You are on page 1of 13

This article was downloaded by: [University of California, San Francisco]

On: 23 March 2015, At: 04:46


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

International Journal of Psychology


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pijp20

The involvement of executive functions in


prospective memory performance of adults
a a b
Mike Martin , Matthias Kliegel & Mark A. McDaniel
a
University of Zurich , Switzerland
b
University of New Mexico , Albuquerque, USA
Published online: 13 May 2010.

To cite this article: Mike Martin , Matthias Kliegel & Mark A. McDaniel (2003) The involvement of executive
functions in prospective memory performance of adults , International Journal of Psychology, 38:4, 195-206, DOI:
10.1080/00207590344000123

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000123

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors
make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability
for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions
and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of
the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of
information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands,
costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or
indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution
in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 2003, 38 (4), 195–206

The involvement of executive functions


in prospective memory performance of adults

Mike Martin and Matthias Kliegel Mark A. McDaniel


University of Zurich, University of New Mexico,
Switzerland Albuquerque, USA
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

T he present study examines the relationship between prospective memory performance and executive functioning. The
four phases of the prospective memory process—intention formation, intention retention, reinstantiation of the
intention, and intention execution—are assumed to require different amounts of executive processing, most of which is
demanded in the phases of intention formation and intention execution. At present, though, it is still unclear whether, and
to what extent, prefrontal executive systems are involved in different kinds of prospective memory tasks, as some findings
suggest that prospective memory might rather rely on nonstrategic processes that are unlikely to depend on prefrontal
executive systems. Therefore, this study focuses on the following questions: (1) to what degree does executive functioning
predict prospective memory performance in different standard prospective memory tasks and, furthermore, are certain
executive measures better predictors than others; (2) are age-related effects in different prospective memory measures due
to individual differences in executive functioning; and (3) do age-related differences in prospective memory exist that are
not explained by individual differences in executive functioning. In a sample of 80 adults (20–80 years), we applied four
instruments to measure prospective memory: a traditional single-task paradigm, two more complex tasks—one time-based
and one event-based—and a highly complex multi-task paradigm. We further assessed a broadly defined construct of
executive functioning using several standard neuropsychological tests. Results showed that executive functioning did not
predict performance in the simple single-task paradigm. However, executive functioning, but not age, predicted perfor-
mance in the two more complex standard tests of prospective remembering, and both executive functioning and age
predicted performance in the most complex paradigm. In sum, the obtained data underline the assumption that frontal/
executive functions are related to prospective memory performance across a range of prospective paradigms. It also seems
clear that age differences in prospective memory performance partially depend on age-related individual differences in
frontal/executive functions.

L a présente étude examine la relation entre la performance de la mémoire prospective et le fonctionnement exécutif. Il est
présumé que les quatre phases du processus de la mémoire prospective (la formation de l’intention, la rétention de
l’intention, la représentation d’une abstraction par l’utilisation d’un exemple concret de l’intention et l’exécution de
l’intention) requièrent des traitements exécutifs d’ampleur variable, la plupart étant plus exigeants dans les phases de la
formation de l’intention et de l’exécution de l’intention. Cependant, actuellement, la façon et l’ampleur avec lesquelles les
systèmes préfrontaux exécutifs sont impliqués dans les différents types de tâches de la mémoire prospective ne sont pas
claires étant donné que les résultats d’étude suggèrent que la mémoire prospective ferait plutôt appel à des processus non
stratégiques étant probablement indépendants des systèmes préfrontaux exécutifs. La présente étude vise à examiner les
questions suivantes: (1) à quel degré le fonctionnement exécutif prédit la performance de la mémoire prospective dans
différentes tâches prospectives standards et, par ailleurs, est-ce que certaines mesures exécutives sont de meilleurs prédicteurs
que d’autres?; (2) est-ce que les effets de l’âge dans les différentes mesures de mémoire prospective sont dus aux différences
individuelles dans le fonctionnement exécutif?; (3) est-ce qu’il existe des différences reliées à l’âge dans la mémoire
prospective qui ne sont pas expliquées par les différences individuelles dans le fonctionnement exécutif? Nous avons

Requests for reprints should be addressed to Mike Martin, Institute of Psychology, Department of Gerontopsychology,
University of Zurich, Schaffhauserstr. 15, CH-8006 Zurich, Switzerland (E-mail: m.martin@psychologie.unizh.ch).
Part of the preparation of this manuscript was supported by DFG grant MA 1896/4-1. Appreciation is expressed to Sonja
Barth for experimental assistance, and to Caroline Moor for helpful comments on a prior version of the manuscript.
© 2003 International Union of Psychological Science
http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/00207594.html DOI: 10.1080/00207590244000205
196 MARTIN, KLIEGEL, McDANIEL

administré, auprès d’un échantillon de 80 adultes (20–80 ans), quatre instruments pour mesurer la mémoire prospective :
un paradigme traditionnel de tâche simple, deux tâches plus complexes—une basée sur le temps et l’autre basée sur les
événements—et un paradigme multi-tâches très complexe. Nous avons également évalué un construit de fonctionnement
exécutif largement défini en utilisant plusieurs tests neuropsychologiques standards. Les résultats ont soulevé que le
fonctionnement exécutif ne prédit pas la performance dans les paradigmes de tâche simple. Cependant, le fonctionnement
exécutif prédit la performance dans les deux tests standards plus complexes de rappel prospectif, ce qui n’est pas le cas de
l’âge. Enfin, à la fois le fonctionnement exécutif et l’âge prédisent la performance dans le paradigme le plus complexe. En
somme, les données obtenues soutiennent la prédiction que les fonctions frontales/exécutives sont reliées à la performance
dans la mémoire prospective à travers une gamme de paradigmes prospectifs. Aussi, il apparaît clairement que les différences
d’âge dans la performance de la mémoire prospective dépendent partiellement des différences individuelles reliées à l’âge
dans les fonctions frontales/exécutives.

