear
synaptic transmission 10 days following training (Fig. 4B). HFS in learning persisted for 10 days and (iii) HFS–LTP remained attenuated
slices from naı̈ve and UP10 animals elicited LTP (naive 10 days after fear conditioning, but LFS–LTP measured in slices from
135.5% ± 3.6%; UP 130.7% ± 4.6%; P > 0.05 between groups) fear-conditioned animals 1 day after training was not present 10 days
while HFS–LTP in slices from FC10 animals remained attenuated following fear learning.
(109.3% ± 2.8%, P < 0.001 between fear-conditioned and controls;
Fig. 4A). These data suggested that the attenuation of HFS–LTP was
associated with the long-term storage of fear memory. Fear learning facilitates synaptic transmission
Previous studies have demonstrated synaptic facilitation at thalamic
sensory input pathways to the LA following fear conditioning
LFS–LTP is absent 10 days after training (McKernan & Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997; Rogan et al., 1997). Other
In addition to attenuation of HFS–LTP, we previously described a low- studies in the cortico-LA pathway, showed that HFS-induced and
frequency stimulation (LFS) dependent form of LTP (LFS–LTP) that pairing-induced LTP are occluded 1 day after fear conditioning
is measured at the cortico-LA synapse 24 h after fear testing (Fig. 5A; (Tsvetkov et al., 2002; Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004). One
Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004). We examined whether this hypothesis is that fear learning induces a long-lasting synaptic
form of plasticity is present 10 days after fear conditioning (Fig. 4B). facilitation at the cortico-LA pathway and that the occlusion of
LFS 1 Hz for 15 min elicited no significant change in fEPSPs in slices HFS–LTP recorded after fear conditioning is due to saturation of
from naı̈ve (n ¼ 8), UP10 (n ¼ 7), and FC10 (n ¼ 10) animals signalling mechanisms necessary to support LTP (Tsvetkov et al.,
(98.8 ± 4.7, 103.3 ± 4.9, and 103.8 ± 3.2, respectively, P > 0.05, 2002). Although definitive proof showing that synaptic facilitation
Fig. 4B). These data suggest that the potentiation of synaptic strength underlies the attenuated HFS–LTP in fear conditioning requires
in response to LFS in the period shortly after fear learning does not extensive occlusion studies (Lledo et al., 1995; Schulz & Fitzgibbons,
persist in long-term fear memory. 1997), the data presented here provide evidence that cortico-LA
synapses are facilitated in fear memory and could contribute to
attenuation of HFS–LTP.
Discussion Paired-pulse facilitation is a form of short-term plasticity that is
The major findings of this study are: (i) fear learning induced synaptic widely believed to rely on enhanced presynaptic function (Schulz
facilitation at the cortico-LA synapse that was expressed in part et al., 1994; Zinebi et al., 2001; Zucker & Regehr, 2002). In this
through an enhanced presynaptic function; (ii) the increased synaptic paradigm, facilitation is thought to be due to a residual Ca++ in the

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783
1780 B. W. Schroeder and P. Shinnick-Gallagher

Fig. 4. Long-term storage of fear memory was associated with occlusion of HFS–LTP. (A) Plot of previous data (Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004) showing
occlusion of HFS–LTP 1 day after fear training. Per cent potentiation of fEPSPs following HFS in slices is graphed with respect to time from naı̈ve (d), UP (s), and
FC (.) animals. (B, upper) fEPSPs before (baseline) and after HFS–LTP stimulation in slices from naı̈ve, unpaired (UP10) and fear-conditioned (FC10) animals. (B,
lower) Graph of percentage potentiation of fEPSPs following HFS is plotted with respect to time in slices from naı̈ve (d), UP10 (s), and FC10 (.) animals (n ¼ 9
animals per group). *1-day data as in (Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004).

