You are on page 1of 12

Acta Psychologica ©2020 Chinese Psychological

2020, Vol. 52, No. 11, 12531265 https://dx.doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.01253

The neural basis of scientific innovation problem finding


1,2 3 1 2 2 2 2
TONG DanDan , LI WenFu , LU Peng , YANG WenJing , YANG Dong , ZHANG QingLin , QIU Jiang
1
( School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Key Laboratory of Behavioral and
Mental Health of Gansu Province, Lanzhou 730070, China)
2
( School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China)
3
( Department of Mental Health, Jining Medical University, Jining 272067, Shandong Province, China)

Abstract
Creative thinking, which refers to the process by which individuals produce a unique, valuable product based on existing knowl-
edge, experience, and multi-perspective thinking activities, is the cornerstone of human civilization and social progress. As an im-
portant part of the creative field, scientific inventions, in particular, require individuals to break the existing state and build
new things in the process of creating them. Therefore, the use of real-life examples of scientific inventions to explore the cognitive
neural mechanism of creative thinking has become a focus of recent research. There have been many studies of creative problem
solving, especially regarding its neural mechanisms. However, less attention has been paid to the issue of problem finding. Hence, the
present study employed resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) and scientific invention problem-finding
materials to identify the neural substrates of the process of scientific innovation problem finding.
In the present study, nine scientific innovation problem situations were selected as materials. Each problem consisted of
three parts: (paradoxical) problem situation, (misleading) old problem, and heuristic prototype. The modified learning-testing
paradigm was used to explore the brain mechanisms of problem finding. Participants were asked to find a new problem based on the
given problem situation and old problem in the testing phase after learning all the heuristic prototypes in the learning phase. A total of
104 undergraduates (mean age = 19.26 ± 0.99) were enrolled in the final experiment. The rs-fMRI data were acquired using an echo
pla- nar imaging (EPI) sequence from a 3-T Siemens Magnetom Trio scanner (Siemens Medical, Erlangen, Germany) at the MRI
center of Southwest University. We used both the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) and resting-state functional
connectivity (RSFC) to measure the local properties of rs-fMRI signals, and then investigated the relationship between ALFF/RSFC
and individ- ual differences in scientific problem finding.
After controlling for age and sex, the results of multiple regression analysis showed that individuals with a high rate of useful
problems had higher spontaneous brain activity in the left medial prefrontal cortex (L-mPFC) and cerebellum. Functional connec-
tivity analysis further found a significant positive correlation between the rate of useful problems and the mPFC-Cuneus functional
connectivity.
Based on these results, we infer that: (1) The mPFC plays an important role in the process of scientific innovation problem finding.
It might be responsive to two aspects: one involved in breaking the thinking set and forming a novel association and another associ-
ated with the extraction and processing of working memory. (2) The cerebellum and the cuneus might be separately involved in the
inter-semantic allocation of attentional resources and divulging.
Key words creativity, scientific innovation problem finding, mPFC, AlFF, RSFC

Adey, 2010). Problem finding is a precursor to problem solving,


1 Introduction
which determines how individuals approach problems and whe-
Creative thinking refers to the process in which individuals, ther they can successfully solve them (Gilhooly, Fioratou,
on the basis of prior knowledge and experience, make use & Henretty, 2011; Holman, 2018). Getzels also stresses the
of multi-angle thinking activities to produce products with impo- rtance of problems being identified and presented in a way
novel and unique characteristics and use value, which is the that produces solutions and argues that creative
corner- stone of human civilization and the driving force of achievements are derived from creative problem-finding
social pro- gress (Hennessey & Amabile, 2010; Jung et al., rather than problem- solving (Getzels, 2011). A large
2013; Stern- berg & Lubart, 1993). Asking questions is the number of researchers have explored the cognitive neural
beginning of innovation, so a growing number of mechanisms of problem-solving from behavioral and
researchers believe that being good at asking questions is neuroscience perspectives (Aziz-Zadeh, Kaplan, & Iacoboni,
an important and critical component of creative thinking 2009; Huang, Fan, & Luo, 2015; Jung-Bee- man et al., 2004;
(Alabbasi & Cramond, 2018; Alabbasi, Paek, Cramond, & Kounios & Beeman, 2013; Qiu et al., 2010; Subramaniam, 2008;
Runco, 2020; Hu, Shi, Han, Wang, & Wu, Knoblich, & Luo, 2013; Xue, Lu, &

Received Date: December 20, 2019


This work was supported by The National Natural Science Foundation of China (31470981; 31771231), the humanities and social sciences research project
of the Ministry of Education (19XJC1900001), the social science planning project of Gansu Province (19YB026), and the “Youth Innovation and tech-
nology program” project of Shandong Province (2019RWF003).
Corresponding author: ZHANG Qinglin, E-mail: zhangql@swu.edu.cn; QIU Jiang, E-mail: qiuj318@swu.edu.cn
The original article is in Chinese, translated by Lingocloud and proofread by the author. The Chinese version shall always prevail in case of any discrepancy
or inconsistency between the Chinese version and its English translation.
Acta Psychologica Sinica
Hao, 2018; Zhao et al., 2013; Yang et al., 2018). However, the creativity (Hu et al., 2010). Ward argues that using examples of
research presented in the problem-finding is not sufficient real-life scientific inventions to study creative thinking has
to some extent, and the study of brain mechanism has a higher ecological validity because it is close to the
received limited attention from researchers (Alabbasi & reality of scientific inventions (Ward, 2007). Zhang has also
Cramond, 2018; Reiter- Palmon & Robinson, 2009; Tong et compiled scientific invention experiment materials to measure
al., 2013; Hu & Han, 2015). scientific creativity based on real-life examples of scientific
Cognitive processing of creative thinking is one of the basic inventions (Zhang et al., 2012; Zhu et al., 2011). Based on this,
problems in creative research, and many researches have been this study suggests that we can combine scientific creativity
carried out on the issue of creative problem-finding (Dietrich & with prob- lem-finding and use examples of creative thinking in
Kanso, 2010; Zhou & Shi, 2005). The most common approach the field of science to develop a measurement tool with high
is to identify the key elements of problem finding by presenting ecological validity.
the question material with different characteristics. Such as pre- According to the theory of “representation transition”, people
senting and discovering Problem Situations (Runco & Okuda, tend to make representations in the way suggested by the prob-
1988), familiar problem situations and unfamiliar problem situ- lem situation and search in the corresponding wrong
ations (Wu & Zhang, 2005), real problem situations problem space. If the method cannot be found in the problem
and non-real problem situations (Runco, Illies, & space for a long time, it is necessary to search for the proper
Reiterpalmon, representation in the meta-level space, enter the correct
2005), object materials and verbal materials (Chen et al., 2006), problem space, and finally solve the problem (Kaplan &
well-structured problem situations and poorly structured prob- Simon, 1990). The theory of “prototype activation” holds that
lem situations (Lee & Cho, 2007), ambivalent problem when a problem solver encounters a mental set, he can
situa- tions and latent problem situations (Chen & Zheng, break the original thought deadlock under the inspiration of
2011), open-ended problem situations and closed-ended seemingly unrelated but in- ternally semantically connected
problem situ- ations (Cheng, Hu, Jia, & Runco, 2016) were prototype materials, and apply the heuristic information to
compared. The result shows that the problem situation is scientific problems to get into the right problem space (Zhang
the key factor in problem-finding, and interactions such as et al., 2012). Therefore, Tong (2017), based on the theory of
hemispheric level of interaction, level of knowledge, and “representation transformation” and “prototype elicitation”,
explicit instruction effect had an impact on creative problem collects examples of recent scien- tific inventions in the
finding (Cheng et al., 2016; Lee & Cho, 2007; Wang et al., scientific field from the perspective of “novelty” and
2017). In addition, cognitive inhibition (Hu et al., 2015), “validity” by using the method of measurement and interview.