E l presente estudio examina la relación entre el desempeño de la memoria prospectiva y el funcionamiento ejecutivo. Se
supone que las cuatro fases del proceso de memoria prospectiva –la formación de la intención, la retención de la
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

intención, la representación de una abstracción por medio de una instancia concreta de la intención y la ejecución de la
intención- requieren diferentes cantidades de procesamiento ejecutivo, del cual hay más demanda en las fases de formación
de la intención y ejecución de la intención. Al presente, sin embargo, no queda claro si los sistemas ejecutivos prefrontales
están involucrados en diferentes tipos de tareas de memoria prospectiva, y en qué medida, ya que algunos hallazgos
sugieren que la memoria prospectiva depende más bien de procesos no estratégicos que es poco probable que dependan de
los sistemas ejecutivos prefrontales. Por consiguiente, el presente estudio se centra en las siguientes preguntas: (1) ¿en qué
medida el funcionamiento ejecutivo predice el desempeño de la memoria prospectiva en diferentes tareas tipificadas de
memoria prospectiva y, es más, si ciertas medidas predicen mejor que otras?; (2) ¿se deben los efectos relativos a la edad
en diferentes medidas de memoria prospectiva, a diferencias individuales en el funcionamiento ejecutivo? y (3) ¿existen
diferencias relativas a la edad en la memoria prospectiva que no pueden explicarse en términos de diferencias individuales
en el funcionamiento ejecutivo? Una muestra de 80 adultos (20–80 años) respondieron a cuatro instrumentos que medían
memoria prospectiva: un paradigma tradicional de una sola tarea, dos tareas más complejas –una basada en tiempo y la otra
en el evento, y un paradigma multi-tarea muy complejo. Se evaluó además un constructo de funcionamiento ejecutivo
definido ampliamente, por medio del uso de varias pruebas neuropsicológicas estándar. Los resultados muestran que el
funcionamiento ejecutivo no predice el desempeño en el paradigma de una sola tarea simple. Sin embargo, el funcionamiento
ejecutivo, mas no la edad, predice el desempeño en dos o más pruebas estándar de recuerdo prospectivo, y tanto el
funcionamiento ejecutivo como la edad predicen el desempeño en el paradigma más complejo. En resumen, los datos
obtenidos subrayan el supuesto de que las funciones frontales/ejecutivas se relacionan con el desempeño de la memoria
prospectiva a lo largo de un espectro de paradigmas prospectivos. También parece ser que las diferencias de edad en el
desempeño de la memoria prospectiva dependen parcialmente de diferencias individuales relativas a la edad en las funciones
frontales/ejecutivas.

Remembering to perform an intended action at a Glisky, Rubin, Guynn, & Routhieaux, 1999; see also
particular point in the future, i.e., prospective memory, Stuss & Benson, 1987). Frontally mediated executive
is essential in the everyday life of older adults, because functions are believed to include planning, interruption
self-initiated acting upon earlier formed intentions is at and inhibition of responses, monitoring of
the core of their independent living. Without it or with environmental events, and flexible initiation of
low levels of performance, appointments (Kvavilashvili, responses to those events (Shimamura, Janowsky, &
1987; Ma. Martin, 1986; West, 1988), medication (Park Squire, 1991). The prospective memory process
& Kidder, 1996), or, more generally, chances to act are consists of the four phases of intention formation,
likely to be missed (Ellis, 1996; Maylor, 1993). intention retention, reinstantiation of the intention, and
However, research focusing on prospective memory execution of the reinstantiated intention (Ellis, 1996;
in old age is in its infancy (cf. Roediger, 1996), with Kliegel, Martin, McDaniel, & Einstein, in press;
several fundamental issues just beginning to receive Kvavilashvili & Ellis, 1996). The theoretical assumption
theoretical and empirical attention. One such issue is that frontal functions are little involved in the
concerns the neuropsychological systems and retention phase, and intimately involved in
processes that support prospective remembering. An performance of the intention formation and intention
initial proposal is that prospective memory execution phase of prospective memory tasks. Older
performance depends on the prefrontal systems and adults’ prospective performance in tasks that
the integrity of the executive functions that these emphasize the phases theoretically making the
systems subserve (Bisiacchi, 1996; Burgess, Veitch, de strongest demands on the executive functions should
Lacy Costello, & Shallice, 2000; Glisky, 1996; McDaniel, be highly related to executive function measures (cf.
PROSPECTIVE MEMORY PERFORMANCE 197

