You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Psychiatric Research 151 (2022) 619–625

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Psychiatric Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jpsychires

Sex differences in long-term fear and anxiety-like responses in a preclinical


model of PTSD
Delara Emtyazi a, Thallita Kelly Rabelo a, Hailey Katzman a, Ana Carolina Campos a,
Mustansir Diwan a, Darryl Gidyk a, Patricia Rabelo dos Santos b, Peter Giacobbe a, c, e,
Nir Lipsman a, c, d, Isabelle Aubert c, f, Clement Hamani a, c, d, *
a
Harquail Centre for Neuromodulation, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Sunnybrook Research Institute, Toronto, ON, M4N 3M5, Canada
b
Department of Medicine, Universidade Federal de Rondônia, Porto Velho, RO, 76801-059, Brazil
c
Hurvitz Brain Sciences Centre, Sunnybrook Research Institute, Toronto, ON, M4N 3M5, Canada
d
Division of Neurosurgery, Department of Medicine, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, M4N 3M5, Canada
e
Department of Psychiatry, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Toronto, ON, M4N 3M5, Canada
f
Department of Laboratory Medicine and Pathobiology, Temerty Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, ON, M5S 1A8, Canada

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: With a high prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in females, studying sex differences in preclinical
Sex models is of substantial importance. We have previously employed behavioural criteria to identify and charac­
Fear terize a subpopulation of rats that presented impaired fear extinction and long-term fear and anxiety responses
Anxiety
following fear conditioning. We now exposed male and female rats to fear conditioning and extinction and
Estrous cycle
segregated the animals into weak- (WE) and strong-extinction (SE) groups based on behavioural scores during
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Extinction extinction. Animals were subsequently tested for tone and context recall, as well as anxiety-like responses in the
Conditioning marble burying and novelty suppression of feeding (NSF) tests. Vaginal lavages were collected to characterize the
phase of the estrous cycle during fear extinction. We found that females had reduced freezing during tone recall
and a lower latency to feed in the NSF test. No differences were found in females undergoing extinction during
high and low estrogen phases of the cycle in any of the performed tests. Overall, the percentage of animals that
presented WE and SE phenotypes was similar in males and females. Both, WE males and females had increased
freezing during tone and context recall. Along with our previous reports, WE males presented anxiety-like re­
sponses, particularly in the NSF compared to SE animals. In contrast, WE females buried less marbles than their
SE mates. Future investigation including a larger number of behavioural tests are certainly required to corrob­
orate our findings and ascertain potential mechanisms to explain the differences observed in our study.

1. Introduction of rodents exposed to a traumatic event (e.g., fear conditioning) exhibit


long-term fear and anxiety-type responses, have high baseline serum
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a debilitating psychiatric corticosterone levels and a disrupted connectivity between the pre­
condition that emerges following exposure to a traumatic event. Females frontal cortical and the basolateral amygdala (Reznikov et al., 2015,
are not only more likely to develop PTSD, but also tend to present more 2018; Vimalanathan et al., 2020).
severe and protracted symptoms (Breslau, 2002; Breslau et al., 1998; We compare long-term fear and anxiety-like responses between fe­
Kilpatrick et al., 2013). While preclinical studies have been instrumental males and male rats, further subdividing the animals according to the
to help us understand the mechanisms and circuitry of fear memory and estrous cycle phase during extinction.
anxiety-type behaviours, they have mainly been conducted in males
(Cohen et al., 2004, 2006; Kessler et al., 1995; Miller and McEwen,
2006; Siegmund and Wotjak, 2006).
We have recently found that, similar to patients with PTSD, 20–30%

* Corresponding author. Sunnybrook Research Institute, 2075 Bayview Av, S126, Toronto, ON, M4N3M5, Canada.,
E-mail addresses: clement.hamani@sunnybrook.ca, c.hamani@sympatico.ca (C. Hamani).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.05.015
Received 5 March 2022; Received in revised form 14 May 2022; Accepted 19 May 2022
Available online 23 May 2022
0022-3956/© 2022 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
D. Emtyazi et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 151 (2022) 619–625

2. Methods increase the statistical power while reducing the number of animals, rats
showing moderate levels of freezing (i.e., 33–67%) were not included in
2.1. Animals the analyses (Reznikov et al., 2015; Vimalanathan et al., 2020).