presynaptic terminal that has not been cleared following the first action collateral-CA1 synapse in the hippocampus following contextual fear
potential and as a result second action potential is augmented, conditioning but hippocampal synaptic facilitation lasts only 7 days
resulting in a greater probability of transmitter release and facilitated following training (Sacchetti et al., 2001). These electrophysiological
synaptic transmission. Conversely, if presynaptic function is already changes are thought to reflect a dynamic process by which memory is
enhanced the ratio of the second to the first event could be reduced initially formed and coded by changes in hippocampal synaptic
either through depletion of the readily releasable pool of vesicles, strength and then conveyed to remote cortical areas for long-term
activation of presynaptic autoreceptors, or decreased by desensitiza- storage (Kim & Fanselow, 1992; Squire & Alvarez, 1995; Anagnost-
tion of postsynaptic receptors initiated by the first synaptic event aras et al., 2001; Nader, 2003). Conversely, synaptic facilitation in the
(Zucker & Regehr, 2002). The data presented here demonstrated PPRs amygdala is present for at least 11 days after fear conditioning
were depressed in the fear conditioning group 1 day after fear (10 days post-testing). It is possible that this enduring facilitation of
conditioning, suggesting that the increase in synaptic strength synaptic transmission provides a synaptic trace for the long-term
recorded 1 day following fear testing is due, at least in part, to storage of emotional memories in the amygdala.
enhanced presynaptic function. The end result of an enhanced
presynaptic function is an increase in transmitter release for a given
stimulus input, which is reflected as an increase in the slope of the HFS-LTP remains occluded
input–output relationship of the fEPSP. These data support previous In addition to alterations in synaptic efficacy and short-term
findings showing that enhanced presynaptic function is associated plasticity, HFS–LTP was attenuated 10 days after fear conditioning
with the maintenance phase of fear memory (Tsvetkov et al., 2002) further suggesting that the cortico-LA synapse is involved in the
and the expression of LTP in the cortico-LA pathway (Huang & long-term storage of fear memory. Similar occlusion of LTP has
Kandel, 1998) as well as thalamo-LA pathway (McKernan & been described at the Schaffer–CA1 synapse in animals that have
Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997). These findings provide an additional been subjected to hippocampal-dependent learning tasks (Sacchetti
mechanistic link between the synaptic changes induced by fear et al., 2002). In that study, HFS–LTP was attenuated immediately
learning and those underlying LTP. following learning; however, complete reinstatement of synaptic
plasticity occurred within 7 days following training. The persistent
occlusion of amygdala HFS–LTP 11 days after cued fear condi-
Enduring synaptic facilitation in long-term fear memory tioning suggests that the cued fear engram in the amygdala may not
The increased synaptic weight and enhanced presynaptic function undergo a similar form of systems consolidation as in the
recorded 1 day after fear conditioning persisted 10 days after testing. hippocampus (Debiec et al., 2002) where the memory trace is
Similar synaptic potentiation has been described at the Schaffer exported to other brain areas for permanent storage. Rather, this

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783
Fear learning and persistent synaptic facilitation 1781

Fig. 5. LFS–LTP was expressed at 1 but not 10 days after fear conditioning. (A) Plot of previous data (Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004) showing occlusion
of LFS–LTP 1 day after fear training. Per cent potentiation of fEPSPs following LFS in slices is plotted with respect to time from naı̈ve (d), UP (s), and FC (.)
animals. (B, upper) fEPSPs before (baseline) and after LFS–LTP stimulation in slices from naı̈ve, unpaired (UP10) and fear-conditioned (FC10) animals. (B, lower)
Graph of percentage potentiation of fEPSPs following LFS is plotted with respect to time in slices from naı̈ve (d), UP10 (s), and FC10 (.) animals (n ¼ 9 animals
per group). *1-day data as in (Schroeder & Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004).

