You are on page 1of 19

Understanding Empathy

Keith Tudor

Abstract by means of which we are enabled to take up


This article considers different understand- any attitude at all towards another mental life”
ings of empathy in psychotherapy and trans- (p. 110). In his use of the term, Freud (1905/
actional analysis, including Carl Rogers’s 1976, 1916/1973) drew on the work of another
contribution to the development and under- German philosopher, Theodor Lipps (1851-
standing of empathy. A review of his neces- 1913), who had transferred the term from aes-
sary and sufficient conditions for therapeutic thetics to psychology.
change clarifies the bilateral and intersubjec- Understanding and Explanation. In psycholo-
tive nature of empathic understanding, em- gy, empathy stands in a tradition that seeks to
pathy, and empathic transactions. Various understand rather than to explain. Johann Droy-
aspects of empathy are elaborated in terms sen (1804-1884), the German historian and
of Stark’s (1999) taxonomy of psychologies, politician, was the first to contrast explanation
and a fourth, two-person- plus psychology, (Erklären) with understanding (Verstehen) (see
is proposed to reflect an empathy that is Van Belle, 2005). This distinction, and the ela-
sociocentric rather than egocentric. boration of Verstehen, is also found in the work
_____ of the German philosopher W ilhelm Dilthey
(1833-1911), who argued for a descriptive and
A Brief History of Empathy analytic psychology. Karl Jaspers, the German
Aesthetics. Empathy has a long history in psychiatrist and existential philosopher, based
aesthetics, dating back to the eighteenth cen- his work on psychopathology (Jaspers, 1913/
tury, and, subsequently, in psychology, psycho- 1963) on this distinction, which, for him, pro-
analysis, and therapy (a term I use to encom- vided an organizing principle for nosology:
pass psychotherapy, counseling, and counseling “The most profound distinction in psychic life
psychology). The term Einfühlung, meaning seems to be that between what is meaningful
aesthetic sympathy and later translated into and allows empathy and what in its particular
English as empathy, was first used in print in way is un-understandable, ‘mad’ in the literal
1873 by the German philosopher Robert Visch- sense” (p. 577). Jaspers’s acceptance of the dis-
er (1847-1933) to designate the projection of tinction between affective disorders and schizo-
human feeling onto the natural world. The term phrenia was, in part, based on the distinction
was and is used to describe aesthetic experi- between those conditions with which one could
ence both in and in response to various art empathize and those that are less or not under-
forms (see Hunsdahl, 1967), an experience and standable. The difference between explanation
process that acknowledges that our emotional and understanding underpins the concept of em-
responses to a piece or form of art are, in ef- pathic understanding in person-centered theory
fect, empathic projections onto an object, a and therapy and distinguishes it from other
work, an event, or even a person. Indeed, in his therapies that seek to explain, analyze, and/or
work on jokes and their relation to the uncon- interpret. Given his training and interest in psy-
scious, Freud (1905/1976) used empathy in this choanalysis, Eric Berne’s own writings reflect
sense of aesthetic sympathy. However, Pigman the tradition of explanation. It is only in the
(1995) has argued that empathy (Einfühlung) past 20 years that a few writers within trans-
played a greater role in Freud’s thinking than is actional analysis have developed the theory and
conveyed in the Standard Edition which does practice of understanding through empathy (see
not translate Einfühlung as empathy in a clinical later in this article). Although both explanation
context (e.g., Freud 1916/1973). Later, Freud and understanding are important in therapy, the
(1920/1955) described empathy as “a mechanism distinction between them informs much thinking

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 39

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

about empathy: Rogers (1951), for instance, based on the work of therapists representing
distinguished between an empathic and a de- three different theoretical orientations (Fiedler,
clarative attitude on the part of the counselor. 1949, 1950), Rogers (1951), in his first explicit
Empathy in Different Theoretical Orienta- reference to the concept of empathy, suggested
tions. Empathy appeared in Jaspers’s existen- that the findings offered “outstanding corro-
tial approach to psychiatry and psychology and boration of empathy and complete understand-
in his work on General Psychopathology (Jas- ing on the part of the therapist” (p. 54). Later in
pers, 1913/1963), in which he emphasized it as the same work, Rogers placed empathy along-
a criterion for diagnosing delusions. Theorists side warmth (later, unconditional positive re-
and clinicians from other schools of psycholo- gard) as two behavioral characteristics that are
gy and psychotherapy have also used and devel- important in creating “a nonthreatening, accept-
oped the concept, most notably Rogers (1951, ing atmosphere” (p. 348), a reference that pre-
1959, 1975/1980a) in client- or person-centered dated his later (Rogers, 1957, 1959) work iden-
psychology; Kohut (1959, 1982) in self psy- tifying six necessary and sufficient conditions
chology; and Jacobson (1964) and W innicott for growth and personality change.
(1965) in object relations theory. More recent- In the seminal paper in which he outlined his
ly, therapists within cognitive behavioral thera- theory of therapy, personality, and interperson-
py have been addressing the literature on em- al relationships, Rogers (1959) defined empath-
pathy and developing their own conceptuali- ic understanding this way: “To perceive the
zation of the nature and function of therapeutic internal frame of reference of another with ac-
empathy (e.g., Thwaites & Bennett-Levy, 2007). curacy, and with the emotional components and
The contributions of a number of theoretical meanings which pertain thereto, as if one were
orientations to the reconsideration of empathy the other person, but without ever losing the ‘as
have been brought together in a useful volume if’ condition” (p. 210). The therapist’s empath-
edited by Bohart and Greenberg (1997). Nowa- ic understanding of the client seems to be help-
days, empathy is perhaps most associated with ful and potent in two ways. The first is that it
self psychology, and its views on empathy seem helps clients to identify, clarify, and then sym-
to receive more attention in theory, practice, bolize or find words for the nuances of their
and teaching/training than do those of Rogers. own experience. T his is consistent with what
In the rest of this part of the article, I review Rogers (1951) wrote about diagnosis: “The
Rogers’s writings on empathy and the develop- purpose of the therapist is to provide the con-
ment of his ideas as well as those of others ditions in which the client is able to make, to
within the person-centered approach, after experience, and to accept the diagnosis of the
which I compare and contrast them with those psychogenic aspects of his maladjustment” (p.
of Kohut. 223). Elsewhere, Rogers (1975/1980a) put it
this way: “True empathy is always free of any
Carl Rogers on Empathy evaluative or diagnostic quality” (p. 154). The
Rogers’s first reference to empathy was in second way in which empathy is powerful
his book Client-Centered Therapy (Rogers, follows from this: A client who feels herself or
1951), although in his first book, Counseling himself accurately understood and still ac-
and Psychotherapy (Rogers, 1942), he did de- cepted feels less alienated, less alone, and more
fine the basic hypothesis of this (then) “newer related to another human being.
approach to psychotherapy” in terms of the ef- The term empathic understanding may be
fect on the client: “Effective counseling con- confusing. In person-centered literature it is
sists of a definitely structured, permissive rela- often used synonymously with empathy and is
tionship which allows the client to gain an un- generally more often used by Rogers in his
derstanding of himself to a degree which ena- writings on the subject (Rogers, 1951, 1959,
bles him to take positive steps in the light of his 1961/1967b, 1975/1980a). It carries the conno-
new orientation” (p. 18). Later, commenting on tation, however, that empathy is only or pre-
research into the ideal therapeutic relationship dominantly a cognitive process. Others since