personality traits (Li et al., 2010; Paletz & Peng, 2009), Then, a series of experiments were carried out to compile the
mood and motivation (Chen, Hu, & Plucker, 2016; Hu & materials of creative scientific problems with more ecological
Zhou, 2010), and school environment (Han et al., 2010; Jia validity, which were more in line with the nature of creative
et al., 2017) are also important internal and external factors thinking and have many indexes such as difficulty, intensity
for creative problem finding. of setting, quantity of inspiration. Besides, the validity of the
The cognitive process of creative problem finding has been experimental materials and paradigms is verified.
described in detail, but there are still some problems. For exa- Creative problem solving has proved to be a complex cogni-
mple, most of the existing experimental materials are divergent tive process involving the whole brain, with the functional
thinking tests evaluated from the dimensions of uniqueness, colla- boration involving different brain regions (Aziz-Zadeh
flexibility, and fluency (Hu & Adey, 2002; Hu et al., et al.,
2010; Shen et al., 2002; Torrance, 1966), and lack of the 2009; Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Kounios & Beeman,
discussion on validity. In addition, there have been more studies 2013; Luo & Niki, 2003; Qiu et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2013; Fan
on crea- tive problem finding processes based on a limited et al.,
number of simple objects, text images, or artificial problem 2014; Li et al., 2016). In the field of creative problem finding,
situations (Chen & Zheng, 2011; Runco et al., 2005; Wang (2013) used electroencephalogram (EEG) analysis to
Runco & Okuda, find that mixed-handedness individuals had higher scores
1988; Torrance, 1966). However, less real-world experimental on creative scientific problem finding and more α waves in
material was used to measure the problem finding. Validity is the frontal and middle frontal regions, which suggested that
one of the most essential characteristics of creative crea- tive scientific problems arose from the involvement of key
thinking and a good indicator of creative ability (Sternberg brain regions such as the frontal lobe, which was
& Lubart, responsible for inhibitory control and conceptual bonding.
1993; Wu et al., 2008), and, compared to the standard divergent Tong et al. (2013) used Functional Magnetic Resonance
thinking test, the real-world questions finding is a better Imaging (fMRI) to expl- ore the brain mechanism of the
pre- dictor of individual creativity (Okuda, Runco, & Berger, problem finding in the context of scientific invention. The
1991). Therefore, to explore the cognitive process and brain results showed that left fusiform Gyrus, left medial frontal
mecha- nism of creative problem-finding, it is necessary to use Gyrus(L-MFG), left lenticular nucl- eus, right cerebellum, and
experi- mental materials which are more suitable to the nature left precentral Gyrus were activated. Tong et al. (2013) used a
of crea- tive thinking and have more ecological validity. similar approach to explore the ques- tion of the scientific
The study of scientific problems is the most typical and invention with or without prototypes and found that the brain
creative activity of human beings, and the field in which activation of the question raised in the prototype elicitation
the creative thinking is most fully embodied and effect was localized in the precuneus and angular gyrus. Zhou
concentrated is the scientific field (Qian, 1999). Scientific (2015) used the Event-related potential (ERP) technique to
creativity is the con- crete expression of general creativity in find significant differences in the amplitude of the P3
scientific disciplines, and its core is scientific creative component of novelty assessment tasks among indivi- duals
thinking, that is, scientific thinking activities with novelty with highly creative scientific questions. It shows that their
and value (Hu & Han, 2015). Besides, problem finding is working memory storage and concept representation upda-
an important stage in scientific
TONG DanDan et al.: The neural basis of scientific innovation problem finding
ting are more efficient, and their attention pattern is more more widely used local spontaneous activity (Wei et al., 2013).
flexible. Thus, they can evaluate the novelty of pictures more Studies
effectively. Although these studies are important for
understanding the nature of creative questions, the
differences in experimental paradigms and techniques and
the paucity of studies make it difficult to corroborate and
support each other’s results. In addi- tion, there is still a lack of
consensus on the neural mechanism of creative science, and the
research on the neural mechanism of scientific creativity still
needs to be strengthened (Hu & Han,
2015). Therefore, it is necessary to study the neural
mecha- nisms of creative problems by using more abundant
brain ima- ging techniques, especially without the limitation
of experi- mental tasks.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging can record spontan-
eous neural activity in the brain without external cognitive
tasks (Biswal, Yetkin, Haughton, & Hyde, 2010; Zuo et
al.,
2010). Therefore, the method is independent of the task
and measures the blood oxygen level of the human brain
at rest, depending on the low frequency fluctuation signal. It is
a good and stable tool for identifying differences in spontaneous
neural activity in the brain (Buckner, 2012; Fulwiler, King, &
Zhang,
2012). Based on the resting State Functional Magnetic
Reso- nance Imaging Technique, previous studies have
explored the neural mechanisms of creative thinking from the
perspective of individual differences, suggesting to some extent
that individuals with different levels of creativity have
specific brain bases. Takeuchi et al. (2012) used the seed-
point-based Resting state functional connectivity (RSFC)
analysis to find a significant positive correlation between
individuals’ performance on div- ergent thinking tasks and
RSFC intensity between the medial prefrontal cortex and the
posterior cingulate cortex. Wei et al. (2014) found a
significant positive correlation between Tor- rance’s
divergent thinking test scores and the strength of the
functional connectivity between the mPFC and the middle
temporal gyrus. Chen et al. (2014) further found that there was
a significant negative correlation between individual
creative achievement test scores and RSFC intensity in bilateral
anterior cingulate and middle frontal gyrus, and that
cognitive flexibility had a mediating effect on the relationship.
Li et al. (2016) used Regional homogeneity (ReHo) and low
frequency amplitude (ALFF) analyses and found that the
correct rate of scientific problem-solving was positively
correlated with the ReHo and ALFF values of left anterior
cingulate. Some researchers used seed-point-based resting
functional connectivity has been used to explore long-distance
functional connectivity in creative thinking or used focus
on the local activity of spontaneous BOLD (Blood oxygen
level dependent) signals in brain regions while lacking a
complete exploration of the brain mechanism of creative
thinking from the perspective of resting state brain imaging.
In addition, some researchers have explored the brain
mechanism of creative thinking by using divergent thinking
tasks, creative achievement questionnaires, and scientific in-
vention problem solving materials, but not from the perspective
of creative problems.
ALFF uses the average of the amplitudes at all frequencies
in the 0.01-0.08 Hz Band to represent the BOLD signal inten-
sity of each voxel, which reflects the level of spontaneous ac-
tivity of each voxel at rest in terms of energy (Zang et
al.,
2007). It reflects individual differences not only in state,
but also in personality, and it is a useful measure of the
have suggested ALFF is related to a variety of cognitive abili-
ties, personality traits, or psychopathology disorders, such as
working memory (Zou et al., 2013), well-being (Luo et al.,
2015), and Alzheimer’s disease (He et al., 2007). RSFC reflects
the connectivity of spontaneous neural signals between
different brain regions and the synergistic effect of the formation
of net- works in different brain regions, and it can reveal the
internal neural circuits of external behavior through well-
connected and integrated brain networks. Moreover, it is a useful
indicator of the spontaneous activity of neural networks in the
brain (Mennes et al. 2010). This approach has been widely used
in studies of individual cognitive abilities, such as creativity
(Takeuchi et al.,
2012). Also, it may reflect associations between various psycho-
pathology disorders, such as social anxiety disorder (Hahn et al.,
2011) and depression (Zeng et al., 2012). Using both ALFF
(resting state spontaneous signal) and RSFC (coordination of
signals from different brain regions), we can effectively and
completely detect the creative problem posed by the stable
functional organization of the brain.