Outbred male (n = 20; 250–275g) and female (n = 30; 200–250 g) 2.3. Anxiety-type behaviour
Sprague-Dawley rats (Charles River, Quebec) were housed in pairs on a
12 h light–dark cycle with water and standard rat chow available ad Marble burying test (MBT). On day 12, rats were placed in a stan­
libitum. All protocols were approved by the Animal Care committee of dard cage containing 5 cm of bedding and 15 identical marbles evenly
the Sunnybrook Research Institute. The timeline for the experiments is arranged in three columns. The number of marbles buried (<50% of
shown in Suppl Fig. 1. visible surface) in a 30 min session was quantified.
Novelty Suppressed Feeding (NSF). Animals were trained to
consume food treats (Honey Nut Cheerios®) in their home cages for 2
2.2. Fear conditioning, extinction and recall
days prior to testing. They were then food deprived for 16h prior to
testing. On the testing day, animals were placed in a novel Plexiglas cage
Fear conditioning and extinction were carried out in an operant
lined, which contained the previously habituated treat. Latency to begin
conditioning chamber (Coulbourn Instruments, Holliston, MA). On day
consuming the food was measured, as previously described (Reznikov
1, rats were presented with six conditioned stimuli (CS; 30sec, 85 dB, 4
et al., 2015).
kHz auditory tones), each co-terminating with a footshock (uncondi­
tioned stimulus, US; 0.8 mA, 0.5s), as previously described (Gidyk et al.,
2.4. Determining the phase of the estrous cycle
2021; Reznikov et al., 2015, 2018; Vimalanathan et al., 2020). On day 2,
rats underwent extinction training, which consisted of 12 presentations
Stages of the estrous cycle were identified based on cytology from
of the CS in the absence of shocks. On days 10 and 11, rats were exposed
vaginal lavages. Briefly, smears were obtained approximately 30 min
to 2 CS-only presentations in a novel context (tone recall) or in the
after fear extinction. Samples were collected using 300 μL of saline via a
original context in the absence of CS or shocks (context recall). %
1000 μL pipette and mounted onto positively charged slides. After
freezing was calculated as (duration of freezing/30sec)*100.
collection, samples were allowed to air-dry overnight prior to staining
with Giemsa. Stages of the estrous cycle were classified according to
2.2.1. Behavioural classification criteria
specific cell structures in the collected material (Ajayi and Akhigbe,
Similar to our previous work, animals were subdivided in two
2020; Marcondes et al., 2002).
groups: Weak Extinction (WE): average percentage of freezing >67%
during the last 2 blocks of extinction (4 trials); Strong Extinction (SE):
average percentage of freezing <33% freezing average during the last 2
extinction blocks (Reznikov et al., 2015; Vimalanathan et al., 2020). To

Fig. 1. Behavioural results according to sex. A) Fear Conditioning and Extinction. No differences were found in the percentage of freezing between females (n = 24)
and males (n = 16) across conditioning (C1–C3) and extinction sessions (E1-E6). B, C) Females had a significant decrease in the percentage of freezing compared to
males during tone recall, while no significant sex differences were found during context recall. D) No differences were observed in the marble burying test between
females and males. E) In the novelty supressed feeding test, females had a significantly lower latency to feed compared to males. Significant trial effects were not
indicated for clarity. Values represent means and SEM. **p ≤ 0.01, *p ≤ 0.05.