40 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

have used a number of descriptors to extend the in harmony with” the client but also in the
concept of empathy beyond cognition or sense of amplifying on something the client has
implied cognition. One such term is visceral said. Although similar to attunement, the differ-
empathy, a phrase that I have been using for ence between resonance and attunement is per-
some 15 years to describe the particular and haps one of directionality: Attunement suggests
often specific somatic sensation experienced in that the therapist attunes to the client; reso-
response to the client and, often quite specifi- nance suggests that the therapist picks up some-
cally, to something she or he is reporting or thing from the client.
disclosing. Such somatic experiencing can be In another description, Rogers (1954/1967c)
helpful in attuning to the client, but it can also defined empathy in the context of acceptance:
be disturbing; either way, the therapist needs to If I say that I “accept” you, but know noth-
monitor such responses and to process their ing of you, this is a shallow acceptance in-
meaning and effects (see Rothschild with Rand, deed, and you realize that it may change if
2008). Cooper (2001) referred to the term em- I actually come to know you. But if I un-
bodied empathy to describe the therapist’s em- derstand you empathically, see you and
bodied attunement, in which the therapist is what you are feeling and doing from your
resonating with “the complex, gestalt-like mo- point of view, enter your private world and
saic of her client’s embodied being . . . [in see it as it appears to you— and still accept
which] the whole of the therapist’s body is you— then this is safety indeed. In this
alive in the interaction, moving and vibrating in climate you can permit your real self to
tandem with the client’s experiencing” (p. 223). emerge, and to express itself in varied and
In transactional analysis, Cornell (Cornell & novel formings as it relates to the world.
Landaiche, 2007) has used the term somatic (p. 358)
resonance to describe something similar. In the same year that Rogers (1959) pub-
The term empathic attunement is sometimes lished the major formulation of his theory,
used synonymously with empathy but, in my Kohut published his paper on “Introspection,
view, confusingly so. More precisely, attune- Empathy, and Psychoanalysis.” In it he defined
ment refers to the therapist’s experience of at- empathy as “vicarious introspection,” by which
tunement and is differentiated from the commu- he meant that it is only through introspection in
nications that flow from that attunement. Some our own experience that we can learn and know
theorists and practitioners, such as Stern (1985) what it might be like for another person in a
and Erskine, Moursund, and Trautmann (1999), similar psychological situation or circumstance.
distinguish between affect attunement and the Later, Kohut (1984) defined empathy as “the
cognitive processes involved in empathy. Ers- capacity to think and feel oneself into the inner
kine and Trautmann (1996) view attunement as life of another person” (p. 82), a definition that
a two-part process involving empathy and the is similar to Rogers’s (1961/1967b) description:
communication of that sensitivity (see later sec- [Empathy occurs] when the therapist is
tion on “Empathy in Transactional Analysis”). sensing the feelings and personal meanings
Attunement requires the therapist to “tune” in which the client is experiencing in each
to or resonate with the client. O’Leary’s (1993) moment, when he can perceive these from
description of empathy caught something of “inside,” as they seem to the client, and
this when he likened empathy to two tuning when he can successfully communicate
forks tuned in the same key: “When one is something of that understanding to his
struck the other picks up the sound emitted by client. (p. 62)
the first while losing nothing of its own essen- Nearly 25 years after his first reference to
tial nature. Empathy is tuning into the wave- empathy, Rogers returned to the subject. Keil
length of the client. Counsellors must attune (1996) suggested that this was because of Rog-
themselves to that particular wavelength” (p. ers’s concerns about misunderstandings and
113). Bohart and Rosenbaum (1995) used the distortions concerning his conception of em-
term in a similar sense of the therapist “vibrating pathy. In an article published in 1975, Rogers

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 41

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

(1975/1980a) reflected that “I would no longer used— that is, the therapist’s or the client’s—
be terming it a ‘state of empathy,’ because I be- and on the degree of new information that is
lieve it to be a process, rather than a state” (p. given.
142). He continued: Rogers (1980c) referred to his person-
An empathic way of being with another centered psychology as an approach (see also
person has several facets. . . . It involves Embleton Tudor, Keemar, Tudor, Valentine, &
being sensitive, moment by moment, to the W orrall, 2004; W ood, 2008) and, indeed, as A
changing felt meanings which flow in the Way of Being (Rogers, 1980c). Interestingly, in
other person. . . . It means temporarily liv- the second edition of his book The Interperson-
ing in the other’s life, moving about it deli- al World of the Infant, Stern (1998) renamed
cately without making judgments; it means what he had previously referred to as “repre-
sensing meanings of which he or she is sentations of interactions that are generalized”
scarcely aware. . . . It includes communi- (or RIGs) (Stern, 1985) as “ways-of-being-
cating your sensings of the person’s world with.” He argued that by doing so he was “de-
as you look with fresh and unfrightened emphasizing the process of formation in favor
eyes at elements of which he or she is fear- of describing the lived phenomenon in a more
ful. (p. 142) experience-near and clinically useful way”
This represents a shift in Rogers’s (1959) think- (Stern, 1998, p. xv). This prefigures and is con-
ing about empathy as an “as if” state/attitude of sistent with his more recent interest in the
the therapist to a description that emphasizes a present moment in psychotherapy and everyday
process between therapist and client (1975/ life (Stern, 2004) and has informed interest in
1980a), one that includes the therapist going the present in cocreative transactional analysis
beyond the experiencing of the client. (This (see Summers & Tudor, 2000, 2005; Tudor,
shift parallels a similar change in Rogers’s ap- 2003, in press).
proach to congruence as identified by Frankel Almost every approach to psychotherapy
and Sommerbeck, 2005, and referred to as claims the therapist’s empathy as central to its
“Rogers I” and “Rogers II.”) effectiveness, and research in neuroscience
Other person-centered practitioners and theo- supports the development of empathy in sup-
rists have developed Rogers’s ideas on empa- porting limbic resonance. However, with rare
thy. Those within the cognitive tradition of exceptions, neither therapists from other orien-
person-centered psychology coined the term tations nor neuroscientists acknowledge the
“empathic responding” in order to stress, as work of Rogers or other person-centered theo-
W exler (1974) put it, “the fact that [empathy] rists in the development of their understanding
is not merely an attitude but a consistent style of this most important human quality and thera-
of behaviors given as responses to the client” peutic attitude. This is due, I think, to four
(p. 96). In this tradition, an empathic response factors:
is a deliberate and organized response to the 1. There is a general view that Rogerian and
client: person-centered thinking and practice is
W hen it is optimal, an empathic response basic and (too) simple.
is a structure or group of structures that 2. There has been a widespread association
more fully captures, and better organizes, of Rogerian and person-centered practice
the meaning of the information in the field with counseling (only) rather than psy-
that the client is processing than had the chotherapy, a distinction that has more to
structure(s) the client had generated him- do with the organization and politics of
self. (p. 97) the activity/profession then any essential
Greenberg and Elliot (1997) expanded this difference (see Tudor, 1997) and one that
and identified five distinct forms of empathic generally is not held within the person-
responding: understanding, evocation, explora- centered approach.
tion, conjecture, and interpretation. These vary 3. There have been many misunderstandings
depending on whose frame of reference is being of and misconceptions about client-centered

42 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

therapy and the person-centered approach that places empathy rather than interpretation at
(see, for instance, Miller, 1989, discussed the center of its method and practice. In this
on page 46 of this article). These have way, self psychology has helped to reclaim and
been identified and addressed by a num- relocate the therapeutic and curative role of
ber of authors (see Barrett-Lennard, 1983; empathy in the wider fields of psychoanalysis
Mearns & Thorne, 2000; Tudor & Merry, and psychotherapy. Furthermore, Kohut’s work
2002; W ilkins, 2003). is generally viewed as providing a bridge be-
4. A certain intellectual isolationism has ex- tween psychoanalysis and humanistic psycholo-
isted whereby practitioners and theorists gy and psychotherapy. From a person-centered
read within and develop their own par- perspective, this is a welcome development,
ticular field but do not necessarily read and, indeed, much of the writing in this field
much outside it or across theoretical echoes Rogers’s own writings and even phrases.
orientations. The result is that they can The contribution of Rogers and other person-
become somewhat parochial in their out- centered theorists to the understanding and
look and misinformed about theories, ide- development of empathy, however, was entire-
as, and developments in other fields and ly unacknowledged by Kohut himself and rare-
approaches. ly acknowledged or referenced by other self
psychologists. Tobin (1991) reminded us that
Heinz Kohut and Carl Rogers Rogers and Kohut were both at the University
Heinz Kohut, at least in his early writings, of Chicago between 1945 and 1957, and, while
viewed empathy as more informative than cura- there is no record of the two men meeting,
tive. Indeed, Stolorow, Atwood, and Brand- Elizabeth Kohut, a psychologist and Kohut’s
chaft (1992) used the term “empathic inquiry” wife, did have some contact with Rogers— and
(p. 2) precisely to emphasize Kohut’s (1959) I, for one, cannot believe that Kohut did not
original conceptualization of analytic empathy know of Rogers’s work, especially on this sub-
as a unique investigatory stance. This does not ject. Tobin also commented that, in his work,
mean that the therapist is asking questions; Kohut referred to few other writers who influ-
rather, it means that she or he is facilitating the enced him, so his lack of acknowledgment of
unfolding and illuminating of the client’s sub- Rogers may not have been a specific lapse or
jective world. Stolorow (1993) suggested that slight.
the term “affective responsiveness” should be Less understandable, however, is the contin-
applied to Kohut’s depiction of empathy as a ued, almost willful lack of sourcing, citing, and
powerful emotional bond in order to distin- referencing of Rogers’s work among more re-
guish between the two aspects of Kohut’s view cent self psychologists. In one volume edited
and definition of empathy. Stolorow clarified by Jackson (1991), only one contributor (Don-
the relationship between these two aspects as ner, 1991) quoted Rogers on empathy (and then
“an essential ingredient of the analyst’s attitude only once), and another (Ornstein, 1991) man-
of empathic inquiry is his commitment continu- aged to discuss acceptance and understanding
ally to investigate the meaning of his affective without citing or referring to Rogers at all. Cor-
responsiveness, or its absence, for the patient” nell and Bonds-W hite (2001) perpetuated the
(p. 32). Erskine, Moursund, and Trautmann same mistake when they stated unequivocally
picked up on this aspect of empathy when they that “the centrality of an empathic stance in
referred to inquiry as a central skill in effective psychotherapy has emerged largely from the
psychotherapy (Erskine et al., 1999; Moursund work of Kohut and other self-psychologists” (p.
& Erskine, 2004). 78).
In general terms, self psychology describes In 1986 Rogers wrote an article in which he
any approach to psychology that places the self offered his own perspective on some of the simi-
as the central concept in its theory. More spe- larities and differences between himself, Kohut,
cifically, it refers to a development in psy- and Erickson. He acknowledged that he shared
chology that has its roots in psychoanalysis but with Kohut similar ideas about fundamental