Studies have shown that there are systematic differences
between high creative problem finding individuals and low
creative problem finding individuals in the ability to
generate novel ideas and suppress irrelevant information
(Tong et al.,
2013; Hu et al., 2015; Wang, 2013; Zhou, 2015). The
medial frontal lobe is thought to be a classic brain region for
generat- ing and evaluating novel ideas, and as a key point
of “cons- cious effort,” it is mainly responsible for thought
processes such as cognitive control (Beaty et al., 2014; Chen et
al., 2014; Fink et al., 2010; Kounios et al., 2006; Luo &
Zhang, 2006; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2011; Talati & Hirsch,
2005). Accord- ingly, we hypothesize that ALFF values in the
mPFC may be associated with creative scientific problem
finding. In addition, some researchers have found that the mPFC,
as the core node of the default network, plays an important role
in the generation of creative ideas by increasing the strength
of its functional connections to other nodes in the default
network (Takeuchi et al., 2012; Wei et al., 2013). Scientific
Creativity is closely related to the frontal lobe, parietal lobe,
and cingulate gyrus (Jung et al., 2010; Limb & Braun,
2008; Shen et al., 2010). Accordingly, we selected the medial
prefrontal lobe as the seed point and hypothesized that the ratio
of creative scientific ques- tions had a significant positive
correlation with the strength of functional connectivity among
Default Network nodes such as the medial prefrontal lobe, the
posterior cingulate/precuneus (Takeuchi et al., 2012), and the
inferior parietal lobe (related to spatial representation and
processing) (Gansler et al., 2011).
Based on these assumptions, this study will use ALFF and
RSFC, which are used in the Functional magnetic resonance
imaging technique, and select the scientific invention problem
with high ecological validity in real life as experimental mate-
rials to explore the relationship between spontaneous brain
activity and creative scientific problem finding.
2 Method
2.1 Participants
In the study, 107 healthy college students were recruited
through online advertisements, 3 participants with excessive
head movement data were deleted, and 104 participants (mean
age 19.26 ± 0.99 years) were used for formal analysis.
There were 32 males (mean age 19.66 ± 1.15 years) and 72
females (mean age 19.08 ± 0.85 years). In order to test
whether the
Acta Psychologica
sample size was reasonable, the software G*Power 3.1 blem in the current test is more similar to the puzzle in
(Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009) was used to life, which makes up for the lack of ecological validity in the
perform the Post hoc statistical efficacy test (effect size (f 2) = pre- vious test materials. The experimental materials used in
0. 25, α = 0. this study were carefully prepared by the research group and
05). The result showed that power = 0.99, indicating that were used in other studies (Tong, 2017). Previous studies
the sample size was sufficient. All participants had a have shown that the rate of validity questions correlates
normal or corrected vision, no history of mental illness or significantly with the score on the creative achievement test,
surgical trauma, computer skills, and no prior exposure to indicating that the material presented by the creative scientific
the material. After entering the laboratory, the participant first invention ques- tion can, to some extent, predict the level of
signed the informed consent, and then the subject introduced the creative achieve- ment in real life, with a certain degree of
experimental proc- edure. The participants completed the effectiveness. In addi- tion, individuals engaged in different
behavior experiment outside the scanning room and then were innovative work (Rese- arching Employees and Operation
sent to the scanning room by the scanner for scanning in a Employees) showed signifi- cant differences in the scores of
resting state. Appropriate remuneration would be paid after scientific invention questions. After covariant age and
the experiment. All experi- mental procedures and education and other additional vari- ables, the results
procedures of the institute have been approved by the showed that researching employees raised more valid
committee on academic ethics. questions than operation employees, and the experi- mental
2.2 Experimental materials results further validated the validity of the experimental
From the material library of creative science problem, 9 materials in the Enterprise Environment (Tong, 2017).
problem situations with appropriate difficulty were selected for 2.3 Collection and evaluation of behavioral data
the formal experiment (Tong, 2017). The experimental E-prime software was used to program and present the exp-
materials were compiled by sifting through examples of erimental materials. Based on the existing research on scientific
scientific inven- tion creation, all of which met the conditions of invention (Wu et al., 2008), this study adopted the “9-to-9”
novelty, easy to understand, and not involving the two-stage experimental paradigm of “learning prototype-posing
knowledge of specialized fields. Each material contained problem” to investigate the brain mechanism of individuals’
potentially contradictory problem situations, old problems with novel validity problem. The process was as follows: First, the
a certain amount of misdirection, and the prototype with Fixation Point “+” was presented in the center of the
heuristic information. A sample of the experimental material: screen with white words on a black background for 0.5 seconds,
Problem situation: There are a lot of asteroids around then nine prototype materials were presented randomly. The
the earth that would be catastrophic if they hit the earth. partici- pants read the prototype materials carefully, and
But the launch of an atomic bomb to destroy the asteroids will then pressed the “1” key after understanding them, and the
bring all kinds of atomic radiation hazards. button jumped to the next prototype material. Each prototype
Old question: How do you deal with the fallout of an atomic material was rend- ered up to 60 seconds and automatically
bomb blowing up an asteroid? jumped to the next when the time was up until all the
Prototype: When we hit a moving billiard ball with a little prototype materials were rendered. Then, the fixation point
force while playing billiards, it will change direction and leave “+” was presented in the center of the screen again, with
the original track. white words on a black back- ground for 0.5 seconds, and
Based on the theory of “transformation of then nine problem situations and old problems were
representation” randomly presented. After 60 seconds, the question situation
and “prototype elicitation,” when individuals encounter and the old question disappeared and were presented with a
problem situations with stereotyped thinking and old problems prompt. On the answer sheet, the participants wrote down the
(thinking deadlocks), they tend to make representations questions in the order in which the questions were presented.
according to the ways suggested by the problem situations After writing the questions, they pressed the “1” button. The
and search in the corresponding error problem space. If an prompt disappeared and moved on to the next question situation
individual wants to ask creative questions, he or she needs until all the problem situations and old probl- ems were
to enter the meta-problem space and search for other presented. The experimental procedure is shown in Figure 1.
appropriate problem representations, then he or she may ask
novel questions. Besides, by activating semantically connected
heuristic information in mind or in the environment and making
connections with problem situations, it is possible to ask novel
and effective creative questions (Holman,
2018; Kaplan & Simon, 1990; Tong, 2017; Zhang et al., 2012).
The situation of implicit contradiction can cause the individual
to think and guess the explanation of the question. When
the setting of the old problems adopts the contradictory
situation of individual reading, the simple question without any
value will first come to mind, which can provide the wrong
thinking set and lead the individual into the wrong problem
space. The pro- vision of prototype can not only help
individuals jump out of the wrong horizontal space, but also Figure 1. Flow chart of the experiment
point out the direction of the problem for individuals to enter Based on the theory of “representation transformation” and
the right problem space. “prototype activation,” the evaluation criteria of creative scien-
The problem of creative scientific invention presents the
ability of materials to focus on how to jump out of the wrong
problem space caused by situations and old problems and then
find the right problem direction. The stereotype of the old pro-
TONG DanDan et al.: The neural basis of scientific innovation problem
tific problem materials are formulated (Kaplan & Simon, 1990; Gauss the image to increase the signal to noise ratio; (6)
Zhang et al., 2012). If the answer is a simple repetition of the the effect of low and high-frequency noise is reduced, and de-
old question or repetition of its meaning, then the individual is linear drift is performed.
