You are on page 1of 18

Psychology & Neuroscience © 2017 American Psychological Association

2017, Vol. 10, No. 3, 345–362 1983-3288/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pne0000102

The Effects of Stress on Prospective Memory: A Systematic Review

Martina Piefke and Katharina Glienke


Witten/Herdecke University

Prospective memory (PM) refers to mnemonic processes, which are directed to the
future. It has been subdivided into different stages of processing: the planning, reten-
tion, performance, and evaluation phase. Moreover, PM can be time- or event-based. It
is well known that retrospective memory (RM) can be affected by stress as seen in
patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, data on the effects of
stress on PM are rare. In this review, available behavioral studies of PM are reviewed
with respect to its vulnerability to stress. Based on the available data, we suggest that
stress may have enhancing or disturbing effects on PM, depending on (a) the stressor
characteristics, (b) whether PM is time- or event-based, and (c) which phase of
processing is affected. Studies in healthy adults indicate rather an increase of PM in
response to acute stress (average effect size of d ⫽ .10). In contrast, studies in PTSD
patients found a deteriorating effect of the disorder on PM performance (average effect
size of d ⫽ ⫺.58). We discuss the putative clinical relevance of a better knowledge of
the relationship between stress and PM for the diagnosis and therapy of PTSD.

Keywords: planning, intentions, neuroendocrinology, stress axis, cortisol

Stress and its influence on cognitive perfor- on the modulation of PM are still rare. The
mance and psychological health has become a focus of the review lies on the effects of stress
central issue in the last decades. Acute and on PM of healthy human subjects, but also
chronic stress is known to be associated with includes the available data on PM in clinical
a range of cognitive disabilities and mental as samples. Several psychological disorders
well as somatoform disorders. This has been (e.g., anxiety disorders and affective disor-
shown by numerous studies in particular for ders such as major depression) are known to
explicit retrospective memory (RM) and in be associated with an aberrant psychobiolog-
patients with posttraumatic stress disorder ical stress response (Yehuda, 2002; Yehuda,
(PTSD; for review see Het, Ramlow, & Wolf, Teicher, Trestman, Levengood, & Siever,
2005; Lupien, Maheu, Tu, Fiocco, & Sch- 1996). However, in this review we only refer
ramek, 2007; Wingenfeld & Wolf, 2015; to PTSD since the disorder (a) results from
Wolf, 2009). The present review addresses the influence of acute stress and (b) has ob-
studies that are concerned with the issue of tained the role of a paradigmatic model in
stress influences on prospective memory psychobiological research on the influence of
(PM), that is, future-directed memory. Al- stress on brain functions and behavior. Given
though PM functions play a key role in our the extended knowledge about the influences
everyday life (e.g., we daily have to plan of stress on RM, we also refer in the Discus-
actions, events, or meetings, try to realize
sion to data on the influence of RM, where
intentions, and bring them into action), data
they are needed to understand the modulation
of PM by stress. Since everyday life demands
are strongly based on PM functions, we pro-
Martina Piefke and Katharina Glienke, Department of
pose that a better knowledge about the mod-
Psychology and Psychotherapy, Witten/Herdecke Univer- ulation of PM by stress may be of high rele-
sity. vance for the scientific and practical
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- evaluation of stress influences on cognitive
dressed to Martina Piefke, Department of Psychology and
Psychotherapy, Witten/Herdecke University, Alfred-Her-
functions and psychological health. We as-
rhausen-Straße 50, 58448 Witten, Germany. E-mail: martina sume that PM is comparably vulnerable to
.piefke@uni-wh.de acute stress as RM. In particular, we hypoth-
345
346 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

esize that acute stress may have enhancing or The Concept of PM


disturbing effects on PM in healthy adults and
patients with PTSD depending on (Hypothe- PM refers to mnemonic processes, which are
sis 1) the stressor characteristics, (Hypothesis directed to the future. These include the plan-
2) whether PM is time- or event-based, and ning, the maintenance (i.e., retention), and the
(Hypothesis 3) which phase of PM processing performance (i.e., realization; execution) of de-
is directly affected by the stressor. Before ferred intentions (Brandimonte, Einstein, &
presenting the results of our review, we pro- McDaniel, 1996; Ellis, 1996; McDaniel & Ein-
vide relevant information on the current stein, 2007; Simons, Schölvinck, Gilbert, Frith,
knowledge of the psychobiological stress re- & Burgess, 2006). Accordingly, PM has been
sponse and the neuropsychological concept of subdivided into differential consecutive stages
PM. We then present the results of our sys- of processing. These are (a) the planning phase,
tematic review including effect sizes where (b) the retention interval, and (c) the perfor-
possible. Based on these results, we discuss mance phase (Ellis, 1996; see Figure 1). Brandi-
enhancing and disturbing modulatory effects monte et al. (1996) proposed that the planning
of stress on PM and their putative clinical phase is the first stage of PM, which comprises
relevance for the diagnosis and therapy of the formation and encoding of future-directed
PTSD. intentions and plans. The following retention
interval is conceptualized as a delay between
planning and performance of intentions. During
The Biological Stress Reaction this interval, intended actions are maintained
and postponed to a future point in time. The
The stress reaction can be conceptualized as retention interval is the critical phase during PM
the adaptive response of an organism aiming since intentions and plans need to be protected
at the reinstantiation of homeostasis (Cannon, against interference (Brandimonte et al., 1996;
1929; Goldstein & Kopin, 2007; Goldstein & Brandimonte, Ferrante, Feresin, & Delbello,
McEwen, 2002). This adaptive response is 2001). In cases where the retention of an in-
physiologically based on two functionally dis- tended action is successful, the performance
tinct stress axes. These are the sympathetic- phase of PM may follow. During the perfor-
adrenal-medullary-system (SAM) and the hypo- mance phase an action, which had been planned
thalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis HPA system earlier, is retrieved, initiated, and put into ac-
(e.g., de Kloet, Joëls, & Holsboer, 2005). Al- tion. However, Brandimonte et al. (1996) pro-
though the SAM and the HPA systems work posed a further stage of PM, during which the
parallel after the confrontation with a stressor, evaluation of planning, maintaining, and exe-
the two systems differ from each other with cuting an action as well as the result of the
respect to the progress and speed of processing, action occurs. According to this point of view,
as well as their biological influences on the PM always includes the recapitulation of mne-
organism. The SAM system is referred to as the monic subprocesses and the evaluation of re-
fast stress axis, which allows within a few sec- sults from actions, which had been planned and
onds for the release of epinephrine from chro- executed earlier.
maffin cells. It builds up the basis for the “flight Note, PM does not need to be successful (see
and fight reaction.” The HPA axis has a slower Figure 1). The action executed during the per-
stress response. In response to stress exposure formance phase may be remembered incorrectly
the hypothalamus produces corticotrophin- such that it can be completely or at least par-
releasing factor (CRF), which in turn stimulates tially wrong. Importantly, the executed action
the pituitary gland to release adrenocorticotro- can be wrong with respect to diverse aspects.
pin (ACTH; Holsboer & Ising, 2010). ACTH These are mainly the time point of initiation, the
stimulates the synthesis of the glucocorticoid content and structure of the action, the planned
cortisol from the adrenal gland. Via a negative order of actions, or a combination thereof.
feedback loop into the central nervous system, Besides the differential stages of PM process-
cortisol mainly contributes to the regulation of ing, event- and time-based PM have been dis-
the stress response (Kirschbaum & Hellham- tinguished (Brandimonte et al., 1996; Einstein
mer, 1994). & McDaniel, 1990; Kliegel, Martin, McDaniel,
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 347

Figure 1. The different successive phases of PM and their functional specificity. It is likely
that the effect of stress on PM depends on the time point of stress induction. Stress induction
before the planning, the retention, or the performance phase may differentially enhance or
compromise actual PM performance. In the case that stress is induced only before the
evaluation phase, the effect will mainly be observed in future situations with PM demands.