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 43

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

human nature, which was, for Kohut, assertive of human beings; about growth and the out-
and affectionate, and about the self and the come of therapy; in their theories about change
restructuring of the self through therapy. It is or cure, disturbance, and transference; and in
also clear that Kohut’s view of empathy as research methodology and therapeutic method
“experience-near” observation equates with (see Table 1).
Rogers’s “as if” definition of empathy. Rogers Regarding empathy, the principle difference
cited Kohut’s (1978) definition of empathy is that, while Rogers and most person-centered
approvingly: “Empathy, the accepting, confirm- practitioners view the therapist’s empathy as, in
ing, and understanding human echo evoked by itself, curative, Kohut (1982) viewed empathy,
the self, is a psychological nutrient without initially at least, as a “mode of observation, at-
which human life as we know and cherish it, tuned to the inner life of man” (p. 396). In this
could not be sustained” (p. 705). Rogers, how- sense, empathy is an investigative stance that
ever, took issue with Kohut on several points: constitutes the “quintessence of psychoanalysis”
1. Rogers pointed out Kohut’s lack of trust (p. 398); in other words, it is an information-
in the actualizing tendency, citing Ko- gathering activity undertaken in order that the
hut’s (1981) paper in which he talked therapist can make good interpretations. In
about his view that the analyst cures by effect, these differences represent the philo-
giving explanations sophical and historical difference between un-
2. There are significant differences with re- derstanding (Verstehen) and explanation (Erk-
gard to therapeutic intent. For Rogers, lären), respectively. At the same time, as Stolo-
being “in tune” was, in itself, healing, row (1993) pointed out, Kohut (1982) also de-
confirming, and growth promoting, where- picted empathy as a “powerful emotional bond
as for Kohut it was a preamble to the between people . . . [and that] empathy per se
curative explanation. . . . has a beneficial, in a broad sense, a thera-
3. W ith regard to Kohut’s view of the thera- peutic effect— both in the clinical setting and in
peutic relationship, according to Rogers human life, in general” (p. 397).
(1986), Kohut’s was cooler and less per- From this we may speculate that the similari-
sonal than Rogers’s. ties between these two approaches may have
4. Rogers viewed Kohut as lacking interest been minimized by the way in which the differ-
in testing his theories. ent traditions of understanding and explanation
Following Rogers, a number of writers have can be and can become polarized. In this age of
commented on the similarities and differences integration, the challenge may be to find a
between the person-centered approach and self genuine integration of both the informative and
psychology: Stolorow (1976), Graf (1984), the explanatory— and, indeed, the curative.
Kahn (1985, 1989a, 1989b, 1996), Bohart One concept that may be useful in this endeav-
(1991), Tobin (1991), W arner (1997, 2000), or is Buber’s (1962, 1963) view of “compre-
Kahn and Rachman (2000), Stumm (2002), and hension” (Umfassung), which, for him, took in
Tudor and W orrall (2006). both poles of empathy: that of being centered
The two psychologies share similar views of in the other and in one’s own existence. Ac-
human nature, although both Kahn (1985) and cording to Buber, comprehending is different
Stumm (2002) have argued that Kohut was from looking at and from observing (and men-
more pessimistic in his view of the human con- tally taking notes)— and, by implication, from
dition, the self and self-development, and the explaining— because it carries a sense of being
importance of the psychological climate. Both existentially affected by something or someone.
approaches draw on phenomenology and phe- Schmid (2001), a person-centered therapist,
nomenological method, similarly acknowledge philosopher, and commentator on Buber, linked
the difference between empathy and emotional this to the importance of presence and argued
identification, and apply their theories to socie- that such comprehension
tal issues. They differ in their philosophical and is only possible if I become present to the
intellectual heritage; their views of the potential other: presence is the fundamental core of

44 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

Table 1
Differences between the Person-Centered Psychology of Rogers and the
Self Psychology of Kohut and Others (Tudor & Worrall, 2006)

Person-Centered Psychology (Rogers) Self Psychology (Kohut)

Philosophical/Intellectual Background
Existential, humanistic frame of reference Background in psychoanalytic ego psychology

The Potential of Human Beings


Belief in the organism’s tendency to actualize Kohut’s concept of the “tragic person” is less
optimistic

Growth and Outcome of Therapy


In the direction of greater independence Interdependence
and separation (autonomy) (from Kohut’s ideas about necessary
dependency of the self on external sources)

Theory of Change/Cure
Expansion of consciousness Strengthening of the self

Self
Changing, perceptual view (Rogers, 1951) As bipolar configuration (Kohut, 1977)

Self-Development
Focus is in the present therapeutic situation Shows that certain categories of parental
behaviors are also necessary attitudes for therapists

Theory of Disturbance—Etiology
Deficit model Dissociation model

Empathy
The therapist’s attempt to perceive the internal Viewed as an “information-gathering activity”
frame of reference of the other is itself curative (Kohut, 1982) . . . and in order to make
(see Rogers, 1959). good interpretations and curative explanations

Transference
The therapist endeavors to understand and accept Transference is a manifestation of the client’s
transference attitudes but does not foster dependence manner of organizing her or his experience

Theory and Research Methodology


Importance given (especially by Rogers) to theories Subjective experience is only accessible
Theories being testable by empirical means and through introspection and empathy
operationalized (see Rogers, 1986)

Re: Therapeutic Method


Against interpretation Against unempathic interpretations but in favor of generic
interpretations linking present events with past experiences

this way of relating and perceiving. . . . Em- two sentences) on the distinction between em-
pathy is an expression of presence, because pathy and intuition, arguing that while empathy
it is, in existential wonderment, related to has a connotation of identification, intuition
what the Other is experiencing. (p. 58) does not, and, rather, that it “has to do with the
automatic processing of sensory perceptions”
Empathy in Transactional Analysis (p. 95). Berne’s lack of reference to empathy is
In his writing, Berne mentioned empathy no accident: He did not consider it a technique,
only once. In an early paper published in 1955 let alone an attitude, and it did not appear as
(Berne, 1955/1977), he commented briefly (in one of his therapeutic operations (Berne, 1966).