suggests that the cortico-LA synapse may play a role in storage of LFS is analogous to slow, delta wave sleep of 1–4 Hz that plays a
long-term fear memory. key role in the consolidation and maintenance of multiple forms of
memory (Kenny et al., 2003; Higgins et al., 2004). In rats, lateral
amygdala projection neurons increase firing during delta wave sleep
LFS–LTP is absent 10 days following fear conditioning (Pare & Gaudreau, 1996), and amygdala inactivation during sleep
The LFS–LTP recorded in amygdala slices from fear-conditioned periods following emotional learning leads to impaired memory
animals 1 day after testing was not present 10 days following fear retention (Rasmussen et al., 2004). Although the mechanisms
conditioning. Current theory suggests that reactivated memories underlying the transient nature of this event and its relationship to
undergo reconsolidation during which they become labile and memory consolidation ⁄ reconsolidation requires further analyses, these
susceptible to protein synthesis inhibitors (Nader et al., 2000). It is data raise the possibility that LFS–LTP recorded 1 day but not 10 days
possible that the difference in response to LFS 1 and 10 days after after fear conditioning may reflect an active consolidation ⁄ reconsol-
fear conditioning may be related to the temporal proximity of the idation process of fear memory within the amygdala during sleep,
recall of the fear memory and electrophysiological examination of which in rats coincides with our recording time frame.
amygdala function. The LFS–LTP may represent a dynamic state Taken together, the present data suggest that altered amygdala
associated with reconsolidation 1 day post-testing but not in long- synaptic physiology is not only associated with the formation of fear
term storage of a stabilized fear memory. Testing this hypothesis memories but also likely plays an important role in the maintenance
requires definitive experimentation but the present data showing an and long-term storage of fear memories. Brain systems outside the
absence of LFS–LTP 10 days following fear conditioning suggest amygdala such as the thalamus and cortex also show neural changes
that amygdala physiology in the period shortly after fear testing with fear conditioning (Weinberger, 1997; Maren et al., 2001), but
may differ from that in the long-term storage of fear memory. It is nearly all changes depend on the amygdala (Quirk et al., 1997;
also possible that testing itself may induce extinction training and Armony et al., 1998; Maren et al., 2001; Pare et al., 2002). A temporal
that LFS may in fact be related to extinction rather than a gradient for auditory fear responses is not evident after post-training
consolidation mechanism. However, previous studies have shown inhibition or lesioning of the amygdala suggesting auditory fear
that extinction of fear-potentiated startle requires approximately 60 memories are stored in the amygdala (Lee et al., 1996; Maren et al.,
presentations of the CS alone (Davis & Astrachan, 1978; Falls 1996). Additionally, a recent behavioural data showed that the
et al., 1992) suggesting that ten presentations of tone during testing amygdala was essential for conditioned freezing behaviour over the
may exert a relatively small influence on our responses. lifetime of the animal whether lesioned 1 day or over 1 years after

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783
1782 B. W. Schroeder and P. Shinnick-Gallagher

