You are on page 1of 15

Review of General Psychology © 2012 American Psychological Association

2012, Vol. 16, No. 3, 241–255 1089-2680/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027158

What’s in a Term? Paranormal, Superstitious, Magical and Supernatural


Beliefs by Any Other Name Would Mean the Same

Marjaana Lindeman and Annika M. Svedholm


University of Helsinki

What are paranormal, superstitious, magical, and supernatural (PSMS) beliefs and what, if anything,
separates them? Currently, use of the concepts is ambiguous and agreement is weak. A literature search
for articles dealing with PSMS beliefs during the last two decades produced conceptual definitions that
we grouped into seven groups. Five groups consisted of definitions that were domain-general, namely
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

false beliefs, belief in scientifically impossible phenomena, and associative biases (covariation bias, laws
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of sympathetic magic, and irrational acts). Two sets of definitions were domain-specific (content-
dependent), namely counterintuitive and intuitive beliefs about physical, biological, and psychological
phenomena. Empirical methods were reviewed to show what kinds of beliefs have been examined under
the rubrics paranormal, supernatural, magical, or superstitious. We concluded that the concepts paranor-
mal, superstitious, magical, and supernatural denote the same thing and that domain-general definitions
are inadequately precise. Defining PSMS beliefs as category mistakes that confuse the distinctive
attributes of mental phenomena, material objects, living, and animate organisms, and the processes
these engage in, fared best in distinguishing PSMS beliefs from other beliefs and covering relevant
beliefs. We suggest researchers sharpen the content of the measures and strive to integrate lines of
research that have so far remained separate.

Keywords: magical, paranormal, supernatural, superstitious

Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027158.supp

Do children have paranormal beliefs? Is belief in God a super- magical, and supernatural should be defined. Consensus is lacking
stition? Is the Devil a magical being? Are horoscopes supernatu- for such basic questions as why belief in immortal souls should be
ral? What exactly do these concepts stand for? considered paranormal (supernatural, magical, superstitious) while
Psychologists have been interested in paranormal, superstitious, the belief that vitamin C prevents flu should not. As researchers,
magical and supernatural topics since the 1800’s, but in the re- we should also be able to specify the relationship between scien-
search literature, these terms are used inconsistently. For example, tific knowledge and claims that are, for instance, supernatural (lat.
the exact same beliefs might be filed under different constructs; super ⫽ above, natura ⫽ nature) or paranormal (gr. para ⫽
belief in witches, for example, has intermittently been labeled against; lat. para ⫽ contrary, outside). Can today’s knowledge be
paranormal (Tobacyk & Milford, 1983), supernatural (Landrine & delegated to the status of a supernatural claim tomorrow and vice
Klonoff, 1994), magical (Pronin, Wegner, McCarthy, & Rodri- versa, or is there something inherently different in paranormal,
guez, 2006), and superstitious (Peltzer & Renner, 2003). Con- supernatural, magical, and superstitious beliefs that separates them
versely, the same construct might be used to refer to very different from scientific knowledge?
beliefs, as in the case of superstition, which has been exemplified In recent years, some reviews of research findings have been
by fear of nonpoisonous snakes (Beck & Forstmeier, 2007) and published (Irwin, 2009; Vyse, 1997) but we know of no previous
belief in an afterlife (Tykocinski, 2008), among others. disquisitions of definitions and measures in this field. The aims of
The main problem, however, is that conceptual agreement is this article are to review how paranormal, supernatural, magical
weak in the whole field: There appears to be no consensus what- and superstitious (PSMS) beliefs have been (a) conceptually and
soever on the way the concepts of paranormal, superstitious, (b) operationally defined, to (c) evaluate the available conceptual
definitions and operationalizations, and (d) to offer a conceptual
definition that fills the requirements of good concept definition. In
This article was published Online First June 11, 2012. particular, we examine two questions that have important im-
Marjaana Lindeman and Annika M. Svedholm, Division of Cognitive plications for the development of both theory and assessment:
Psychology and Neuropsychology, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, Uni- (a) Are there reasons to differentiate the concepts paranormal,
versity of Helsinki, Finland.
supernatural, magical and superstitious from each other and if
This study was supported by grants from Nylands Nation and the
Finnish Cultural Foundation awarded to Annika M. Svedholm.
there are, how do the constructs differ from each other? And,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marjaana (b) How have PSMS beliefs been understood to differ from
Lindeman, Division of Cognitive Psychology and Neuropsychology, Insti- other beliefs?
tute of Behavioural Sciences, P. O. Box 9, 00014 University of Helsinki, We first briefly describe the etymological roots of the four
Helsinki, Finland. E-mail: marjaana.lindeman@helsinki.fi concepts. The following chapters present the criteria by which

241
242 LINDEMAN AND SVEDHOLM

studies were included in the review, and give examples of the Work concerning childhood beliefs was included only if it
different ways in which the concepts have been hierarchically explicitly addressed even adult beliefs or if the definition held
ordered. We then proceed to a review of conceptual definitions for particular relevance for adult beliefs. However, methods used to
PSMS beliefs. The next section focuses on methods for measuring assess beliefs in children were not included, as they were seldom
the beliefs. We conclude the article with a synthesis and evaluation comparable to the methods used with adults.
of the available approaches and proposals for defining and assess- Out of the four terms, the search on “magic” returned by far the
ing PSMS beliefs. largest number of results, but the smallest number of articles to be
included in the review. The unselected articles used the word
A Short Etymology “magic” as a metaphor, to refer to perceptual illusions or tricks, or
to something enchanting, surprising, or exceptional (e.g., the bas-
The concept of magic (gr. magos ⫽ one of the members of the ketball player Earvin Johnson).
learned and priestly class) is traced back to the Middle Ages when The difficulty of defining magical, superstitious, paranormal,
some people were assumed to have exceptional characteristics, and supernatural beliefs was widely acknowledged. Many authors
such as an ability to control events in nature. In the course of time,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

chose not to attempt definition at all. It is possible that the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

anyone who for one reason or another was exceptional could be researchers assumed that people are often capable of identifying
considered a magician, including menstruating or pregnant the beliefs at a common sense level, as brought up in one definition
women, doctors, actors, blacksmiths, individuals with unusual of superstitions: They are phenomena that most members of soci-
looks, or, prototypically, priests (Mauss, 1902/1972). ety would identify and recognize as falling into this category
Superstition, also a medieval concept, has always denoted a (Campbell, 1996). Many authors instead supplied examples of the
false belief, assumed to be held by ignorant lay people (lat. phenomena that the beliefs concern, especially in connection with
superstitio ⫽ standing over a thing in amazement or awe). The paranormal beliefs (e.g., witchcraft, spiritualism, astrology, and
concept may have been brought up by Bishop Agobard around 820 precognition). Thus, many of the search results were left out of the
in reference to pagans and non-Christians (Kieckhefer, 1989). review. The present review focuses on those articles that included
Consequently, whereas the concept of magic still carries an en- a conceptual definition, either formulating one explicitly, or using
chanting spell, the term superstition has a strongly pejorative one in such a way that it could be identified.
flavor (Jahoda, 1970): Few people like to self-identify as super- To organize the text, we classified the definitions into seven, not
stitious. completely mutually exclusive groups based on similarity. Five of
Supernatural is by origin a religious concept and it was intro- the groups consisted of domain-general definitions in that the
duced after the Middle Ages to denote something that belongs to beliefs were defined independently from their contents, namely (a)
a realm or system higher than that of nature (Supernatural, n.d.). false beliefs, (b) belief in scientifically impossible phenomena, and
Durkheim (1912/1964) suspected that people in prescientific so- associative biases, that is, (c) covariation bias, (d) laws of sympa-
cieties probably did not see anything as supernatural because only thetic magic, and (e) irrational acts. Two sets of definitions were
in a scientific age can one have a conception of a natural order to domain-specific (content-dependent), namely (f) counterintuitive
which the supernatural may be opposed. and (g) intuitive beliefs about physical, biological, and psycholog-
As “supernatural” carried a religious nuance, the English spiri- ical phenomena.
tual movement of the 19th century and the Society for Psychical In the course of collecting conceptual and operational defini-
Research coined the concept of paranormal. The society was
tions of PSMS beliefs from the literature, we also surveyed dif-
established in 1882 to scientifically investigate events whose op-
ferent hierarchical orderings of the concepts. Often no explicit
eration is outside the scope of the known laws of nature: first,
assertions were made concerning the relations between concepts,
survival after death, and later also phenomena such as telepathy
and many authors simply used two or more concepts in parallel or
and precognition (Paranormal., n.d.). As Rice (2003) has noted,
stated that one concept is superordinate to another. Below we
many Christians differentiate supernatural from paranormal phe-
present some examples of these orderings.
nomena because while some paranormal phenomena (e.g., psycho-
kinesis) are not officially endorsed by the church, other supernat-
ural phenomena (e.g., miracles, guardian angels, and God) are Hierarchical or Nonhierarchical Concepts?
accepted as real.
Some authors used all four concepts synonymously without
Conceptual Definitions making differences between them (e.g., King, Burton, Hicks, &
Drigotas, 2007; Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007). Some others con-
nected three of the four concepts together, be they paranormal,
Search Criteria
superstitions, and supernatural (Otis & Alcock, 1982), paranormal,
The PsycINFO® electronic database was used to conduct a superstitions, and magical (Schulter & Papousek, 2008), or super-
literature search for articles that had full text available, concerned stitions, magical, and supernatural (Gbadamosi, 2005; Wiseman &
human participants, were published in English in peer-reviewed Watt, 2004). Suggested parallels between two of the four concepts
journals between 1990 and the third week of September 2011, and included supernatural and magical beliefs (Kurtz, 1992; McGarry
had one of the main constructs (paranormal, supernatural, super- & Newberry, 1981), supernatural and superstitious beliefs (Tyko-
stition/superstitious or magic/magical) in the title. We comple- cinski, 2008), superstitious and magical beliefs (Case, Fitness,
mented the selected articles with books and articles that we knew Cairns, & Stevenson, 2004; Pronin et al., 2006), magical and
of based on our earlier work in this field. paranormal beliefs (Brugger & Graves, 1997), and paranormal
DEFINING PSMS BELIEFS 243