& Einstein, 2001). According to this distinction, event-based PM. Note, however, that on the
event- and time-based PM are considered to behavioral level in everyday life the distinction
represent two different forms of PM, which are between time- and event-based PM may some-
based on differential cognitive (e.g., Kliegel et times be difficult due to the frequent entangle-
al., 2001) and neurofunctional processes (e.g., ment of the two forms of PM. For example,
Cheng, Wang, Xi, Niu, & Fu, 2008; Okuda et time-based PM may sometimes also be trig-
al., 2007). Event-based PM is supposed to depend gered by external cues (i.e., events) such as the
mainly on external cues. Based on this assump- alarm of a clock.
tion, a specific event acts as the reminder for the For an ongoing PM task paradigm, underly-
performance of an action planned earlier. For ex- ing cognitive strategies for PM have been mod-
ample, the planning and realization of the intake eled in a “multiprocess framework” (McDaniel
of medicine after lunch relies on event-based & Einstein, 2000). The multiprocess framework
PM processes. In contrast, time-based PM is supposes two cognitive strategies of PM. These
assumed to possess only a weak dependence are either strategic monitoring or spontaneous
from external cues. Rather, it is based on a retrieval. Strategic monitoring describes a
priori planned time points and/or time periods. mainly top-down controlled memory process,
The planning and realization of the intake of which holds the intention active in mind and
medicine at 12 a.m. is an example for time- scans the environment for the “PM cue” (e.g.,
based PM processes. The differentiation be- the cue that elicits the execution of an intention;
tween event- and time-based PM is supported i.e., Guynn, 2003). Spontaneous retrieval means
by the findings of neuroimaging studies of that the retrieval of an intention is triggered
Okuda et al. (2007) and Cheng et al. (2008). spontaneous by a PM cue (McDaniel & Ein-
The authors concordantly reported differential stein, 2000). In consequence, spontaneous re-
frontal brain regions to be either associated with trieval is mainly associated with bottom-up pro-
time- or event-based PM. In particular, they cesses (Cona, Scarpazza, Sartori, Moscovitch,
found that the right superior frontal gyrus, the & Bisiacchi, 2015; McDaniel & Einstein, 2000;
anterior medial frontal lobe, and the anterior Scullin, McDaniel, & Shelton, 2013). Scullin et
cingulate gyrus involved time-based PM pro- al. (2013) modified the multiprocess framework
cesses. In contrast, the left superior frontal by introducing a dynamic component. Thus, in
gyrus was associated with the processing of ongoing experimental tasks the underlying PM
348 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

strategies were either strategic monitoring or feedback loop as well as the HPA axis in pa-
spontaneous retrieval but in the modified frame- tients with PTSD (Yehuda, 2002). The DST
work the processes are understood dynamically allows for the investigation of the physiological
depending on the context. stress response by the application of a low dose
Although there is evidence that PM is of a synthetic cortisol (dexamethasone). The
strongly associated with executive functions, data thus suggest that patients with PTSD have
working memory (WM), and RM (e.g., Addis, an intact negative feedback loop and HPA axis,
Wong, & Schacter, 2007; Schacter, Addis, & but that the organism is unable to activate the
Buckner, 2007), it is widely accepted as a dis- functional circuit of the HPA axis autono-
tinct cognitive function (Graf & Uttl, 2001). mously (Yehuda, 2002).
This view is supported for example by clinical Chronic stress may result from sustained ex-
studies in neurological patients, which demon- posure to stressors and/or from a single expo-
strated selective disturbances of PM in the pres- sure to uncontrollable acute traumatic stress.
ence of relatively intact executive and memory While acute everyday stress can normally be
functions (West, McNerney, & Krauss, 2007). managed without negative influences on a per-
son’s health, chronic stress frequently leads to
Stress and Memory Processing in Patients aberrations of the physiological stress response
With PTSD and associated psychological disorders such as
PTSD (Miller, Chen, & Zhou, 2007). It is likely
A classic clinical explanatory model for the that alterations of the slow HPA axis rather than
formation of psychological disorders is the dia- deviant responses of the SAM system are rele-
thesis–stress model (Zubin & Spring, 1977). vant in PTSD since the regulating function of
The model assumes that a psychological disor- the negative feedback loop by cortisol is more
der is the result of a combination of acute or related to the HPA axis than the SAM system
chronic stress that is appraised as a threat, and a (Yehuda et al., 1996). PTSD is well known to
person’s predisposition for the development of be accompanied by cognitive and affective dis-
a psychological disease (Lazarus & Folkman, turbances, in particular in the memory domain.
1984). According to this account, stress plays a It is therefore likely that PM processing is also
critical role in the genesis—and most likely also affected in patients with PTSD.
for the maintenance— of psychological disor-
ders. In this context a differentiation between Method
acute and chronic stress needs to be considered.
Acute stress may be caused by threats of the According to the aims of the review we had
recent past or expected demands in the recent six main inclusion criteria and four main exclu-
future. In patients with Posttraumatic stress dis- sion criteria for relevant studies.
order (PTSD), the acute physiological stress Inclusion criteria:
response is mainly characterized by diminished
(1) For healthy adults only studies that refer to PM were
levels of cortisol (i.e., Elzinga et al., 2008; included into this review. However, since clinical
Mason, 1968; Yehuda et al., 1996) caused by an studies on PM are very rare and studies found a
acute and traumatic stress situation. Normally, significant association between PM and episodic fu-
when being confronted with a stressor, a high ture memory (i.e., Terrett et al., 2015), for studies
with PTSD patients we also included studies that
release of cortisol occurs in the organism. Cor- relied on episodic future memory.
tisol then inhibits the neural structures, which (2) Only studies of adults were included. Studies on chil-
are involved in the regulation of cortisol release dren were excluded from this review since develop-
(in particular the hypothalamus and the pituitary mental aspects of the biological stress reaction in
gland). This functional circuit is referred to as a children (Elmlinger, Kühnel, & Ranke, 2002; Kud-
ielka, Buske-Kirschbaum, Hellhammer, & Kirsch-
negative feedback loop. It has been suggested baum, 2004) and their relevance to the influence of
that this negative feedback loop is interrupted in stress on PM would be a topic of a separate review.
patients with PTSD, and that this is due to the (3) For studies that report a psychoendocrinological
low basal cortisol level associated with the dis- marker, we review only published data concerning the
stress hormone cortisol. Although there is evidence
order (Yehuda, 2002). Interestingly, studies us- that other hormones and neurotransmitters (e.g., en-
ing the dexamethasone suppression test (DST) dorphins and epinephrine; i.e., Cahill & Alkire, 2003)
demonstrated a normal function of the negative may also play crucial roles in the psychoendocrino-
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 349

logical processing of stress, cortisol is known to be the 2016. For studies on the influence of stress on
key player in the biological stress response. Cortisol is PM in healthy adults, bibliographic research
also well known to affect memory functions, due to
the wide distribution of cortisol binding glucocorti- ranged from 2005 to 2016, and from 2013 to
coid receptors (GC-receptors) to neural substrates that 2016 for studies on PM in patients with PTSD.
are associated with memory functions in the brain Literature research took place from October
(i.e., Dedovic, Duchesne, Andrews, Engert, & Pruess- 2015 until December 2016. The review does not
ner, 2009; Lupien et al., 2007; Wingenfeld & Wolf,
2015; Wolf, 2009). Moreover, cortisol is the stress
require an ethical approval.
modulator that has most intensely been investigated to Calculation of Effect Sizes
date. Studies on other neuromodulators involved in
the processing of stress are only referred to in cases
where there are relevant for the understanding of Effect sizes were calculated for overall PM
cortisol functions in psychoneuroendocrinological re- performance and for time- and event-based PM
sponses to acute stress. separately. Our analysis of effect sizes relied on
(4) Only studies of patients with PTSD were included in Hedges’ g (gHedges), which describes the differ-
the review. PTSD has become a paradigmatic model
of chronic alterations of the HPA axis and their influ- ence of means of an experimental group and a
ences on PM performance. Typically, patients with control group standardized by a pooled standard
PTSD show consistently reduced cortisol levels deviation (SD; Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Positive
caused by a dysfunction of the HPA axis (i.e.Yehuda, gHedges indicate better, negative gHedges indicate
2002, 2006; Yehuda et al., 1996). The picture of deteriorating PM in response to stress. As sug-
alterations of the HPA axis in PTSD patients (com-
pared to other psychiatric disorders) is consistent (i.e., gested by Cohen (1988), an effect size of .20
Meewisse, Reitsma, de Vries, Gersons, & Olff, 2007; was classified as small, .50 as moderate, and .80
Peeters, Nicolson, & Berkhof, 2004; Yehuda et al., as large. Where available, calculations of effect
1996). sizes were based on means and SD of means. To
(5) All studies of patients with PTSD included to this
review were controlled clinical trials.
control for overestimation of the effect sizes,
(6) The patients investigated in the studies did not have each gHedges was adjusted and converted to Co-
confounding comorbid disorders other than depres- hen’s d value with respect to Hedge’s formula
sion such as, for example, personality disorders. (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). In two cases effect
Exclusion criteria: sizes were calculated on the basis on correla-
Studies were excluded when: tional coefficients. For these studies, Fischer’s z
was adjusted and converted into Cohens d. Es-
(1) they exclusively used indirect memory measures (e.g., pecially in the clinical studies, ds were available
electroencephalography without behavioral data); for multiple PM measures (e.g., PM specificity).
(2) they administered synthetic glucocorticoids (e.g., dexa- In cases where more than one measure of the
methasone) or included participants who received phar-
macological treatment;
same dependent variable was available, ds were
(3) they included patients with comorbid disorders other first calculated for each single variable and then
than depression (e.g., borderline personality disorder, averaged to one d called PM performance. For
psychotic disorders etc.); time- and event dependent PM we also calcu-
(4) participants of the study met criteria for traumatic brain lated separate ds.
injury.
Statistical Analysis of Effect Sizes
Literature Research
Average effect sizes were calculated for PM
Keywords that were used in the systematic for each study reported. A confidence interval
bibliographic research were prospective mem- of 95% was defined as the level of significance
ory, future memory, planning phase, retention (95% CI). To investigate whether the effect
phase, retrieval phase, execution phase, imple- sizes were consistent across studies all ds were
mentation of intentions, retrospective memory, summarized in a weighted average effect size,
consolidation, retrieval, stress, cortisol, post- with its standard deviation and 95% CI. To test
traumatic stress disorder, PTSD, monitoring, whether the ds share a common effect size, the
spontaneous retrieval and combinations there- Cochran Q test was consulted to prove homo-
of. The databases used for literature research geneity. A significant result indicates heteroge-
were PubMed, PsycINFO, PubPsych, and neous distributions of ds, which points out sig-
Google Scholar. Publication years included in nificant differences between the studies. To
the bibliographic research ranged from 2005 to identify putative mediating factors of the ds
350 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