training (Gale et al., 2004). Our data provide direct electrophysiolog- Huang, Y.Y. & Kandel, E.R. (1998) Postsynaptic induction and
ical evidence to support a role for the amygdala in the long-term PKA-dependent expression of LTP in the lateral amygdala. Neuron, 21,
169–178.
storage and expression of fear memory. Huang, Y.Y., Martin, K.C. & Kandel, E.R. (2000) Both protein kinase A and
These findings have significant implications as the locus of storage mitogen-activated protein kinase are required in the amygdala for the
for fear memories and the synaptic alterations induced by these macromolecular synthesis-dependent late phase of long-term potentiation.
memories may guide the development of pharmacological and J. Neurosci., 20, 6317–6325.
behavioural treatment modalities for anxiety disorders. It has been Kenny, P.J., Gasparini, F. & Markou, A. (2003) Group II metabotropic and
alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionate (AMPA) ⁄ kainate
recently suggested that patients with learned anxiety disorders, such as glutamate receptors regulate the deficit in brain reward function associated
post-traumatic stress disorder, suffer from an inability to form effective with nicotine withdrawal in rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther., 306, 1068–
extinction engrams (Rothbaum & Davis, 2003). This study identifies 1076.
modifications of amygdala synaptic physiology that may serve as Kim, J.J. & Fanselow, M.S. (1992) Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of
fear. Science, 256, 675–677.
potential therapeutic targets for intervention in the treatment of learned Lee, Y., Walker, D. & Davis, M. (1996) Lack of a temporal gradient of
anxiety disorders in patients with established disease. retrograde amnesia following NMDA-induced lesions of the basolateral
amygdala assessed with the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behav.
Neurosci., 110, 836–839.
Acknowledgements Lledo, P.M., Hjelmstad, G.O., Mukherji, S., Soderling, T.R., Malenka, R.C. &
Nicoll, R.A. (1995) Calcium ⁄ calmodulin-dependent kinase II and long-term
We would like to thank Drs Joel Gallagher, Geoff Swanson, and Sebastian potentiation enhance synaptic transmission by the same mechanism. Proc.
Pollandt and Mr Michael Scott and Ms Julie Ross for their helpful reviews. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 92, 11175–11179.
Supported by MH58327 and F30 MH067458. Maren, S. (1999) Neurotoxic basolateral amygdala lesions impair learning and
memory but not the performance of conditional fear in rats. J. Neurosci., 19,
8696–8703.
Abbreviations Maren, S. (2000) Reply to Vazdarjanova. TINS, 23, 345–346.
Maren, S., Aharonov, G. & Fanselow, M.S. (1996) Retrograde abolition of
CS, conditioned stimulus; FC, fear-conditioned; fEPSPs, field excitatory conditional fear after excitotoxic lesions in the basolateral amygdala of rats:
postsynaptic potentials; HFS–LTP, high-frequency stimulation dependent LTP; absence of a temporal gradient. Behav. Neurosci., 110, 718–726.
IO, input–output; LA, lateral amygdala; LTP, long-term potentiation; LFS–LTP, Maren, S., Yap, S.A. & Goosens, K.A. (2001) The amygdala is essential for the
low-frequency stimulation dependent LTP; PPR, paired-pulse ratio; US, development of neuronal plasticity in the medial geniculate nucleus during
unconditioned stimulus; UP, unpaired. auditory fear conditioning in rats. J. Neurosci., 21, RC135.
Mckernan, M.G. & Shinnick-Gallagher, P. (1997) Fear conditioning induces a
lasting potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro. Nature, 390, 607–611.
Nader, K. (2003) Memory traces unbound. TINS, 26, 65–72.
References Nader, K., Schafe, G.E. & Ledoux, J.E. (2000) The labile nature of
Anagnostaras, S.G., Gale, G.D. & Fanselow, M.S. (2001) Hippocampus and consolidation theory. Nature Rev. Neurosci., 1, 216–219.
contextual fear conditioning: recent controversies and advances. Hippocam- Pare, D., Collins, D.R. & Pelletier, J.G. (2002) Amygdala oscillations and the
pus, 11, 8–17. consolidation of emotional memories. Trends Cogn. Sci., 6, 306–314.
Armony, J.L., Quirk, G.J. & Ledoux, J.E. (1998) Differential effects of Pare, D. & Gaudreau, H. (1996) Projection cells and interneurons of the
amygdala lesions on early and late plastic components of auditory cortex lateral and basolateral amygdala: distinct firing patterns and differential
spike trains during fear conditioning. J. Neurosci., 18, 2592–2601. relation to theta and delta rhythms in conscious cats. J. Neurosci., 16,
Bauer, E.P., Schafe, G.E. & Ledoux, J.E. (2002) NMDA receptors and L-type 3334–3350.
voltage-gated calcium channels contribute to long-term potentiation and Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. (1998) The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates.
different components of fear memory formation in the lateral amygdala. Academic Press, San Diego, CA.
J. Neurosci., 22, 5239–5249. Quirk, G.J., Armony, J.L. & Ledoux, J.E. (1997) Fear conditioning enhances
Cahill, L., Weinberger, N.M., Roozendaal, B. & Mcgaugh, J.L. (1999) Is the different temporal components of tone-evoked spike trains in auditory cortex
amygdala a locus of ‘conditioned fear’? Some questions and caveats. and lateral amygdala. Neuron, 19, 613–624.
Neuron, 23, 227–228. Rasmussen, K., Hsu, M.A. & Vandergriff, J. (2004) The selective mGlu2 ⁄ 3
Campeau, S. & Davis, M. (1995) Involvement of subcortical and cortical receptor antagonist LY341495 exacerbates behavioral signs of morphine
afferents to the lateral nucleus of the amygdala in fear conditioning measured withdrawal and morphine-withdrawal-induced activation of locus coeruleus
with fear-potentiated startle in rats trained concurrently with auditory and neurons. Neuropharmacology, 46, 620–628.
visual conditioned stimuli. J. Neurosci., 15, 2312–2327. Rodrigues, S.M., Bauer, E.P., Farb, C.R., Schafe, G.E. & Ledoux, J.E. (2002)
Chapman, P.F., Kairiss, E.W., Keenan, C.L. & Brown, T.H. (1990) Long-term The group I metabotropic glutamate receptor mGluR5 is required for fear
synaptic potentiation in the amygdala. Synapse, 6, 271–278. memory formation and long-term potentiation in the lateral amygdala.
Davis, M. & Astrachan, D.I. (1978) Conditioned fear and startle magnitude: J. Neurosci., 22, 5219–5229.
effects of different footshock or backshock intensities used in training. Rogan, M.T., Staubli, U.V. & Ledoux, J.E. (1997) Fear conditioning induces
J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process, 4, 95–103. associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature, 390, 604–
Debiec, J., Ledoux, J.E. & Nader, K. (2002) Cellular and systems 607.
reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron, 36, 527–538. Rothbaum, B.O. & Davis, M. (2003) Applying learning principles to the
Falls, W.A., Miserendino, M.J. & Davis, M. (1992) Extinction of fear- treatment of post-trauma reactions. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 1008, 112–121.
potentiated startle: blockade by infusion of an NMDA antagonist into the Sacchetti, B., Lorenzini, C.A., Baldi, E., Bucherelli, C., Roberto, M., Tassoni,
amygdala. J. Neurosci., 12, 854–863. G. & Brunelli, M. (2001) Long-lasting hippocampal potentiation and
Fendt, M. (2001) Injections of the NMDA receptor antagonist aminophospho- contextual memory consolidation. Eur. J. Neurosci., 13, 2291–2298.
nopentanoic acid into the lateral nucleus of the amygdala block the Sacchetti, B., Lorenzini, C.A., Baldi, E., Bucherelli, C., Roberto, M., Tassoni,
expression of fear-potentiated startle and freezing. J. Neurosci., 21, 4111– G. & Brunelli, M. (2002) Time-dependent inhibition of hippocampal LTP in
4115. vitro following contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci., 15,
Gale, G.D., Anagnostaras, S.G., Godsil, B.P., Mitchell, S., Nozawa, T., Sage, 143–150.
J.R., Wiltgen, B. & Fanselow, M.S. (2004) Role of the basolateral amygdala Schafe, G.E., Atkins, C.M., Swank, M.W., Bauer, E.P., Sweatt, J.D. & Ledoux,
in the storage of fear memories across the adult lifetime of rats. J. Neurosci., J.E. (2000) Activation of ERK ⁄ MAP kinase in the amygdala is required for
24, 3810–3815. memory consolidation of pavlovian fear conditioning. J. Neurosci., 20,
Higgins, G.A., Ballard, T.M., Kew, J.N., Richards, J.G., Kemp, J.A., Adam, G., 8177–8187.
Woltering, T., Nakanishi, S. & Mutel, V. (2004) Pharmacological manipula- Schroeder, B.W. & Shinnick-Gallagher, P. (2004) Fear memories induce a
tion of mGlu2 receptors influences cognitive performance in the rodent. switch in stimulus response and signaling mechanisms for long-term
Neuropharmacology, 46, 907–917. potentiation in the lateral amygdala. Eur. J. Neurosci., 20, 549–556.