beliefs and superstitions (Dudley, 1999; Kane, Core, & Hunt; cluded that paranormal beliefs concern things outside the bounds
Wain & Spinella, 2007). of established science (Hines, 1988) and that for paranormal be-
Other researchers, in turn, saw some of the concepts as more liefs to be explicable, major revisions would need to be made to the
general than others. For example, according to Subbotsky (2004), basic limiting principles of science (Tobacyk & Milford, 1983).
magical events involve supernatural ones and according to Yo- Associative biases. In addition to thinking of magical beliefs
rulmaz, Inozu, and Gultepe (2010), magical thinking encompasses as false, Tylor considered magical beliefs to be based on biases in
paranormal beliefs. It was also argued that superstitions are rooted associative thinking: “. . . Having come to associate in thought
in (Keinan, 2002) or derivatives of (Einstein & Menzies, 2004) those things which he found by experience to be connected in fact,
magical thinking. However, other researchers argued the opposite [man] proceeded erroneously to invert this action, and conclude
and proposed that superstitions involve magical thinking (Zusne & that association in thought must involve similar connexion in
Jones, 1989) and that belief in magic is a consequence of super- reality” (Tylor, 1871/1958, p.116). According to Tylor, a “savage
stition (Mowen & Carlson, 2003). At other times, paranormal mind” therefore believes, for instance, that because cocks crow
beliefs were suggested to include superstitions (Dudley, 2000; when the sun rises, the sun will rise if one makes the cock crow.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Tobacyk, 1983) and magical thinking (Peltzer, 2003), and super- In the articles we reviewed, definitions that rested on the idea of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

natural beliefs to include superstitions (Johnson, 2005; Talmont- associative biases were abundant. These definitions are here cat-
Kaminski, 2009), magical beliefs (Legare & Gelman, 2008) and egorized into three clusters: covariation bias, the laws of sympa-
paranormal beliefs (Weeks, Weeks, & Daniel, 2008). The ways thetic magic, and irrational acts carried out for positive outcomes.
researchers described the actual meaning of these terms are de- Covariation bias. A number of researchers defined paranor-
scribed next. Besides their current expressions, the review covers mal, magical and superstitious (albeit not supernatural) beliefs as
the historical background of the seven approaches. erroneous perceptions of causality. Paranormal beliefs, for exam-
ple, were suggested to arise when remarkable coincidences are
Domain-General Definitions wrongly attributed to paranormal causes rather than chance
(Musch & Ehrenberg, 2002), when two closely occurring events
False beliefs. Sir Edward Tylor, one of the founders of are interpreted in a flawed way (Alcock, 1990), and in order to
anthropology, delineated superstitions and magical thinking as
bridge associative gaps between two temporally coinciding events
unfounded ideas and pernicious delusions (Tylor, 1871/1958,
(Gianotti, Mohr, Pizzagalli, Lehmann, & Brugger, 2001).
1871/1974). Similarly, most authors whose work is included in this
In a similar way, superstitions were defined as resting on illu-
review considered magical, superstitious, paranormal, or supernat-
sory pattern perception; for example, on irrational illusory corre-
ural beliefs to be mistakes.
lations (Haselton & Nettle, 2006) or the identification of a coherent
Some defined the beliefs simply as errors, without specifying
and meaningful interrelationship among a set of random or unre-
how they are erroneous more than describing them as irrational,
lated stimuli (Whitson & Galinsky, 2008). Defined this way,
illogical, or illusory. This type of definition was occasionally
superstition was suggested to exist in all kinds of organisms
evoked when discussing magical (e.g., Berenbaum, Boden, &
capable of associative learning (Beck & Forstmeier, 2007). Several
Baker, 2009) or paranormal (e.g., Marks, 1986; Sharps, Matthews,
authors defined superstition in terms of the incorrect establishment
& Asten, 2006) beliefs. Most often, this definition was used when
discussing superstitions. Characteristically, superstitions were con- of cause and effect (Foster & Kokko, 2009; Matute, Yarritu, &
ceived of as tenets founded on ignorance (Jahoda, 1970), an Vadillo, 2011; Zebb & Moore, 2003). Zebb and Moore (2003)
irrational attitude of mind (Parida, 1962) and wrong ideas about pointed out that superstitious presupposed causal links are not
external reality (Beck & Forstmeier, 2007). Superstitions were also founded on empirical evidence and people may, in fact, trust them
defined as beliefs that are barely articulated (Campbell, 1996), and even despite evidence to the contrary.
not based on reason or knowledge (Ng, Chong, & Du, 2010). Definitions of the covariation bias type were equally numerous
Belief in scientifically impossible phenomena. Following in the literature on magical beliefs. The classical sociologists
Tylor’s (1871/1958) classification of magic as pseudoscience and Hubert and Mauss described magical beliefs as gigantic variations
Broad’s (1953) conceptualization of paranormality as a phenom- on the theme of the principle of causality (as cited in Lévi-Strauss,
enon that violates the fundamental and scientifically founded prin- 1962/2004). Magical beliefs were also defined as judgments about
ciples of nature, many current authors considered the beliefs false what goes with what in experience (Shweder, 1977), a defiance of
in the specific sense of contradicting scientific knowledge. For culturally accepted laws of causality (Einstein & Menzies, 2004;
example, magical beliefs were defined as breaking scientific laws Rees, Draper, & Davis, 2010), the incorrect establishment of cause
of causality (Keinan, 1994) and superstitions were considered to be and effect (Parris, Kuhn, Mizon, Benattayallah, & Hodgson,
groundless beliefs or practices that deviate from the enlightened 2009), precausal explanations (Subbotsky, 2004), forms of causal-
views adopted by scientists and the general public in the commu- ity that violate basic limiting principles of science (Peltzer, 2003),
nity one lives in (Vyse, 1997). and the linking of thoughts and external events in a way that
This type of definition was most commonly evoked for beliefs cannot be rationally explained by physical laws or culturally
in paranormal phenomena (e.g., Dag, 1999; Darwin, Neave, & acceptable explanations (Bocci & Gordon, 2007).
Holmes, 2011; Dudley, 2000; Hergovich, 2003; MacDonald, 1995; Laws of sympathetic magic. In the late 19th century, the
Peltzer, 2003; Roberts & Seager, 1999). For example, Irwin (2009) social anthropologist Sir James Frazer elaborated Tylor’s ideas of
recommended that the term paranormal belief should only apply to association into two laws of sympathetic magic: the law of simi-
belief in phenomena that are scientifically impossible, not only larity (homeopathic or imitative magic) and the law of contact
scientifically unacceptable. Other variations on this theme in- (contagious magic).
244 LINDEMAN AND SVEDHOLM

The law of similarity holds that an effect resembles its cause, outcome of a given course of events (e.g., Damisch, Stoberock, &
that like produces like, or more generally, that superficial resem- Mussweiler, 2010).
blance indicates deep resemblance (Frazer, 1922/1963; Nemeroff
& Rozin, 2000). Magicians may thus infer that they can produce Domain-Specific Definitions
any effect they desire, for example rain, merely by imitating it. The
law of contagion holds that things that have once been in contact During recent years, PSMS beliefs have increasingly been de-
continue to influence each other. The alleged influence of the fined in terms of domain-specific knowledge. This perspective
source on the target may be negative (i.e., contamination) or draws from the view that the human mind contains fully or partly
positive, and the influence is assumed to remain after the physical independent modules specialized to process evolutionarily signif-
contact has ceased because an essence has been transferred from icant information (e.g., Chomsky, 1980; Fodor, 1983). The defi-
the source to the target (Frazer, 1922/1963; Rozin & Nemeroff, nitions in this category are guided by the premise that instead of
2002). The French sociologist Marcel Mauss (1902/1972) added general reasoning processes, the beliefs derive from those domain-
that the law also expresses itself as the belief that a person’s specific mental processes that from early childhood onward make
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

essence is found in images, symbols and parts, such as teeth, us understand physical, biological and, in particular, psychological
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

saliva, sweat, nails, or hair. Current-day examples of the laws phenomena. Two types of definitions, one focusing in counterin-
include a reluctance to wear Hitler’s sweater because of the idea of tuition and the other on intuition, have approached the beliefs from
contagion and a reluctance to consume tasty food if it is fashioned the perspective of domain-specific cognition.
to look like a disgusting item, such as body excretions (Rozin & Counterintuitive beliefs. One of the few forums in which the
Nemeroff, 1990). essence of supernatural beliefs has been deeply analyzed is the
Irrational acts. Writing on magic, Mauss (1902/1972) con- modern cognitive science of religion (CSR). One prevalent per-
cluded that referring to errors, associations, or sympathetic magic spective in CSR characterizes supernatural representations as on-
does not produce a complete and satisfactory conceptualization of tological violations based on domain-specific cognitive processes
magic. According to him, researchers should pay attention to and considers these representations counterfactual and counterin-
magical rites, because magic’s exclusive aim is to produce results. tuitive (Atran & Norenzayan, 2005a; Barrett, 2000; Boyer, 2001;
In a similar way, anthropologist Malinowski (1948/1992; see also Boyer, 2003; Pyysiäinen, 2004).
Frazer, 1922/1963) underlined that magic should be defined as a Supernatural beliefs are characterized as counterintuitive be-
body of purely practical acts that are performed as a means to an cause they violate intuitive ontology about entities such as man-
end. made objects, a living thing (plants and animals), and especially, a
In the reviewed articles, several authors likewise defined mag- person (an intentional agent; Barrett, 2000; Boyer, 2003). Atran
ical thinking and superstitions as irrational behaviors carried out to and Norenzayan (2005b; see also Sperber, 1990) specified that in
promote positive outcomes or avoid negative ones. For example, CSR, “counterintuitiveness” refers to something that is contrary to
magical thinking was defined as an action directed toward the what common sense would suggest. Any reasonable statement
achievement of a particular outcome with no logical relationships drawing on domain-specific knowledge—for example, a closing
between the action and the outcome (Albas & Albas, 1989) and as door, a sleeping dog, and a confused student (Norenzayan, Atran,
a belief in the ability to influence events at a distance with no Faulkner, & Schaller, 2006), is considered intuitive. In contrast, a
known physical explanation (Pronin et al., 2006; Zusne & Jones, statement is defined as counterintuitive and supernatural if it
1989). describes an entity in one ontological category with a property that
Out of the four concepts, the one that authors most often used it according to common sense should not have or by denying it a
when discussing irrational acts was superstition. Many referred to property it should have (Boyer, 2001; Norenzayan et al., 2006;
Skinner’s (1948) famous experiments in which conditioned pi- Pyysiäinen, 2004). A spirit is a special kind of person who never
geons adopted baseless rituals, such as thrusting their heads into dies and a magic wand is a special kind of artifact. Other examples
one of the corners of their cage to get food from a food hopper. of supernatural concepts include a statue that listens to prayers, a
According to Skinner, his experiments metaphorically demonstrate cursing frog, and a thirsty door (Atran & Norenzayan, 2005a;
superstition (in quotes in the title) because the birds behaved as if Barrett & Nyhof, 2001; Boyer, 2001).
there were a causal relation between their behavior and the pre- Intuitive beliefs. In contrast to the CSR tradition emphasiz-
sentation of food, although such a relation was lacking. ing the counterintuitiveness of supernatural concepts, other
In a similar vein, other authors defined superstitions as irrational domain-specific approaches consider PSMS beliefs intuitive, that
practices (Jahoda, 1970), and behaviors carried out to promote is, based on intuitive physics, biology and psychology. For exam-
positive outcomes or avoid negative ones despite a lack of any ple, Bloom (2007, see also Bloom & Weisberg, 2007) maintained
supporting scientific evidence (Ciborowski, 1997) and even when that because people intuitively make sense of psychological enti-
the outcomes are in fact determined by chance (Rothbaum, Weisz, ties in different terms than physical entities, people implicitly
& Snyder, 1982; Scheibe & Sarbin, 1965). An example of a more come to think of souls and bodies as distinct; what runs counter to
thorough definition comes from sports psychology: Superstitious one’s intuitions is scientific information.
rituals are actions that are repetitive, formal, sequential, nonordi- Many definitions in this category echoed Piaget’s observations
nary, and potent in that the rituals are believed to be powerful in about children’s magical thinking. Piaget (1929/1951) described
controlling supernatural beings or force (Womack, 1992). Some magical thinking as intuitive thinking that confuses mental and
authors included not only actions but also objects (e.g., a lucky physical, and living and lifeless phenomena. This confusion finds
charm) and circumstances not related to a course of events among expressions in nominal realism (names emanate from the objects
the factors that a superstitious person believes might influence the they name), artificialism (all things are manufactured for a pur-
DEFINING PSMS BELIEFS 245