620
D. Emtyazi et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 151 (2022) 619–625

2.5. Statistical analyses 3.2. Weak vs strong extinction

Mixed design ANOVA was used to analyze freezing scores during Once freezing time during fear conditioning and extinction was
conditioning and extinction sessions with group as between-subjects and scored, animals were subdivided in strong-extinction, weak-extinction
time as within-subjects factors (Sidak post hoc). In our initial experi­ or interthreshold groups (Reznikov et al., 2015; Vimalanathan et al.,
ment, groups (between-subjects) were males vs females. In our second 2020). In females, 7 and 17 out of 30 animals were respectively classi­
experiment, groups were WE vs SE, with separate analyses conducted for fied as WE (23%) and SE (57%). Six animals were characterized as
males and females. In our third experiment, groups were proestrus vs intrathreshold (20%) and excluded from further analyses. This propor­
metestrus/diestrus. Independent t-tests were used for recall trials, MBT, tion was similar to that of WE, SE and intrathreshold males, which
and NSF. The Fisher’s exact test was used to compare the proportion of comprised 25% (5/20), 55% (11/20) and 20% (4/20) of the population
WE/SE animals. Unless otherwise specified, data are reported as mean (p = 0.99).
± standard error of the mean (SEM). Statistical significance was set at p In females, significant trial-block [F(2,44) = 167.6, p < 0.001] and
≤ 0.05. group effects (strong vs. weak-extinction) [F(1,22) = 7.79, p = 0.01] but
no trial x group interaction [F(2,44) = 2.74, p = 0.08] were found
3. Results during conditioning (Fig. 2A). Males had a significant trial-block effect
[F(2,28) = 87.16, p < 0.001], but no group effect [F(1,14) = 0.67, p =
3.1. Behavioural characteristics of female and male rats 0.42] or trial x group interaction [F(2,28) = 0.21, p = 0.82; ] (Fig. 2B).
Post-hoc tests revealed that WE females froze significantly more than SE
During conditioning, trial x group interaction [F(2,76) = 6.56, p = animals in block C2 of the conditioning phase (p < 0.05).
0.002] and trial block effect were found to be significant [F(2,76) = During extinction, the effects of trial-block [F(5, 110) = 6.28, p <
279.6, p < 0.001], while no group effect was noted (Female vs. Male) [F 0.0001], group [F(1,22) = 43.52, p < 0.0001] and interaction trial x
(1,38) = 0.001, p = 0.97] (Fig. 1A). During extinction, a significant trial- group [F(5, 110) = 2.60, p = 0.03] were all found to be significant in
block effect [F(5, 190) = 23.30, p < 0.0001] but no group effect [F females. In males, significant trial-block [F(5, 70) = 9.78, p < 0.0001]
(1,38) = 1.05, p = 0.32] or trial x group interaction [F(5,190) = 1.86, p and group effects [F(1,14) = 24.59, p = 0.0002], but no trial x group
= 0.10] were observed (Fig. 1A). interaction [F(5, 70) = 0.42, p = 0.83] were found. Post-hoc tests
While females froze significantly less during the tone recall (females revealed that WE females froze significantly more than SE animals in E2-
21.2 ± 4.3% vs. males 44.1 ± 4.6%; p = 0.001; Fig. 1B) no sex differ­ E6. In males, significant differences were found in E2, E3, E5 and E6
ences were recorded during context recall (females 30.6 ± 5.7% vs. (Fig. 2A and B).
males 38.8 ± 5.3%; p = 0.30; Fig. 1C). Both females (6.8 ± 0.7) and Freezing in SE females was significantly lower than in WE animals
males (6.4 ± 0.6) buried a similar number of marbles (p = 0.19; during tone recall (13.7 ± 3.5% vs 39.4 ± 9.4%; p = 0.03; Fig. 3A) and
Fig. 1D). The latency to feed in the NSF was significantly lower in fe­ context recall (22.7 ± 5.9% vs 49.8 ± 10.9%; p = 0.004; Fig. 3B). In
males (females 242.0 ± 34.7 s) compared to males (347.9 ± 36.79 s; p males, freezing was also significantly lower in SE compared to WE ani­
= 0.05; Fig. 1E). mals during tone recall (34.9 ± 5.7% vs 64.4 ± 5.7%; p = 0.0006;
Fig. 3E) and context recall (32.1 ± 5.4% vs 58.0 ± 7.1%,; p = 0.008;
Fig. 3F).

Fig. 2. Fear Conditioning and Extinction in Weak


(WE) and Strong Extinction animals (SE). A) During
conditioning, WE females (n = 7) had a reduced
percentage of freezing compared to SE (n = 17) ani­
mals in C2. During extinction, WE females froze
significantly more than SE females in all sessions. B)
In males, no difference was found between the WE (n
= 5) and SE groups (n = 11) during conditioning. In
contrast, WE males froze significantly more than SE
animals during E2, E3, E5 and E6. Significant trial
effects were not indicated for clarity. Values represent
means and SEM. **p ≤ 0.01, *p ≤ 0.05.

621
D. Emtyazi et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 151 (2022) 619–625

Fig. 3. Long-term fear and anxiety-like behaviour in Weak (WE) and Strong Extinction (SE) animals. Both WE females (n = 7) and males (n = 5) had significantly
higher freezing scores during tone recall (A, E) and context recall compared to SE animals (n = 17 females, n = 11 males; B, F). In the marble burying test, WE females
buried less marbles than their SE mates (C), whereas a trend was noticed for WE males to bury more marbles compared to SE animals (G). In the novelty supressed
feeding test, no differences were found between WE and SE females (D), whereas WE males showed a significantly higher latency to feed (H). Values represent means
and SEM. ***p ≤ 0.001, **p ≤ 0.01, *p ≤ 0.05.