West, 1996). However, there is virtually no empirical (for which no external reminders are implemented) are
work that has attempted to evaluate this assumption. dependent on self-initiated monitoring in the intention
Further, the limited work that is available shows execution phase of the prospective task, an
somewhat mixed findings. Some studies report relations assumption consistent with age-related deficits in
between frontal processes and prospective monitoring patterns and subsequent prospective
remembering in adults (Bisiacchi, 2000; Burgess, 2000a; memory performance (Einstein, McDaniel, Richardson,
Burgess et al., 2000; Kopp & Thöne, 2000; McDaniel Guynn, & Cunfer, 1995) and with increased time-based
et al., 1999), whereas others do not necessarily performance as a function of the importance of the task
implicate frontal processes (Bisiacchi, 1996; Cockburn, (Kliegel et al., 2001). Based on the idea that prefrontal
Keene, & Hope, 2000; Mi. Martin, Kliegel, McDaniel, functions control monitoring (Shallice & Burgess,
& Einstein, 2000). 1991; Shimamura et al., 1991), prefrontal systems would
These mixed patterns suggest several possibilities. be expected to be significantly associated with time-
One clear possibility is that prospective memory in based prospective memory. No published study has
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

adulthood involves a range of processes that depend yet examined this prediction1, and for this reason one
on the particular instantiation of the prospective memory of our prospective tasks was a standard laboratory
task (cf. Einstein & McDaniel, 1990; McDaniel & time-based task (Einstein, Smith, McDaniel, & Shaw,
Einstein, 2000). While some prospective memory tasks 1997; Kliegel et al., 2001).
appear to require prefrontal executive processes of Event-based prospective memory involves remem-
planning, monitoring, or flexibility in response bering to perform the intended action when some exter-
preparation, others seem to rely on relatively nal event occurs (such as remembering to give a
nonstrategic processes, i.e., processes that might not message to a colleague when passing her office). Thus,
depend on prefrontal executive systems (Guynn, in contrast to time-based prospective memory, event-
McDaniel, & Einstein, 2001; Kliegel, Martin, McDaniel, based tasks have external cues that can stimulate
& Einstein, 2001; Kliegel, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2000; retrieval of the intended action. In a sense, the event-
McDaniel & Einstein, 2000; McDaniel, Robinson- based task is similar to cued recall (McDaniel & Einstein,
Riegler, & Einstein, 1998). The amount of required 1993), and thereby may attenuate the degree of self-
executive processing is likely to depend on the degree initiated retrieval or monitoring required to remember the
to which the dependent prospective measures focus intended action at the appropriate moment in the
on the intention formation and/or execution phases intention execution phase of the prospective task
versus the retention phase. The more relative weight is (Einstein & McDaniel, 1990, 1996). For instance, in an
given to intention formation and intention execution, event-based prospective memory task introduced by
the stronger should be the relation to executive Einstein et al. (1997), participants were instructed to
functioning. press a particular function key on a computer keyboard
To inform this possibility, the first goal of the when a specified target item appeared in a word-rating
present study was to investigate the contribution of task. Age-related declines on the task were modest
prefrontal executive processes to performance in four (Einstein et al., 1997, Experiment 1), and varying the
standard prospective memory tasks. As a importance of the task did not change performance levels
consequence, we test if differences in executive (Kliegel et al., 2001). Both findings suggest that relatively
functioning are related to differences in prospective few strategic retrieval processes support this event-
memory performance in young and old adults. Unlike based task. To evaluate this idea, our second and third
earlier studies with only old adults (e.g., McDaniel et prospective memory tasks were two standard event-
al., 1999), we compare prospective memory performance based prospective memory tasks. As a standard
and its relation to executive functioning between laboratory task, the event-based task used in Kliegel et
young and old adults in four different prospective al. (2001; cf. Einstein et al., 1997) was administered. In
memory tasks. addition, as a standard clinical event-based prospective
The selected prospective memory tasks differentially memory task, the Remembering-a-Belonging subtest
represent several major components on which such from the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test (RBMT;
tasks can vary. One critical component is whether the Wilson, Cockburn, & Baddeley, 1985; for applications:
task is time-based or event-based (Einstein & e.g., Cockburn & Smith, 1988, 1991, 1994; Evans &
McDaniel, 1990, 1996). Time-based prospective
memory involves remembering to perform the intended
action at a certain time or after a certain amount of time 1
Bisiacchi (1996) tested time-based prospective memory
has elapsed (e.g., remembering to take cookies out of but did not separate it from event-based prospective memory
the oven in 10 minutes). Presumably, time-based tasks in relating performance to neuropsychological variables.
198 MARTIN, KLIEGEL, McDANIEL

Wilson, 1992; Hon, Huppert, Holland, & Watson, 1998; made similar proposals (e.g., Glisky, 1996) and there is
Huppert & Beardsall, 1993; Wisemann, Ratcliff, Chase, some preliminary data to support this hypothesis (Kliegel
Laporte, Robertson, & Colantonio, 2000) was applied. et al., 2000) but, to date, this hypothesis has not been
In this task, participants are required to demand the specifically tested. The idea is that the age-related
return of an item at the end of the session before leaving differences found in prospective memory will be
the room. Because there is only a single event-based eliminated once the variance in performance due to
action that has to be remembered, we reasoned that frontal/executive functioning is taken into account. In
performance on the RBMT should not rely on strategic this study, by using several prospective memory tasks
executive control processes in the intention execution on which either significant age-related differences have
phase of the task. been established (time-based, e.g., Einstein et al., 1995;
Our fourth prospective memory task was a complex Park, Hertzog, Kidder, Morrell, & Mayhorn, 1997;
prospective memory task patterned on that used by complex prospective memory, Kliegel et al., 2000, 2001,
Kliegel et al. (2000; see also Burgess, 1996; Burgess et 2002) or tendencies for age-related differences are
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