pose), and participation (all objects and entities in the natural nonmaterial mental contents (e.g., thoughts, beliefs, and desires),
world are involved in a system of mystic participation). material objects have an independent existence in space; and that
Piaget’s notion of participation was rearticulated by many au- intentional acts are committed by animate beings but physical
thors in the current review (e.g., Subbotsky, 2001; Zusne & Jones, events take place through the transfer of unintentional force or
1989), especially those studying thought-action fusion (TAF). energy (for details, see Carey & Spelke, 1994; Kalish, 1999;
TAF, typically associated with obsessive– compulsive disorder, Wellman & Gelman, 1998).
refers to intrusive beliefs that certain thoughts cause particular We suggested that intuitively mixing up these properties lays the
events (“If I think about an accident, then the accident will hap- ground for adopting PSMS beliefs found in one’s culture. Com-
pen”). Many authors suggested that TAF is a specific type of mon examples include beliefs that thoughts can move external
magical thinking (Amir, Freshman, Ramsey, Neary, & Brigidi, objects, that force or energy can live (e.g., Feng shui, Chi), that
2001; Einstein & Menzies, 2004; Rassin, Merckelbach, Muris, & minds can exist independently of bodies and operate as animate
Schmidt, 2001) but it was also noted that inconsistent definitions entities (e.g., angels, devils and ghosts), and that chance events
of magical thinking have hindered better understanding of the have a purpose and may be intentionally modified (e.g., rainmak-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

relationship between TAF and magical thinking (Berle & ing rites, dice rolling rituals).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Starcevic, 2005).
Piaget’s ideas have been applied to supernatural beliefs as well.
For example, Kelemen (2004; Kelemen & Rosset, 2009) charac- Operational Definitions
terized children’s tendency for “promiscuous” teleological reason-
ing in a similar way as Piaget described artificialism, that is, as an Table 1 offers a comprehensive list of the various belief objects
intuitive tendency to explain all kinds of natural phenomena by that researchers offered as instances of PSMS beliefs in the re-
reference to a human-like purpose, such as assuming that clouds viewed articles. Belief objects are on separate rows. The four
are made for the purpose of bringing us rain. columns show the numbers of different measures used to assess
In the same way, Guthrie (1993) suggested that the concept of each belief object classified by the concept that the measure was
God is best understood as an expression of a general, spontaneous, purported to assess (paranormal, magical, superstition, or super-
and unconscious interpretative tendency to attribute human char- natural). While Table 1 shows the numbers of measures, an all-
acteristics to nonhuman things and events, that is, by anthropo- inclusive table listing all the specific measures can be found in the
morphism. Anthropomorphism (gr. anthropo ⫽ human) is a term supplemental online material. In incongruous cases, measures
closely linked with animism, the former deriving from theological were categorized based on their names. For example, Eckblad and
texts and the latter from (Tylor’s, 1871/1974) writings on magical Chapman’s (1983) Magical Ideation scale was counted as a mea-
thinking. Anthropomorphism means ascription of human attributes sure of magical thinking even though some authors who utilized
to anything nonhuman, whereas animism (lat. anima ⫽ soul, spirit, the scale exclusively used some other concepts (e.g., paranormal
breath) denotes belief in the existence of souls and attribution of a beliefs) in their texts. In cases where the measure had no name and
soul, life and consciousness to inanimate objects and processes. the authors discussed two concepts (e.g., paranormal and magical),
Some scholars brought up that mental-physical and living- the measure was categorized according to the first mentioned
lifeless confusions are involved in supernatural and magical beliefs concept. Measures that were slight reformulations of an estab-
more generally. Wellman and Gelman (1998) characterized chil- lished measure, that were insufficiently described, or that com-
dren’s magical thinking as ontological confusions in fundamental prised only overall items (e.g., “Are you a superstitious person?”)
knowledge about physical, biological, and psychological phenom- were excluded from the table.
ena, in which the attributes of one domain are stretched to inap- The measures mainly comprised questionnaires (especially for
propriate realms. Similarly, Nemeroff and Rozin (2000) defined paranormal beliefs) and experiments (especially for superstitions),
magic as an intuition or belief in the existence of imperceptible although some other methods (e.g., interviews) were also used.
forces or essences that transcend the usual boundary between the The most often used questionnaires turned out to be (in alphabet-
mental/symbolic and physical/material. Several other authors also ical order) the Anomalous Experiences Inventory (Gallagher, Ku-
espoused the idea of intuitive confusions between mental and mar, & Pekala, 1994), the Australian Sheep-Goat scale (Thal-
physical in supernatural beliefs, but many did not discuss the bourne & Delin, 1993), the Magical Ideation scale (Eckblad &
meaning of the term in more detail (e.g., Bering, 2006; Bjorklund, Chapman, 1983), and the Paranormal Belief Scale (Tobacyk &
Blasi, & Periss, 2010; Bloom, 2007; Hood, 2009). Milford, 1983) and its revision (Tobacyk, 2004).
We have earlier argued that the idea of confused domain- In addition, several authors had constructed their own question-
specific knowledge should be applied to both paranormal, super- naires, particularly for paranormal beliefs. This is manifested in
natural, magical, and superstitious beliefs among adults (Lindeman the great number of methods for measuring paranormal beliefs.
& Aarnio, 2007; Lindeman et al., 2008; Lindeman & Saher, 2007). Conversely, most questionnaire studies on magical beliefs used the
We defined the beliefs as involving category mistakes, in which Magical Ideation scale, resulting in a smaller number of methods
the core ontological distinctions between the categories of physi- for magical beliefs. Measures for supernatural beliefs were fewer
cal, biological, and psychological phenomena are blurred. Thus, as well, probably because these are more often assessed under the
we posed that the scope of PSMS beliefs should be limited to rubric of religious beliefs, which were not included in our search
confusions of those ontological distinctions that children learn criteria.
without explicit instruction irrespective of culture, that is, core Many of the belief objects in Table 1 are rather self-explanatory.
knowledge. These distinctions include, for example, understanding Others are in need of more explication, particularly the following:
the fundamentals of contamination and healing; that in contrast to the laws of similarity and contagion, luck-related rituals, control of
246 LINDEMAN AND SVEDHOLM

Table 1
Number of Assessment Methods by Belief Object and Concept Being Measured (Paranormal, Superstitious, Magical, or Supernatural
Beliefs)

Concept being measured

Belief object Paranormal Superstition Magical Supernatural

Extrasensory perception (e.g., telepathy, precognition) 30 7 0 5


Card reading, palm reading 9 2 0 0
Precognitive dreams 13 0 1 1
Psychokinesis or levitation 14 0 0 1
Extraordinary life forms (e.g., the snowman of Tibet) 8 0 0 0
Extraterrestrial life (e.g., UFOs) 18 1 1 3
Omens (e.g., breaking a mirror, black cats) 5 7 1 0
Amulets and charms 2 10 3 0
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Lucky numbers, colors or days 6 11 0 0


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Luck-related rituals 2 10 4 1
Control of chance events by thoughts or behavior 0 3 5 2
Astrology, lunar effects and horoscopes 18 4 1 4
Feng shui 1 1 1 0
Plants’ ability to sense and communicate 3 0 0 1
Cosmic forces and energies (e.g., aura, pyramid power) 4 0 2 4
Law of contagion 0 0 16 1
Law of similarity 0 0 12 0
Spiritual healing 8 1 1 1
Alternative medicine 3 0 0 0
Gods 5 1 0 8
Spiritual beings other than gods (e.g., angels, devils) 9 0 1 7
Creationism 3 1 0 0
Prayer 0 1 0 2
Fate 0 2 0 1
Afterlife, reincarnation, karma 15 2 1 8
Communication with the dead 13 1 0 2
Hell and/or Heaven 5 0 0 0
Astral projection (spirit separated from the body) 9 0 0 1
Ghosts, haunting, poltergeist 13 2 0 1
Witches and bewitchment (incl. spells, curses, etc.) 11 1 8 3
Déjà vu 4 0 0 1
Illusory rule detection 0 8 1 0
Altered states of consciousness (e.g., hallucinations) 5 0 1 0