In the marble burying tests, SE females had a 77% significant in­ metestrus/diestrus 248.1 ± 44.50 s; p = 0.60; Fig. 4E) between groups.
crease in the number of buried marbles compared to WE animals (7.8. ±
0.7 vs 4.4. ± 1.3; p = 0.03; Fig. 3C). In contrast, SE males buried 29% 4. Discussion
less marbles than their WE mates (5.7 ± 0.7 vs 8.0 ± 1.1; p = 0.08;
Fig. 3G). Experiments in the current study were conducted to test whether sex
In the NSF test, while no differences were found between WE and SE differences could be observed in our proposed model of PTSD. Since
females (255.4 ± 88.0 s vs 236.4 ± 35.2 s; p = 0.81; Fig. 3D), SE males estrogen during fear extinction was shown to reduce freezing during
had a 36% significantly lower latency to feed compared with WE ani­ recall (Chang et al., 2009; Milad et al., 2009), we hypothesized that
mals (295.5 ± 40.3 s vs 463.0 ± 49.7 s; p = 0.03; Fig. 3H). females extinguished in high hormonal phases of the cycle would show
reduced fear and anxiety-type responses compared to animals extin­
3.3. Estrous cycle guished during low hormonal phases. Along this line, we expected this
latter group to have a behavioural pattern similar to that observed in
In the proestrus group (5 in total), 1 female was classified as WE males. These hypotheses, however, were not confirmed by our results, as
(20%) and 4 as SE (80%). Similarly (p > 0.99), 5 out of 15 metestrus/ no behavioural differences were found in animals undergoing extinction
diestrus females (33%) were classified as WE and 10 as SE (67%). during different phases of the estrous cycle. In addition, females froze
During conditioning, a significant effect of trial block [F(2,36) = less during tone recall and had a lower latency to feed in the NSF
92.93; p < 0.0001], but no group effect (proestrus vs. metestrus/dies­ compared to males. A novel aspect of our study was that males pre­
trus) [F(1,18) = 0.80; p = 0.38] or trial x group interaction [F(2,36) = senting the extinction deficient phenotype developed long-term fear and
1.38; p = 0.27] were observed (Fig. 4A). During extinction, a significant anxiety-like behaviour, whereas only the former was observed in fe­
effect of trial-block [F(5, 90) = 7.09; p < 0.0001] but no group effect [F males. In contrast to males, WE females buried less marbles. As defen­
(1,18) = 0.01; p = 0.92] or trial x group interaction [F(5,90) = 0.82; p = sive burying is an innate behavioural response, this may suggest a
0.54] were found (Fig. 4A). shifted coping strategy, rather than an increase in anxiety.
Freezing during tone recall (proestrus 21.3 ± 10.6%; metestrus/ Sex differences have been a major subject of study in rodents and
diestrus 22.6 ± 5.9%; p = 0.92; Fig. 4B) and context recall (proestrus humans undergoing fear conditioning and extinction paradigms (Dalla
32.1 ± 12.2%; metestrus/diestrus 27.6 ± 6.9 &; p = 0.75; Fig. 4C) was and Shors, 2009; Stark et al., 2006). In the literature, a series of rodent
similar in females extinguished during proestrus or low estrogen phases studies have shown enhanced conditioned fear in males, while others
of the cycle. Similarly, no differences were found in the number of demonstrated no sex differences (Maren et al., 1994; Pryce et al., 1999).
buried marbles (proestrus 6.0 ± 1.6; metestrus/diestrus 7.4 ± 0.8; p = Similar to the former, human reports suggest that men exhibit a trend
0.42; Fig. 4D) or the latency to feed (proestrus 295.2 ± 71.8 s; towards an increased conditioned fear relative to women (Milad et al.,

622
D. Emtyazi et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 151 (2022) 619–625

Fig. 4. Behavioural results according to the estrous cycle. Females were subdivided into proestrus (n = 5) and metestrus/diestrus groups (n = 15). No differences
were found between groups during A) fear conditioning and extinction, B) tone recall, C) context recall, D) marble burying, and E) novelty supressed feeding.
Significant trial effects were not indicated for clarity. Values represent means and SEM.