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 45

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

As Cornell and Bonds-W hite (2001) pointed have written about empathy in books, notably
out, Erskine, Moursund, and Trautmann (1999);
His [Berne’s] transactional analysis was Moursund & Erskine (2004); and Hargaden
intended to unsettle a client’s familiar, de- and Sills (2002). In this section I review these
fensive frame of reference through de- ideas in the chronological order of their devel-
scription, confrontation, interpretation, and opment.
humor. It seems quite clear that Berne’s The Empathic Transaction (Clark). In her
intent, consistent with a classical psycho- 1991 article, Clark introduced the concept of
analytic position, was to alter the intrapsy- the “empathic transaction,” suggesting that “it
chic structure and function of the client demonstrates the importance of regulating the
through clarifying interventions, not through intensity and directness of transactions during
offering a corrective relationship. (p. 73) different phases of treatment (p. 92).” Clark did
Berne’s transactional analysis thus reflects, in not quote Rogers directly, but she cited another
the language of Stark’s (1999) meta-analysis of author, Miller (1989), who referred to Rogers
modes of therapeutic action, a “one-person” but misrepresented him. Specifically, Miller
psychology (for further discussion of this, see claimed that Rogers identified three variables
page 49). that were necessary and sufficient when, in
In the 20 years following Berne’s death, fact, Rogers identified six: psychological con-
there appears to have been a similar lack of in- tact between client and therapist, client incon-
terest within transactional analysis in the sub- gruence, therapist congruence, unconditional
ject of empathy— or, perhaps, the elaboration positive regard (or acceptance), empathic
of empathy. Indeed, in over 35 years of the understanding, and the client’s perception of
Transactional Analysis Journal (TAJ) and over the therapist’s acceptance and empathy. Ac-
1,500 articles, there are only two specifically cording to Clark, an empathic transaction com-
on the subject of empathy: Payton, Morriss, prises two parts: “the therapist’s expression of
and Beale (1979) and Clark (1991), the former understanding of the patient’s experience and
of which is a report on the effects of transac- the patient’s confirmation that he or she has
tional analysis instruction on students’ ability been understood” (p. 92). Drawing on Berne’s
to recognize empathic responses and to discri- (1963) ideas about phases of treatment, Clark
minate empathic from nonempathic responses. talked about the intensity of the therapist’s
A review of TAJ articles published between transactions in different phases of therapy: de-
January 1971and January 2008 (see Summers, contamination and deconfusion. Although Clark
2008) reveals some interesting statistics: did not diagram this transaction, from what she
• W hile only two articles have “empathy” said about “direct transactions,” I think her
or “empathic” in their titles, some 253 men- description of the empathic transaction is accu-
tion empathy, empathic, or empathetic. rately represented in Figure 1.In this analysis of
• Interest in the subject appears to have in- transactions, however, Clark did not account
creased over the years; an analysis of for the first transaction, that is, the stimulus
these 253 articles by decade reveals the from the client.
following distribution: 1970s— 9 articles, Empathy and Integration (Erskine, Mour-
1980s— 53, 1990s—87, and in the seven sund, and Trautmann). In their book Beyond
years from January 2000 to January 2008 Empathy, Erskine, Moursund, and Trautmann
— 104. (1999) characterized the qualities of attune-
• Of the 253 articles that mention the sub- ment and involvement and the skill of inquiry
ject, only 36 have three or more mentions as central to effective psychotherapy. They based
of empathy, empathic, or empathetic, and all three elements on empathy but claimed that
of these, only 8 deal with the subject in all go “beyond” empathy in some way, al-
any detail, all of which have been written though it is not clear precisely how or in what
in the last 17 years. ways. In a subsequent book, Moursund and Ers-
In addition, some transactional analysis authors kine (2004) reviewed the literature on empathy;

46 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

Figure 1
The Empathic Transaction (based on Clark, 1991)

cited Rogers’s development of empathy (al- and intensifications of the empathic process”
though they, too, misrepresent his theory of (p. 95) and categorize them as “beyond empa-
therapeutic conditions); and drew on Tropp and thy” (p. 95). The authors, however, appear
Stolorow’s (1997) work to advocate for an ac- ambiguous— and ambivalent— about whether
tive, inquiring kind of empathy. M oursund and the activities they are promoting are aspects,
Erskine (2004) claimed that for relationship- subdivisions, or extensions of empathy. This
focused integrative psychotherapists, “empathy ambiguity is important not only in terms of the
provides a foundation upon which additional development of theory but also because of the
therapeutic activities are built” (p. 95); they centrality of empathy in their approach to trans-
again identified these as attunement, inquiry, actional analysis and psychotherapy.
and involvement. Moursund and Erskine com- From Therapeutic Operations to Empathic
mented that these activities are “enhancements Transactions (Hargaden and Sills). In their book

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 47

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

Figure 2
The Empathic Transaction (Hargaden & Sills, 2002, p. 33)

book Transactional Analysis: A Relational Per- Hargaden and Sills (2002) went on to re-
spective, Hargaden and Sills (2002) drew on frame Berne’s (1966) therapeutic operations in
Clark’s work on the empathic transaction and terms of empathic transactions, as part of which
diagrammed the empathic transaction as repre- they added “holding” to Berne’s sequence (be-
senting “a series of complementary transactions fore crystallization). They made the point that
between the Adult of the client and the thera- empathic transactions are fundamentally differ-
pist and complementary ulterior transactions ent from the original Bernean concept of opera-
between the client’s Child and the therapist’s tions and argued that there are a number of ad-
Adult” (p. 33) (see Figure 2). They emphasized vantages to their reformulation:
the importance of the ulterior transaction in 1. It brings empathy into the context of such
responding “with empathy and respect to the operations or transactions.
contaminated Adult of the client” and in estab- 2. The use of the word transaction acknowl-
lishing an empathic bond that, eventually, “makes edges the connectedness of both therapist
it possible for the client to feel secure enough and client: In other words, empathy is not
at an ‘unthought’ level to revive unmet needs something that can simply be given, it
and suppressed developmental needs” (p. 33). also needs to be received. This is a point

48 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

that Rogers (1957, 1959) emphasized and by the therapist’s interpretations. In this mode
that I discuss further later in this article. or model, the therapist is not a participant in a
Some may argue that this sense of con- relationship but, according to Stark (1999), an
nectedness is implicit in the concept of a “objective observer of the patient” (p. xvi). She
transaction because it comprises both a continued, “The therapist conceives of her posi-
stimulus and a response; thus, a stimulus tion as outside the therapeutic field and of her-
that does not elicit a response is not a self as a blank screen onto which the patient
transaction and does not make a connec- casts shadows that the therapist then interprets”
tion. However, while the stimulus-response (p. xvi). This mode is more often associated
model of transactions derives from be- with the interpretive model of classical psycho-
haviorism, Hargaden and Sills’s (2002) analysis and ways in which empathy is used to
conceptualization of transactions draws interpret, evoke, and inquire, whether from a
on a more relational and intersubjective hermeneutic perspective or not, so as to keep
understanding and, thereby, gives greater the therapist as a distant “as if” figure (see
significance to the connection between Keil, 1996). However, many cognitive behav-
people in the communication of empathy. ioral therapists, as well as humanistic thera-
3. The use of the word transaction acknowl- pists, also use empathy in this way, or, perhaps
edges the ulterior, nonverbal element to more accurately, their empathy reflects a one-
the exchange. person psychology. W hen we say to a client,
4. The use of the phrase empathic transac- “You look sad,” we are, in effect and in es-
tions acknowledges the use of subliminal sence, offering an interpretation based on some
thinking. observation— usually of her or his eyes, face,
In their reworking of Berne’s therapeutic op- and posture—as well as the content and tone of
erations, Hargaden and Sills (2002) developed what the person is saying. Furthermore, there is
the concept of empathic transactions as a form at least one form of empathy— hermeneutic
of analysis and interpretation and thus devel- empathy (Keil, 1996)— that describes the situa-
oped a Kohutian perspective on empathy. tion when the therapist understands more than
Elsewhere (Tudor, 2011), I have developed the client and differs from the client in what she
and diagrammed a cocreative perspective on or he understands. On the basis that therapy is
the empathic transaction. a hermeneutic or interpretive activity, Keil ar-
gued that empathic understanding necessarily
Empathy in a Relational Context has to go beyond the “as if” understanding and
From the discussion so far, it is clear that one-to-one relationship, by which he means a
there are different understandings and defini- relationship based on a one-person psychology.
tions of empathy. To clarify the significance of Keil went on to identify a number of steps in
these differences, I develop Stark’s (1999) tax- realizing hermeneutic empathy, beginning with
onomy of psychologies, by which she distin- the therapist perceiving and recognizing her or
guished between three different, although mu- his own reactions to the behavior and expres-
tually enhancing, modes of therapeutic action. sion of the client and, later, recognizing the
Table 2 notes Stark’s different psychologies, importance of nonacceptance and nonunder-
with the addition of a two-person-plus psy- standing.
chology, and summarizes their respective dif- One-and-a-Half-Person Psychology. Stark
ferent influences, which I refer to as centered- (1999) introduced what she referred to as the
ness and foci. Other columns in Table 2 give second mode of psychology by means of a brief
examples of different views of empathy that history of the shift in the etiology of psycho-
reflect each mode and notes criticisms or limi- pathology from nature to nurture, a shift that
tations of each mode. was influenced by object relations and self psy-
One-Person Psychology. In the first mode, chology. As a result, Stark argued, “The locus
the goal of treatment is a strengthening of the of therapeutic action shifted from insight by
client’s ego through insight facilitated primarily way of interpretation to a corrective experience

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 49

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

Table 2
Understandings of Empathy in Terms of Stark’s Modes of Therapeutic Action

Psychology or Influence, Focus Empathy (Examples) Criticisms/


Mode of Centeredness, Limitations
Therapeutic and
Action (based Relationship
on Stark, 1999)