Note. More information about the assessment methods can be found in the online supplementary table. [http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027158.supp]

miscellaneous chance events by thoughts or behavior, and illusory The second object of belief in need of explanation are luck-
rule detection. related rituals. The operational measures that we found included,
First, belief in the law of contagion was often operationalized as first, well-known habits such as keeping one’s fingers crossed
the degree of reluctance to touch or eat objects that have been in (Damisch et al., 2010) and knocking on wood in response to
contact with something unpleasant, or conversely, as the willing- hearing stressful questions (Keinan, 2002). Some studies used an
ness to choose things that have been in contact with something or open question format and found many more private and idiosyn-
someone pleasant. Examples include the degree of reluctance to cratic rituals, such as cutting one’s hair short to permit knowledge
drink juice if a sterilized, dead cockroach has been dropped in it for to flow freely around the head, and eating special food such as a
5 seconds (Rozin, Millman, & Nemeroff, 1986) and the evaluation carrot muffin from a certain vendor for luck (Albas & Albas,
of chocolate chip cookies depending on how closely they were 1989). Athletes’ rituals to guarantee their physical condition in
placed to a pack of sanitary towels (Morales & Fitzsimons, 2007). games were also often used as measures of belief in lucky rituals.
Similarly, the law of similarity was typically operationalized as Examples include selecting the same golf ball after a successful
the degree of reluctance to touch, eat, or do things considered putt (Van Raalte, Brewer, Nemeroff, & Linder, 1991), batting
similar to something unpleasant, and conversely, as the willingness rituals among baseball players (Ciborowski, 1997), going to bed
to choose things that the participant associated with pleasant early, doing things in a fixed order, checking one’s appearance in
things. For instance, Rozin et al. (1986) measured preferences for the mirror, and wearing lucky items of clothing before a game
chocolate fudge that was in the form of dog feces, and participants’ (Bleak & Frederick, 1998; Schippers & Van Lange, 2006).
willingness to hold a fake rubber pool of vomit between their lips. Third, belief in the controllability of miscellaneous chance
In addition, people’s reactions to throwing darts at a picture of a events by thoughts or behavior was used as a belief object in
baby (King et al., 2007) or Hitler versus John F. Kennedy (Rozin operationalizations of magical, superstitious, and supernatural be-
et al., 1986) were examined. liefs. To illustrate, Wohl and Enzle (2002) assessed magical think-
DEFINING PSMS BELIEFS 247

ing by asking participants to rate their prospects of winning in a Although some slight trends emerged in the usage of the four
lottery depending on if they had chosen the lottery tickets them- concepts, the trends reflected the etymological histories of the
selves or if a computer program had randomly selected their ticket. concepts more than any theoretical underpinnings. Researchers
In Landrine and Klonoff’s (1994) study, participants’ supernatural who were influenced by anthropologists examining traditional
beliefs were operationalized, among others, as a belief that com- societies tended to use magic-related concepts in their work. In
mitting sinful acts causes illness. turn, researchers such as Skinner (1948) and his followers, who
Last, when “illusory rule detection” was the object of belief, were interested in how false beliefs and irrational behaviors are
superstition was operationalized, for example, as participants’ il- formed and maintained in the Western world, preferred the concept
lusory beliefs that a stimulus forms a pair with another stimulus of superstition.
(Freeman et al., 2009), or that by pressing specific buttons or keys Paranormal was a concept somewhat more characteristic of
they would earn points (Rudski, Lischner, & Albert, 1999) or stop studies addressing phenomena that once interested the scholars in
an uncontrollable noise (Matute, 1995). the Society for Psychical Research. Finally, whereas other re-
As a whole, Table 1 shows that operationalizations of paranor- searchers equated the four concepts quite liberally, scientists who
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mal, superstitious, magical, or supernatural beliefs were not sys- were interested in religious-like beliefs (e.g., afterlife beliefs)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tematically different from each other, as most objects of belief typically used the concept of supernatural exclusively. These his-
were studied under at least three of the concepts. The few weak but torical connotations may explain why it sometimes feels inappro-
observable trends that were found included that beliefs related to priate to replace one concept with another, for example, to say that
positive or negative outcomes (i.e., luck) and illusory rule detec- belief in God is a superstition or that children have paranormal
tion were often labeled as superstitions, and beliefs related to the beliefs.
laws of contagion and similarity were exclusively labeled magical. It is thus probable that the interrelationship of the concepts
In addition to the belief objects in Table 1, even counterintuitive supernatural, magical, paranormal, and superstition is similar to
representations and intuitive core knowledge confusions were em- the interrelationship of the concepts of mind, spirit, and soul. That
pirically assessed. Counterintuitive ideas were operationalized in is, they are concepts that have different connotations and yet
experiments by the evaluation of expressions such as a flying denote the same thing. Although it is not uncommon for different
healthy cow, a confused table, and an impatient fist (Norenzayan origins and varying historical associations to give similar concepts
et al., 2006; Upala, Gonce, Tweney, & Slone, 2007). Examples of a distinct character (e.g., an atheist may be called just but not
operationalizations of core knowledge confusions included ques- righteous; Merriam-Webster’s dictionary of synonyms, n.d.), us-
tionnaire and EEG studies on processing statements such as “a age of scientific concepts should be more exact and empirically
force lives in nature” and “the house knows its history” (Lindeman justifiable. It would not be possible to have some researchers study
& Aarnio, 2007; Lindeman et al., 2008). These studies were not memory as a process of the soul, others study memory as a process
included in Table 1 because counterintuitiveness studies concen- of the spirit and yet others, in isolation, as a process of the mind.
trated on the memorability of counterintuitive ideas and core This review suggests that this may currently be the case in research
knowledge confusion studies addressed the confusions as corre- on paranormal, superstitious, magical, and supernatural beliefs.
lates of conventional PSMS beliefs, that is, neither type of state- Therefore, researchers concerned with the meaning of paranor-
ment was intended as a measure of the PSMS beliefs per se. mal, magical, superstitious, or supernatural should pay attention to
all four concepts. For the present, researchers on PSMS beliefs
only sporadically allude to studies conducted under another con-
Conclusions struct, even when the objects of belief are similar. Ignoring the
similarities between the concepts has maintained conceptual con-
The review showed that definitional and assessment foci vary
fusion and hindered us from advancing our understanding of the
widely. Near full agreement was found only on the assumption that
psychology of the beliefs. Unless providing convincing arguments,
the beliefs are unfounded. Even so, it was possible to find common
researchers working under one of the terms should not ignore
threads in how the beliefs have been conceptually defined, whether
research results on the three other concepts, and arguments that
separation of these four concepts is justified, how the beliefs have
researchers put forward on one concept should also apply to the
been understood to differ from other beliefs, and how the beliefs
three others.
have been empirically assessed. We drew four major conclusions,
discussed next.
Domain-General Definitions Do Not Work
Paranormal, Supernatural, Magical, and Supernatural Five of the seven definition types represented domain-general
Beliefs Are Not Fundamentally Different views of the PSMS beliefs and two were expressions of domain-
specific definitions. In the concluding discussion, we focus on the
The literature revealed no essential reasons to set the concepts differences between domain-general and domain-specific views.
paranormal, magical, superstitious, and supernatural apart. Argu- Domain-general definitions described the beliefs as false beliefs,
ments concerning which of the terms should be taken as synonyms beliefs in scientifically impossible phenomena, beliefs building on
and which should not, were seldom presented. In cases where one a covariation bias, biases following the laws of sympathetic magic,
of the concepts was conceptually defined, its likeness or unlike- or as irrational acts. Some of these definitions concerned only sets
nesss to the other concepts was not elaborated. Furthermore, no of the beliefs or their manifestations. Identifying the beliefs as
consistent differences between the operationalizations of PSMS irrational acts, e.g., excludes beliefs that allow the individual to be
beliefs were found. passive, such as beliefs in omens, astrology, and telepathy. Con-
248 LINDEMAN AND SVEDHOLM

sidering the beliefs to be the result of a covariation bias, in turn, phasized the beliefs’ counterintuitiveness, while those with a back-
does not encompass beliefs in single phenomena such as devils or ground in psychology mostly emphasized their intuitiveness.
an afterlife. Finally, although the laws of sympathetic magic have Researchers advocating the counterintuitive definitions sug-
wide applicability as they illustrate how magical thinking may turn gested that supernatural concepts, for example a being who never
up in unexpected areas such as disgust, the laws confine only to a dies, are counterintuitive because they violate intuitive ontology
subset of magical beliefs, as Rozin and Nemeroff (2002) them- about entities such as solid objects, lifeless organisms, living
selves note. things, animate beings, events, and abstract objects (Atran &
Except for Rozin and Nemeroff, the literature search found no Norenzayan, 2005a; Boyer, 2001; Norenzayan et al., 2006). A
other authors who explicitly addressed the scope of their defini- challenge for defining PSMS beliefs as counterintuitive is the
tions. The other treatments leave open the question: How should unusual way the words intuition and counterintuition are used,
beliefs not covered by a particular definition be labeled? For making it difficult to integrate this work with other scientific work
example, if one commits to the definition of superstition as cova- on intuition where intuition stands for a conscious judgment based
riation bias, should belief in devils be considered nonsuperstitious? on automatic and holistic processing of information, which might
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Despite these problems, domain-general definitions as a whole be opposed to a concurrent judgment based on rational knowledge
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