2006, 2009, 2010). Our study corroborates these findings, inasmuch as associated with the phase of the estrous cycle during extinction.
males presented slightly higher freezing scores, albeit not significantly Controversial findings have been reported on the potential associa­
different, compared to females in C3. tion between the estrous cycle and defensive burying. While a few
In rodents, sex differences during fear extinction were largely re­ studies have shown that females in estrus exhibit less impulsive burying
ported in studies that considered the estrous cycle and hormonal profiles compared to animals in the diestrus (Llaneza and Frye, 2009), others
(Cossio et al., 2016; Maeng et al., 2015; Milad et al., 2009). Proestrus failed to show an influence of the estrous cycle on this behavioural trait
extinguished females had less freezing during recall than (Zhao et al., 2021). Corroborating the former, the administration of
metestrus-extinguished animals (Milad et al., 2009). The administration progesterone or estrogen + progesterone to ovariectomized rats
of estrogen to rodents either systemically or focally (e.g., into the hip­ decreased impulsive burying (Llaneza and Frye, 2009), suggesting a
pocampus) facilitates extinction and reduces freezing during recall hormonal influence. Overall, we found neither sex differences nor a
(Chang et al., 2009; Milad et al., 2009). In contrast, a few studies re­ strong influence of the estrous cycle on marble burying, but an opposite
ported no relationship between fear extinction and the estrous cycle trend in WE females and males. While the former showed an
(Graham, 2018; Lesuis et al., 2018). Along with these findings, we show anxiolytic-like response, SE males buried 29% less marbles (non-sig­
no sex differences or an influence of the estrous cycle during extinction. nificant trend) than WE animals. Overall, the use of defensive burying as
One important sex difference observed in our study was the lower a surrogate of anxiety-like behaviour has limitations (de Brouwer et al.,
freezing scores recorded in females during tone recall. In the literature, a 2019; Gidyk et al., 2021; Nicolas et al., 2006; Njung’e and Handley,
few reports showed no apparent differences in recall between sexes or 1991). Though animals receiving anxiolytic and antidepressant treat­
even an increased freezing in females (Fenton et al., 2014, 2016; Leb­ ments tend to bury less marbles, burying objects considered to be
ron-Milad et al., 2013). Differences between our study and others may dangerous, threatening or associated with an unpleasant experience is
be related to the number of extinction sessions and the timeframe be­ considered an innate rodent behaviour (de Brouwer et al., 2019; Gidyk
tween extinction and recall. In addition, freezing during recall may also et al., 2021; Nicolas et al., 2006; Njung’e and Handley, 1991; Reznikov
vary according to reproductive behaviour, as only nulliparous females et al., 2016). At present, we do not have a clear explanation for the sex
have lower freezing during recall in the proestrus phase (Tang and differences observed in our study. It is possible that, instead of an in­
Graham, 2020). crease in anxiety, WE females may present a shift in coping strategy,
In the current study, we show that a similar proportion of females rather than an increase in anxiety. This, however, remains to be
undergoing fear conditioning had extinction deficits and prolonged fear demonstrated in future studies.
responses (WE phenotype) compared to males. Despite these findings, Also interesting were the sex differences observed in novelty
however, major sex differences were observed in defensive burying and supressed feeding. This test is proposed for the study of anxiety-like
in the novelty supressed feeding test. While WE males had a significantly behaviour because rodents have neohypophagia when exposed to a
higher latency to feed compared to SE animals, WE females had a novel stressful environment. As commonly conducted, however, it also
reduction in defensive burying. Performance in neither test was has a motivational component (Bambico et al., 2015; Bodnoff et al.,