al., 2000). We selected this task because (1) participants evident (event-based, cf. Cherry & LeCompte, 1999;
are required to execute a set of six intended actions Einstein et al., 1997; Huppert, Johnson, & Nickson, 2000;
rather than a single action after the retention (i.e., delay Maylor, 1996; Park et al., 1997; but cf. Kliegel et al.,
phase); (2) because the appropriate delayed execution 2000, for different findings concerning the RBMT), we
of the set of actions involves event-based and time- are able to test a more complete version of this
based characteristics; and (3) because planning is hypothesis. Alternatively, it might be that there is some
explicitly required in the intention formation phase of age-related component of prospective memory
this task. We reasoned that these complex demands in performance that is not mediated by frontal/executive
both the intention formation and the intention execution processes. That is, even after factoring out effects of
phases should require the most robust degree of frontal/executive processes, age deficits may remain. If
prefrontal executive involvement, if such involvement so, then we will have support for the assumption that
indeed is central to prospective memory performance in prospective memory signals age-related decline that is
the respective task phases. differentiated from typical markers of age-related frontal
Another possibility for the mixed patterns from the decline (cf. Wecker et al., 2000).
initial investigations of the importance of prefrontal Overall, we expect the weakest effect of executive
executive processes in prospective remembering is that functioning differences on the RBMT performance, a
executive functioning may have been inadequately medium effect of executive functioning differences on
captured by the measures used. As noted above, the standard experimental event- and time-based
prefrontal systems are thought to support a wide array paradigm performances, and a strong effect of executive
of executive functions, and it is unlikely that one functioning differences on the performance in the
measure will adequately capture the integrity of these complex prospective paradigm. In addition, when
executive functions. Accordingly, we used a broadly groups of young, old/high executive functioning, and
defined construct of executive functioning based on a old/low executive functioning groups are compared, we
set of three executive functioning tasks from clinical and expect no effects of age and executive functioning
experimental literature, rather than restricting focus to differences in the RBMT performance, effects of
just one specific measure (for a similar procedure see executive functioning differences on the performances
McDaniel et al., 1999). Further, we used assessments in the experimental event- and time-based paradigm
that are thought to index particular components of performances, and effects of executive functioning
frontal function, such as planning, inhibition, monitoring differences and age on the performance in the complex
ongoing activity, and cognitive and response flexibility, prospective paradigm.
that are theoretically related to prospective memory The difference in the relations can be explained by
performance as sketched above. the differential emphases that these prospective
A second goal was to explore a corollary hypothesis memory paradigms lay on the intention formation and
to the idea that prefrontal processes are intimately the intention execution phases. Whereas the RBMT
involved in prospective memory performance. Based focuses on the retention and reinstantiation component
on the idea that frontal functioning may show in a least demanding environment, the standard
preferential decline with age (e.g., Schretlen et al., 2000; experimental paradigms focus on the intention execution
Wecker, Kramer, Wisniewski, Delis, & Kaplan, 2000; in a somewhat demanding task environment. Finally,
West, 1996), we suggest that age-related decline in the complex prospective paradigm maximizes the
prospective memory will be significantly associated with interindividual differences in the intention formation and
decline in frontal/executive functioning. Others have the intention execution phases in a demanding task
PROSPECTIVE MEMORY PERFORMANCE 199

environment, thus theoretically requiring the largest break and some other tasks. Using example sheets, the
involvement of executive functions. tasks and the rules of our modified six-elements task
were explained to the participant. Specifically, the
METHOD participants were asked to remember to carry out six
subtasks in a 6-minute period of time. The six subtasks
Participants were divided into two similar sets (sets A and B) of three
(word finding, solving arithmetic problems, and writing
The 80 participants in this study, including 40 young down the names of pictures). We designed each subtask
and 40 old adults, completed the battery of tests in an so that it would need more than 1 minute to complete.
average time of 120 minutes, distributed over two testing After the subtasks were explained, participants were told
sessions an average of 1 week apart. The young where the material for these subtasks was stored (in the
participants were students of the University of Mainz second drawer of the participant’s table, divided in three
who volunteered. The old participants were community file-folders according to the task type) and that there
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