still bound to the old question space, such as: “How to reduce 2.5 Statistical analysis
the effects of atomic radiation? /How to make a nuclear weapon Calculation and analysis of ALFF. Using the REST
with low radiation?” The point is 0. If the answer is not tied to (Rest- ing-state FMRI data toolkit) toolkit (Song et al., 2011),
the problem space of the old question but to the future and is accord- ing to the method of Zang et al. (2007), the time series
imaginative and novel, such as: “Is a planet colliding with the of each voxel in the whole brain is Fourier transform, and then
Earth more dangerous or is atomic radiation more dangerous? the area under the peak of the frequency domain power
Why did the asteroid hit the earth?” The point is 1. If the ans- spectrum is squ- ared, the resulting value can represent the
wer is not stuck in the old problem space and presents a scien- amplitude of the signal oscillation. At the same time, the
tific hypothesis with some idea of how to solve the square root of frequ- ency between 0.01 to 0.08 of each voxel
problem, which is valid or valuable, for example: “if a was averaged, that is, the ALFF value. To achieve
particular trajec- tory can be designed to keep the asteroid off standardization, the ALFF value of each voxel is divided by the
the planet by itself? / Can other forces be used to knock the whole-brain mean and normalized to obtain a standardized
asteroid off its course?” The point is 2. The rate of novelty ALFF value for each voxel (Biswal et al.,
problems refers to the rate of items with a non-zero score in the 2010). In order to explore the relationship between creative
total test items, and the rate of novelty validity problems refers scientific problem-finding ability and low-frequency amplitude,
to the rate of items with 2 scores in the total test items. The a voxel-wise analysis of variance (ANCOVA) was used to
higher the rate of novelty problem, the more likely the analyze the whole brain. Age and gender were irrelevant
participants can break through the old cognitive model, jump variables, and creative science problem finding scores was
out of the wrong question space, and ask novel questions. variables of interest. Multiple Linear regression was used to
Besides, the higher the rate of novelty validity problem, detect the problem and propose the relationship between the
the more likely the participants can form effective score and ALFF. The signi- ficance was corrected by
connections related to task goals and raise valuable topological FDR, and the significance level was p < 0.05 (two
questions through cognitive processes such as thought reor- tails) (Chumbley & Friston, 2009).
ganization or prototype elicitation. Calculation and analysis of RSFC. Also, using the
2.4 Static data acquisition and preprocessing REST toolkit and based on seed point correlation analysis, the
The Resting State images were obtained with Siemens regions of interest (ROI) were the mPFC (mPFC, x = 0, Y = 54,
Medical 3 Tesla magnetic resonance Imaging. Block foam pads Z = 8) (Beaty et al., 2014; Whitfield-gabrieli & Nieto-Castanon,
and earplugs were used to reduce head movement and instru- 2012), which were selected from anatomically represented in
ment noise. Before scanning, the participants were asked to the clas- sical literature and significantly associated with
change the special clothing in the laboratory, and, at the same ALFF in the study separately, and a circle with a radius of 6
time, the metal jewelry was removed to avoid the influence of mm was establi- shed by taking the coordinates of the region of
the metal objects on the security and image quality of the parti- interest as the center. The calculation method was as follows:
cipants. During the scan, participants were asked to lie First, the time series of all voxels in each region of interest
back with their eyes closed and relax, keeping their heads still were extracted, and the whole brain signal, white matter, brain
and resting their eyes closed throughout the scan (Wang et al., crest fluid, and six head motion parameters were used as
2011). meaningless variables for regression. Then, the correlation
The whole-brain plane Gradient Echo planar imaging (EPI) coefficients between each seed point and other brain regions
was used for scanning. The relevant scanning parameters were were calculated by voxel-wise Correlation Analysis and
above: repetition time (TR) = 2000ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, Fisher’s r-to-z transformation. Finally, we used multivariate
flip angle = 90°, field of view (FOV) =220 mm × 220 Regression analysis. Age and gender were independent
mm, thickness = 3 mm, gap = 1 mm, Voxel size =3.4 mm × 3.4 variables, scores on creative science questions were
mm × presented as variables of interest, and the correlation bet- ween
4 mm, and acquisition Matrix = 64×64. The total scanning time the strength of functional connectivity and creative science
was 8 minutes and 4 seconds. A total of 242 consecutive questions was calculated, further testing for the existence
im- ages were obtained. of specific network connections can significantly predict the
Based on Matlab, Data Processing Assistant for ability of creative scientific problem-finding. The
Rest- ing-State fMRI software (DPARSF) is used to significance level was also adjusted by topological FDR
process Data (Yan & Zang, 2010). The steps are as and was p < 0.05(two tails) (Chumbley & Friston, 2009). Use
follows: (1) check the data quality of the rest state to see G*Power Software to calculate the effect of multivariate
whether there is incomplete scanning or artifact of the regression analysis results (Faul et al., 2009).
participant data; (2) convert the original DICOM data into
analyzable Nifti data format; (3) in order to obtain a stable MRI 3 Results
image, the data of the first 10 time points are eliminated, and 3.1 Behavioral data results
the remaining stable data are gradually subjected to slice The rate of novelty problem and the rate of novelty validity
timing and head motion correction. Then, delete translation problem were tested by an independent sample t-test. The
greater than 3 mm and rotation greater than 3 degrees (delete 3 results showed that there were no significant gender differences
participants) and remove physiological noise; (4) standardize in the two variables (ps > 0.05). Specifically, the accuracy rate
the processed data to the MNI (Montreal neu- rologic institute) of nov- elty problem raised by male participants was 0.86 ±
Standard Spatial Template by using DARTEL (ancestral 0.19, and that of female participants was 0.90 ± 0.10, with no
morphological registration through expanded Lie Algebra), significant difference between male and female participants
and the normalized voxel size is 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm; (5) 6 (p = 0.184). Besides, the novelty validity problem rate of male
mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) is used to participants
Acta Psychologica

Table 1
Mean and standard deviation of the rate of creative scientific problem finding
Measurement index Mean number 95% CI Standard deviation Minimum value Maximum value Score range
Novelty problem 0.89 0.86 ~ 0.91 0.14 0.11 1.00 0.89
Validity problem 0.33 0.28 ~ 0.38 0.26 0.00 0.89 0.89
Table 2
ALFF and novel validity questions were presented with rates significantly correlated to brain regions
MNI ES
Cerebral area Hemispheres T value
(maximum point) (Voxels) f 2
X Y Z
ALFF is positively correlated with the
proposed rate of novel validity problem
vmPFC L −6 12 −18 3.51 74 0.19
anterior cerebellum R 9 −42 −30 4.53 124 0.25

Figure 2. ALFF values and novel validity questions were presented in a rate-significant correlation between brain regions.
was 0.33 ± 0.27, and that of female participants was 0.33 cerebellum (Table 2 and Figure 2). No regions of the brain were
± found to be significantly associated with novelty problem find-
0.26. There was no significant difference between male ing rates.
and female participants (p = 0.945). Then, the correlation 3.2.2 Results of RSFC analysis
analysis between the rate of novelty problem and the rate The functional connections between the whole brain and the
of novelty validity problem was carried out. The results seed sites in the ALFF-behavioral region (L-mPFC) and
showed that the correlation between the two columns was the creative thinking region (R-mPFC) in the classical
significant r = 0.343, p < 0.001. The average and standard literature were analyzed by multiple regression analysis,
deviation of creative science problem formulation rates are respectively (Beaty et al., 2014). The results showed that
shown in Table 1. after controlling the covariates such as age and sex, the
3.2 Brain imaging data functional connectivity between the R-mPFC and the left
3.2.1 ALFF results Cuneus were positively cor- related with the rate of novel
The rate of ALFF and novelty problem and novelty validity validity questions finding (x = −3,
problem were analyzed by multiple regression analysis, and sex 2
Y = −90, Z = 33; mass size = 249 Voxel; t = 4.03; f = 0.13; r =
and age were controlled as covariables. The results showed that 0.33; p < 0.001; Figure 3). The stronger the functional
after controlling for covariates, the rate of new validity connection between medial prefrontal lobe and left Cuneus, the
ques- tions was positively correlated with the ALFF values of higher the rate of novel validity problem finding. There was no
the left ventromedial prefrontal (L-vmPFC) and the right significant
anterior cer-
TONG DanDan et al.: The neural basis of scientific innovation problem

Figure 3. Functional connectivity networks with seed points were able to predict the regions of the brain that presented novel validity
problems.