One-person Unidirectional Structural “Accustom yourself to give careful


psychology influence conflict, attention to what others are
impaired saying, and try your best to enter
÷ capacity, into the mind of the speaker”
and (Aurelius, 1964, p. 104). The endless
Patient/client- relation- tolerance of
centered ships within “I and my colleagues realized that the client’s
the person this empathic listening provided needs can
“You” one of the least clouded windows appear
relationship into the workings of the human “quintessen-
psyche, in all its complex mystery” tionally nar-
(Rogers, 1974/1980b, p. 50). cissistic” (H.
Hargaden,
One-and-a-half- Unidirectional Structural Empathy “involves a continuous personal
person influence deficit, process of checking with the client communica-
psychology corrective to see if understanding is
÷l² experience, complete and accurate. It is
tion, 3
October
consolida- carried out in a manner that is 2005)
Person- ting the self personal, natural, and free
centered flowing: it is not a mechanical kind
of reflection or mirroring” (Rogers
“I-It” & Raskin, 1989, p. 189).
relationship

Two-person Bidirectional On “Empathic understanding— “Psycho-


psychology influence interactive understanding with a person, not therapy can
engage- about him” (Rogers, 1952/1967a, be con-
ment, p. 332) sidered to be
enact- an attempt to
ments, the “When you live in the shadow of carry out
Persons- therapeutic insanity, the appearance of phenome-
centered, relation- another mind that thinks and talks nology
relationship- ship, as yours does is something close à deux”
centered including to a blessed event. Like Robinson (Loewenthal
failures Crusoe’s discovery of footprints & Snell,
“I-Thou,” on the sand” (Pirsig, 1974/1991, 2003, p. 12).
“Thou-I,” “We” p. 263).
relationship

Two-person-plus Multidirectional Context, Empathy . . . [is] “in itself a


psychology influence environ- healing agent. It is one of the
ment most potent aspects of therapy,
because it releases, it confirms, it
brings even the most frightened
client into the human race. If a
person can be understood, he or
Socio-centered she belongs” (Rogers, 1986, p.
129).
“I-Thou-Them,”
“We-in-context”
relationship

50 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

by way of the real [therapeutic] relationship” for mirroring and twinship, can sometimes
(p. xvii). However, this is more of an “I-It” seem to be quintessentially narcissistic.”
relationship than an “I-Thou” or “Thou-I” rela- Two-Person Psychology. The third psycholo-
tionship and hence the designation of this mode gy identified by Stark (1999) conceives of
as a “one-and-a-half-person” psychology. In this therapy as “interactive engagement with an
mode, empathy may be viewed—and experienced authentic other” (p. xix). In other words, what
—as a way of being on the part of the therapist heals is the therapeutic relationship itself, faci-
by means of which the client may emerge with litated by empathic attunement and resonance.
greater self-understanding of herself or himself, In this mode, client and therapist create and co-
others, and the world. The classical Rogerian create an interactive dyad in which each affects
empathic listening, together with reflection, is the other— and each recognizes that she or he
an example of this mode, and, indeed, Stark affects the other. This is sometimes referred to
classified Rogers (1961/1967b) as a therapist in as a person-to-person relationship or, following
this mode or model. One example of empathy Buber (1937), an I-Thou relationship. In this
in this mode is when, as therapists, we check mode, more attention is paid to the processing
out our understanding of what the client has of inevitable empathic failure and misattune-
said or meant. Brodley (2001), a leading expo- ment on the part of the therapist (e.g. ,“I think
nent of the nondirective approach to therapy, I missed you there”) as well as to ruptures in
was explicit about this: the relationship. This is reflected in the person-
I make empathic understanding responses centered literature that has an interactional
primarily in order to check my accuracy orientation (see van Kessel & van der Linden,
with my clients. . . . I want to know wheth- 1993) and focuses on empathy as an aspect of
er my inner understandings, that I have “the between” or the interpersonal encounter (see
communicated as best I can, are accurate Barrett-Lennard, 1997; Myers, 1999; Schmid,
according to the client. Empathic respon- 2006), perspectives that highlight the reflective
ses express my perceptions of my client’s and cocreative nature of empathy. Although this
intended communications. (p. 19) has echoes of Sullivan’s interpersonal therapy,
Arguably, working in this mode requires the and Rogers (1951) acknowledged the “threads
therapist to have the kind of “comprehension” of interconnectedness with [certain] modern
to which Buber referred (described earlier). formulations of psychoanalytic thinking” (p. 4),
There are criticisms of empathy or how em- including Sullivan (along with Horney and
pathy can be used in these two modes. Slavin Alexander and French), Rogers only specifi-
and Kriegman (1992) argued that for the thera- cally cited Sullivan (1945) with reference to his
pist to try to remain exclusively attuned to views on the forward direction of the organism.
dominant themes and meanings in the client’s Schmid (2001, 2006), in particular, made an
subjective world can be a “self-protective stra- important contribution to the development of a
tegy on behalf of the therapist” (p. 252), one dialogical person-centered therapy. He made
that can be experienced by the client as con- the point (Schmid, 2006) that, as it is the other
trolling and emotionally impersonal. Indeed, who calls the “I” into service, this relationship
“as if” empathy can be reflective of what Deutsch is more accurately described as a “Thou-I” re-
(1934) described as an “as-if personality,” in lationship— which, of course, also reflects a fo-
which affective relationships with others are cus or centeredness on the client. More broad-
barred due to a pseudoidentification. O’Hara ly, this mode reflects the widespread interest in
(1997) viewed these conceptions of empathy as the therapeutic relationship across theoretical
modernist and egocentric. In a contribution to orientations and what Mitchell (2000) referred
an Internet discussion group, Helena Hargaden to as a “relational turn” (p. xiii) in psychothera-
(personal communication, 3 October 2005) py. In this mode, empathy, from the therapist’s
suggested that “therapy based on person-centered point of view, is for and with the other and with
or self psychological perspectives, with their the relationship and hence the therapist’s com-
focus on endlessly tolerating the client’s needs ments on the relationship (e.g., “It seems like

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 51

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

we both talked past each other there”). In the see Bohart & Tallman, 1999; Bozarth, 1998;
context of sociocentric psychology, as distinct Duncan, Miller, & Sparks, 2004; Miller, Hub-
from egocentric psychology, O’Hara (1997) bard, & Duncan, 1995). These encompass the
coined the phrase “relational empathy,” which, client’s active participation in therapy and her
she argued, offers a view of empathy as contex- or his proactive choice and realistic expecta-
tual awareness and as a state—or process— of tions of therapy as well as her or his levels of
consciousness that is based on and reflects a psychosocial functioning, secure styles of
relational knowing. She contended that “em- attachment, psychological mindedness, and so-
pathy has a more respected role in sociocentric cial support. As Horney (1945/1999) put it,
human relations than it typically does in ego- “Life itself still remains a very effective thera-
centric cultures” (p. 303) and that in such rela- pist” (p. 240). One example of empathy in this
tions and cultures empathy is not “a poor rela- mode is Rogers’s (1986) definition of empathy
tion to objective interpretation” (p. 303). as “in itself a healing agent” (p. 129). He
The criticism of this mode of therapeutic ac- continued, “It is one of the most potent aspects
tion is encapsulated in Loewenthal and Snell’s of therapy, because it releases, it confirms, it
(2003) suggestion that “psychotherapy can be brings even the most frightened client into the
considered to be an attempt to carry out phe- human race. If a person can be understood, he
nomenology à deux” (p. 12). or she belongs” (p. 129). Elsewhere, Rogers
A Fourth Mode? Two-person psychology, (1975/1980a) described one of the profound
psychotherapy, and other activities help healing consequences of empathy succinctly: “Empathy
and change take place in a social context, and dissolves alienation” (p. 151). It is no accident
that social context has an evident effect on that those therapists and others— such as com-
what happens in the consulting room— and, in- munity and youth workers as well as political
deed, even on whether the client gets to and activists who focus on the social/political world
continues to attend therapy. Furthermore, for — often work and organize in groups and com-
some therapists— and, within transactional munities, and that those who analyze alienation
analysis, those who view TA as a social psy- (see, for instance, Agel, 1971) and psycho-
chology and who draw on the radical psychia- pathology as forms of alienation (see, for in-
try tradition— the context that clients bring into stance, Tudor & W orrall, 2006), often work
the consulting room is an important focus for therapeutically in and with groups.
therapy. Given the significance of client factors W e can understand more fully the signifi-
and extratherapeutic factors, and in acknowl- cance of this social, connecting aspect of em-
edgment of an interest in the social context of pathy if we also refer to Angyal (1941), a psy-
therapy— what I have referred to as another, chologist whose work influenced Rogers. Ang-
social “turn” in the field of psychotherapy (see yal explored the concept of homonomy, which
Tudor, 2010)— I propose a fourth mode of he defined as “the tendency to conform to, unite
therapeutic action: a two-person-plus psycholo- with, participate in, and fit into superindividual
gy. This phrase and mode acknowledges the wholes” (p. 182). He suggested that this ten-
significance and impact of these factors and of dency or trend, this urge to belong to some-
the social context of the client, the therapist, thing larger than oneself, is “a source of pro-
and the therapy— and of the therapist’s and cli- found motivation for human behavior” (p.
ent’s empathic relationship with such factors. 182). As human beings, we tend or trend to-
Two-Person-Plus Psychology. Alongside the ward both autonomy and homonomy in a world
interest and development in ideas about the that is heteronomous or other. As Angyal put it,
therapeutic relationship or therapeutic relating “The organism lives in a world in which things
(see Summers & Tudor, 2000; Tudor, 2008b), happen according to laws which are heterono-
there is an increasing acknowledgment of the mous from the point of view of the organism”
importance to psychotherapy outcome of client (p. 33). Empathic understanding, resonance,
factors as well as other extratherapeutic factors and, specifically, visceral and relational em-
(i.e., factors outside therapy that are therapeutic; pathy are ways of satisfying this trend toward