dwelling volunteers. As Table 1 indicates, despite small would be a few rules to follow. The rules explicated that
differences between the groups with respect to gender, earlier problems would be given more points than later
health, or years of formal schooling, which are to be ones in each subtask, that each of the six subtasks would
expected when recruiting samples of young and old be given equal weight, and that doing two subtasks (A)
adults, the two groups seem largely comparable. and (B) of the same type one after the other was not
allowed. When they were aware of the rules, i.e., were
Instruments able to recall them perfectly, the participants were told
that they would have to remember to work on all six
Prospective memory measures tasks, and that they would have to open the drawer and
initiate these tasks by themselves after answering the
The complex or multitask prospective memory
question about their date of birth in the “participant
paradigm (MTPM; Kliegel et al., 2000, Kliegel, Martin,
information” questionnaire that had previously been
McDaniel, & Einstein, 2002a; Martin et al., 2000) is a
explained. Finally, the participants were asked to verbally
modified six-elements task after Shallice and Burgess
develop a plan for the multitask prospective memory
(1991). The task consists of three phases: an
paradigm. After a filled delay (in which inhibition and
introduction phase, in which the participants are
event-based prospective memory were assessed), the
required to develop a plan for executing the prospective
experimenter made sure that all participants
task, a delay phase, in which individual difference
reinstantiated the original execution intention, i.e.,
variables can be assessed, and a performance phase, in
participants not initiating the multitask prospective
which the multitask prospective memory paradigm had
memory paradigm by themselves were prompted by the
to be executed. In the introduction phase, participants
experimenter. This way, the focus of the prospective
were told that at a certain point—when they would have
memory task is clearly on the intention planning and the
to fill out a personal information questionnaire—they
execution phases, whereas retention and reinstantiation
would be required to execute a set of six tasks. The
are kept stable between participants. After having
participants were informed that this would take place
worked on the six elements task for 6 minutes, the
during the second part of the experiment, after a short
participants filled out the rest of their “participant
information” questionnaire. We also interviewed all
TABLE 1 participants about how well they remembered the
Demographic characteristics of young and old adults instructions and their original plans. Finally, participants
Young adults Old adults were debriefed by the experimenter. The dependent
(N = 40) (N = 40) variables are the number of subtasks started (MTPM/
Age M = 24.8 M = 69.3 performance) and the percentage of recalled executable
SD = 2.0 SD = 5.6 actions of the original plan from the planning phase of
Range = 22–31 Range = 60–80 the MTPM as an indicator of retrospective memory (note
Gender that for experimental purposes we tried to minimize age
Female N = 21 N = 30
Male N = 19 N = 10
differences in this measure).
Years of education M = 13.2 M = 12.2 From experimental psychology literature, we selected
SD = 0.9 SD = 2.1 two paradigms. One involves giving the person a
Health a M = 3.9 M = 3.4 specific cue that will be presented during their ongoing
SD = 0.9 SD = 0.7 activity, thus putting a strong emphasis on the execution
a
Rated on a scale from 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent). phase of the task. For this laboratory version of an event-
200 MARTIN, KLIEGEL, McDANIEL

based prospective memory task, all participants were Executive functioning measures
given a slightly modified standard prospective memory
paradigm introduced by Einstein et al. (1997). The In order to measure executive functions, a number of
ongoing task was a computerized word rating task, in instruments have been developed and are currently in
which 26 words (e.g., house, phone, love, war, etc.) had clinical and research use. We selected three prominent
to be rated on four dimensions (concreteness, familiarity, measures, trying to capture the essential aspects of
pleasantness, and seriousness). On each trial one word executive functioning. The calculated factor score
was presented with one dimension and a rating scale derived from the raw scores can be used for individual
for 5 seconds on the computer screen. The rating had analyses and to split the groups into young, old/high
to be done by pressing the corresponding number key executive functioning, and old/low executive
on the computer keyboard. All 26 words were presented functioning.
four times in the same order with changing rating The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton,
dimensions. The presentation order of the rating Chelune, Talley, Kay, & Curtiss, 1993) is a computerized
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

dimensions was randomized but one dimension was test to measure the ability to shift cognitive strategies
never presented twice in a row. Hence, 104 trials were or sets in response to changing environmental
presented to every participant. The prospective memory contingencies (e.g., Greve, Brooks, Crouch, & Williams,
task was to press a target key whenever the German 1997). In the test, participants have to sort a deck of 64
word “Gespräch” (i.e., conversation) appeared on the cards to match 1 of 4 stimulus cards. The computer tells
screen as a word to be rated. The target word appeared the participant if the answer is right or wrong. Once the
every 2 minutes, and the task lasted 8 minutes and 40 participant has made 10 consecutive “correct” matches
seconds. to the initial sorting principle (i.e., colour), the sorting
Another experimental prospective memory task principle is changed to form or number without a
involves requiring the subject to respond after a warning, requiring the participant to use the feedback
particular time interval, thus also emphasizing the to develop a new sorting rule or, in other words, to shift
execution phase of the prospective task. For this sets. The dependent variable is the number of categories
laboratory version of a time-based prospective memory participants have completed after having sorted 64 cards.
task, the materials were identical to those used in the We included a colour–word version of the Stroop
event-based version with two exceptions: The task (cf. Stroop, 1935; for applications cf. Houx, Jolles,
prospective memory task was to press a target key on & Vreeling, 1993) in order to measure inhibition (e.g.,
the computer keyboard every 2 minutes after having Dempster, 1992; Spieler, Balota, & Faust, 1996). In this
started. The response was scored as correct if it task, the word stimuli consisted of four colour names
occurred within a time window of plus or minus 5 (red, blue, green, and yellow) written in black ink (trial 1:
seconds. Again, all instructions and materials were read the words as fast as you can), the four colour names
presented on the computer screen, and the task lasted 8 printed in colour bars (trial 2: name the colours as fast
minutes and 40 seconds. In addition, participants were as you can), and the four colour names printed in
told that they could monitor the time by pressing a mismatching colours (trial 3: name the colours the words
yellow key, when a time counter clock would appear for are written in as fast as you can). Each Stroop condition
2 seconds. began with practice of the top line consisting of 5 items,
Our fourth standard measure of prospective memory followed by timed performance on the 20 test items
was selected from clinical/assessment literature. The (consisting of 5 rows of 4 items each). The dependent
task was the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test variable is the time difference between the baseline
(RBMT; Wilson et al., 1985), from which we selected the (colour bars) and the interference condition.
Remembering-a-Belonging subtest. In this test, the The Tower of London (TOL) is a planning task in which
experimenter requests a personal belonging from the difficulty can be varied by using different end states that
participant at the beginning of the experimental session have to be achieved starting from a given starting state
and instructs the participant to demand the return of (e.g., Morris, Evenden, Sahakian, & Robbins, 1987; Ward
the item at the end of the session before leaving the & Allport, 1997). The participants’ task is to make the
room, without any additional reminding on the part of fewest possible moves of different-sized disks to reach
the experimenter. The dependent variable is the correct the end state. The dependent variable is the difference
request to return the belonging after a delay of between the minimum number of moves and the number
approximately 45 minutes. In this test, there is little of moves the participants made.
emphasis on the intention planning and intention Results from the principal components analysis
execution phases, but, instead, the retention and with z-scores of all three executive measures sug-
reinstantiation of the intention are emphasized. gested a single executive factor representing the
PROSPECTIVE MEMORY PERFORMANCE 201