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783
Fear learning and persistent synaptic facilitation 1783

Schulz, P.E., Cook, E.P. & Johnston, D. (1994) Changes in paired-pulse Vazdarjanova, A., Cahill, L. & Mcgaugh, J.L. (2001) Disrupting basolateral
facilitation suggest presynaptic involvement in long-term potentiation. J. amygdala function impairs unconditioned freezing and avoidance in rats.
Neurosci., 14, 5325–5337. Eur. J. Neurosci., 14, 709–718.
Schulz, P.E. & Fitzgibbons, J.C. (1997) Differing mechanisms of expression for Wallace, K.J. & Rosen, J.B. (2001) Neurotoxic lesions of the lateral nucleus of
short- and long-term potentiation. J. Neurophysiol., 78, 321–334. the amygdala decrease conditioned fear but not unconditioned fear of a
Shinnick-Gallagher, P., Mckernan, M.G., Xie, J. & Zinebi, F. (2003) predator odor: comparison with electrolytic lesions. J. Neurosci., 21, 3619–
L-type voltage-gated calcium channels are involved in the in vivo and 3627.
in vitro expression of fear conditioning. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 985, 135– Weinberger, N. (1997) Learning-induced receptive field plasticity in the
149. primary auditory cortex. Sem. Neurosci., 9, 59–67.
Squire, L.R. & Alvarez, P. (1995) Retrograde amnesia and memory con- Zinebi, F., Russell, R.T., Mckernan, M. & Shinnick-Gallagher, P. (2001)
solidation: a neurobiological perspective. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 5, Comparison of paired-pulse facilitation of AMPA and NMDA synaptic
169–177. currents in the lateral amygdala. Synapse, 42, 115–127.
Tsvetkov, E., Carlezon, W.A., Benes, F.M., Kandel, E.R. & Bolshakov, V.Y. Zinebi, F., Xie, J., Liu, J., Russell, R.T., Gallagher, J.P., Mckernan, M.G. &
(2002) Fear conditioning occludes LTP-induced presynaptic enhancement of Shinnick-Gallagher, P. (2003) NMDA currents and receptor protein are
synaptic transmission in the cortical pathway to the lateral amygdala. downregulated in the amygdala during maintenance of fear memory.
Neuron, 34, 289–300. J. Neurosci., 23, 10283–10291.
Vazdarjanova, A. (2000) Does the basolateral amygdala store memories for Zucker, R.S. & Regehr, W.G. (2002) Short-term synaptic plasticity. Annu. Rev.
emotional events? TINS, 23, 345–346. Physiol., 64, 355–405.

ª 2005 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies, European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1775–1783

You might also like