set out the characteristic and essential features of the beliefs in a (Evans, 2008; Hodgkinson, Langan-Fox, & Sadler-Smith, 2008).
reasonably satisfying way: they are unfounded beliefs in scientif- Due to its weaknesses, criticism against reliance on the concept of
ically impossible phenomena that may manifest themselves as counterintuitiveness has been raised even within CSR. For exam-
associative biases either in thinking or in behavior. Although the ple, Barrett (2008) has brought up that difficulties in identifying
domain-general definitions thus make clear what the concepts what are and are not counterintuitive concepts has made empirical
apply to, they have one important limitation. Because the defini- studies irregular and difficult to evaluate. Likewise, Boyer (2001)
tions do not distinguish PSMS beliefs from other unfounded be- has argued that the term counterintuitive may be misleading.
liefs, they are too broad in scope to be useful as such. From the The other domain-specific approaches considered PSMS beliefs
point of view of theory formulation, domain-general definitions intuitive. These approaches are characterized by the idea of early
would imply that the same causal processes, mediators and mod- and undeveloped intuitive conceptions of psychological, biologi-
erators that lie behind stereotypes about homosexuals (a covaria- cal, and physical phenomena that retain their autonomous power
tion bias), living beyond one’s means (an irrational act), knowl- into adulthood, making individuals susceptible to PSMS beliefs
edge about microbial contamination and vaccination (following (e.g., Bering, 2006; Bjorklund et al., 2010; Bloom, 2007; Hood,
the laws of contagion and similarity), the belief that dolphins are 2009; Kelemen, 2004). This approach is conceptually clear and
fish (a false belief), and the belief that colors are attributes of theoretically fruitful because it suggests links to cognitive devel-
things in themselves (a scientifically impossible claim) would also opment and to dual-process theories of cognition that address two
account for superstitious, magical, paranormal, or supernatural information processing modes: intuitive and analytical reasoning.
beliefs. Dual-process theories (for a review see Evans, 2008) offer a
In Aristotle’s terminology, domain-general definitions define framework for investigating PSMS beliefs at different levels of
PSMS beliefs only by genus, that is, by a wider category of beliefs consciousness. They also suggest plausible mechanisms for the
with which the beliefs share common characteristics but not by general observation that PSMS beliefs (e.g., belief in immortal
differentia, that is, the attributes that set the beliefs apart from souls) may coexist side by side or even contravene later acquired
other related beliefs. To be useful, a scientific definition should understanding and rational knowledge (e.g., “Death is final”).
satisfy both criteria. We argue that catching the differentia of the However, arguing that the conceptions are intuitive (a domain-
beliefs by appeal to domain-general characteristics of the beliefs is general feature of cognitive processes) does not help to clarify the
an impossible task. By definition, domain-general processes are nature of PSMS beliefs, nor how they differ from other beliefs or
cognitive processes that apply to all thought, irrespective of its intuitions. As PSMS beliefs may also transpire with much reflec-
content. Yet, it is expressly the content that differentiates PSMS tion and conscious thinking, we see no reason that intuitiveness
beliefs from other unfounded beliefs. For this reason, the domain- (or, counterintuitiveness) should be incorporated into the definition
specific definitions presently appear more promising. of PSMS beliefs.
An important question is what kinds of ontological violations
make a belief a PSMS belief. In the cognitive science of religion,
Domain-Specific Definitions Catch The Genus and The the coverage of the term supernatural is extended to a wide set of
Differentia phenomena, encompassing not only familiar supernatural phenom-
ena like gods, immortal souls, and statues that listen to prayers, but
An idea that we encountered in several articles was that PSMS also occurrences such as a flying cow (Upala et al., 2007), a car
beliefs involve misunderstandings where the attributes of physical, that can drip through a sieve (Barrett, 2000), and a melting grand-
biological, and psychological are mixed. Few researchers intended father (Atran & Norenzayan, 2005a). While these are delightful
this to be an actual definition and mentioned it as more of a examples of ontological violations found, for example, in fairy
sidenote, but we hold that this approach in fact catches the essence tales, they do not easily fit in with the prevailing views about what
of PSMS beliefs and argue that the idea of confused domain- a supernatural phenomenon is.
specific knowledge, after some specifications, applies to all para- We, in turn, have earlier suggested that PSMS beliefs should be
normal, supernatural, magical, and superstitious beliefs. limited to confusions of universally learned, early developing
Two types of domain-specific definitions were found: research- understanding about the most fundamental distinctive properties of
ers representing the cognitive science of religion most often em- psychological, biological, and physical objects and processes (Lin-
DEFINING PSMS BELIEFS 249

deman & Aarnio, 2007; Lindeman et al., 2008). Although delim- describes a physical event with properties from an ontologically
iting the scope of thinking about PSMS beliefs in important ways, basic category it does not belong to, namely physical objects
this definition is not entirely coherent either, as the term “psycho- (Carey, 1985; Chi & Hausmann, 2003). Categories such as mental
logical phenomena” taps two different classes of phenomena: phenomena, material objects, living things, animate beings, and
mental phenomena and animate organisms. These should be dis- intentional and unintentional events are all ontologically basic
tinguished because animate organisms are material entities that can categories, and their distinct properties cannot be borrowed to
exert causal force on external objects and events but mental characterize an entity in another ontological category without
phenomena do not: although mental phenomena can affect the making a category mistake (Carey, 1985; Keil, 1979; Ryle, 1949).
body, they do not, as representations, have independent existence The definition thus provides a criterion for judging which
and they cannot exert mechanical causal force directly on the beliefs should be categorized PSMS beliefs and which should
external world as material entities do. In addition, because psy- not. It excludes not only other unfounded beliefs (e.g., vitamin
chology, biology, and physics are labels of scientific fields and not C prevents flu) but also all other beliefs that have the potential
domains that serve as explanatory frames in everyday life, it may to be true. Considered from this point of view, many phenom-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

be appropriate to abandon these concepts from the definition. ena suggested to be paranormal (magical, supernatural) are not
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Based on these arguments, we suggest that paranormal, supernat- paranormal after all. For example, biological contagion beliefs
ural, magical and superstitious beliefs are defined as category (even false ones such as the reluctance to wear clothes touched
errors confusing core knowledge about mental phenomena, mate- by an individual with AIDS), similarity beliefs without a cate-
rial objects, living and animate organisms, and the processes these gory error (e.g., red drinks improve hemoglobin), the ideas that
engage in (for examples, see Table 2). For the present, this defi- there is life on other planets, that crop circles are caused by the
nition seems more precise and parsimonious than the previous as actions of extraterrestrials, that handwriting reveals one’s per-
it answers the challenges brought up, as discussed below. sonality, and that there are mysterious disappearances in the
First, the definition has the most to say about the differences Bermuda triangle, should not be classified as PSMS beliefs as
between PSMS and other unsupported beliefs. Conceptualizing the they do not include category mistakes.
beliefs as category mistakes means that PSMS beliefs are not The definition also specifies why paranormal, supernatural,
ordinary mistakes. An example of an ordinary mistake is the and magical phenomena are scientifically impossible and out-
assumption that a car is broken. Even though this assumption side the scope of the laws of nature, providing an explanation
might be flawed (the car just ran out of gas), it has the potential to for these oft-cited expressions. The criterion that paranormal
be right. In contrast, the argument that a thunderstorm is broken is statements are empirically untestable does not suffice. For
not an ordinary mistake. It is a category mistake, because it example, the statement “Ants that weigh 100 kg exist” is

Table 2
Examples of Category Mistakes Involving Core Knowledge, The Resulting Paranormal, Superstitious, Magical, and Supernatural
Beliefs, and Related Concepts

Inappropriate attribute attached


Entities and processes to the entity or process Examples of beliefs Related concepts

MENTAL
Minds Independent existence, living Afterlife, gods, angels, demons (and Animism
demonical possession, e.g., witches),
ghosts, haunting, poltergeist
Mental states Contagiousness A person’s mentality residing in worn Law of contagiona
objects
Healing Spiritual healing
Thoughts (beliefs, desires) Moving external objects Psychokinesis, levitation
Ability to bring about external Thinking about an accident makes it Thought-action fusion, participation
events likely to happen between thought and things
Symbols Material (indexical) relation to The label poison means that the Law of similaritya, nominal realism
a material object contents of a bottle are poisoned
Temporal (indexical) relation Tarot cards, palm reading, omens, signs
to future events of the zodiac
Symbolic acts Ability to bring about external Rituals, spells, curses, voodoo, black Law of similaritya
events magic
MATERIAL WORLD
Sensation of objects in the world Mentally (internally) driven Clairvoyance, precognition,
retrocognition, telepathy
Plants Having a mind The spirit of trees Animism, antropomorphisma
Artifacts Having a mind, living Bleeding, weeping and listening statues Animism, antropomorphisma,
participation between objects
Force and energy Living, healing, goal-directed Chi, vital force, healing energy Animism, antropomorphisma
Random events Goal-directed, intentionally Purpose, fate, creationism, intelligent Artificialism, promiscuous
planned design teleological reasoning
a
Note. The concepts extend to nonparanormal phenomena as well (e.g., regarding a pet mouse as ambitious is anthropomorphism but not paranormal).
250 LINDEMAN AND SVEDHOLM

empirically untestable, but not paranormal. Paranormal, magi- events, and the symbolic content attached to amulets or rituals may
cal, and supernatural phenomena are impossible phenomena appear as a means to affect external events.
because they are described with a distinctive property of an Furthermore, the definition of PSMS beliefs as confusions of
ontologically basic category that they do not belong to, and, core knowledge makes it possible to trace the paranormal and
therefore, circumstances in which the arguments could be true nonparanormal components in pseudosciences and traditional
do not exist. This does not, however, mean that phenomena that schools of thought, such as astrology, Feng shui, and oriental
are viewed as supernatural today could never turn out to be real. medicine. Usually, these teachings are categorized as paranormal
Schizophrenia was once considered to result from demon pos- only because their arguments have not been scientifically proven.
session, and dreams were taken as an indication of the soul Yet, lack of proof alone does not make an argument paranormal.
detaching itself from the body. Phenomena that have been Moreover, these teachings include several reasonable tenets, in-
considered supernatural may thus turn out to be natural and cluding the ideas that planets might influence earthly events and
explicable by science, but this can only happen through con- that we should keep our kitchens clean. What is paranormal in
ceptual change about the ontological properties of the entities these doctrines are the assumptions that the placement of furniture
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

involved. affects a flowing, living, spiritual force or energy, that this kind of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