common variance of all three executive tests and RESULTS


explaining 57.6% of the variance. Component
loadings were in the expected directions and The first goal of our study was to assess and represent
ranged between .70 to .80. Using the standardized a wide range of typical standard prospective memory
factor score (for a similar procedure see McDaniel tasks as well as of frontal executive processes. Table 2
et al., 1999), a three-way median split between all summarizes the results in these prospective and execu-
participants in the study resulted in a young adult tive measures.
group and two old adult groups. The young adults The results indicate increasing age differences
had the lowest mean score, i.e., the highest per- the more strongly frontal/executive functions are
formance (M = –0.74, SD = 0.39), the old adults with hypothesized to be involved in performing the
high executive performance were close to the mean prospective task. There are age differences for all
of the distribution (M = –0.02, SD = 0.42), and the executive measures, for the laboratory event- and
old adults with low executive performance showed time-based tasks, and marked age effects for the
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

the highest mean score, i.e., the lowest perform- MTPM/performance measure. There was no age ef-
ance (M = 1.36, SD = 0.72). All group differences fect in the single action event-based task from the
were significant. RBMT.
In addition, we intended to explore the extent to which
Procedure individual differences in executive functioning are
associated with individual differences in prospective
In the course of the first session, the participants were memory performance. Therefore, we conducted a
given the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test and the correlation analysis for young and old adults with the
time-based prospective memory task. After a short prospective memory measures and the executive
break of 5 minutes the TOL as well as the WCST were functioning factor score. The results can be found in
administered. Table 3.
In the course of the second session, participants The results indicate that only in older adults were
were first instructed in the complex or multitask pro- the laboratory event- and time-based prospective
spective paradigm, then had to develop their plan for memory tasks as well as the complex prospective
this task, and were next tested on the Stroop task as memory measure significantly correlated with executive
well as on the event-based prospective memory task. functioning. The RBMT measure was not correlated to
After a short break of 5 minutes, participants worked the executive factor score in either age group.
on a distractor task. After completing the distractor A second goal was to investigate if individual differ-
task, they were given the “participant information” ences rather than age per se explain most of the age-
questionnaire as the cue for the multitask prospective related variance in prospective memory tasks. This
memory paradigm. After working for 6 minutes on the would be suggested if, after controlling for individual
complex prospective memory task, participants fin- differences in nonexecutive functions, age-related
ished the participant-information questionnaire, re- differences in prospective memory are eliminated once
sponded to the questions asking what respondents the variance in performance due to executive functioning
remembered from their original plans, and were de- is taken into account. Alternatively, there might be some
briefed by the experimenter. age-related component of prospective memory

TABLE 2
Age differences in executive functioning, and prospective memory measures

Young adults Old adults


(N = 40) (N = 40) F-value η2
Executive measures
WCST 5.8 (0.9) 4.0 (1.8) 31.98*** .296
Stroop 10.3 (4.4) 26.4 (14.0) 38.12*** .328
Tower of London 3.5 (3.4) 9.4 (5.7) 29.17*** .280
Prospective memory measures
RBMT (% correct) 62.5 82.5 0.48 .007
Lab. event-based 3.79 (0.5) 3.15 (1.5) 6.35* .077
Lab. time-based 3.89 (0.4) 2.97 (2.1) 6.32* .081
MTPM/performance 5.50 (0.8) 2.83 (1.3) 127.18 *** .620
*
p < .05; **p < .01; p < .001.
***
202 MARTIN, KLIEGEL, McDANIEL

TABLE 3
Correlations between prospective memory measures and executive functioning
for young and old adults
Prospective memory task
RBMT Event-based Time-based MTPM/Performance
Young/Old Young/Old Young/Old Young/Old
Executive functioning
(factor score) .17 / .20 .14 / .40* .32+ / .32* .19 / .41**
+
p < .10; * p < .05; **p < .01.