52 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

homonomy for both client and therapist. W hen In this fourth mode, the focus of the thera-
one understands the other, both belong to pist’s empathy is with the client’s relationship
something larger and more inclusive than either with the extratherapeutic factors that support or
does individually. W hile the concept of autono- detract from her or his life, including her or his
my is familiar and well-articulated in transac- relationship with therapy. The therapist pays
tional analysis, from Berne (1964/1968) on- attention to the client’s sense of connection
ward, there is, apart from my work, only one with and/or disconnection from her or his world
other reference in the transactional analysis and of belonging to and/or alienation from the
literature to the concept of homonomy in an world, including relationships with her or his
excellent article written by Massey (2007). faith, ancestors, and natural environment (e.g.,
Rogers and Angyal knew of each other’s work: land, river, and mountains). Some therapists
Rogers (1951, 1959) cited Angyal (1941), and explore these relationships with the client by
Angyal (1965/1973) referred to Rogers when physically moving out of the frame of the con-
he acknowledged that sulting room into the environment. W hether or
with patients whose emotions are not, or not client and therapist supplement or comple-
no longer, completely submerged or falsi- ment talking with walking, it is clear that two-
fied, a sensitive recognition of all their person-plus psychology acknowledges the so-
feelings by the therapist can do much to cial, cultural, and environmental context of the
increase the patient’s awareness of his client and that empathy in this mode acknowl-
mental states. Because of this greater dif- edges the holistic and contextual client.
ferentiation, and also because the therapist
naturally resonates to genuine undistorted Summary
feelings, this process also furthers their This article has argued that the different
growth. This is one reason why reflection meanings of empathy have not been made ex-
of feelings advocated and practiced by plicit in the transactional analysis literature and
Rogers and his followers often proves so that a fuller understanding of Carl Rogers’s
effective. (p. 290) work on empathy, in the context of his neces-
W ithin transactional analysis there are no sary and sufficient conditions for therapy, ex-
journal articles on the subject of belonging, al- pands the understanding of empathy and its ap-
though it is the subject of some discussion in plications in TA. The article has sought to clari-
articles by Caracushansky (1980), Caracushan- fy different perspectives on empathy in trans-
sky and Giampeitro (1987), and Shadbolt actional analysis, an analysis that also serves as
(2004). Maslow (1954) placed belonging be- background for a separate article that elabo-
low but, reading from the bottom up, before rates a cocreative perspective on empathy
self-actualization in his hierarchy of needs, and (Tudor, in press). Finally, the article suggests
it is implied in some of the relational needs an addition to Stark’s (1999) taxonomy of dif-
identified by Erskine and Trautmann (1996) ferent psychologies, one that acknowledges the
(e.g., to feel validated, affirmed, and signifi- impact and contribution of extratherapeutic fac-
cant; to be accepted by a stable, dependable, tors and context on the client. This perspective
and protective other; to have an impact on oth- reflects and enhances transactional analysis as
ers; and to express love). Moiso (1999) expli- a social psychology.
citly identified the drive to belong as one of the
three drives that derive from Berne’s concept Keith Tudor, M .A., M.Sc., CQSW, Dip.
of human hungers. He recognized this as the Psychotherapy, Certified Transactional Ana-
first permission essential to the child and, lyst (psychotherapy), Teaching and Super-
therefore, that the injunction Don’t Belong, vising Transactional Analyst (psychotherapy),
together with Don’t Be and D on’t, is a proto- is an associate professor and program leader
injunction. Similarly, Allen and Allen (1999) in the Department of Psychotherapy, Auckland
considered that permission to feel that one be- University of Technology, Aotearoa New Zea-
longs is one of a number of major permissions. land. He can be reached at Keith Tudor,

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 53

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

Private Bag 92006, Auckland 1020, New Zea- therapy work: The process of active self-healing.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
land; e-mail: keith.tudor@aut.ac.nz . The auth-
Bozarth, J. D. (1998). Research on psychotherapy outcome
or is grateful to two anonymous reviewers for and the person-centered approach. In J. D. Bozarth,
their helpful comments on this article; to Bill Person-centered therapy: A revolutionary paradigm
Cornell for his editorial facilitation and his (pp. 163-173). Ross-on-Wye, England: PCCS Books.
suggestion of the term “two-person-plus psy- Brodley, B. T. (2001). Observations of empathic under-
standing in a client-centered practice. In S. Haugh & T.
chology”; and, as ever, to Mick Worrall for his Merry (Eds.), Empathy (pp. 16-37). Ross-on-Wye, Eng-
contribution to the development of these ideas land: PCCS Books.
and their collaboration on this subject in the Buber, M. (1937). I and thou (R. G. Smith, Trans.). Edin-
context of their writing and various publica- burgh, Scotland: T. & T. Clark.
Buber, M. (1962). Werke. Erster band: Schriften zur
tions on person-centered psychology. philosophie [Works. Volume one: Philosophical writ-
ings]. Munich, Germany: Lambert Schneider Verlag.
REFERENCES Buber, M. (1963). Werke. Dritter band: Schriften zum
Agel, J. (Ed.). (1971). The radical therapist. New York, chassidismus [Works. Volume three: Writings on Ha-
NY: Ballantine Books. sidism]. Munich, Germany: Lambert Schneider Verlag.
Allen, J. R., & Allen, B. A. (1999). On receiving the 1998 Caracushansky, S. (1980). Self-actualization and cure.
Eric Berne memorial award for theory. Transactional Transactional Analysis Journal, 10, 322-325.
Analysis Journal, 29, 11-13. Caracushansky, S. R., & Giampeitro, A. C. (1987). The
Angyal, A. (1941). Foundations for a science of person- use of myths and fairy tales in a Bernian approach to
ality. New York: Commonwealth Fund. psychotherapy. Transactional Analysis Journal, 17,
Angyal, A. (1973). Neurosis and treatment: A holistic 277-285.
theory. New York, NY: Wiley. (Original work pub- Clark, B. D. (1991). Empathic transactions in the decon-
lished 1965) fusion of child ego states. Transactional Analysis Jour-
Aurelius, M. (1964). Meditations (M. Staniforth, Trans.). nal, 21, 92-98.
Harmondsworth, England: Penguin. Cooper, M. (2001). Embodied empathy. In S. Haugh & T.
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1983). Understanding the person- Merry (Eds.), Empathy (pp. 218-229). Ross-on-Wye,
centered approach to therapy: A reply to questions and England: PCCS Books.
misconceptions. In E. McIlduff & D. Coghlan (Eds.), Cornell, W. F., & Bonds-White, F. (2001). Therapeutic
The person-centered approach and cross-cultural com- relatedness in transactional analysis: The truth of love or the
munication: An international review (Vol. I, pp. 99- love of truth. Transactional Analysis Journal, 31, 71-83.
113). Dublin, Ireland: Center for Cross-Cultural Com- Cornell, W. F., & Landaiche, N. M., III. (2007). Why body
munication. psychotherapy: A conversation. Transactional Analysis
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1997). The recovery of empathy: Journal, 37, 256- 262.
Towards others and self. In A. C. Bohart & L. S. Green- Deutsch, H. (1934). Uber einen typus der pseudoaffek-
berg (Eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions in tivitaet [More than one type of pseudoeffectiveness].
psychotherapy (pp. 103-121). Washington, DC: Ameri- Internationale Zeitschrift für Ärztliche Psychoanalyse,
can Psychological Association. 20, 323-335.
Berne, E. (1963). The structure and dynamics of organiza- Donner, S. (1991). The treatment process. In H. Jackson
tions and groups. New York, NY: Grove Press. (Ed.), Using self psychology in psychotherapy (pp. 51-
Berne, E. (1966). Principles of group treatment. New 70). Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
York, NY: Grove Press. Duncan, B. L., Miller, S. D., & Sparks, J. A. (2004). The
Berne, E. (1968). Games people play: The psychology of heroic client: A revolutionary way to improve effective-
human relationships. Harmondsworth, England: Pen- ness through client-directed, outcome-informed thera-
guin. (Original work published 1964) py. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Berne, E. (1977). Primal images and primal judgment. In Embleton Tudor, L., Keemar, K., Tudor, K., Valentine, J.,
E. Berne, Intuition and ego states: The origins of trans- & Worrall, M. (2004). The person-centred approach:
actional analysis (P. McCormick, Ed.) (pp. 67-97). San A contemporary introduction. Basingstoke, England:
Francisco, CA: TA Press. (Original work published Palgrave.
1955) Erskine, R. G., Moursund, J. P., & Trautmann, R. L.
Bohart, A. C. (1991). Empathy in client-centered therapy: (1999) Beyond empathy: A therapy of contact-in-
A contrast with psychoanalysis and self psychology. relationship. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel.
Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 31(1), 34-48. Erskine, R. G., & Trautmann, R. L. (1996). Methods of an
Bohart, A. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (Eds.). (1997). Empathy integrative psychotherapy. Transactional Analysis
reconsidered: New directions in psychotherapy. Wash- Journal, 26, 316-328.
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. Fiedler, F. E. (1949). A comparative investigation of early
Bohart, A., & Rosenbaum, R. (1995). The dance of empa- therapeutic relationships created by experts and non-
thy: Empathy, diversity, and technical eclecticism. The experts of the psychoanalytic, non-directive, and Ad-
Person-Centered Journal, 2, 5-29. lerian schools. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Bohart, A. C., & Tallman, K. (1999). How clients make University of Chicago, Chicago, IL.