While differentiating PSMS beliefs from other unsupported force is the reason for physical and mental health, and that the
beliefs, the suggested definition still covers a wide variety of symbolic contents of the zodiac (e.g., aggression and energy linked
PSMS beliefs, as exemplified in Table 2. The table also shows that with Mars) have causal influence on people’s personalities and
other belief-related, more specific concepts can be incorporated destinies.
into this conceptualization. Animism, anthroporphism, various
forms of participation, nominal realism, TAF, artificialism, pro- Assessment Methods Hit The Target Better Than
miscuous teleological reasoning, and the magical laws of conta- The Definitions
gion and similarity— concepts that are currently restricted to use in
highly specialized lines of research and whose interrelationships While the conceptual formulations about what separates PSMS
have seldom been analyzed—all refer to a particular type of core beliefs from other unfounded beliefs were often blurry, the oper-
ational definitions showed less ambiguity. Most of the reviewed
knowledge confusion.
measures assessed beliefs in phenomena whose paranormality no
Analyzing PSMS beliefs in terms of inappropriate ontological
significant majority of people would doubt. Indeed, the measures
attributes attached to mental phenomena, material objects, and
typically concentrated on key areas of belief, such as ESP, astrol-
living and animate organisms also helps us to identify what exactly
ogy, spiritual (incorporeal) beings, and luck-related beliefs. Even
is paranormal in PSMS beliefs more accurately than defining the
though none of the reviewed measures were designed with the
beliefs merely as being generally against scientific knowledge. We
category error definition in mind, examination of the belief objects
illustrate this by beliefs in ESP and omens and some examples of
reveals that most of them fit this definition well, as can be seen in
pseudosciences (also see Table 2).
Table 2.
Belief in ESP (e.g., telepathy, precognition and clairvoyance)
However, some assessment methods raised the question if what
includes the idea that it is possible to gain information about
they were measuring are really PSMS beliefs. For example, some
objects, physical events, or other people’s thoughts without the single items in questionnaires (e.g., “I believe in the unconscious”,
sense organs of the human body (e.g., sight and hearing). Belief in Gallagher et al., 1994) and belief objects related to extraterrestrial
ESP thus treats sensing and interpreting (i.e., perceiving) informa- life are not obvious manifestations of PSMS beliefs. It is also
tion as purely mental phenomena. In a similar way, small children doubtful whether measures such as illusory rule detection are
do not appreciate that the senses, such as vision and hearing, are accurate ways to assess PSMS beliefs (e.g., a belief that one can
necessary conditions for informational access, but see beliefs and find a numerical cue with a keyboard to stop an uncontrollable
knowledge as arising purely mentally within the individual, like noise if one is induced to find one, Matute, 1995). Whether one
desires (Flavell, 2004; Robinson, Haigh, & Pendle, 2008; Wim- accepts the idea that PSMS beliefs must include category errors or
mer, Hogrefe, & Perner, 1988). not, it is difficult to see what is paranormal in these beliefs and
Omens, in turn, might be understood as confusions between abilities. Whatever conceptual definition proves best supported,
symbols and indexes, that is, between mental and material. Omens, we suggest that future work be conceptually more rigorous and
for example celestial bodies and their relative positions in astrol- that assessment methods be chosen with an emphasis on the
ogy, have symbolic contents. A symbol is a mental representation specific nature of PSMS beliefs.
based on social conventions and thus it bears no realistic connec- Some measures assessed experiences such as déjà vu and altered
tion to its referent. An index, in contrast, is a sign that has a states of consciousness (e.g., hallucinations or out-of-body expe-
physical and temporal causal connection to its existing or upcom- riences). Reports of these kinds of experiences are questionable as
ing external referent; it is like a fragment torn away from the object measures of PSMS beliefs because they may, but do not necessar-
(Peirce, 1955). For example, smoke is an index of fire and dark ily, inform us about participants’ beliefs. Individuals who experi-
clouds are indexes of coming rain. Signs that are symbols for ence disruptions in consciousness and alterations in the perception
adults (e.g., names) are initially indexical for young children of self or surroundings may well find the experiences as valid
(Homer & Nelson, 2005; Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 2005; Piaget, indications of paranormal phenomena. However, some other indi-
1929/1951). Similarly, for adults with a tendency to hold PSMS viduals may interpret these kinds of occurrences as signs of ex-
beliefs, the symbolic content attached to a spread of Tarot cards haustion and maintain a skeptical attitude when describing them:
may appear as an index, that is, causally connected to future “It was as if my mind was separated from my body,” in which case
DEFINING PSMS BELIEFS 251

it is unjustified to call these individuals paranormal believers. the claim that science cannot explain supernatural phenomena
Therefore, we recommend that future studies should not straight- because they belong to another world, a world beyond the scope of
forwardly equate experiences with belief. science. For example, the arguments that life continues after death
Questions about behavior were often used as measures of belief, or that walking under a ladder brings bad luck belong to the realm
motivated by the observations that sometimes beliefs are implicit of science because they are general propositions about the ways
and unconscious, and that people often explicitly reject the sug- the world works, and especially because they challenge prevailing
gestion that they believe in magic but nevertheless allow it to scientific knowledge.
influence their thinking and actions (Campbell, 1996; Ciborowski, Over the last decades, research emphasis on the beliefs has
1997; Subbotsky, 2001). However, acts need not be driven by shifted from domain-general thinking to domain-specific concep-
belief; people may well understand that their actions are irrational, tions. This new line of work has sharpened the scope of research
for example. Especially luck-related rituals could be motivated by in important ways; after all, it is the content that is the crux of the
a host of reasons other than PSMS belief, such as cultural habits or matter in the beliefs. Defining paranormal, superstitious, magical,
comfort seeking. Only if an individual believes that the symbolic and supernatural beliefs as category errors of core knowledge, as
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

content of the act (e.g., luck attached to a ball) has causal influence we suggest, has a high heuristic value as it demonstrates the role
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

on future outcomes, does the act deserve to be labeled superstition. of universal domain-specific knowledge as the common founda-
Thus, researchers employing behavioral methods to measure tion of PSMS beliefs across time and cultures. In addition, the
PSMS beliefs should proceed with caution and verify that the definition has theoretical utility because it holds the most promise
behaviors indeed have a superstitious meaning for the particular for integrating the various definitions of the beliefs and it offers
participants being studied. clues to the psychological processes behind the tendency to hold
The last issue we would like to draw attention to is the cultural these beliefs.
specificity of the beliefs and the differences in the meaning of Whichever definition scholars are working under, we hope that
certain beliefs even within contemporary Western cultures. Some the so far miscellaneous operationalizations are in the future con-
extraordinary life forms (e.g., Bigfoot and the abominable snow- fined to a more concise set of contents, and that conceptual
man of Tibet) included in widely used questionnaires are not distinctions are more empirically justified than they have so far
necessarily familiar or considered paranormal everywhere. For been. We also hope that the so far separate research tracts could be
example, we thought that the Loch Ness Monster is simply a merged for a synthesis. Only by reaching such a synthesis can the
horrible animal until we learned from a helpful reviewer that field further our understanding of these extraordinary beliefs.
Nessie seems to have sprung directly from the water or been
dropped from the sky. Thus, depending both on the content of the
belief and the definition adopted, beliefs in Nessie may or may not References
demonstrate a PSMS belief. (Because children do not have uni-
Albas, D., & Albas, C. (1989). Modern magic: The case of examinations.
versal predispositions to understand animal reproduction and life
The Sociological Quarterly, 30, 603– 613. doi:0.1111/j.1533-
span, we prefer to refer to these kinds of creatures with concepts
8525.1989.tb01537.x
such as myths, legends, or folktales, but not as PSMS beliefs). Alcock, J. E. (1990). Science and supernature. A critical appraisal of
Furthermore, some beliefs might be dated, such as the belief that parapsychology. New York, NY: Prometheus Books.
a black cat crossing brings bad luck. On the other extreme, belief Amir, N., Freshman, M., Ramsey, B., Neary, E., & Brigidi, B. (2001).
in mind reading does not necessarily denote PSMS belief anymore, Thought-action fusion in individuals with OCD symptoms. Behaviour
as our informed participants familiar with recent brain imaging Research and Therapy, 39, 765–776. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967
studies (famously publicized as “brain reading”) have alerted us to. (00)00056-5
Whenever culture-specific beliefs are not the focus of the study, Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2005a). Religion’s evolutionary landscape:
we judge it advisable to replace culture-specific items (e.g., “Do Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral
you believe that the No. 13 is unlucky?”) with culture-free coun- and Brain Sciences, 27, 713–730. doi:10.1017/S0140525X04000172
Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2005b). Why minds create gods: Devotion,
terparts (e.g., “Do you believe that some numbers are unlucky?”).
deception, death, and arational decision making. Behavioral and Brain
This would minimize confounding variance in the dependent vari- Sciences, 27, 754 –770. doi:10.1017/S0140525X04470174
ables while preserving the gist of the items. Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 29 –34. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01419-9
Epilogue Barrett, J. L. (2008). Coding and quantifying counterintuitiveness in reli-
gious concepts: Theoretical and methodological reflections. Method and
The heterogeneity of research traditions may explain why the Theory in the Study of Religion, 20, 308 –338. doi:http://dx.doi.org/
psychological appeal of paranormal, magical, superstitious, and 10.1163/157006808X371806
supernatural beliefs is so poorly understood. However, we cannot Barrett, J. L., & Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading non-natural concepts: The
formulate theories about these beliefs and we cannot assess the role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of
cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 69 –100. doi:
beliefs with valid measures if we are not even in the clear on what
10.1163/156853701300063589
the object of research is. One major reason for lack of fertile theory
Beck, J., & Forstmeier, W. (2007). Superstition and belief as inevitable
formation on the origin and development of the beliefs is that the by-products of an adaptive learning strategy. Human Nature, 18, 35– 46.
meaning of the terms paranormal, superstitious, magical, and su- doi:10.1007/BF02820845
pernatural has not been brought into question. Concept definition Berenbaum, H., Boden, M. T., & Baker, J. P. (2009). Emotional salience,
is of particular importance also because these concepts bear on the emotional awareness, peculiar beliefs, and magical thinking. Emotion, 9,
scope and limits of science. As scientists we cannot subscribe to 197–205. doi:10.1037/a0015395
252 LINDEMAN AND SVEDHOLM