performance on particular prospective memory tasks significantly contribute to the explanation of 77% of
that is not mediated by frontal/executive processes. In the variance. The results are also supported by four
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

this case, age-related differences in prospective memory separate ANOVA analyses with the three groups of
will still exist after the variance in performance due to young, old/high executive functioning, and old/low
frontal/executive processes is taken into account. We executive functioning as independent variables and the
conducted stepwise regression analyses with prospec- four prospective measures as the dependent variables.
tive memory performances as the dependent variables. There was no effect of group membership for the RBMT
We entered the nonexecutive measures of education: task, F(2, 75) = 1.35, p > .25, or for the event-based task,
retrospective memory and health in the first step, the the time-based task, and the complex memory task, F(2,
frontal/executive functioning measures in the second 75) = 4.75, p < .05; F(2, 75) = 4.30, p < .05; and F(2, 75) =
step, and chronological age in the third step. This al- 69.98, p > .001, respectively. Post-tests reveal no
lowed us to examine whether executive functioning ex- significant mean differences in the RBMT task, similar
plains variance in prospective memory performance performances in the young and old/high executive
above and beyond the influence of nonexecutive meas- functioning vs. old/low executive functioning groups
ures. In addition, our procedure allows us to examine if in the event- and time-based task, and significant
the increase in explained variance (∆ R2) due to adding differences between all three groups in the complex
age to the equation is significant. The results are dis- prospective memory task.
played in Table 4.
The results indicate that, with the exception of the DISCUSSION
RBMT, individual differences in executive functioning
explain a significant amount of variance of prospective A recent focus in prospective memory research has been
memory performance even after controlling for the to consider the role of frontally mediated executive
influence of nonexecutive measures. In contrast, adding functions for prospective remembering (Burgess, 2000b;
age to the regression does not lead to a significant Burgess et al., 2000; Glisky, 1996; McDaniel et al., 1999).
increase in explained variance in performance in the However, the findings are disparate, with some studies
laboratory event- and time-based prospective memory reporting relations between frontal processes and
tasks. However, regarding the complex prospective prospective remembering (e.g., Burgess, 2000a; McDaniel
memory task all three predictor groups—nonexecutive et al., 1999), whereas others do not necessarily implicate
measures, executive measures, and age—did frontal processes (Cockburn et al., 2000; Mi. Martin et al.,

TABLE 4
Regression summaries for four prospective memory performance measures as dependent variables

Prospective memory measures


RBMT Event-Based Time-Based MTPM/Performance
Predictors R 2
∆R 2
R 2
∆R 2
R 2
∆R 2
R2 ∆R2
Step 1
Nonexecutive measures .01 .01 .05 .05 .06 .06 .51*** .51***
Step 2
Executive functioning score .04 .03 .27 ** .22 ** .22 * .17 * .62*** .11 **
Step 3
Age .10 .06 .29 ** .02 .22 * .00 .77 *** .15***
*
p < .05; p < .01;
**
p < .001.
***
PROSPECTIVE MEMORY PERFORMANCE 203

2000). Therefore, the first goal of our study was to exam- high executive functioning, and old/low executive func-
ine and clarify the contribution of prefrontal executive tioning. Our finding may help to explain the disparate
processes to performance in four standard prospective findings from the prospective memory literature: Studies
memory tasks. These tasks were taken from the experi- finding no age effects might have used prospective
mental and clinical literature, and differ with respect to memory tasks focusing mainly on the retention and/or
the hypothesized involvement of executive processes of reinstantiation phase of the tasks, thus requiring only a
planning, inhibition, and self-initiated monitoring. In par- minimal involvement of frontal/executive functions (such
ticular, whereas the RBMT emphasizes the retention and as the Remembering-a-Belonging test), whereas studies
reinstantiation phases, the standard time-and event- finding age differences might have used prospective
based tasks focus on the intention execution phase. tasks with a larger involvement of frontal/executive
Finally, the complex prospective paradigm focuses on functions. For example, in studies where participants were
both the intention formation and the intention execution asked to send back postcards on specified dates and
phases, thus theoretically making the largest demands on were allowed to use the postcards as external reminders
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