origin, they were attributed to the factors in- overall PM for significant increased cortisol lev-
cluded in our hypotheses: (a) stressor character- els.
istics, (b) PM type, and (c) PM phase. This Stress influence on event-dependent PM.
differentiation could be accomplished only for As displayed in Table 1, Nakayama and col-
the studies with healthy participants. In the leagues (2005); Nater et al. (2006) and Walser
studies of PTSD patients, too little studies ad- et al. (2013) reported event-dependent PM per-
dressing each factor were available. For the formance to be unaffected by an alteration in
studies of nonclinical samples, we pooled mea- stress level. Within the real-life related para-
sures of overall PM performance after the in- digm of Glienke and Piefke (2016) an improve-
duction of acute experimental stress procedures ment in event-dependent PM was found after an
(Glienke & Piefke, 2016; Nater et al., 2006; acute increase in cortisol after laboratory stress
Walser, Fischer, Goschke, Kirschbaum, & Ples- exposure.
sow, 2013) with PM measures without experi- Stress influence on time-dependent PM.
mentally induced stress (Ihle et al., 2014; Ihle, In behavioral studies, Nater et al. (2006) and
Schnitzspahn, Rendell, Luong, & Kliegel, 2012; Glienke and Piefke (2016) reported better time-
Nakayama, Takahashi, & Radford, 2005). based PM performance after the induction of
Moreover, we pooled measures (a) of time- psychosocial stress. In another study, Ihle et al.
based PM (Glienke & Piefke, 2016; Ihle et al., (2014) found that a relaxation intervention de-
2014; Nater et al., 2006), (b) event based PM creased subjective and physiological stress lev-
(Glienke & Piefke, 2016; Nakayama et al., els but had no implication on time-based PM.
2005; Nater et al., 2006; Walser et al., 2013), Both, histological studies as well as behav-
and (c) across time- and event-based PM types ioral and psychophysiological research in hu-
(Glienke & Piefke, 2016; Ihle et al., 2012). mans indicate that stress most likely alters PM
Because studies that differentiated between processing.
phases of PM are very rare we could not include
this factor into the statistical analyses. To test Analysis of Effect Sizes for the Nonclinical
for a publication bias we used a significance test Samples
according to Begg (1994). A Spearman corre-
lation (rs) was applied to test to assess standard- As shown in Table 1 we found a broad range of
ized effect sizes and their variances. A publica- effects sizes from d ⫽ ⫺1.42 (Ihle et al., 2012) to
tion bias is indicated by a significant positive d ⫽ .61 (Nater et al., 2006) across all studies in
(rs). Publication biases were calculated sepa- healthy adults. The integration of effect sizes re-
rately for nonclinical and clinical samples. vealed an average weighted effect size of d ⫽ .10
(.01 ⱕ d ⱕ.18) for all studies. The homogeneity
Results analysis showed a higher Q (22.11) than the crit-
ical value of ␹2 distribution (␹28 ⫽ 11.42; p ⬎ .05),
Stress Influences on PM indicating heterogeneous distributions of ds. This
indicates that the ds do not share a common un-
Histological studies show evidence for the derlying effect size and may therefore result from
existence of cortisol-binding glucocorticoid re- the investigation of different populations, the use
ceptors in some neural structures, which have of different study designs, stressors, and tasks, or
repeatedly been associated with PM. These are combinations thereof. As depicted in Table 2,
mainly the frontal pole and medial temporal results of laboratory studies applying standardized
lobe (Bremner, 2006; de Quervain et al., 2003; acute experimental stressors show average effect
Kremen et al., 2010). Given these neuroanat- sizes (p ⬍ .05), which are significantly different
omical and neurobiological conditions, it is rea- from zero. In contrast, for studies that measured
sonable to assume that the release of cortisol different forms of basal stress, a significant heter-
will change PM performance. Research of the ogeneity of distributions and a nonsignificant av-
last years found diverging impact of stress on erage effect size was evident. When separating
PM in healthy participants. For instance, in time- and event- dependent PM, we also found
overall PM, Ihle et al. (2012) reported better PM significant average effect sizes and homogeneity
performance under lower stress levels, whereas of distributions for each type of PM. Moreover,
Glienke and Piefke (2016) found enhanced there was a significant heterogeneity and a non-
Table 1
Summary of Descriptive Facets and Average Effect Sizes of PM Performance for the Included Nonclinical Studies
Number of
subjects (age in Stress induction Stress PM phase affected Stress effect
Study years ⫽ Mean) Subjects method measurement Type of PM PM task by stress on PM Hedges g/d
PM
Nakayama 34 么 Healthy — Basal salivary Event-based Ongoing task — No association d event-based ⫽ .21
et al. (M ⫽ 19.00) subjects cortisol PM paradigm between
(2005) cortisol and
event-based
PM
Nater et al. 20 么 Healthy TSST (cross-over Induced salivary Time-based PM Ongoing task — Stress d event-based PM ⫽ .26
(2006) (M ⫽ 24.45) subjects design) cortisol & event- PM paradigm improved d time-based PM ⫽ .61
time-based
PM
Ihle et al. 8 么; 12 乆 Young and old — Subjective rating No difference Everyday Retrieval and Lower stress d PM performance ⫽
(2012) (M ⫽ 22.50) healthy PM execution phase levels ⫺1.42
9 么; 10 乆 subjects task resulted in
(M ⫽ 68.20) better PM
PM
Walser Controls Healthy TSST Induced salivary Event-based Ongoing task Retrieval and Stress had no d event-based ⫽ .01
et al. 20 么; 21乆 subjects cortisol PM paradigm execution phase effect on
(2013) (M ⫽ 21.96) event-based
Stressed PM
20 么; 21乆
(M ⫽ 21.96)
PM
Ihle et al. 3 么; 30 乆 Young and old — Subjective rating Time-based PM Ongoing task — Stress had no d time-based ⫽ .11
(2014) (M ⫽ 20.80) healthy and blood paradigm effect on
12 么; 29 乆 subjects pressure time-based
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY

(M ⫽ 65.20) PM
performance
Glienke & Controls Healthy SECPT Induced salivary Time-based PM Computerized Planning phase Stress d PM ⫽ .34
Piefke 24 么 subjects cortisol & event- real life- improved
(2016) (M ⫽ 23.64) based PM related PM time-based
Stressed task PM &
25 么 event-based d event-based PM ⫽ .17
(M ⫽ 23.95) PM d time-based PM ⫽ .27
Note. Hedges g/d ⫽ (average) effect size of a particular study. TSST ⫽ trier social stress test; PM ⫽ Prospective Memory.
351
352 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