Bering, J. M. (2006). The folk psychology of souls. Behavioral and Brain Dudley, R. (2000). The relationship between negative affect and paranor-
Sciences, 29, 453– 462. doi:10.1017/S0140525X06009101 mal belief. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 315–321. doi:
Berle, D., & Starcevic, V. (2005). Thought–action fusion: Review of the 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00100-2
literature and future directions. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 263– Durkheim, E. (1912/1964). The elementary forms of the religious life.
284. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2004.12.001 London, UK: George Allen & Unwin.
Bjorklund, D., Blasi, C., & Periss, V. (2010). Lorenz revisited: The Eckblad, M., & Chapman, L. J. (1983). Magical ideation as an indicator of
adaptive nature of children’s supernatural thinking. Human Nature, 21, schizotypy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 215–
371–392. doi:10.1007/s12110-010-9099-8 225. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.51.2.215
Bleak, J., & Frederick, C. M. (1998). Superstitious behavior in sport: Einstein, D. A., & Menzies, R. G. (2004). The presence of magical thinking
Levels of effectiveness and determinants of use in three collegiate sports. in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42,
Journal of Sport Behavior, 21, 1–15. 539 –549. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00160-8
Bloom, P. (2007). Religion is natural. Developmental Science, 10, 147– Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment
151. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00577.x and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278. doi:
Bloom, P., & Weisberg, D. S. (2007). Childhood origins of adult resistance 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629
to science. Science, 316, 996 –997. doi:10.1126/science.1133398
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Flavell, J. H. (2004). Development of knowledge about vision In C. B.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bocci, L., & Gordon, P. K. (2007). Does magical thinking produce neu- Keasey (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and
tralising behaviour? An experimental investigation. Behaviour Research children. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. (Vol. 25, pp. 43–76).
and Therapy, 45, 1823–1833. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.02.003 Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained. The evolutionary origins of religious Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology.
thought. New York, NY: Basic Books. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Boyer, P. (2003). Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain Foster, K. R., & Kokko, H. (2009). The evolution of superstitious and
function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 119 –124. doi:10.1016/S1364- superstition-like behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biolog-
6613(03)00031-7 ical Sciences, 276, 31–37. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0981
Broad, C. D. (1953). Religion, philosophy, and psychical research. New Frazer, J. G. (1922/1963). The golden bough. A study in magic and
York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & Co. religion. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Brugger, P., & Graves, R. E. (1997). Testing vs. believing hypotheses:
Freeman, T. P., Morgan, C. J. A., Klaassen, E., Das, R. K., Stefanovic, A.,
Magical ideation in the judgment of contingencies. Cognitive Neuropsy-
Brandner, B., & Curran, H. V. (2009). Superstitious conditioning as a
chiatry, 2, 251–272. doi:10.1080/135468097396270
model of delusion formation following chronic but not acute ketamine in
Campbell, C. (1996). Half-belief and the paradox of ritual instrumental
humans. Psychopharmacology, 206, 563–573. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-
activism: A theory of modern superstition. E-journal of British Journal
1564-x
of Sociology, 47, 151–166. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/
Gallagher, C., Kumar, V. K., & Pekala, R. J. (1994). The Anomalous
591121
Experiences Inventory: Reliability and validity. Journal of Parapsychol-
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. London, UK: MIT
ogy, 58, 402– 428.
Press.
Gbadamosi, G. (2005). Ritualism, symbolism and magic in consultancy
Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and concep-
practice: An exploratory investigation. Management Decision, 43,
tual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the
1129 –1146. doi:10.1108/00251740510626236
mind. Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 169 –200).
Gianotti, L. R. R., Mohr, C., Pizzagalli, A., Lehmann, D., & Brugger, P.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
(2001). Associative processing and paranormal beliefs. Psychiatry and
CBO9780511752902.008
Case, T. I., Fitness, J., Cairns, D. R., & Stevenson, R. J. (2004). Coping Clinical Neurosciences, 55, 595– 603. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1819
with uncertainty: Superstitious strategies and secondary control. Journal .2001.00911.x
of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 848 – 871. doi:10.1111/j.1559- Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds. New York, NY: Oxford University
1816.2004.tb02574.x Press.
Chi, M. T. H., & Hausmann, R. G. M. (2003). Do radical discoveries Haselton, M., & Nettle, D. (2006). The paranoid optimist: An integrative
require ontological shifts? In L. V. Shavinina (Ed.), International hand- evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychol-
book on innovation (Vol. 3, pp. 430 – 444). Dordrecht, Holland: Elsevier ogy Review, 10, 47– 66. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_3
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York, NY: Columbia Hergovich, A. (2003). Field dependence, suggestibility and belief in para-
University Press. normal phenomena. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 195–
Ciborowski, T. (1997). “Superstition” in the collegiate baseball player. 209. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00022-3
Sport Psychologist, 11, 305–317. Hines, T. (1988). Pseudoscience and the paranormal: A critical examina-
Dag, I. (1999). The relationships among paranormal beliefs, locus of tion of the evidence. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.
control and psychopathology in a Turkish college sample. Personality Hodgkinson, G. P., Langan-Fox, J., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2008). Intuition: A
and Individual Differences, 26, 723–737. doi:10.1016/S0191- fundamental bridging construct in the behavioural sciences. British
8869(98)00184-6 Journal of Psychology, 99, 1–27. doi:10.1348/000712607X216666
Damisch, L., Stoberock, B., & Mussweiler, T. (2010). Keep your fingers Homer, B. D., & Nelson, K. (2005). Seeing objects as symbols and
crossed! Psychological Science, 21, 1014 –1020. doi:10.1177/ symbols as objects: Language and the development of dual representa-
0956797610372631 tion. In B. D. Homer & C. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), The development of
Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. social cognition and communication (pp. 29 –52). Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Per- baum.
sonality and Individual Differences, 50, 1289 –1293. doi:10.1016/ Hood, B. M. (2009). SuperSense: From superstition to religion- the brain
j.paid.2011.02.027 science of belief. London, UK: Constable and Robinson.
Dudley, R. (1999). The effect of superstitious belief on performance Irwin, H. J. (2009). The psychology of paranormal. A researcher’s hand-
following an unsolvable problem. Personality and Individual Differ- book. Hertfordshire, UK: University of Hertfordshire Press.
ences, 26, 1057–1064. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00209-8 Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for
DEFINING PSMS BELIEFS 253

language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367–371. doi: nal of Experimental Psychology, Section B, 48, 142–157. doi:10.1080/
10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01542.x 14640749508401444
Jahoda, G. (1970). The psychology of superstition. Oxford, UK: Penguin. Matute, H., Yarritu, I., & Vadillo, M. A. (2011). Illusions of causality at the
Johnson, D. D. P. (2005). God’s punishment and public goods: A test of the heart of pseudoscience. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 392– 405.
supernatural punishment hypothesis in 186 world cultures. Human Na- doi:10.1348/000712610X532210
ture, 16, 410 – 446. doi:10.1007/s12110-005-1017-0 Mauss, M. (1902/1972). A general theory of magic. London, UK: Rout-
Kalish, C. (1999). What young children’s understanding of contamination ledge & Kegan Paul.
and contagion tells us about their concepts of illness. In M. Siegal & McGarry, J. J., & Newberry, B. H. (1981). Beliefs in paranormal phenom-
C. C. Peterson (Eds.), Children’s understanding of biology and health ena and locus of control: A field study. Journal of Personality and Social
(pp. 99 –130). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi: Psychology, 41, 725–736. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.41.4.725
10.1017/CBO9780511659881.006 Merriam-Webster’s Dictionary of Synonyms. (n.d.). Introductory matter.
Kane, M. J., Core, T. J., & Hunt, R. R. (2010). Bias versus bias: Harnessing Survey of the history of English synonyms. Retrieved from http://
hindsight to reveal paranormal belief change beyond demand character- www.merriam-webster.com/cgi-bin/book.pl?synonym.htm&2
istics. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 206 –212. doi:10.3758/ Morales, A. C., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2007). Product contagion: Changing
PBR.17.2.206 consumer evaluations through physical contact with “disgusting” prod-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological ucts. Journal of Marketing Research, 44, 272–283. doi:10.1509/
perspective. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. jmkr.44.2.272
Keinan, G. (1994). Effects of stress and tolerance of ambiguity on magical Mowen, J. C., & Carlson, B. (2003). Exploring the antecedents and
thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 48 –55. consumer behavior consequences of the trait of superstition. Psychology
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.1.48 and Marketing, 20, 1045–1065. doi:10.1002/mar.10108
Keinan, G. (2002). The effects of stress and desire for control on super- Musch, J., & Ehrenberg, K. (2002). Probability misjudgment, cognitive
stitious behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 102– ability, and belief in the paranormal. British Journal of Psychology, 93,
108. doi:10.1177/0146167202281009 169 –178. doi:10.1348/000712602162517
Kelemen, D., & Rosset, E. (2009). The human function compunction: Nemeroff, C., & Rozin, P. (2000). The makings of the magical mind. The
Teleological explanation in adults. Cognition, 111, 138 –143. doi: nature and function of sympathetic magical thinking. In K. Rosengren,
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.001 C. Johnson, & P. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the impossible: The develop-
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Psychological Sci- ment of magical, scientific, and religious thinking in contemporary
ence, 15, 295–301. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00672.x society (pp. 1–34). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:
Kieckhefer, R. (1989). Magic in the middle ages. Cambridge, UK: Cam- 10.1017/CBO9780511571381.002
bridge University Press. Ng, T., Chong, T., & Du, X. (2010). The value of superstitions. Journal of
King, L. A., Burton, C. M., Hicks, J. A., & Drigotas, S. M. (2007). Ghosts, Economic Psychology, 31, 293–309. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.12.002
UFOs, and magic: Positive affect and the experiential system. Journal of Norenzayan, A., Atran, S., Faulkner, J., & Schaller, M. (2006). Memory
Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 905–919. doi:10.1037/0022- and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintui-
3514.92.5.905 tive narratives. Cognitive Science, 30, 531–553. doi:10.1207/
Kurtz, P. (1992). The new skepticism. New York, NY: Prometheus Books. s15516709cog0000_68
Landrine, H., & Klonoff, E. A. (1994). Cultural diversity in causal attri- Otis, L. P., & Alcock, J. E. (1982). Factors affecting extraordinary belief.
butions for illness: The role of the supernatural. Journal of Behavioral Journal of Social Psychology, 118, 77– 85. doi:10.1080/00224545
Medicine, 17, 181–193. doi:10.1007/BF01858104 .1982.9924420
Legare, C. H., & Gelman, S. A. (2008). Bewitchment, biology, or both: Paranormal. (n.d.). In Oxford English dictionary. Retrieved from http://
The co-existence of natural and supernatural explanatory frameworks www.oed.com
across development. Cognitive Science, 32, 607– 642. doi:10.1080/ Parida, G. (1962). Superstitions among college students in India. Journal of
03640210802066766 Social Psychology, 57, 3–10. doi:10.1080/00224545.1962.9710898
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1962/2004). The savage mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford Parris, B. A., Kuhn, G., Mizon, G. A., Benattayallah, A., & Hodgson, T. L.
University Press. (2009). Imaging the impossible: An fMRI study of impossible causal
Lindeman, M., & Aarnio, K. (2007). Superstitious, magical, and paranor- relationships in magic tricks. Neuroimage, 45, 1033–1039. doi:10.1016/
mal beliefs: An integrative model. Journal of Research in Personality, j.neuroimage.2008.12.036
41, 731–744. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.06.009 Peirce, C. S. (1955). Logic as semiotic: The theory of signs. In J. Buchler
Lindeman, M., Cederström, S., Simola, P., Simula, A., Ollikainen, S., & (Ed.), Philosophical writings of Peirce (pp. 98 –110). New York, NY:
Riekki, T. (2008). Sentences with core knowledge violations increase the Dover Publications.
size of N400 among paranormal believers. Cortex, 44, 1307–1315. Peltzer, K. (2003). Magical thinking and paranormal beliefs among sec-
doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.07.010 ondary and university students in South Africa. Personality and Indi-
Lindeman, M., & Saher, M. (2007). Vitalism, purpose and superstition. vidual Differences, 35, 1419 –1426. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)
British Journal of Psychology, 98, 33– 44. doi:10.1348/ 00359-8
000712606X101808 Peltzer, K., & Renner, W. (2003). Superstition, risk-taking and risk per-
MacDonald, W. L. (1995). The effects of religiosity and structural strain on ception of accidents among South African taxi drivers. Accident Analysis
reported paranormal experiences. E-journal of Journal for the Scientific & Prevention, 35, 619 – 623. doi:10.1016/S0001-4575(02)00035-0
Study of Religion, 34, 366 –377. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/ Piaget, J. (1929/1951). The child’s conception of the world. London, UK:
stable/1386885 Routledge & Kegan.
Malinowski, B. (1948/1992). Magic, science and religion. Prospect Pronin, E., Wegner, D. M., McCarthy, K., & Rodriguez, S. (2006). Every-
Heights, IL: Waveland Press. day magical powers: The role of apparent mental causation in the
Marks, D. F. (1986). Investigating the paranormal. Nature, 320, 119 –124. overestimation of personal influence. Journal of Personality and Social
doi:10.1038/320119a0 Psychology, 91, 218 –231. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.2.218
Matute, H. (1995). Human reactions to uncontrollable outcomes: Further Pyysiäinen, I. (2004). Magic, miracles, and religion. Walnut Creek, CA:
evidence for superstitions rather than helplessness. The Quarterly Jour- AltaMira Press.
254 LINDEMAN AND SVEDHOLM