54 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

Fiedler, F. E. (1950). The concept of an ideal therapeutic Kahn, E., & Rachman, A. W. (2000). Carl Rogers and
relationship. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 14, Heinz Kohut: A historical perspective. Psychoanalytic
239-245. Psychotherapy, 17(2), 294-312.
Frankel, M., & Sommerbeck, L. (2005). Two Rogers and Keil, W. W. (1996). Hermeneutic empathy in client-
congruence: The emergence of therapist-centred therapy centered therapy. In U. Esser, H. Pabst, & G-W. Speier-
and the demise of client-centred therapy. In B. Levitt er (Eds.), The power of the person-centered approach.
(Ed.), Embracing non-directivity (pp. 40-61). Ross-on- New challenges—perspectives—answers (pp. 65-80).
Wye, England: PCCS Books. Köln, Germany: GwG.
Freud, S. (1955). Group psychology and the analysis of the Kohut, H. (1959). Introspection, empathy and psycho-
ego. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition analysis: An examination of the relationship between
of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud modes of observation and theory. Journal of American
(Vol. 18, pp. 67-143 ). London, England: Hogarth Psychoanalytic Association, 7, 459-483.
Press. (Original work published 1920) Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York,
Freud, S. (1973). Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. In NY: International Universities Press.
S. Freud, The Pelican Freud library. Vol. 1: Introductory Kohut, H. (1978). The psychoanalyst in the community of
lectures on psychoanalysis (J. Strachey & A. Richards, scholars. In P. H. Ornstein (Ed.), The search for self:
Eds.; J. Strachey, Trans.) (pp. 31-517). Harmondsworth, Selected writings of H. Kohut: 1950-1978 (Vol. 1, pp.
England: Pelican. (Original work published 1916) 685-724). New York, NY: International Universities Press.
Freud, S. (1976). Jokes and their relation to the uncon- Kohut, H. (1981). On empathy. In P. H. Ornstein (Ed.),
scious. In S. Freud, The Pelican Freud library. Vol. 6: The search for self (Vol. 4, pp. 525-535). New York,
Jokes and their relation to the unconscious (J. Strachey NY: International Universities Press.
& A. Richards, Eds.; J. Strachey, Trans.) (pp. 31-302). Kohut, H. (1982). Introspection, empathy, and the semi-
Harmondsworth, England: Pelican. (Original work pub- circle of mental health. International Journal of Psy-
lished 1905) choanalysis, 63, 395-407.
Graf, C. L. (1984). A comparison of Carl Rogers’ and Kohut, H. (1984). How does analysis cure? Chicago, IL:
Heinz Kohut’s humanistic theories of the fully function- University of Chicago Press.
ing person. In A. S. Segrera (Ed.), Proceedings of the Loewenthal, D., & Snell, R. (2003). Post-modernism for
first international forum on the person-centered ap- psychotherapists. Hove, England: Brunner-Routledge.
proach. Oaxtepec, Mexico: Universidad Iberamericana. Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New
Greenberg, L. S., & Elliot, R. (1997). Varieties of empath- York, NY: Harper & Row.
ic responding. In A. C. Bohart, & L. S. Greenberg Massey, R. F. (2007). Reexamining social psychiatry as a
(Eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions in psy- foundational framework for transactional analysis: Con-
chotherapy (pp. 167-186). Washington, DC: American sidering a social-psychological perspective. Transac-
Psychological Association. tional Analysis Journal, 37, 51-79.
Hargaden, H., & Sills, C. (2002). Transactional analysis: Mearns, D., & Thorne, B. (2000). Person-centred therapy
A relational perspective. London, England: Routledge. today: New frontiers in theory and practice. London,
Horney, K. (1999). Our inner conflicts: A constructive England: Sage.
theory of neurosis. New York, NY: Norton. (Original Miller, I. J. (1989). The therapeutic empathic communica-
work published 1945) tion (TEC) process. American Journal of Psychothera-
Hunsdahl, J. B. (1967). Concerning einfuhlung (empathy): py, 4, 531-545.
A concept analysis of its origin and early development. Miller, S., Hubble, M., & Duncan, B. (1995, March/
Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 3, April). No more bells and whistles. The Family Therapy
180-191. Networker, 53-63.
Jackson, H. (Ed.). (1991). Using self psychology in psy- Mitchell, S. A. (2000). Relationality: From attachment to
chotherapy. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson. intersubjectivity. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
Jacobson, E. (1964). Self and the object world. New York, Moiso, C. (1999). Being and belonging: An appreciation
NY: International Universities Press. and application of Berne’s social psychiatry. The Script,
Jaspers, K. (1963). General psychopathology (J. Hoenig 29(9), 1.
& M. W. Hamilton, Trans.). Chicago, IL: University of Moursund, J. P., & Erskine, R. G. (2004). Integrative psy-
Chicago Press. (Original work published 1913) chotherapy: The art and science of relationship. New
Kahn, E. (1985). Heinz Kohut and Carl Rogers: A timely York, NY: Thomson/Wadsworth/Brooks/Cole.
comparison. American Psychologist, 40, 893-904. Myers, S. (1999). Empathy: Is that what I hear you saying?
Kahn, E. (1989a). Carl Rogers and Heinz Kohut. On the Person-Centered Journal, 6(2), 11-152.
importance of valuing the “self.” In D. W. Dettick & S. O’Hara, M. (1997). Relational empathy: Beyond modern-
P. Dettick (Eds.), Self psychology: Comparisons and ist geocentricism to postmodern holistic contextualism.
contrasts (pp. 213-228). Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic In A. C. Bohart, & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Empathy re-
Press. considered: New directions in psychotherapy (pp. 295-
Kahn, E. (1989b). Carl Rogers and Heinz Kohut: Toward a 319). Washington, DC: American Psychological Asso-
constructive collaboration. Psychotherapy, 26(4), 555-563. ciation.
Kahn, E. (1996). The intersubjective perspective and the O’Leary, E. (1993). Empathy in the person-centred and
client-centered approach: Are they one at their core? gestalt approaches. British Gestalt Journal, 2, 111-
Psychotherapy, 33(1), 30-42. 114.