Table 2
Results of Pooled Averaged Effect Sizes
Significance test for average Analysis of effect size
effect size heterogeneity
d 95% CI ␹2 df Q
Stress characteristics
Acute stress (n ⫽ 6) .24ⴱ (.19 ⱕ d ⱕ .28) 5.47 5 2.59
Basal stress (n ⫽ 3) ⫺.21 (⫺.44 ⱕ d ⱕ .02) 5.88 2 14.82ⴱ
PM type
Time-based PM (n ⫽ 3) .28ⴱ (.21 ⱕ d ⱕ .34) 2.6 2 1.57
Event-based PM (n ⫽ 4) .13ⴱ (.10 ⱕ d ⱕ .16) 6.64 3 .55
Overall PM (n ⫽ 2) ⫺.35 (⫺.76 ⱕ d ⱕ .07) .80 1 14.65ⴱ
Note. Integration was done with respect to our hypotheses for healthy adults.ⴱ Indicate that
the average effect size is significantly different to 0 or the ds of one class of data has a
heterogeneous distribution. PM ⫽ Prospective Memory.

significant average effect size for studies that did Studies on the impact of stress on different
not differ between time and event-dependent PM. phases of PM in patients with PTSD have not
This indicates that the differentiation of effect been published yet.
sizes for time- and event-based PM may explain in
part the differential effects of stress on PM re- Analysis of Effect Sizes for the Clinical
ported in published studies. Data from studies that Samples
did not distinguish between time- and event-
dependent only indicate that stress may have dif- As shown in Table 3 we found a broad range
ferential effects on overall PM. The test of publi- of effects sizes for the effect of PTSD on PM
cation biases showed a significant negative from d ⫽ ⫺1.99 (Brown et al., 2013) to d ⫽ .14
correlation of the standard effect sizes and their (Brown et al., 2014). The integration of effect
variance (r ⫽ ⫺.25; p ⬍ .05), indicating the sizes revealed an average weighted effect size
absence of any publication bias. of d ⫽ ⫺.58 (⫺.67 ⱕ d ⱕ ⫺.49) for all studies.
The homogeneity analysis showed a lower Q
Cognitive Disturbances (14.86) than the critical value of the ␹2 distribution
Associated With PTSD (␹28 ⫽ 17,49; p ⬎ .05), indicating a homogeneous
distribution of ds. The ds thus share a common
Numerous researchers investigated RM in underlying effect size and therefore seem to
patients with PTSD (for a review, see Buck- come from similar populations and experimen-
ley, Blanchard, & Neill, 2000). Studies of tal conditions. The analysis of effect sizes of
retrospective real life memory consistently results from studies of patients with PTSD thus
reported that PTSD is associated with impaired does not indicate statistical differences in the
autobiographical memory (e.g., Beblo et al., 2006; effect of PTSD on PM between the studies.
LaGarde, Doyon, & Brunet, 2010; McNally,
2006; Piefke et al., 2008). These findings are of Discussion
particular interest since autobiographical mem-
ory deeply integrates retrospective and PM pro- Based on the available literature, this review
cesses (Schacter et al., 2007). As displayed in analyzed the effects of stress on PM in healthy
Table 3, a similar picture of alterations in pa- adults and patients with PTSD depending on (a)
tients with PTSD is detectable for PM perfor- the stressor characteristics, (b) whether PM is
mance. All authors reported difficulties in PM time- or event-based, and (c) which phase of
performance for PTSD patients (Brown et al., PM processing is directly affected by the stres-
2013, 2014; Kleim, Graham, Fihosy, Stott, & sor. Results indicate that the effects of stress on
Ehlers, 2014; McFarland et al., 2016; Scott et PM are modulated by stressor characteristics
al., 2016). and different types (i.e., time- and event-
Table 3
Summary of Descriptive Facets and Average Effect Sizes of PM Performance for the Included Clinical Studies
Number of PM phase
subjects (age in affected by
Study years ⫽ Mean) Subjects Type of PM PM task stress Stress effect on PM nd Hedges g/d
PM performance
Kleim et al. PTSD PTSD vs. non- Imaged future Future-based — PTSD patients 2 d ⫽ ⫺.47
(2014) 10 么; 20 乆 PTSD events autobiographical imagined future
(M ⫽ 42.30) subjects memory events fewer
Non-PTSD test specific.
10 么; 10 乆
(M ⫽ 37.00)
Brown et al. PTSD Combat veterans Imaged future Autobiographical — PTSD patients 2 d PM performance ⫽
(2013) 12 么 with PTSD events memory overgenerate future ⫺1,99
(M ⫽ 30.25) vs. non-PTSD test events.
Non-PTSD combat
16 么 veterans
(M ⫽ 31.38)
performance
Brown et al. PTSD Combat veterans Imaged future Autobiographical — PTSD generated more 2 d PM ⫽ .14
(2014) 12 么 with PTSD events interview external, than
Non-PTSD vs. non-PTSD (AI) episodic internal
16 么 combat details of imagined
(M ⫽ 31.38; veterans future.
M ⫽ 30.25) Greater symptom
severity leads to
fewer episodic
details.
performance
Scott et al. PTSD Combat veterans Time-based PM Memory for — PTSD patients show 0 d PM ⫽ ⫺.58
(2016) 34 么; 6 乆 with PTSD and event- intention poorer PM,
vs. non-PTSD based PM task especially for time-
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY

(M ⫽ 35.2)
Non-PTSD combat (MIST) based PM.
35么; 3 乆 veterans d event-based PM ⫽ ⫺.39
(M ⫽ 32.9) d time-based PM ⫽ ⫺.56
McFarland et al. PTSD Combat veterans Event-based Ongoing — Elevated PTSD 3 d PM performance ⫽ ⫺.96
(2016) 35 么, 5 乆 with PTSD PM task symptoms are
(M ⫽ 39.6) paradigm associated with
more difficulties in
event-based PM.
Note. Nd ⫽ total number of ds; Hedges g/d ⫽ (average) effect size of a particular study. PTSD ⫽ posttraumatic stress disorder; PM ⫽ Prospective Memory.
353
354 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

dependent) of PM in healthy adults. The calcu- were required to mark words that could be clas-
lation of effect size demonstrates that the ex- sified as clothing (neutral) or words that are
perimental induction of short acute stress may classified as names of symptoms of physical
improve PM. For studies that investigated basal disorders (negative emotional). Based on the
stress neither an improving nor a deteriorating study designs they are also different in their
association between stress levels and PM per- capabilities to standardize experimental condi-
formance could be detected. Effect sizes for the tions. Therefore, it is reasonable that the com-
influence of stress on distinct phases of PM bination of different stressor characteristics and
could not be calculated because too few studies task demands may explain the different results
are published on this issue. Effects sizes for for the influence of basal stress on PM. In
studies of patients with PTSD demonstrate a another study, Ihle et al. (2014) investigated
deteriorating effect of the disorder on overall whether aging-related differences in time-based
PM. Based on the results of our review we will PM performance may be due to higher basal
now discuss our hypotheses on the modulating levels of stress in the elderly (relative to young
factors of PM in healthy adults and patients with adults) during a laboratory testing situation. Ihle
PTSD. et al. (2014) also measured basal stress niveau
but even lowered the basal stress levels by con-
Healthy Adults ducting a relaxation intervention. In their exper-
imental paradigm, participants performed a
Effects of stressor characteristics. We hy- working memory task while pressing a com-
pothesized that the reported diverging effects of puter button every minute to test time-depen-
stress on PM performance may be a result of dent PM. Half of the older and half of the
different stress characteristics. Ihle et al. (2012) younger participants accomplished a relaxation
and Nakayama, Takahashi, and Radford (2005) intervention before the time-based PM task. Ihle
both investigated the influence of basal stress et al. (2014) found that the relaxation interven-
levels on PM (i.e., stress levels in the absence of tion decreased subjective and physiological
stress treatment). Nakayama et al. (2005) found stress levels in both young and older partici-
no effect of different basal stress levels whereas pants. However, stress levels did not differ be-
Ihle and collegueas (2012) reported better PM tween the age groups such that the amount of
performance under lower stress levels. How- stress could not explain aging-related differ-
ever, only Nakayama et al. (2005) relied their ences in PM performance in this laboratory
results on the measured stress hormone cortisol. context. This study underpins the results of Na-
Ihle et al. (2012) differentiated their results on kayama et al. (2005) because both tested PM
either subjective stress ratings. Supposedly, sub- under controlled conditions and found no effect
jective stress measures and cortisol measurement of basal stress on PM, although, again different
describe different stress reactions. Subjective forms of stress were measured.
stress ratings usually contain the SAM-system. Glienke and Piefke (2016); Nater et al.
Bodily changes during the fast stress reaction are (2006) and Walser et al. (2013) induced system-
much better detectable for subjects and are re- atically and standardized acute stress by social
flected by physiological measure like blood pres- stress tests. While Glienke and Piefke (2016)
sure, than the HPA-axis that results in the release applied the socially evaluated cold pressure test
in cortisol. Therefore, different influence of (SECPT), Nater et al. (2006) and Walser et al.
stress may be a result of different stress reac- (2013) conducted the trier social stress test
tions that were measured. Certainly Ihle et al. (TSST). Both stress exposure methods resulted
(2012) applied an everyday PM task, which in a reliable cortisol increase (Kirschbaum,
aimed at assessing retrieval and execution of Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993; Schwabe, Haddad,
intentions over a time period of 5 days in a & Schachinger, 2008). Nater et al. (2006) re-
naturalistic study design. Participants formed ported selectively improved time-based PM
and encoded their intentions during their every- performance after the induction of psychosocial
day routine. Encoded intentions had to be re- stress, with no such effect on event-based PM
trieved and executed on the following day. Na- (see Figure 2). Nater and colleagues (2006)
kayama et al. (2005), in contrast, tested PM by implied an ongoing word-rating task, wherein
an event-dependent PM paradigm. Participants the participant was asked to either press a target
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 355