correlation between the strength of functional connectivity individuals with highly creative questions are better at
in the Left Medial prefrontal (L-mPFC) and other brain supp- ressing irrelevant information and are more likely to
regions and the rate at which valid questions were posed. activate conceptual network connections in mind (Hu et al.,
4 Discussion 2015; Wang,
2013; Zhou, 2015). Kounios et al. (2006) found that
For the first time, the problem situation of scientific priming for insight was associated with significant activation
invention with high ecological validity was selected as the of the medial frontal lobe, which might be associated with
experimental material, using high spatial Resolution Resting increased top-down cognitive control, such as inhibition of
State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Technique and thinking, choosing the right strategy, and ignoring the wrong
widely used ALFF and RSFC, exploring the functional basis of one. Dar- saud et al. investigated the neural mechanisms of
the brain raised by questions in the situation of scientific delayed epiphanies using the digital reduction task and
invention, and thus provi- ding data support for further found that the mPFC might be involved in semantic
understanding the nature of creative thinking. It was found that associations, rules finding, and other creative related cognitive
the intensity and functional connec- tivity of resting processes (Darsaud et al.,
spontaneous neural activity reflected individual differences in 2011; Lang et al., 2006). In addition, several studies have hypo-
the questions posed in the situation of scientific invention. thesized that the medial frontal lobe is involved in
Specifically, in the ALFF analysis, the higher the rate at cognitive processes such as conflict processing, response
which novel validity questions were posed, the greater the selection, and inhibition control (Moore et al., 2009;
spontaneous activity in the left medial prefrontal lobe and the Takeuchi et al., 2012). Therefore, this study concludes that the
right anterior cerebellum. In a further RSFC analysis, a medial prefrontal lobe may help the high-problem speaker to
significant positive correlation was found between the proposed better suppress irrelevant information, break away from the
novel validity problem ratio and the strength of functional constraint of the old problem space, integrate the semantic
connectivity between the Right Medial prefrontal lobe and the meaning of each sentence, estab- lish semantic connection
left cuneiform lobe. The results of two kinds of resting between seemingly remote problem situations and heuristic
state brain image data showed that the mPFC played an information (prototype or original kno- wledge of the
important role in the problem-finding process of scientific individual), and then lead to constructive or sci- entifically
invention. hypothetical questions.
Creative thinking is the process of producing novel and app- In addition, previous studies have shown that highly creative
licable ideas or products. Therefore, both the general field scientific problems suggest that individuals are better able
of creative thinking (e.g., divergent thinking, convergent to store stimuli separately in working memory based on their
thinking, etc.) and domain-specific creative thinking (such mea- ning in the task, thus improving the efficiency of
as scientific creativity, artistic creativity, etc.) involve the information processing (Zhou, 2015). Howard-jones et al.
generation and evaluation of novel ideas (Sternberg & (2005) used the fabrication task to explore the neural
Lubart, 1993; Feist, mechanisms of creative perspectives and found that the
1998). Both of them have also been found to be associated with fabrication condition signific- antly activated both medial
activity in the mPFC and middle frontal gyrus, which is resp- prefrontal Lobes as opposed to the fabrication of non-creative
onsible for novel ideas (Darsaud et al., 2011; Hao et al., 2013; Stories, which might reflect that the medial prefrontal lobe
Gilbert et al., 2010; Fink et al., 2010; Limb & Braun, was related to the storage of working memory and the
2008). Therefore, consistent with the previous results, the retrieval of situational memory. In addition, the medial
positive correlation between medial prefrontal spontaneity and prefrontal lobe is thought to play an important role in
the rate of creative scientific problem finding in the current manipulating information in working memory (Cairo et al.,
study can be understood to be related to the generation of novel 2004). In this study, rather than simply repeating the old prob-
ideas. lem, the process of finding appropriate heuristic archetypes in
However, scientific creativity follows stronger logical rules, mind, making novel connections, and forming new
relies more on purposeful information processing that excludes problems with scientific hypotheses increased the working
all extraneous information, and places more emphasis on reca- memory load. Therefore, the medial frontal lobe may be
pitulating and synthesizing existing seemingly disparate know- involved in the proc- ess of information storage and retrieval in
ledge and the ability to create new scientific knowledge (Bai et working memory.
al., In addition, Beaty et al. (2014) believed that the emergence
2014; Hu & Han, 2015). Previous studies have also found that of new ideas was related to the Default Mode Network (DMN)
Acta Psychologica
of the classic Brain Areas (such as medial prefrontal, posterior uneus Lobes, play a prominent role in mental imagery. In addi-
cingulate, and bilateral inferior parietal lobule). Takeuchi et al. tion, previous research has shown that the precuneus, together
(2012) explored the relationship between creative thinking and with the prefrontal cortex, controls source memory, in
functional connectivity between the MPFC and other brain which the source of the memory is recalled. The function of the
regions. It was found that there was a significant positive corre- prec- uneus in this process is to provide sufficient context
lation between divergent thinking test scores and the strength of for the prefrontal cortex to choose the correct past memory
functional connectivity between the two Default (Lunds- trom, Ingvar, & Petersson, 2005). In scientific
Network Nodes: the MPFC and posterior cingulate. Wei et creativity, such episodic memory is associated with the recall of
al. (2014) further found a significant positive correlation an individual’s heuristic knowledge (Luo et al., 2013; Qiu et al.,
between Torra- nce divergent thinking test scores and the 2010). These results demonstrate that the precuneus is involved
strength of the func- tional connectivity between the MPFC and in informa- tion retrieval and visual retrieval through the
the middle temporal gyrus, which was also the default network representation of physical images in the brain. Based on
node. Therefore, the MPFC, as the core node of the default this, this study conc- ludes that individuals with high-
front of the network, played an important role in the creativity questions may have more vivid visual mental imagery
generation of creative ideas. In addition, Takeuchi et al. (2010) and better episodic memory, which makes heuristic
used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) to find a significant prototypical information better stored and extracted. Thus,
positive correlation between the structural integrity of the functional connections between the medial frontal lobe
MPFC white matter and the scores of divergent thinking and the wedge lobe provide the basis for these cognitive
tests. Structural connectivity is the basis for functional functions.
connectivity between brain regions (Greicius et al., Previous studies have shown that individuals with high
2003), and higher individual white matter structural or low creative problems difference in the allocation of attention
integrity makes stronger functional connectivity possible (AU resources. The concrete manifestation is that the highly creative
Duong et al., question raises the individual attention resources allocation and
2005). Therefore, the enhancement of medial prefrontal functi- the attention pattern transformation to be more flexible and so
onal connectivity in high problem finding individuals may on (Feist, 1998; Wang, 2013; Zhou, 2015). Qiu et al.
be partly related to the higher structural integrity of the (2010) used word puzzles as experimental material to explore
MPFC white matter in high-creative individuals. the brain mechanisms of the preparation and resolution stages of
The process of scientific activity also involves the transfer insight problems and found that the cerebellum was
and transformation of information from one domain to another, significantly activ- ated by the problem solving state. From
involving a great deal of abstract reasoning (Bai et al., this, the researchers speculated that the Cerebellum might
2014; Hu & Han, 2015). Previous research has found that play a role in attention resource allocation, perceptual
compared with the temporal lobe and motor cortex are activated reorganization, and information retrieval. Tong et al. (2013)
by artistic creativity, there was more activation of the found that the problem raised in the scientific invention context
parietal cortex in scientific creativity (Gilbert et al., 2010; also involved the left medial frontal lobe and the right
Luo et al., 2013; O'Boyle et al., 2013; Shen et al., 2010). Cerebellum, in which the right cerebellum might cooperate
Moreover, the precuneus is a common activated brain region with the medial frontal lobe to regulate and allocate
in reasoning and prob- lem-solving tasks ((Ferstl & Yves von attention resources. Houk (2005) supported the Cereb- ellum
Cramon, 2001; Luo et al., was responsible for maintaining an individual’s ongoing
2013; Qiu et al., 2010). In the RSFC analysis of the behavior or thought and provided corrective functions for ongo-
present study, there was a significant positive correlation ing tasks. Li (2014) further found that the density of gray matter
between the rate of proposed novel validity problem and the in the posterior cerebellum of the high academic achievement
strength of the functional connection between the right medial group was significantly higher than that of the Control Group,
frontal lobe and the left cuneiform lobe, which maintained a which might reflect better language integration and
high consistency with the existing results and verified the response inhibition in the high academic achievement group.