623
D. Emtyazi et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 151 (2022) 619–625

1988; Dulawa and Hen, 2005; Santarelli et al., 2003; Stedenfeld et al., Appendix A. Supplementary data
2011). Since animals are often food-deprived, they experience the
conflict of being driven to eat in a fearful novel open space. Several Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
antidepressant treatments reduce the latency to feed in this paradigm org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.05.015.
(Bambico et al., 2015; Bodnoff et al., 1988; Dulawa and Hen, 2005;
Santarelli et al., 2003; Stedenfeld et al., 2011). In our study, females had References
a lower latency to feed compared to males, with no effect of the estrous
cycle being recorded. In addition, only SE males had an anxiolytic-like Ajayi, A.F., Akhigbe, R.E., 2020. Staging of the estrous cycle and induction of estrus in
experimental rodents: an update. Fertil. Res. Pract. 6, 5. https://10.1186/s40738-02
effect compared to WE animals. We note, however, that the latency to 0-00074-3.
feed in both WE and SE females was similar to that observed in SE males, Auta, T., Hassan, A.T., 2016. Alteration in oestrus cycle and implantation in Mus
and approximately 45–50% lower than in WE males. In previous reports, musculus administered aqueous wood ash extract of Azadirachta indica (neem).
Asian Pac. J. Reprod. 5, 188–192.
female rats were shown to consume a higher amount and derive a more Bambico, F.R., Bregman, T., Diwan, M., Li, J., Darvish-Ghane, S., Li, Z., Laver, B.,
rewarding experience from palatable foods than males (Klump et al., Amorim, B.O., Covolan, L., Nobrega, J.N., Hamani, C., 2015. Neuroplasticity-
2013; Sinclair et al., 2017). That said, when exposed to a novel context, dependent and -independent mechanisms of chronic deep brain stimulation in
stressed rats. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e674. https://10.1038/tp.2015.166.
females seem to habituate as well as males, consuming less food in a Bodnoff, S.R., Suranyi-Cadotte, B., Aitken, D.H., Quirion, R., Meaney, M.J., 1988. The
novel context (Greiner and Petrovich, 2020). The contrasting results effects of chronic antidepressant treatment in an animal model of anxiety.
between these latter findings and ours may be related to the fact that we Psychopharmacology (Berl) 95, 298–302. https://10.1007/BF00181937.
Breslau, N., 2002. Gender differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder.
have habituated the animals to palatable foods before testing in a novel
J. Gend. Specif. Med. 5, 34–40.
context. Similar to a previous report (Zhao et al., 2021), we found no Breslau, N., Kessler, R.C., Chilcoat, H.D., Schultz, L.R., Davis, G.C., Andreski, P., 1998.
association between the estrous cycle and latency to feed. Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: the 1996 detroit area
One aspect that needs to be discussed regarding our estrous cycle survey of trauma. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 55, 626–632. https://10.1001/archpsyc
.55.7.626.
experiments is that group assignment was based on vaginal lavage Chang, Y.J., Yang, C.H., Liang, Y.C., Yeh, C.M., Huang, C.C., Hsu, K.S., 2009. Estrogen
samples collected during extinction, and not during tests to measure modulates sexually dimorphic contextual fear extinction in rats through estrogen
anxiety-type behaviour. We chose to collect vaginal samples at that time receptor beta. Hippocampus 19, 1142–1150. https://10.1002/hipo.20581.
Cohen, H., Matar, M.A., Richter-Levin, G., Zohar, J., 2006. The contribution of an animal
point due to the strong influence of the estrous phase on extinction model toward uncovering biological risk factors for PTSD. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
(Milad et al., 2009). The estrous cycle in female rodents lasts 4–5 days, 1071, 335–350. https://1071/1/335[pii]10.1196/annals.1364.026.
but small individual variations may occur (Ajayi and Akhigbe, 2020; Cohen, H., Zohar, J., Matar, M.A., Zeev, K., Loewenthal, U., Richter-Levin, G., 2004.
Setting apart the affected: the use of behavioral criteria in animal models of post
Auta and Hassan, 2016). Under these circumstances, though it is likely traumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1962–1970 [pii]. http
that differences in the estrous phase between groups extinguished in s://10.1038/sj.npp.1300523.
high vs low estrous phases would be maintained during anxiety testing, Cossio, R., Carreira, M.B., Vasquez, C.E., Britton, G.B., 2016. Sex differences and estrous
cycle effects on foreground contextual fear conditioning. Physiol. Behav. 163,
we cannot guarantee that females were tested in the same phases of the 305–311. https://10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.026.
cycle during marble burying and NSF. Dalla, C., Shors, T.J., 2009. Sex differences in learning processes of classical and operant