Figure 2. Differential effects of stress on time-based PM. In a study by Nater et al. (2006)
it is shown that time-based PM was improved by the stressor, while there was no effect of
stress on event-based PM. Nater et al. (2006) did not differentiate between the distinct phases
of PM illustrated in Figure 1. It is most likely that the effects of stress on PM may also depend
on the time point of stress induction (i.e., before the planning, the retention, the performance,
or the evaluation phase).

key every 2 min (time-dependent PM) or when- real life-like PM task that involved the plan-
ever a target word appeared on the screen ning, retention, and performance of intentions
(event-dependent PM). Walser et al. (2013) in- during a fictional holiday week. Half of the
tended to explore the missing effect of stress on subjects were stressed with the SECPT before
event-based PM in the paradigm of Nater et al. the planning of intentions, and the other half of
(2006). In their study, the authors specified and the participants underwent a control procedure
expanded the event-depended PM condition by at the same time. Glienke and Piefke (2016)
including (a) response times into their analyses found that the performance of participants
and (b) implementing an additional deactivation treated with the SECPT procedure before the
phase into the paradigm. The latter experimen- planning phase remained constantly better time-
tal variation allowed them to test potential ef- and event-dependent PM retrieval over the per-
fects of stress on intention-deactivation after the formance phase, while performance of controls
completion of each PM trial. This approach is declined.
based on the view that after having completed a With respect to the hypothesis that the stres-
PM challenge intentions, which are no longer sor characteristics may explain at least in part,
relevant, are deactivated to avoid interference we also have to take into account some impor-
with the demands of following PM tasks. Fol- tant differences between the two most fre-
lowing this account, Walser and colleagues quently applied experimental stressors in neu-
(2013) aimed at exploring whether stress may roendocrinological and neuropsychological
lead to changes of intention-deactivation by research: the SECPT and the TSST. Both stres-
measuring if participants responded to PM cues sors have repeatedly been shown to induce ro-
that were required in the PM block before. bust effects on the physiological marker cortisol
Neither in event-dependent PM performance, (Giles, Mahoney, Brunyé, Taylor, & Kanarek,
nor in the response times, Walser et al. (2013) 2014; Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Schwabe et al.,
found differences between the stress and the 2008), on mood and emotion processing (e.g.,
control groups. Likewise, intention-deactivation Giles et al., 2014), and on cognitive functions
after completion of each trial did not differ (e.g., Het et al., 2005; Schwabe, Joëls,
between the two groups. Given these results, the Roozendaal, Wolf, & Oitzl, 2012; Schwabe,
authors proposed that processes like intention Wolf, & Oitzl, 2010). Conceptually, the TSST
retrieval and intention-deactivation may pre- represents a social stressor, whereas the SECPT
serve even under acute stress conditions. Re- includes both a social stressor component and—
cently Glienke and Piefke (2016) separated PM with the ice cold water condition—a physiolog-
in its different phases and applied a complex ical component. Additionally, the TSST proce-
356 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

dure lasts 15 min whereas the SECPT is much besides a common frontal activation, different
shorter with its 3-min stress procedure. It has even-related potentials (ERPs) for event- and
repeatedly been shown that the TSST increases time-dependent PM. Especially in event-
cortisol levels immediately after stress induc- based PM the authors found ERPs that re-
tion, while the SECPT increases cortisol within flected increased recruitment of resources re-
20 min post stress treatment (Giles et al., 2014; quired for checking the occurrence PM cues.
Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Schwabe et al., 2008). This is in line with the assumption that event-
The differences between the SECPT and the dependent PM is a self-initiated execution of
TSST in the magnitude of induced cortisol lev- intentions in response to an external PM cue,
els were only marginal (Giles et al., 2014). whereas time-dependent PM reflects a more
Note, however, that the SECPT induced across internal self-initiated execution to a specific
studies slightly higher levels of cortisol than the time and is therefore of higher cognitive de-
TSST, although the SECPT stress procedure mand (Nater et al., 2006). Strategic monitor-
lasts only one-fifth of the duration of the TSST ing may be recruited in cases where PM cues
stress procedure. Available data on the effects possess a low salience and demand less cog-
of the SECPT and TSST stress induction meth- nitive resources (Cona et al., 2015; McDaniel
ods thus suggest that a combined social and & Einstein, 2000; Scullin et al., 2013). In
physiological stressor (represented by the consequence, it is reasonable that strategic
SECPT) is slightly more efficient in the induc- monitoring is more likely the underlying
tion of cortisol release than a mere social stres- strategy of event-dependent PM; however, the
sor (represented by the TSST). Since different more demanding a PM task is, the fewer
experimental paradigms were applied in the resources are available for strategic monitor-
studies on PM, which used either the SECPT or ing. Spontaneous retrieval has been explained
the TSST, one cannot decide whether the re- by a reflexive association between the PM cue
ported differences in cortisol release depended and the intention stored in PM. However,
on differential stressor characteristics, distinct spontaneous retrieval may also be a result of
PM paradigms, or both (Glienke & Piefke, the salience of the PM cue (Cona et al., 2015;
2016; Nater et al., 2006; Walser et al., 2013). McDaniel & Einstein, 2000; Scullin et al.,
Moreover, issues related to stressor characteris- 2013). PM tasks with high demands like time-
tics interfere with different time points of stress dependent PM and salient PM cues would
induction during PM processing in the reviewed thus be most likely to provoke spontaneous
studies (Glienke & Piefke, 2016; Nater et al., retrieval (Cona et al., 2015; McDaniel & Ein-
2006; Walser et al., 2013). Based on the cur- stein, 2000; Scullin et al., 2013). Time-
rently available data, we propose that stressor dependent PM may thus be likely vulnerable
characteristics may at least in part explain the for stress. The analysis of effect sizes revealed
varying results of studies on the effects of stress significantly increased time- and event-dependent
on PM performance. Importantly, the calcula- PM in response to stress; however, there was a
tion of effect sizes demonstrates that studies (nonsignificant) tendency toward a stronger im-
applying a standardized acute experimental provement of time-dependent than event-depen-
stressor demonstrate comparably improving ef- dent PM. Effect sizes of studies that did not
fects on PM (with effect sizes significantly distinguish between time- and event-dependent
greater than zero). In contrast, effect sizes for PM were heterogeneous (i.e., they did not differ
studies investigating the influence of basal from zero). In summary, the calculation of ef-
stress levels on PM do not indicate a similarity fect sizes corroborated our hypothesis that time-
between the measured effects. It is likely that in and event-dependent PM are differentially af-
these studies the stressor characteristics are not fected by stress in terms of a distinct degree of
sufficiently defined, such that they may not ex- improvement.
plain the varying reported effects of stress on The differential experimental paradigms used
PM performance. to measure time- and event-dependent PM,
Effects of PM type. We also hypothesized however, also need to be considered in the
that time- and event-dependent PM may be debate on putative differences between time-
differentially affected by stress. Cona, Ar- and event-dependent PM in the vulnerability to
cara, Tarantino, and Bisiacchi (2012) found, stress. For example, Nakayama et al. (2005) and
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 357