reliability of the exp- erimental results. Moreover, Fink et al. Therefore, this study concludes that when individuals pose
(2014) found that the flexibility and fluency scores of questions based on a problem situation that contains
verbal imagination creativity tests were positively correlated contradictions, highly creative problem-makers make better use
with the gray matter density of the right cuneus and precuneus. of the cerebellum to keep the goal of questioning in mind,
The completion of the verbal imagination creativity test allocate more attention to the contradictions or gaps in the
requires the generation of flexible ideas, the use of diverse situation, reorganize these contra- dictions more deeply, and
information, and problem-oriented solutions (rather than then integrate language to find new and effective scientific
unsystematic and unrealistic illusions). Therefore, this study problems.
concludes that in the present situation of creative scientific In this study, the question situation of the scientific
problem-finding, the reasoning process, viewpoint inven- tion with high ecological validity was used as the
generation, and problem-oriented thinking associated with experimental material, and the analysis method based on low
precuneus make it possible to pose novel and effective frequency amp- litude and static state function connection was
problems. adopted with the Functional magnetic resonance imaging
Andreasen and Ramchandran (2012) found that cuneus cor- technique to explore the brain mechanism of creative
relates with the performance of Word Association tasks, which scientific problems. The res- ults show that the validity of
might reflect a strong association between these brain regions the novel problem is realized through the cooperative
and visual imagery. Shen et al. (2012) also found association between mPFC and other brain regions, which
that “non-verbal” visual spatial information processing provides some evidence for further rev- ealing the neural
networks composed of cuneus and precuneus facilitated the mechanism of the scientific creative problem. Moreover, it is
effective conversion of problem representations. These the first study to analyze the problem of scient- ific invention
results suggest that the parietal-occipital regions, such as the systematically by using the resting state data index.
cuneus and prec-
TONG DanDan et al.: The neural basis of scientific innovation problem
The current study also has some shortcomings, such as bility by a combined voxel-based morphometry and resting-state
using only the more basic analysis methods of the resting state functional connectivity study. Neuroimage, 102, 474–483.
functi- onal magnetic resonance imaging and lack of other Cheng, L. F., Hu, W. P., Jia, X. J., & Runco, M. A. (2016). The different
modal magnetic resonance imaging and more complex network role of cognitive inhibition in early versus late creative problem find-
analysis. Therefore, diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI), ing. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity & the Arts, 10(1), 32–41.
Dynamic Causal Modeling (DCM), and small-world network Chumbley, J. R., & Friston, K. J. (2009). False discovery rate revisited:
can be used in fut- ure research to help us explore the neural FDR and topological inference using Gaussian random fields. Neuro-
mechanisms of crea- tive scientific questions more image, 44(1), 62–70.
Darsaud, A., Wagner, U., Balteau, E., Desseilles, M., Sterpenich, V.,
comprehensively. Besides, the current study is a cross-
Van- dewalle, G., ... Maquet, P. (2011). Neural precursors of delayed
sectional study with adults as partici- pants. In order to
insight. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(8), 1900–1910.
explore the relationship between brain develo- pment and
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroima-
creative problems, we should consider the Longitu- dinal study ging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5),
with children and adolescents as subjects. Therefore, future 822.
research can also be carried out longitudinal follow-up Fan, L. Y., Fan, X. F., Luo, W. C., Wu, G. H., Yan, X., Yin, D. Z., …
studies to explore creative questions and propose changes Xu, D. R. (2014). An explorative fMRI study of human creative
in brain mechanisms over time. In addition, brain activity is thinking using a specially designed iCAD system. Acta Psychologica
inse- parable from the creative environment. To fully Sinica,
describe the cognitive neural mechanisms of creative problems, 46(4), 427–436.
future rese- arch must also consider the interaction between Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical
the brain and multiple environmental systems in the creative power analyses using g*power 3.1: Tests for correlation and
process and conduct multi-level research. regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160.
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and
References artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4),
290–309.
Alabbasi, A. M. A., & Cramond, B. (2018). The creative problem
Ferstl, E. C., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2001). The role of coherence and
finding hierarchy: A suggested model for understanding problem
cohesion in text comprehension: An event- related fMRI study.
finding. Creativity. Theories–Research-Applications, 5(2), 197–229.
Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 325–340.
Alabbasi, A. M. A., Paek, S. H., Cramond, B., & Runco, M. A. (2020).
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Gebauer, D., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., &
Problem finding and creativity: A meta-analytic review. Psychology
Ebner, F. (2010). Enhancing creativity by means of cognitive stimu-
of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(1), 3–14
lation: Evidence from an fmri study. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1687–1695.
Andreasen, N. C., Ramchandran, K. (2012). Creativity in art and
Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Hutterer, L., Steiner, E., Benedek, M., Weber,
science: Are there two cultures? Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience,
B., ... Weiss, E. M. (2014). Gray matter density in relation to diff-
14(1),
erent facets of verbal creativity. Brain Structure and Function,
49–54.
219(4), 1263–1269.
Au Duong, M. V., Boulanouar, K., Audoin, B., Treseras, S., Ibarrola, D.,
Fulwiler, C. E., King, J. A., & Zhang, N. (2012). Amygdala–
Malikova, I., ... Cozzone, P. J. (2005). Modulation of effective con-
orbito- frontal resting-state functional connectivity is associated
nectivity inside the working memory network in patients at the ear-
with trait anger. Neuroreport, 23(10), 606–610.
liest stage of multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage, 24(2), 533–538.
Gansler, D. A., Moore, D. W., Susmaras, T. M., Jerram, M. W., Sousa,
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2009). “Aha!”:
J., & Heilman, K. M. (2011). Cortical morphology of visual
The neural correlates of verbal insight solutions. Human Brain
creativity. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2527–2532.
Mapping,
Getzels, J. W. (2011). Problem-finding and the inventiveness of solutions.
30(3), 908–916.
Journal of Creative Behavior, 9(1), 12–18.
Bai, X. J., Gong, Y. B., Hu, W. P., Han, Q., & Yao, H. J. (2014). The
Gilbert, S. J., Zamenopoulos, T., Alexiou, K., & Johnson, J. F. (2010).
inhi- bitory mechanism of individuals with different scientific
Involvement of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ill-structured
creativity. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 12(2), 151–155.
design cognition: An fMRI study. Brain Research, 1312(2), 79–88.
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Wilkins, R. W., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Silvia, P.
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., & Henretty, N. (2011). Verbalization and
J., ... Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Creativity and the default network: A
problem solving: Insight and spatial factors. British Journal of Psy-
functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuro-
chology, 101(1), 81–93.
psychologia, 64C (2), 92.
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functi-
Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). Func-
onal connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the def-
tional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using
ault mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
echo-planar mri. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 34(4), 537–541. ences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253–258.