Rassin, E., Merckelbach, H., Muris, P., & Schmidt, H. (2001). The Subbotsky, E. (2001). Causal explanations of events by children and adults:
thought-action fusion scale: Further evidence for its reliability and Can alternative causal modes coexist in one mind? British Journal of
validity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 537–544. doi:10.1016/ Developmental Psychology, 19, 23– 45. doi:10.1348/026151001165949
S0005-7967(00)00031-0 Subbotsky, E. (2004). Magical thinking in judgments of causation: Can
Rees, C. S., Draper, M., & Davis, M. C. (2010). The relationship between anomalous phenomena affect ontological causal beliefs in children and
magical thinking, thought-action fusion and obsessive-compulsive adults? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 123–152.
symptoms. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3, 304 –311. doi:10.1348/026151004772901140
doi:10.1521/ijct.2010.3.3.304 Supernatural. (n.d.). In Oxford English dictionary. Retrieved from http://
Rice, T. (2003). Believe it or not: Religious and other paranormal beliefs www.oed.com
in the United States. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42, Talmont-Kaminski, K. (2009). Effective untestability and bounded ratio-
95–106. doi:10.1111/1468-5906.00163 nality help in seeing religion as adaptive misbelief. Behavioral and
Roberts, M. J., & Seager, P. B. (1999). Predicting belief in paranormal Brain Sciences, 32, 536 –537. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999135X
phenomena: A comparison of conditional and probabilistic reasoning. Thalbourne, M. A., & Delin, P. S. (1993). A new instrument for measuring
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 443– 450. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099- Sheep-Goat variable: Its psychometric properties and factor structure.
0720(199910)13:5⬍443::AID-ACP592⬎3.0.CO;2-K Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 59, 172–186.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Robinson, E. J., Haigh, S. N., & Pendle, J. E. C. (2008). Children’s Tobacyk, J. (1983). Cognitive complexity and paranormal beliefs.
working understanding of the knowledge gained from seeing and feel- E-journal of Psychological Reports, 52, 101–102. Retrieved from http://
ing. Developmental Science, 11, 299 –305. doi:10.1111/j.1467- psycnet.apa.org/PsycINFO/1983–32745-001
7687.2008.00676.x Tobacyk, J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. E-journal of Inter-
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world national Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 23, 23, 94 –98. Retrieved
and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. Jour- from http://www.transpersonalstudies.org/ImagesRepository/ijts/
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5–37. doi:10.1037/0022- Downloads/
3514.42.1.5 Tobacyk, J., & Milford, G. (1983). Belief in paranormal phenomena:
Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of Assessment instrument development and implications for personality
sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1029 –
and Social Psychology, 50, 703–712. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.4.703 1037. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.5.1029
Rozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. (1990). The laws of sympathetic magic: A Tykocinski, O. E. (2008). Insurance, risk, and magical thinking. Person-
psychological analysis of similarity and contagion. In J. W. Stigler & ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1346 –1356. doi:10.1177/
R. A. Shweder (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative 0146167208320556
human development (pp. 205–232). New York, NY: Cambridge Univer- Tylor, E. B. (1871/1958). The origins of culture. New York, NY: Harper &
sity Press. Row.
Rozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. J. (2002). Sympathetic magical thinking: The Tylor, E. B. (1871/1974). Primitive culture: Research into the development
contagion and similarity “heuristics.” In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom. New York, NY:
Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive Gordon Press.
judgment (pp. 201–216). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Upala, M. A., Gonce, L. O., Tweney, R. D., & Slone, D. J. (2007).
Rudski, J. M., Lischner, M. I., & Albert, L. M. (1999). Superstitious rule Contextualizing counterintuitiveness: How context affects comprehen-
generation is affected by probability and type of outcome. E-journal of sion and memorability of counterintuitive concepts. Cognitive Science,
The Psychological Record, 49, 245–246. Retrieved from http:// 31, 415– 439. doi:10.1080/15326900701326568
web.ebscohost.com/ehost Van Raalte, J. L., Brewer, B. W., Nemeroff, C. J., & Linder, D. E. (1991).
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London, UK: Hutchinson. Chance orientation and superstitious behavior on the putting green.
Scheibe, K. E., & Sarbin, T. R. (1965). Towards a theoretical conceptu- Journal of Sport Behavior, 14, 41–50.
alisation of superstition. E-journal of British Journal for the Philosophy Vyse, S. A. (1997). Believing in magic: The psychology of superstition.
of Science, 16, 143–158. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/ New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
686154 Wain, O., & Spinella, M. (2007). Executive functions in morality, religion,
Schippers, M. C., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2006). The psychological and paranormal beliefs. International Journal of Neuroscience, 117,
benefits of superstitious rituals in top sport: A study among top 135–146. doi:10.1080/00207450500534068
sportspersons. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 2532–2553. Weeks, M., Weeks, K. P., & Daniel, M. R. (2008). The implicit relation-
doi:10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00116.x ship between religious and paranormal constructs. Journal for the Sci-
Schulter, G., & Papousek, I. (2008). Believing in paranormal phenomena: entific Study of Religion, 47, 599 – 611. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906
Relations to asymmetry of body and brain. Cortex, 44, 1326 –1335. .2008.00429.x
doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.022 Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in
Sharps, M. J., Matthews, J., & Asten, J. (2006). Cognition and belief in foundational domains. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychol-
paranormal phenomena: Gestalt/feature-intensive processing theory and ogy. Cognition, perception, and language (Vol. 2, pp. 523–573). New
tendencies toward ADHD, depression, and dissociation. The Journal of York, NY: Wiley.
Psychology, 140, 579 –590. doi:10.3200/JRLP.140.6.579-590 Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases
Shweder, R. A. (1977). Likeness and likelihood in everyday thought: illusory pattern perception. Science, 322, 115–117. doi:10.1126/
Magical thinking in judgments about personality. E-journal of Current science.1159845
Anthropology, 18, 637– 658. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/ Wimmer, H., Hogrefe, G. J., & Perner, J. (1988). Children’s understanding
openurl of informational access as source of knowledge. Child Development, 59,
Skinner, B. F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimen- 386 –396. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/pss/1130318. doi:
tal Psychology, 38, 168 –172. doi:10.1037/h0055873 10.2307/1130318
Sperber, D. (1990). The epidemiology of beliefs. In C. Fraser & G. Gaskell Wiseman, R., & Watt, C. (2004). Measuring superstitious belief: Why
(Eds.), The social psychological study of widespread beliefs (pp. 25– 44). lucky charms matter. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1533–
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 1541. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.009
DEFINING PSMS BELIEFS 255

Wohl, M. J. A., & Enzle, M. E. (2002). The deployment of personal luck: Zebb, B., & Moore, M. (2003). Superstitiousness and perceived anxiety
Sympathetic magic and illusory control in games of pure chance. Per- control as predictors of psychological distress. Journal of Anxiety Dis-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1388 –1397. doi:10.1177/ orders, 17, 115–130. doi:10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00176-7
014616702236870 Zusne, L., & Jones, W. H. (1989). Anomalistic psychology: A study of
Womack, M. (1992). Why athletes need ritual: A study of magic among magical thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
professional athletes. In S. J. Hoffman (Ed.), Sport and religion (pp.
191–202). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetic Books.
Yorulmaz, O., Inozu, M., & Gultepe, B. (2010). The role of magical
thinking in obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms and cognitions in Received November 12, 2011
an analogue sample. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Revision received January 9, 2012
Psychiatry, 42, 198 –203. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.11.007 Accepted January 9, 2012 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

You might also like