Ihle et al. (2014) applied experimental ongoing during which phase of PM it may deteriorate
task paradigms, whereas Ihle and colleagues performance.
(2012) implemented a rather naturalistic PM
paradigm. Nater et al. (2006) and Walser and Patients With PTSD
colleagues (2013) applied an ongoing PM task,
while Glienke and Piefke (2016) used a com- Although PTSD represents a paradigmatic
plex computerized real-life-related PM task. model in psychobiological research on the in-
Ihle and colleagues (2012) did not differentiate fluence of stress on brain functions and behav-
between time-based or event-based PM. Na- ior, results of studies on PM in PTSD patients
kayama et al. (2005) and Walser et al. (2013) cannot easily be analyzed and interpreted with
investigated event-dependent PM, and Ihle et al. reference to studies of the modulation of PM by
(2014) solely addressed time-depenendent PM. stress in healthy participants. This is mainly due
Nater et al. (2006) as well as Glienke and Piefke to the lack of psychobiological measures such
(2016) differentiated between time- and as cortisol concentration and heart rate and the
event-dependent PM and investigated the in- application of different PM paradigms in stud-
fluence of stress on both types of PM. In ies of healthy participants and PTSD patients.
paradigms where ongoing tasks are used, Nonetheless, the calculation of effect sizes for
time-dependent PM is supposedly more cog- overall PM performance indicates that PTSD
nitive demanding than event-dependent PM, has a deteriorating effect on PM (average effect
and may therefore be more vulnerable to size of d ⫽ ⫺.58).
stress. Paradigms possessing a higher com- Effects of stressor characteristics. With
plexity may increase the cognitive demands regard to our hypothesis that stressor character-
of event-dependent PM. Task complexity may istics may explain enhancing or disturbing ef-
therefore explain at least in part why studies fects on PM, it needs to be considered that
applying naturalistic paradigms found that stressor characteristics in PTSD patients need to
event-dependent PM was also affected by be defined by the traumatic event that caused
stress. Following these assumptions, both PM the disorder. The results of our review demon-
types and study designs may explain the di- strate that available studies focusing on PM in
verging patterns of results on the influence of patients with PTSD recruited combat veterans
stress on PM in healthy adults. for their investigations— except the study of
Effects of PM phase. As displayed in Ta- Kleim et al. (2014; see Table 3). Characteristics
ble 1 only three studies are published that of the stress situation causing the disorder were
differentiated between the phases of PM probably widely comparable in the studies of
(Glienke & Piefke, 2016; Ihle et al., 2012; combat veterans, such that we can assume that
Walser et al., 2013). Given the differences in the characteristics of combat-related traumatic
stressor characteristics and experimental de- stress may at least in part explain disturbing
signs, results of the three studies cannot be effects of PTSD on PM performance.
compared with each other concerning the im- None of the studies addressed the role of the
pact of stress on different phases of PM. For stress hormone cortisol. In prior research, it is
the same reasons, effect sizes could not be discussed that low-dose cortisol treatment re-
calculated for the influence of stress on dif- duces cortisol concentrations in PTSD patients
ferent PM phases. However, following the and improves symptoms (Aerni et al., 2004).
assumption of the “multiprocess framework” Based on this assumption, a short and moderate
(Cona et al., 2015; McDaniel & Einstein, stress exposure that actives the HPA-axis
2000; Scullin et al., 2013; see also Introduc- should have a similar effect in patients with
tion) for the retrieval and execution phases of PTSD. The putative relevance of this hypothesis
PM, it is likely that the effects of stress on the for PM capacities in PTSD patients needs to be
retrieval phase is different from the influence further investigated.
of stress exerted on the planning and retention Effects of PM type. Only two studies on
phases of PM. Since PM has a high relevance PM in combat veterans with PTSD are pub-
in everyday life, future research is needed to lished, which differentiated between time- and
further explore during which phase of PM event-dependent PM (Scott et al., 2016; McFar-
acute stress may have enhancing effects, and land et al., 2016). Scott and colleagues (2016)
358 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

consulted the Memory for Intention Test cifically in response to positive, but not to neg-
(MIST; Kamat et al., 2014; Raskin, 2009; Scott ative cues relative to the control group. Al-
et al., 2016) and found significantly poorer PM though the study design and the PM task used
performance in PTSD patients for time- by Kleim et al. (2014) are not directly compa-
dependent PM (but not for event-dependent rable to those applied in the PM studies in
PM) compared to combat veterans without combat veterans, the results fit in the overall
PTSD. The MIST consists of eight PM trials picture of data on the effect of war-related
over 30 min that are counterbalanced by PM PTSD on PM.
type (i.e., time-based vs. event-based), response In summary, different study paradigms make
(i.e., physical vs. verbal), and delay (i.e., 2 min it difficult to interpret the effect of PTSD on
vs. 15 min; Kamat et al., 2014; Raskin, 2009; time- and event-dependent PM performance.
Scott et al., 2016). McFarland et al. (2016) However, the pattern of results from studies of
applied an ongoing PM task similar to the one PTSD patients and the respective effect sizes
used by Nater et al. (2006), but only for event- clearly indicate that PTSD deteriorates overall
dependent PM. They found a negative correla- PM performance.
tion between the severity of PTSD symptoms Studies on the impact of stress on different
and event-based PM performance. Other au- phases of PM (see Hypothesis 3) in patients
thors used imaginative task procedures (Brown with PTSD have not been published yet. We
et al., 2013, 2014; Kleim et al., 2014) that did know from RM in healthy adults and PTSD
not distinguish between time- and event- patients that stress may have different effects on
dependent PM. Brown et al. (2013) presented memory phases (e.g., Het et al., 2005; Wingen-
neutral word cues to combat veterans who were feld et al., 2012; Wingenfeld & Wolf, 2015;
instructed to generate autobiographical memo- Wolf, 2009). Future research is needed to clar-
ries or imagine future autobiographical events. ify whether similar effects can be found for PM.
Data were analyzed for episodic specificity and It may be of particular interest for the treatment
content. Events of the future were reported of the disorder to examine putative differential
more overgeneral by combat veterans with positive or negative effects of distinct types of
PTSD compared to combat veterans without short acute stressors on each phase of PM in
PTSD. Brown et al. (2014) employed the auto- patients with PTSD (Wingenfeld et al., 2012).
biographical interview (Levine, Svoboda, Hay, This would probably enable us to develop more
Winocur, & Moscovitch, 2002) to investigate precise and efficient rehabilitation interven-
autobiographical memory and imagined future tions.
event narratives generated by combat veterans
with and without PTSD. Responses were coded Conclusions
for the number of episodic and semantic details.
Compared to combat veterans without PTSD, The present review demonstrates that a short
those with PTSD generated more semantic than acute stressor may have enhancing effects on
episodic details when recalling past or imagin- PM in healthy humans, depending on the char-
ing future events. Interestingly, fewer episodic acteristics of the stressor and the type of PM.
details were associated with greater symptom With regard to the stressor attributes, available
severity. Kleim et al. (2014) were the only au- data are confounded with the use of differential
thors who did not investigate combat veterans in PM paradigms and the induction of stress at
their study. In the PTSD patients included in different time points during PM processing. In
their investigation, the disorder resulted from summary, however, the results of our review
assault and motor vehicle accidents. The stres- indicate that variations of stressor characteris-
sor characteristics therefore differed from those tics may in part be relevant for the effects of
of studies of combat veterans with war-related stress on PM (Giles et al., 2014). Moreover,
PTSD. The PM task used by Kleim et al. (2014) time-dependent PM appears to be more vulner-
addressed the effects of cues with differential able to stress. This may be due to the higher
emotional valence on the accuracy of autobio- cognitive demands (e.g., memory load, executive
graphical PM in PTSD patients and healthy capacity) in time-dependent relative to event-
controls. They reported that PTSD patients dependent PM. Interestingly, there is some evi-
imagined future events less accurately and spe- dence across studies that different basal cortisol
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 359