Buckner, R. L. (2012). The serendipitous discovery of the brain's default Hahn, A., Stein, P., Windischberger, C., Weissenbacher, A., Spindeleg-
network. Neuroimage, 62(2), 1137–1145. ger, C., Moser, E., ... Lanzenberger, R. (2011). Reduced rest-
Cairo, T. A., Liddle, P. F., Woodward, T. S., & Ngan, E. T. (2004). The ing-state functional connectivity between amygdala and orbitofron-
inf- luence of working memory load on phase specific patterns of tal cortex in social anxiety disorder. Neuroimage, 56(3), 881–889.
cortical activity. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 21(3), Han, Q., Hu, W. P., Liu, J., Jia, X. J., & Adey, P. (2013). The influence
377. of peer interaction on students creative problem- finding ability.
Chen, B. R., Hu, W. P., & Plucker, J. A. (2016). The effect of mood on Creativity Research Journal, 25(3), 248–258.
pro- blem finding in scientific creativity. The Journal of Hao, X., Cui, S., Li, W. F., Yang, W. J., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. L. (2013).
Creative Beh- avior, 50(4), 308–320. Enhancing insight in scientific problem solving by highlighting the
Chen, L. J., Zhang, Q. L., & Cai. Z., (2006). The effect of the stimuli- functional features of prototypes: An fMRI study. Brain Research,
pre- senting mode on children's problem-finding in different fields. 1534:46–54.
Journal of Psychological Science, (02), 43–46. He, Y., Wang, L., Zang, Y. F., Tian, L., Zhang, X. Q., Li, K., & Jiang, T.
Chen, L. J., & Zheng, X. (2011). The exploratory study of cognitive (2007). Regional coherence changes in the early stages of Alzhei-
stages in problem-finding process. Psychological Exploration, 31(4),
332–337. Chen, Q. L., Yang, W. J., Li, W. F., Wei, D. T., Li, H. J., Lei,
Q., ... Qiu, J. (2014). Association of creative achievement with cognitive
flexi-
Acta Psychologica
mer’s disease: A combined structural and resting-state functional of scientific innovation problems solving. Acta Psychologica
MRI study. Neuroimage, 35(2), 488–500. Sinica,
Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Social Science 48(4), 331–342.
Electronic Publishing, 61(1), 569–598. Limb, C. J., & Braun, A. R. (2008) Neural substrates of spontaneous
Holman, D. M. (2018). What am I supposed to do? Problem Finding musi- cal performance: An fMRI Study of Jazz improvisation. PLoS
and its impact on Problem Solving. UC Merced. ONE,
Houk, J. C. (2005). Agenss of the mind. Biological Cybernetics, 92(6), 3(2), e1679.
427–437. Lundstrom, B. N., Ingvar, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2005). The role of
Howard-Jones, P. A., Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Samuel, E. A., Summers, precuneus and left inferior frontal cortex during source memory ep-
I. R., & Claxton, G. (2005). Semantic divergence and creative story isodic retrieval. Neuroimage, 27(4), 824–834.
generation: An fMRI investigation. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, Luo, J., & Niki, K. (2003). Function of hippocampus in “insight”
25(1), 240–250. of problem solving. Hippocampus, 13(3), 316–323.
Hu, W. P., & Adey, P. (2002). A scientific creativity test for secondary Luo, J., & Zhang, X. L. (2006). From the impasse to the breakthrough:
school students. International Journal of Science Education, 24(4), The brain basis for insightful problem solving. Advances of Psycho-
389–403. logical Science, 14(4): 484–489.
Hu, W. P., Cheng, L. F., Jia, X. J., Han, M., & Chen., Y. H. (2015). The Luo, J. L., Li, W. F., Qiu, J., Wei, D. T., Liu, Y. J., & Zhang, Q. L. (2013).
influence of Cognitive inhibition to Creative Scientific Problem Neural basis of scientific innovation induced by heuristic prototype.
Finding: Mediating effect of field cognitive style. Studies of Psych- PLoS ONE, 8(1), e49231.
ology and Behavior, 13(6), 721–728. Luo, Y. M., Li, B. L., Liu, J., Bi, C. Z., & Huang, X. T. (2015).
Hu, W. P., & Han, K. K. (2015). Theoretical research and Amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in happiness: A resting-
practical exploration of adolescents’scientific creativity. state fMRI study. Chinese Science Bulletin, 60(2), 170–178.
Psychological Deve- lopment and Education. 31(1), 44–50. Mennes, M., Kelly, C., Zuo, X. N., Martino, A. D., Biswal, B. B., Castel-
Hu, W. P., Shi, Q. Z., Han, Q., Wang, X. Q., & Adey, P. (2010). lanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2010). Inter- individual differences in
Creative scientific problem finding and its developmental trend. resting-state functional connectivity predict task-induced BOLD
Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 46–52. activity. Neuroimage, 50(4), 1690–1701.
Hu, W. P., & Zhou, P. (2010). The influences of motivation on creative Moore, D. W., Bhadelia, R. A., Billings, R. L., Fulwiler, C., Heilman, K.
scientific problem finding ability of first year students in high M., Rood, K. M. J., & Gansler, D. A. (2009). Hemispheric connecti-
school. Psychological Development and Education. (01), 34–39. vity and the visual–spatial divergent-thinking component of
Huang, F., Fan, J., & Luo, J. (2015). The neural basis of novelty and creativity. Brain & Cognition, 70(3), 267–272.
app- ropriateness in processing of creative chunk decomposition. O’Boyle, M. W., Cunnington, R., Silk, T. J., Vaughan, D., Jackson, G.,
Neuroi- mage, 113, 122–132. Syngeniotis, A. & Egan, G. F. (2005). Mathematically gifted male
Jia, X. J., Hu, W. P., Cai, F. C., Wang, H. H., Li, J., Runco, M. A., & adolescents activate a unique brain network during mental rotation.
Chen, Y. H. (2017). The influence of teaching methods on creative Cognitive Brain Research, 25(2), 583–587.
problem finding. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 24, 86–94. Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A., & Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the
Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., finding and solving of real-world problems. Journal of Psychoedu-
Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., ... Kounios, J. (2004). Neural acti- cational Assessment, 9(1), 45–53.
vity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS biology, Paletz, S. B. F., & Peng, K. (2009). Problem finding and contradiction:
2(4), e97. Examining the relationship between naive dialectical thinking, ethni-
Jung, R. E., Mead, B. S., Carrasco, J., & Flores, R. A. (2013). The stru- city, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 21(2–3), 139–151.
cture of creative cognition in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Qian, Z. H. (1999). A brief discussion of scientific problems. Journal of
Neuroscience, 7(2), 330. Jiangsu University (Social Science Edition), (2), 9–12.
Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Bockholt, J. H., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Qiu, J., Li, H., Jou, J., Liu, J., Luo, Y., Feng, T., ... Zhang, Q. (2010).
Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Neural correlates of the “Aha” experiences: Evidence from an fMRI
Human Brain Mapping. 31, 398–409. study of insight problem solving. Cortex, 46(3), 397–403.
Kaplan, C. A., & Simon, H. A. (1990). In search of insight. Cognitive Reiter-Palmon, R., & Robinson, E. J. (2009). Problem identification
psychology, 22(3), 374–419. and construction: What do we know, what is the future? Psychology
Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2013). The cognitive neuroscience of of Aesthetics Creativity & the Arts, 3(1), 43–47.
insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 71–93. Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., & Reiter-palmon, R. (2005). Explicit instruc-
Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subramaniam, tions to be creative and original: A comparison of strategies and crit-
K., Parrish, T. B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2006). The prepared mind eria as targets with three types of divergent thinking tests.
neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent Korean Journal of Thinking & Problem Solving, 15(1), 5–15.
solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science, 17(10), 882–890. Runco, M. A., & Okuda, S. M. (1988). Problem discovery, divergent
Lang, S., Kanngieser, N., Jaśkowski, P., Haider, H., Rose, M., & Verle- thinking, and the creative process. Journal of Youth and
ger, R. (2006). Precursors of insight in event-related brain potentials. Adoles- cence, 17(3), 211–220.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2152. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Adler, N., Aharon-Peretz, J., Perry, D., & Mayse-
Lee, H., & Cho, Y. (2007). Factors affecting problem finding depending less, N. (2011). The origins of originality: The neural bases of crea-
on degree of structure of problem situation. Journal of Educational tive thinking and originality. Neuropsychologia, 49(2), 178–185.