In summary, we found that sex differences in tone recall, and the NSF conditioning. Physiol. Behav. 97, 229–238. https://10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.035.
de Brouwer, G., Fick, A., Harvey, B.H., Wolmarans, W., 2019. A critical inquiry into
were unrelated to the estrous cycle. In addition, major sex differences
marble-burying as a preclinical screening paradigm of relevance for anxiety and
were observed between WE and SE, with males presenting long-term obsessive-compulsive disorder: mapping the way forward. Cognit. Affect Behav.
anxiety-type behaviour in the NSF and females presenting an Neurosci. 19, 1–39. https://10.3758/s13415-018-00653-4.
anxiolytic-type response in the marble burying test. Future investigation Dulawa, S.C., Hen, R., 2005. Recent advances in animal models of chronic antidepressant
effects: the novelty-induced hypophagia test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 771–783.
including a larger number of tests, oophorectomized animals and hor­ https://10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.017.
monal administration is certainly required to corroborate our findings Fenton, G.E., Halliday, D.M., Mason, R., Bredy, T.W., Stevenson, C.W., 2016. Sex
and ascertain potential mechanisms to explain the sex differences differences in learned fear expression and extinction involve altered gamma
oscillations in medial prefrontal cortex. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 135, 66–72. http
observed in our study. s://10.1016/j.nlm.2016.06.019.
Fenton, G.E., Pollard, A.K., Halliday, D.M., Mason, R., Bredy, T.W., Stevenson, C.W.,
Role of funding 2014. Persistent prelimbic cortex activity contributes to enhanced learned fear
expression in females. Learn. Mem. 21, 55–60. https://10.1101/lm.033514.113.
Gidyk, D.C., Diwan, M., Gouveia, F.V., Giacobbe, P., Lipsman, N., Hamani, C., 2021.
The funders had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, or Investigating the role of CB1 endocannabinoid transmission in the anti-fear and
preparation of the manuscript. anxiolytic-like effects of ventromedial prefrontal cortex deep brain stimulation.
J. Psychiatr. Res. 135, 264–269. https://10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.029.
Graham, B.M., 2018. Postnatal stress is associated with impaired fear conditioning and
Contributors extinction, and heightened hippocampal fibroblast growth factor 2, in mother rats.
Horm. Behav. 105, 110–114. https://10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.08.005.
Greiner, E.M., Petrovich, G.D., 2020. The effects of novelty on food consumption in male
DE was responsible for executing the experiments, analyzing data
and female rats. Physiol. Behav. 223, 112970. https://10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.
and preparing the manuscript. TR, ACC, PRS, HK, MD and DG helped 112970.
conducting some of the behavioural experiments. PG, NL and IA pro­ Kessler, R.C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., Nelson, C.B., 1995. Posttraumatic
vided critical input. CH has planned the studies, analyzed data and stress disorder in the national comorbidity survey. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 52,
1048–1060. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012.
prepared the final version of the manuscript. Kilpatrick, D.G., Resnick, H.S., Milanak, M.E., Miller, M.W., Keyes, K.M., Friedman, M.J.,
2013. National estimates of exposure to traumatic events and PTSD prevalence using
Declaration of competing interest DSM-IV and DSM-5 criteria. J. Trauma Stress 26, 537–547. https://10.1002/
jts.21848.
Klump, K.L., Racine, S., Hildebrandt, B., Sisk, C.L., 2013. Sex differences in binge eating
The authors declare no conflicts of interest related to this study. patterns in male and female adult rats. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46, 729–736. https://
10.1002/eat.22139.
Lebron-Milad, K., Tsareva, A., Ahmed, N., Milad, M.R., 2013. Sex differences and estrous
Acknowledgments cycle in female rats interact with the effects of fluoxetine treatment on fear
extinction. Behav. Brain Res. 253, 217–222. https://10.1016/j.bbr.2013.07.024.
This work was supported by funds from Veteran Affairs Canada and Lesuis, S.L., Catsburg, L.A.E., Lucassen, P.J., Krugers, H.J., 2018. Effects of corticosterone
on mild auditory fear conditioning and extinction; role of sex and training paradigm.
the Harquail Centre for Neuromodulation. IA is supported by a Tier 1 Learn. Mem. 25, 544–549. https://10.1101/lm.047811.118.
Canada Research Chair in Brain Repair and Regeneration. Llaneza, D.C., Frye, C.A., 2009. Progestogens and estrogen influence impulsive burying
and avoidant freezing behavior of naturally cycling and ovariectomized rats.
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93, 337–342. https://10.1016/j.pbb.2009.05.003.