levels do not modulate PM performance. In Beblo, T., Driessen, M., Mertens, M., Wingenfeld,
contrast, acute short stress induction and the K., Piefke, M., Rullkoetter, N., . . . Woermann,
corresponding differential increases of cortisol F. G. (2006). Functional MRI correlates of the
may improve PM performance (Glienke & recall of unresolved life events in borderline per-
Piefke, 2016). Concerning a putative relation- sonality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 36,
845– 856. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S003329170
ship between stress induction and the different
6007227
phases of PM, only three studies are published Begg, K. D. (1994). Publication bias. In H. Cooper &
that differentiated between the distinct phases of L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research
PM. Moreover, stressor characteristics and ex- synthesis (pp. 399 – 409). New York, NY: Russell
perimental designs highly differed between Sage Foundation.
these studies such that one hardly may compare Brandimonte, M. A., Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel,
and interpret the study results with respect to the M. A. (1996). Prospective memory: Theory and
impact of stress on PM phases. Note, in every- applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
day life stress may occur in all phases of PM. It Brandimonte, M. A., Ferrante, D., Feresin, C., &
would thus be interesting to explore in future Delbello, R. (2001). Dissociating prospective
research during which phases of PM acute stress memory from vigilance processes. Psicológica,
may enhance or deteriorate PM. The knowledge 22, 97–113.
about beneficial effects of stress on certain Bremner, J. D. (2006). Traumatic stress: Effects on
phases of PM may also be relevant for clinical the brain. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 8,
445– 461.
samples. On the clinical side, data on influences
Brown, A. D., Addis, D. R., Romano, T. A., Marmar,
of acute stress on PM in patients with PTSD are C. R., Bryant, R. A., Hirst, W., & Schacter, D. L.
rare. These mainly relate PTSD symptoms to (2014). Episodic and semantic components of au-
declined PM functions. The influence of a short tobiographical memories and imagined future
acute stressor on the differential phases of PM events in post-traumatic stress disorder. Memory,
has not been investigated yet. Interestingly, 22, 595– 604. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211
some recent studies suggest that the level of .2013.807842
cortisol may be an important biomarker for di- Brown, A. D., Root, J. C., Romano, T. A., Chang,
agnostic and therapeutic considerations in the L. J., Bryant, R. A., & Hirst, W. (2013). Overgen-
treatment of patients with PTSD. Therefore, eralized autobiographical memory and future
future research on the psychological and neu- thinking in combat veterans with posttraumatic
roendocrinological interactions underlying dis- stress disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
turbances of PM in patients with PTSD pos- Experimental Psychiatry, 44, 129 –134. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.11.004
sesses a central clinical and scientific relevance.
Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Neill, W. T.
It would be of particular interest to investigate (2000). Information processing and PTSD: A re-
how cortisol levels induced by a short acute view of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychol-
stress exposure at different time points (i.e., ogy Review, 20, 1041–1065. http://dx.doi.org/10
before the planning, retention, or retrieval/ .1016/S0272-7358(99)00030-6
execution phases) influences PM performance Cahill, L., & Alkire, M. T. (2003). Epinephrine en-
in patients with PTSD. hancement of human memory consolidation: Inter-
action with arousal at encoding. Neurobiology of
Learning and Memory, 79, 194 –198. http://dx.doi
References .org/10.1016/S1074-7427(02)00036-9
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Cannon, W. B. (1929). Bodily changes in pain, hun-
Remembering the past and imagining the future: ger, fear and rage (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Common and distinct neural substrates during Appleton.
event construction and elaboration. Neuropsycho- Cheng, H.-D., Wang, K., Xi, C. H., Niu, C. S., & Fu,
logia, 45, 1363–1377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j X.-M. (2008). Prefrontal cortex involvement in the
.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016 event-based prospective memory: Evidence from
Aerni, A., Traber, R., Hock, C., Roozendaal, B., patients with lesions in the prefrontal cortex. Brain
Schelling, G., Papassotiropoulos, A., . . . de Quer- Injury, 22, 697–704. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
vain, D. J. (2004). Low-dose cortisol for symptoms 02699050802263035
of posttraumatic stress disorder. The American Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the
Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1488 –1490. http://dx behavioral sciences. New York, NY: Lawrence
.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.161.8.1488 Erlbaum.
360 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

Cona, G., Arcara, G., Tarantino, V., & Bisiacchi, mood, HPA axis, and autonomic response: Com-
P. S. (2012). Electrophysiological correlates of parison of three psychosocial stress paradigms.
strategic monitoring in event-based and time-based PLoS ONE, 9(12), e113618. http://dx.doi.org/10
prospective memory. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e31659. .1371/journal.pone.0113618
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0031659 Glienke, K., & Piefke, M. (2016). Acute social stress
Cona, G., Scarpazza, C., Sartori, G., Moscovitch, M., before the planning phase improves memory per-
& Bisiacchi, P. S. (2015). Neural bases of prospec- formance in a complex real life-related prospective
tive memory: A meta-analysis and the “Attention memory task. Neurobiology of Learning and Mem-
to Delayed Intention” (AtoDI) model. Neurosci- ory, 133, 171–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm
ence and Biobehavioral Reviews, 52, 21–37. http:// .2016.06.025
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.007 Goldstein, D. S., & Kopin, I. J. (2007). Evolution of
Dedovic, K., Duchesne, A., Andrews, J., Engert, V., concepts of stress. Stress, 10, 109 –120. http://dx
& Pruessner, J. C. (2009). The brain and the stress .doi.org/10.1080/10253890701288935
axis: The neural correlates of cortisol regulation in Goldstein, D. S., & McEwen, B. (2002). Allostasis,
response to stress. NeuroImage, 47, 864 – 871. homeostats, and the nature of stress. Stress, 5,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05 55–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10253890290
.074 012345
de Kloet, E. R., Joëls, M., & Holsboer, F. (2005). Graf, P., & Uttl, B. (2001). Prospective memory: A
Stress and the brain: From adaptation to disease. new focus for research. Consciousness and Cog-
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 463– 475. http:// nition, 10, 437– 450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/
dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1683 ccog.2001.0504
de Quervain, D. J.-F., Henke, K., Aerni, A., Treyer, Guynn, M. J. (2003). A two-process model of stra-
V., McGaugh, J. L., Berthold, T., . . . Hock, C. tegic monitoring in event-based prospective mem-
(2003). Glucocorticoid-induced impairment of de- ory: Activation/retrieval mode and checking. In-
clarative memory retrieval is associated with re- ternational Journal of Psychology, 38, 245–256.
duced blood flow in the medial temporal lobe. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000178
European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1296 – Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Methods
1302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003 for Meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
.02542.x Het, S., Ramlow, G., & Wolf, O. T. (2005). A meta-
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal analytic review of the effects of acute cortisol
aging and prospective memory. Journal of Exper- administration on human memory. Psychoneu-
imental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cog- roendocrinology, 30, 771–784. http://dx.doi.org/
nition, 16, 717–726. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.005
0278-7393.16.4.717 Holsboer, F., & Ising, M. (2010). Stress hormone
Ellis, J. A. (1996). Prospective memory or the real- regulation: Biological role and translation into
ization of delayed intentions: A conceptual frame- therapy. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 81–
work for research. In M. Brandimonte, G. Einstein, 109, C1–C11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev
& M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: .psych.093008.100321
Theory and applications (pp. 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: Ihle, A., Kliegel, M., Hering, A., Ballhausen, N.,
Lawrence Erlbaum. Lagner, P., Benusch, J., . . . Schnitzspahn, K. M.
Elmlinger, M. W., Kühnel, W., & Ranke, M. B. (2014). Adult age differences in prospective mem-
(2002). Reference ranges for serum concentrations ory in the laboratory: Are they related to higher
of lutropin (LH), follitropin (FSH), estradiol (E2), stress levels in the elderly? Frontiers in Human
prolactin, progesterone, sex hormone-binding Neuroscience, 8, 1021. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/
globulin (SHBG), dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate fnhum.2014.01021
(DHEAS), cortisol and ferritin in neonates, chil- Ihle, A., Schnitzspahn, K., Rendell, P. G., Luong, C.,
dren and young adults. Clinical Chemistry and & Kliegel, M. (2012). Age benefits in everyday
Laboratory Medicine, 40, 1151–1160. http://dx.doi prospective memory: The influence of personal
.org/10.1515/cclm.2002.202 task importance, use of reminders and everyday
Elzinga, B. M., Roelofs, K., Tollenaar, M. S., Bakvis, stress. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition,
P., van Pelt, J., & Spinhoven, P. (2008). Dimin- 19, 84 –101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825585
ished cortisol responses to psychosocial stress as- .2011.629288
sociated with lifetime adverse events: A study Kamat, R., Weinborn, M., Kellogg, E. J., Bucks,
among healthy young subjects. Psychoneuroendo- R. S., Velnoweth, A., & Woods, S. P. (2014).
crinology, 33, 227–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Construct validity of the Memory for Intentions
j.psyneuen.2007.11.004 Screening Test (MIST) in healthy older adults.
Giles, G. E., Mahoney, C. R., Brunyé, T. T., Taylor, Assessment, 21, 742–753. http://dx.doi.org/10
H. A., & Kanarek, R. B. (2014). Stress effects on .1177/1073191114530774
EFFECTS OF STRESS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY 361

Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1994). Sali- McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic
vary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: and automatic processes in prospective memory
Recent developments and applications. Psycho- retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cog-
neuroendocrinology, 19, 313–333. http://dx.doi nitive Psychology, 14, S127–S144. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/0306-4530(94)90013-2 .org/10.1002/acp.775
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospec-
(1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’—A tool for tive memory: An overview and synthesis of an
investigating psychobiological stress responses in emerging field. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76 – McFarland, C. P., Clark, J. B., Lee, L. O., Grande,
81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000119004 L. J., Marx, B. P., & Vasterling, J. J. (2016).
Kleim, B., Graham, B., Fihosy, S., Stott, R., & Event-based prospective memory among veterans:
Ehlers, A. (2014). Reduced specificity in episodic The role of posttraumatic stress disorder symptom
future thinking in posttraumatic stress disorder. severity in executing intentions. Journal of Clini-
Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 165–173. http:// cal and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38, 251–
dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702613495199 260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015
Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Ein- .1102203
stein, G. O. (2001). Varying the importance of a McNally, R. J. (2006). Cognitive abnormalities in
prospective memory task: Differential effects posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends in Cognitive
across time- and event-based prospective memory. Sciences, 10, 271–277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
Memory, 9, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ .tics.2006.04.007
09658210042000003 Meewisse, M.-L., Reitsma, J. B., de Vries, G.-J.,
Kremen, W. S., O’Brien, R. C., Panizzon, M. S., Gersons, B. P. R., & Olff, M. (2007). Cortisol and
Prom-Wormley, E., Eaves, L. J., Eisen, S. A., . . . posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: Systematic
Franz, C. E. (2010). Salivary cortisol and prefron- review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of
Psychiatry, 191, 387–392. http://dx.doi.org/10
tal cortical thickness in middle-aged men: A twin
.1192/bjp.bp.106.024877
study. NeuroImage, 53, 1093–1102. http://dx.doi
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it
.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.026
goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and
Kudielka, B. M., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Hellham-
the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in
mer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2004). HPA axis
humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 25– 45.
responses to laboratory psychosocial stress in
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25
healthy elderly adults, younger adults, and chil- Nakayama, Y., Takahashi, T., & Radford, M. H.
dren: Impact of age and gender. Psychoneuroen- (2005). Cortisol levels and prospective and retro-
docrinology, 29, 83–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ spective memory in humans. Neuroendocrinology
S0306-4530(02)00146-4 Letters, 26, 599 – 602.
LaGarde, G., Doyon, J., & Brunet, A. (2010). Mem- Nater, U. M., Okere, U., Stallkamp, R., Moor, C.,
ory and executive dysfunctions associated with Ehlert, U., & Kliegel, M. (2006). Psychosocial
acute posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Re- stress enhances time-based prospective memory in
search, 177, 144 –149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j healthy young men. Neurobiology of Learning and
.psychres.2009.02.002 Memory, 86, 344 –348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, ap- .nlm.2006.04.006
praisal, and coping. New York, NY: Springer. Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Yama-
Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & dori, A., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007).
Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiograph- Differential involvement of regions of rostral pre-
ical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic frontal cortex (Brodmann area 10) in time- and
retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17, 677– 689. event-based prospective memory. International
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677 Journal of Psychophysiology, 64, 233–246. http://
Lupien, S. J., Maheu, F., Tu, M., Fiocco, A., & dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.009
Schramek, T. E. (2007). The effects of stress and Peeters, F., Nicolson, N. A., & Berkhof, J. (2004).
stress hormones on human cognition: Implications Levels and variability of daily life cortisol secre-
for the field of brain and cognition. Brain and tion in major depression. Psychiatry Research,
Cognition, 65, 209 –237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ 126, 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres
j.bandc.2007.02.007 .2003.12.010
Mason, J. W. (1968). A review of psychoendocrine Piefke, M., Pestinger, M., Arin, T., Kohl, B., Kastrau,
research on the sympathetic-adrenal medullary F., Schnitker, R., . . . Flatten, G. (2008). The
system. Psychosomatic Medicine, 30(Suppl.), neurofunctional mechanisms of traumatic and non-
631– 653. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006842- traumatic memory in patients with acute PTSD
196809000-00022 following accident trauma. Neurocase, 13, 342–
362 PIEFKE AND GLIENKE

357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/135547907018 Experimental Psychology, 69, 310 –323. http://dx


51494 .doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1054294
Raskin, S. (2009). Memory for Intentions Screening Walser, M., Fischer, R., Goschke, T., Kirschbaum,
Test: Psychometric properties and clinical evi- C., & Plessow, F. (2013). Intention retrieval and
dence. Brain Impairment, 10, 23–33. http://dx.doi deactivation following an acute psychosocial stres-
.org/10.1375/brim.10.1.23 sor. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e85685. http://dx.doi.org/
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. 10.1371/journal.pone.0085685
(2007). Remembering the past to imagine the fu- West, R., McNerney, M. W., & Krauss, I. (2007).
ture: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neu- Impaired strategic monitoring as the locus of a
roscience, 8, 657– 661. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ focal prospective memory deficit. Neurocase, 13,
nrn2213 115–126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1355479070
Schwabe, L., Haddad, L., & Schachinger, H. (2008). 1399247
HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold- Wingenfeld, K., Driessen, M., Terfehr, K., Schlosser,
N., Fernando, S. C., Otte, C., . . . Wolf, O. T.
pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 890 –
(2012). Cortisol has enhancing, rather than impair-
895. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03
ing effects on memory retrieval in PTSD. Psycho-
.001 neuroendocrinology, 37, 1048 –1056. http://dx.doi
Schwabe, L., Joëls, M., Roozendaal, B., Wolf, O. T., .org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.002
& Oitzl, M. S. (2012). Stress effects on memory: Wingenfeld, K., & Wolf, O. T. (2015). Effects of
An update and integration. Neuroscience and cortisol on cognition in major depressive disorder,
Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1740 –1749. http://dx posttraumatic stress disorder and borderline per-
.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.002 sonality disorder—2014 Curt Richter Award win-
Schwabe, L., Wolf, O. T., & Oitzl, M. S. (2010). Memory ner. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 51, 282–295.
formation under stress: Quantity and quality. Neurosci- http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.009
ence and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 584–591. http:// Wolf, O. T. (2009). Stress and memory in humans:
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.015 Twelve years of progress? Brain Research, 1293,
Scott, J. C., Woods, S. P., Wrocklage, K. M., 142–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2009
Schweinsburg, B. C., Southwick, S. M., & Krystal, .04.013
J. H. (2016). Prospective memory in posttraumatic Yehuda, R. (2002). Posttraumatic stress disorder. The
stress disorder. Journal of the International Neu- New England Journal of Medicine, 346, 108 –114.
ropsychological Society, 22, 724 –734. http://dx http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra012941
.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617716000564 Yehuda, R. (2006). Advances in understanding neu-
Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., & Shelton, J. T. roendocrine alterations in PTSD and their thera-
(2013). The Dynamic Multiprocess Framework: peutic implications. Annals of the New York Acad-
Evidence from prospective memory with contex- emy of Sciences, 1071, 137–166. http://dx.doi.org/
tual variability. Cognitive Psychology, 67, 55–71. 10.1196/annals.1364.012
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.07.001 Yehuda, R., Teicher, M. H., Trestman, R. L., Leven-
Simons, J. S., Schölvinck, M. L., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, good, R. A., & Siever, L. J. (1996). Cortisol reg-
C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Differential com- ulation in posttraumatic stress disorder and major
ponents of prospective memory? Evidence from depression: A chronobiological analysis. Biologi-
cal Psychiatry, 40, 79 – 88. http://dx.doi.org/10
fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1388 –1397. http://
.1016/0006-3223(95)00451-3
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01
Zubin, J., & Spring, B. (1977). Vulnerability—A new
.005
view of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
Terrett, G., Rose, N. S., Henry, J. D., Bailey, P. E., chology, 86, 103–126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Altgassen, M., Phillips, L. H., . . . Rendell, P. G. 0021-843X.86.2.103
(2015). The relationship between prospective
memory and episodic future thinking in younger Received March 29, 2017
and older adulthood. The Quarterly Journal of Accepted July 18, 2017 䡲

You might also like