Research, 101(2), 113–123. Shen, J. L., Hu, W. P., & Lin, C. D. (2002). Constructing of the scien-
Li, H. Y., Hu, W. P., & Shen, J. L. (2010). School environment effects tific creativity test for adolescent. Psychological Development and
on the relations between adolescents' personality and creative Education, 16(4), 76–81.
scientific problem finding. Journal of Psychological Science, (5), Shen, W. B., Liu, C., & Wang, Y. J. (2010). Neurophysiological basis of
132–136. artistic creativity. Advances in Psychological Science, 18(10),
Li, W. F. (2014). The neural basis of creativity via multi-modal brain 520–1528.
imaging investigation. Unpublished doctor’s thesis. China: Shen, W. B., Luo, J., Liu, C., & Yuan, Y. (2012). One decade for insi-
Southwest University. ghtful brain: New advances on neural correlates of insight (in Chin-
Li, W. F., Tong, D. D., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. L. (2016). The neural basis ese). Chinese Science Bulletin, 57(21), 1948–1963.
Song, X. W., Dong, Z. Y., Long, X. Y., Li, S. F., Zuo, X. N., Zhu, C.
TONG DanDan et al.: The neural basis of scientific innovation problem
Z., ... Zang, Y. F.. (2011). REST: A toolkit for resting-state functi- ctivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks.
onal magnetic resonance imaging data processing. Plos One, 6(9), Brain Connect, 2(3), 125–141.
e25031. Wu, L. L., Knoblich, G., & Luo, J. (2013). The role of chunk tightness
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1993). Investing in creativity. and chunk familiarity in problem solving: Evidence from ERPs and
Psycholo- gical Inquiry, 4(3), 229–232. FMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 34(5), 1173–1186.
Subramaniam, K. (2008). The behavioral and neural basis for the facili- Wu, Z. Z., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. L. (2008). Exploring the mechanism for
tation of insight problem-solving by a positive mood. Northwestern prototype elicitation effect in insight. Psychological Development
University. and Education. 24(1), 31–35.
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., Nagase, T., Nouchi, Wu, Z. Z., & Zhang, Q. L. (2005). Effects of familiarity of fields and
R., & Kawashima, R. (2012). The association between resting questioning-constrained on the creativity of question- asking of
functional connectivity and creativity. Cerebral Cortex, 22(12), under- graduates. Chinese Journal of Clinical Rehabilitation. (44),
2921–2929. 61–63.
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Xue, H., Lu, K. L., & Hao, N. (2018). Cooperation makes two less-
Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2010). White matter creative individuals turn into a highly-creative pair. Neuroimage,
structures associated with creativity: Evidence from diffusion 172, 527–
tensor imaging. Neuroi- mage, 51(1), 11. 537.
Talati, A., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Functional specialization within the Yan, C. G., & Zang, Y. F. (2010). DPARSF: A MATLAB toolbox for
medial frontal gyrus for perceptual go/no-go decisions based on “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Frontiers in Systems
“what, ” “when, ” and “where” related information: An fMRI study. Neuroscience, 4, 13.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(7), 981–993. Yang, W. J., Jin, Y. L., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. L. (2018). The effect of pro-
Tong, D. D. (2017). The study of cognitive neural mechanisms of totype difficulty and semantic similarity on the prototype activation.
creative scientific problems. Unpublished doctor’s thesis. China: Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(3), 260–269.
Southwest university. Zang, Y. F., He, Y., Zhu, C. Z., Cao, Q. J., Sui, M. Q., Liang, M., ...
Tong, D. D., Dai, T. E., Li, W. F., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. L. (2013). An Wang, Y. F. (2007). Altered baseline brain activity in children with
fMRI study for problem-finding in scientific inventional situation. ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain &
Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(7), 740–751. Devel- opment, 29(2), 83–91.
Tong, D. D., Li, W. F., Dai, T. E., Nusbaum, H. C., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. Zeng, L. L., Shen, H., Liu, L., Wang, L. B., Li, B. J., Fang, P., ... Hu, D.
L. (2013). Brain mechanisms of valuable scientific problem finding W. (2012). Identifying major depression using whole-brain func-
inspired by heuristic knowledge. Experimental Brain Research. 228, tional connectivity: A multivariate pattern analysis. Brain A Journal
437–443. of Neurology, 135(Pt 5), 1498.
Torrance, E. P. (1966). The torrance tests of creative Zhang, Q. L., Tian, Y., Qiu, J. (2012). Automatic activation of proto-
thinking- norms-technical manual research edition-verbal tests, type representation in insight: The sources of inspiration. Journal of
Forms A and B-Figural Tests, Forms A and B. Princeton, NJ: Southwest University (Natural Science Edition), 34(9), 144–149.
Personnel Press. Zhao, Q. B., Zhou, Z. J., Xu, H. B., Chen, S., Xu, F., Fan, W. L., & Han,
Wang, B. T. (2013). Effects of hemispheric interaction on creative L. (2013). Dynamic neural network of insight: A functional mag-
scientific problem finding ability. Unpublished master’s netic resonance imaging study on solving chinese riddles. Plos One,
thesis. Shanxi Normal University, China. 8(3), e59351.
Wang, B. T., Duan, H. J., Han, Q., & Hu, W. P. (2017). The influence of Zhou, D., & Shi, J. N. (2005). Creative process: From the information
inter-hemispheric interaction on creative scientific problem finding processing perspective. Advances in Psychological Science.
ability. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 15(2), 92–102. 13(6),
Wang, Z. Q., Yan, C. G., Zhao, C., Qi, Z. G., Zhou, W., Lu, J., ... Li, Q. 721–727.
(2011). Spatial patterns of intrinsic brain activity in mild cognitive Zhou, H. (2015). Research on the difference of novelty evaluation pro-
impairment and alzheimer’s disease: A resting-state functional MRI cess of college students with different creative science problem
study. Human Brain Mapping, 32(10), 1720–1740. finding ability. Unpublished master’s thesis. Shaanxi Normal Uni-
Ward, T. B. (2007). Creative cognition as a window on creativity. versity, China.
Methods, 42(1), 28. Zhu, D., Luo, J. L., Zhu, H. X., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. L. (2011). The
Wei, D. T., Yang, J. Y., Li, W. F., Wang, K. C., Zhang, Q. L., & Qiu, J. effect of prototype inspiration in the process of scientific innovation
(2014). Increased resting functional connectivity of the medial pre- thinking. Journal of Southwest University (Social Sciences Edition),
frontal cortex in creativity by means of cognitive stimulation. 37(5), 144–149.
Cortex, Zou, Q. H., Ross, T. J., Gu, H., Geng, X. J., Zuo, X. N., Elliot Hong, L.,
51(1), 92–102. ... Yang, Y. H. (2013). Intrinsic resting-state activity predicts
Wei, L. Q., Duan, X. J., Zheng, C. Y., Wang, S. S., Gao, Q., Zhang, Z. working memory brain activation and behavioral performance.
Q., … Chen, H. F. (2013). Specific frequency bands of amplitude Human Brain Mapping, 34(12), 3204–3215.
low-frequency oscillation encodes personality. Human Brain Map- Zuo, X. N., Martino, A. D., Kelly, C., Shehzad, Z. E., Gee, D. G., Klein,
ping, 35(1), 331–339. D. F., ... Milham, M. P. (2010). The oscillating brain: Complex and
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., & Nieto-Castanon, A. (2012). A functional reliable. Neuroimage, 49(2), 1432–1445.
conne-

You might also like