624
D. Emtyazi et al. Journal of Psychiatric Research 151 (2022) 619–625

Maeng, L.Y., Cover, K.K., Landau, A.J., Milad, M.R., Lebron-Milad, K., 2015. Protocol for Reznikov, R., Binko, M., Nobrega, J.N., Hamani, C., 2016. Deep brain stimulation in
studying extinction of conditioned fear in naturally cycling female rats. JoVE. htt animal models of fear, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder.
ps://10.3791/52202. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 2810–2817. https://10.1038/npp.2016.34.
Marcondes, F.K., Bianchi, F.J., Tanno, A.P., 2002. Determination of the estrous cycle Reznikov, R., Diwan, M., Nobrega, J.N., Hamani, C., 2015. Towards a better preclinical
phases of rats: some helpful considerations. Braz. J. Biol. 62, 609–614. https://10 model of PTSD: characterizing animals with weak extinction, maladaptive stress
.1590/s1519-69842002000400008. responses and low plasma corticosterone. J. Psychiatr. Res. 61, 158–165. https://10.
Maren, S., De Oca, B., Fanselow, M.S., 1994. Sex differences in hippocampal long-term 1016/j.jpsychires.2014.12.017.
potentiation (LTP) and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats: positive correlation Santarelli, L., Saxe, M., Gross, C., Surget, A., Battaglia, F., Dulawa, S., Weisstaub, N.,
between LTP and contextual learning. Brain Res. 661, 25–34. Lee, J., Duman, R., Arancio, O., Belzung, C., Hen, R., 2003. Requirement of
https://10.1016/0006-8993(94)91176-2. hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301,
Milad, M.R., Goldstein, J.M., Orr, S.P., Wedig, M.M., Klibanski, A., Pitman, R.K., 805–809. https://10.1126/science.1083328.
Rauch, S.L., 2006. Fear conditioning and extinction: influence of sex and menstrual Siegmund, A., Wotjak, C.T., 2006. Toward an animal model of posttraumatic stress
cycle in healthy humans. Behav. Neurosci. 120, 1196–1203. https://10.1037/0 disorder. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, 324–334. https://1071/1/324 [pii] 10.11
735-7044.120.5.1196. 96/annals.1364.025.
Milad, M.R., Igoe, S.A., Lebron-Milad, K., Novales, J.E., 2009. Estrous cycle phase and Sinclair, E.B., Hildebrandt, B.A., Culbert, K.M., Klump, K.L., Sisk, C.L., 2017. Preliminary
gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience 164, evidence of sex differences in behavioral and neural responses to palatable food
887–895. https://10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.011. reward in rats. Physiol. Behav. 176, 165–173. https://10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.0
Milad, M.R., Zeidan, M.A., Contero, A., Pitman, R.K., Klibanski, A., Rauch, S.L., 42.
Goldstein, J.M., 2010. The influence of gonadal hormones on conditioned fear Stark, R., Wolf, O.T., Tabbert, K., Kagerer, S., Zimmermann, M., Kirsch, P., Schienle, A.,
extinction in healthy humans. Neuroscience 168, 652–658. https://10.1016/j.neuro Vaitl, D., 2006. Influence of the stress hormone cortisol on fear conditioning in
science.2010.04.030. humans: evidence for sex differences in the response of the prefrontal cortex.
Miller, M.M., McEwen, B.S., 2006. Establishing an agenda for translational research on Neuroimage 32, 1290–1298. https://10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.046.
PTSD. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, 294–312. https://1071/1/294 [pii] 10.11 Stedenfeld, K.A., Clinton, S.M., Kerman, I.A., Akil, H., Watson, S.J., Sved, A.F., 2011.
96/annals.1364.023. Novelty-seeking behavior predicts vulnerability in a rodent model of depression.
Nicolas, L.B., Kolb, Y., Prinssen, E.P., 2006. A combined marble burying-locomotor Physiol. Behav. 103, 210–216. https://10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.001.
activity test in mice: a practical screening test with sensitivity to different classes of Tang, S., Graham, B.M., 2020. Hormonal, reproductive, and behavioural predictors of
anxiolytics and antidepressants. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 547, 106–115. https://10.1016/j fear extinction recall in female rats. Horm. Behav. 121, 104693. https://10.1016/j.
.ejphar.2006.07.015. yhbeh.2020.104693.
Njung’e, K., Handley, S.L., 1991. Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model of Vimalanathan, A., Gidyk, D.C., Diwan, M., Gouveia, F.V., Lipsman, N., Giacobbe, P.,
anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 38, 63–67. https://10.1016/0091-3057(91) Nobrega, J.N., Hamani, C., 2020. Endocannabinoid modulating drugs improve
90590-x. anxiety but not the expression of conditioned fear in a rodent model of post-
Pryce, C.R., Lehmann, J., Feldon, J., 1999. Effect of sex on fear conditioning is similar for traumatic stress disorder. Neuropharmacology 166, 107965. https://10.1016/j.neu
context and discrete CS in Wistar, Lewis and Fischer rat strains. Pharmacol. Biochem. ropharm.2020.107965.
Behav. 64, 753–759. https://10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00147-1. Zhao, W., Li, Q., Ma, Y., Wang, Z., Fan, B., Zhai, X., Hu, M., Wang, Q., Zhang, M.,
Reznikov, R., Bambico, F.R., Diwan, M., Raymond, R.J., Nashed, M.G., Nobrega, J.N., Zhang, C., Qin, Y., Sha, S., Gan, Z., Ye, F., Xia, Y., Zhang, G., Yang, L., Zou, S., Xu, Z.,
Hamani, C., 2018. Prefrontal cortex deep brain stimulation improves fear and Xia, S., Yu, Y., Abdul, M., Yang, J.X., Cao, J.L., Zhou, F., Zhang, H., 2021. Behaviors
anxiety-like behavior and reduces basolateral amygdala activity in a preclinical related to psychiatric disorders and pain perception in C57BL/6J mice during
model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 1099–1106. different phases of estrous cycle. Front. Neurosci. 15, 650793. https://10.338
https://10.1038/npp.2017.207. 9/fnins.2021.650793.

625

You might also like