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 55

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


KEITH TUDOR

Ornstein, A. (1991). When the patient is demanding. In H. dialogical person-centered psychotherapy. Person-
Jackson (Ed.), Using self psychology in psychotherapy Centered & Experiential Psychotherapies, 5(4), 240-
(pp. 153-166). Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson. 254.
Payton, O. D., Morriss, R. L., & Beale, A. V. (1979). Ef- Shadbolt, C. (2004). Homophobia and gay affirmative
fects of TA instruction on empathy, self-esteem, con- transactional analysis. Transactional Analysis Journal,
trol. Transactional Analysis Journal, 9, 200-204. 34, 113-125.
Pigman, G. W. (1995). Freud and the history of empathy. Slavin, M. O., & Kriegman, D. (1992). The adaptive
The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 76, 237- design of the human psyche. New York, NY: Guilford.
256. Stark, M. (1999). Modes of therapeutic action. Northvale,
Pirsig, R. M. (1991). Zen and the art of motorcycle main- NJ: Jason Aronson.
tenance: An inquiry into values. London, England: Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant:
Vintage. (Original work published 1974) A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psy-
Rogers, C. R. (1942). Counseling and psychotherapy: chology. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Newer concepts in practice. Boston, MA: Houghton Stern, D. N. (1998). The interpersonal world of the infant:
Mifflin. A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psy-
Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. London, chology (Rev. ed.). New York, NY: Basic Books.
England: Constable. Stern, D. N. (2004). The present moment: In psycho-
Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient condi- therapy and everyday life. New York, NY: Norton.
tions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Con- Stolorow, R. D. (1976). Psychoanalytic reflections on
sulting Psychology, 21, 95-103. client-centered therapy in the light of modern con-
Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality and ceptions of narcissism. Psychotherapy: Theory, Re-
interpersonal relationships, as developed in the client- search & Practice, 13, 26-29.
centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A Stolorow, R. D. (1993). Thoughts on the nature and
study of a science. Vol. 3: Formulation of the person therapeutic action of psychoanalytic interpretation. In
and the social context (pp. 184-256). New York, NY: A. Goldberg (Ed.), The widening scope of self psy-
McGraw-Hill. chology: Progress in self psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 31-
Rogers, C. R. (1967a). Dealing with breakdowns in 44). Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
communication—interpersonal and intergroup. In C. R. Stolorow, R. D., Atwood, G. E., & Brandchaft, B. (1992).
Rogers, On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of Three realms of the unconscious and their therapeutic
psychotherapy (pp. 329-337). London, England: Con- transformation. Psychoanalytic Review, 79, 25-30.
stable. (Original work published 1952) Stumm, G. (2002). The person-centered approach and self
Rogers, C. R. (1967b). On becoming a person: A thera- psychology. In J. C. Watson, R. N. Goldman, & M. S.
pist’s view of psychotherapy. London, England: Con- Warner (Eds.), Client-centered and experiential psy-
stable. (Original work published 1961) chotherapy in the 21st century: Advances in theory,
Rogers, C. R. (1967c). Toward a theory of creativity. In C. research and practice (pp. 108-126). Ross-on-Wye,
R. Rogers, On becoming a person: A therapist’s view England: PCCS Books.
of psychotherapy (pp. 347-359). London, England: Sullivan, H. S. (1945). Conceptions of modern psychiatry.
Constable. (Original work published 1954) Washington, DC: W. A. White Foundation.
Rogers, C. R. (1980a). Empathic: An unappreciated way Summers, G. (2008). TAJdisk v.3 [CD]. Available from
of being. In C. R. Rogers, A way of being (pp. 137- http://www.tajdisk.co.uk .
163). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. (Original work Summers, G., & Tudor, K. (2000). Cocreative transaction-
published 1975) al analysis. Transactional Analysis Journal, 30, 23-40.
Rogers, C. R. (1980b). In retrospect: Forty-six years. In C. Summers, G., & Tudor, K. (2005). Introducing co-creative
R. Rogers, A way of being (pp. 46-69). Boston, MA: TA. Retrieved 16 February 2011 from http://www.
Houghton Mifflin. (Original work published 1974) co-creativecoaching.co.uk/downloads/downloads.php
Rogers, C. R. (1980c). A way of being. Boston, MA: Thwaites, R., & Bennett-Levy, J. (2007). Conceptualizing
Houghton Mifflin. empathy in cognitive behaviour therapy: Making the
Rogers, C. R. (1986). Rogers, Kohut, and Erickson: A per- implicit explicit. Behavioural and Cognitive Psycho-
sonal perspective on some similarities and differences. therapy, 35, 591-612.
Person-Centered Review, 1(2), 125-40. Tobin, S. A. (1991). A comparison of psychoanalytic self
Rogers, C. R., & Raskin, N. J. (1989). Person-centered psychology and Carl Rogers’ person-centered therapy.
therapy. In R. J. Corsini, & D. Wedding (Eds.), Current Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 30, 9-33.
psychotherapies (pp. 155-194). Itasca, IL: Peacock. Tropp, J. L., & Stolorow, R. D. (1997). Therapeutic em-
Rothschild, B., with Rand, M. (2008). Help for the helper: pathy: An intersubjective perspective. In A. C. Bohart
The psychophysiology of compassion fatigue and & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New
vicarious trauma. New York, NY: Norton. directions in psychotherapy (pp. 279-291). Washing-
Schmid, P. S. (2001). Comprehension: The art of not ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
knowing. Dialogical and ethical perspectives on em- Tudor, K. (1997). Counselling and psychotherapy: An
pathy as dialogue in personal and person-centred issue of orientation. ITA News, 47, 40-42.
relationships. In S. Haugh & T. Merry (Eds.), Empathy Tudor, K. (2003). The neopsyche: The integrating adult
(pp. 53-71). Ross-on-Wye, England: PCCS Books. ego state. In C. Sills, & H. Hargaden (Eds.), Ego states
Schmid, P. (2006). The challenge of the other: Towards (Key concepts in transactional analysis: Contemporary

56 Transactional Analysis Journal

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016


UNDERSTANDING EMPATHY

views) (pp. 201-231). London, England: Worth Pub- Warner, M. S. (1997). Does empathy cure? A theoretical
lishing. consideration of empathy, processing and personal
Tudor, K. (2008b). Verbal being: From being human to narrative. In A. C. Bohart, & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.),
human being. In B. Levitt (Ed.), Reflections on human Empathy reconsidered: New directions in psycho-
potential: Bridging the person-centred approach and therapy (pp. 125-140). Washington, DC: American
positive psychology (pp. 68-83). Ross-on-Wye, Eng- Psychological Association.
land: PCCS Books. Warner, M. S. (2000). Person-centred therapy at the dif-
Tudor, K. (2010, 3 July). Psyche and society: Social and ficult edge: A developmentally based model of fragile
personal effectiveness. Paper presented at the 9th Wold and dissociated process. In D. Mearns, & B. Thorne,
Conference for Person-Centered and Experiential Person-centred therapy today: New frontiers in theory
Psychotherapy and Counselling, Rome, Italy. and practice (pp. 144-171). London, England: Sage.
Tudor, K. (in press). Empathy reconsidered: A cocreative Wexler, D. A. (1974). A cognitive theory of experiencing,
perspective. Transactional Analysis Journal. self-actualization and therapeutic process. In D. A.
Tudor, K., & Merry, T. (2002) Dictionary of person- Wexler, & L. N. Rice (Eds.), Innovations in client-
centred psychology. London, England: Whurr. centered therapy (pp. 49-116). New York, NY: Wiley.
Tudor, K., & Worrall, M. (2006). Person-centred therapy: Wilkins, P. (2003). Person-centred therapy in focus.
A clinical philosophy. London, England: Routledge. London, England: Sage.
Van Belle, H. A. (2005). Philosophical roots of person- Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes and
centered therapy in the history of western thought. The the facilitating environment: Studies in the theory of
Person-Centered Journal, 12(1-2), 50-60. emotional development. London: Hogarth Press/
van Kessel, W., & van der Linden, P. (1993). Die aktuelle Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
beziehung in der klientenzentrierten psychotherapie Wood, J. K. (2008). Carl Rogers’ person-centered ap-
[The here-and-now relationship in client-centered thera- proach: Toward an understanding of its implications.
py]. GwG-Zeitschrift, 90, 19-32. Ross-on-Wye, England: PCCS Books.

2011 World TA Conference


Bilbao, Spain * 7-9 July 2011
Cosponsored by ITAA, EATA, and ATAA

Training Endorsement Workshop: 2-4 July


Exams: 5-6 July
ITAA Board Meetings: Mon. & Tues. 4-5 July and the morning of Sun. 10 July

It is a great pleasure to invite you to come to Bilbao—a small city in the Basque country of northern
Spain—to celebrate together our 2011 Transactional Analysis World Conference. The conference will be
held in the antique buildings of Deusto University, situated on the Nervion River across from the Guggen-
heim Museum. At the TA Conference we are going to face and explore the universal experience of “The
Challenge of Growth.” The transactional analysis concepts of awareness, intimacy, spontaneity, and,
obviously, autonomy are all about the challenge to grow at every developmental stage in our lives. The
entire organizational team joins me in encouraging you to come and share with us this opportunity for
learning and thinking together. You will find a warm atmosphere and feel very welcomed in our city,
Bilbao. Here you will enjoy our famous gastronomy, the picturesque old town, and perhaps even visiting
our beaches and mountains. It is an honor for us to tell you, “Ongi Etorri Bilbo´ ra!” which in the Basque
language means “Bienvenid@ a Bilbao!” or “You are very welcome to Bilbao!”
Amaia Máuriz-Etxabe, Chairperson

www.TAbilbao2011AT.com

Vol. 41, No. 1, January 2011 57

Downloaded from tax.sagepub.com at Auckland Univ of Technology on May 24, 2016

You might also like