executive processes. (e.g., Patton & Meit, 1993; West, 1988), or in a number of
On the part of the executive processes, mixed results event-based tasks not requiring strategic monitoring of
concerning the involvement of executive processes in the task environment (Einstein & McDaniel, 1990), there
prospective memory performance reported in the were no age differences in performance (cf. McDaniel &
literature could have been caused by the use of a Einstein, 2000, for a theoretical discussion on the role of
particular measure focusing on a particular executive strategic monitoring in prospective memory
function. Therefore, in our study, we used a broadly performance). In the light of the present results, this is
defined construct of executive functioning by including not surprising, assuming that external cues in a familiar
measures of planning, inhibition, monitoring, and environment minimize the need to plan to execute an
cognitive and response flexibility. intention (one relies on the efficiency of the cue) or to
The results improve our understanding of the monitor a complex environment while trying to realize the
neuropsychological processes involved in prospective delayed intention, i.e., to engage in frontal/executive
memory performance in two important ways. First, we processing. In contrast, studies in which participants had
had expected increasing age differences in prospective to switch between a number of different prospective
memory performance the more executive functions are actions or were engaged in demanding ongoing task
hypothesized to be involved, i.e., the more the intention activities (e.g., Kliegel et al., 2000; Mi. Martin & Schumann-
formation and intention execution phases of the Hengsteler, 2001; Maylor, 1996; Park et al., 1997) showed
prospective task are emphasized. Based on the idea age differences. Given a large number and high complexity
that frontal functioning may show preferential decline of task demands, this could be explained by assuming
with age (e.g., Schretlen et al., 2000; West, 1996), we that the execution of the delayed intentions in these
suggested that age-related decline in prospective studies requires the use of frontal/executive functions
memory will be observable in prospective tasks that like planning, attention switching, or inhibition.
are shown to be significantly associated with frontal/ Second, although the near-ceiling performance of the
executive functioning. Therefore, we expected age- younger adults prevents us from making strong
related deficits in frontal/executive functions and in statements about potential age differences, when the
prospective memory performance across the indicators correlations between executive functioning and
for each construct. The results clearly demonstrate prospective memory performance are compared between
age-related differences in prospective memory young and old adults, it seems that interindividual
performance in three of the four indicators of differences in executive functioning are particularly
prospective memory performance. Only the predictive of prospective memory performance in old age,
Remembering-a-Belonging subtest from the Rivermead but not in young adults. This might be because executive
Behavioral Memory Test, a prospective memory test processes become increasingly important as the overall
taken from the assessment literature, did not reveal an level of cognitive processes declines in old age (e.g.,
age-related performance difference. Similarly, there are Verhaeghen & Salthouse, 1997). Age differences in the
age-related differences in three indicators of frontal/ correlation between executive functioning and
executive functioning. prospective memory would, thus, become obvious in
One may conclude that there is a clear relationship prospective memory tasks making relatively few demands
between the amount of involvement of frontal/executive for storage and cued retrieval of material, but, instead,
functions and the degree to which age effects occur. requiring planning, inhibition, and attention switching.
This was supported by the results from ANOVAs Therefore, a high level of prospective memory
examining differences between the groups of young, old/ performance in older adults compared to young adults
204 MARTIN, KLIEGEL, McDANIEL

seems to depend more strongly on effective executive related frontal decline (Wecker et al., 2000). In contrast,
processes. Another explanation could be that different results from the most complex experimental paradigm
constellations of executive processes are involved in suggest that individual differences in nonexecutive
prospective memory performance in young and old measures, individual differences in frontal/executive
adults. This has been suggested by Bisiacchi (1996) and functioning, and individual age differences all contrib-
has recently been supported by fMRI studies showing ute to the prediction of prospective memory perform-
that different activations are present for younger and ance. Hence, in this paradigm, there is support for the
older adults on identical working memory tasks (Mitchell, assumption that prospective memory signals age-related
Johnson, Raye, & D’Esposito, 2000; Rypma & decline that is differentiated from typical markers of age-
D’Esposito, 2001). However, results are precursory, and related frontal decline.
only future research combining more difficult prospective As an explanation, one might offer that in the complex
tasks to prevent ceiling performance in the younger prospective memory task overall processing demands
adults and a broader spectrum of executive functioning are most diverse and at a relatively high level. Thus, a
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

measures might be able to examine the executive processes number of different nonexecutive processes might be
involved in prospective memory performance in more involved, and their predictive power in explaining
detail. performance becomes substantial. For example, this may
The second goal of our study was to examine if be true for acquired problem-solving strategies that
individual differences in frontal/executive functions might support performance in the complex prospective
rather than age per se explain most of the age-related memory task. Still, to perform well in this prospective
variance in prospective memory performance (e.g., memory task also requires the on-line processes of
Cherry & LeCompte, 1999; Kliegel et al., 2000). In other planning, inhibition, and switching between the tasks.
words, we examined if, after controlling for the effects Thus, executive processes play an important role in
of nonexecutive influences such as education, health, predicting prospective memory performance even after
or retrospective memory, significant portions of the age- controlling for individual differences in nonexecutive
related differences in prospective memory performance measures.
are eliminated once the variance in performance due to Our results indicate that even after controlling for
frontal/executive process is taken into account, or if, age-related declines in nonexecutive and executive
alternatively, some age deficits remain even after processes, there are still age-related differences in
controlling for nonexecutive skills and after factoring processes involved in complex prospective memory
out effects of frontal/executive processes. performance that have not been captured by our
The results demonstrate that a substantial proportion instruments. There are at least two possible explanations
of the variance in prospective memory performance can of this finding. First, there might be other nonexecutive
be explained by differences in executive functioning factors, e.g., motivational or emotional processes, that
above and beyond the influence of nonexecutive are contributing to age-related differences in complex
functions. This was at least true for the event-based task, prospective memory performance that have not been
the time-based task, and the MTPM task. There was no examined in our study (cf. Kliegel et al., 2001, 2002b,
significant predictor for the performance in the simple this issue). Second, there might be executive processes
single-task RBMT measure. In addition, regarding the other than the ones examined in our three standard
most complex multitask prospective memory measure, measures that are important for complex prospective
adding age to the regression equation further improved memory performance. As a consequence, our findings
the prediction of prospective memory performance. In suggest a need for measurement instruments capturing
this paradigm only, nonexecutive measures did explain a the full range of nonexecutive and executive processes
significant amount of the prospective performance at first. potentially related to complex prospective memory
This suggests that frontal/executive functioning is an performance. Based on our data, reasoning about the
important predictor for prospective memory performance, exact mechanisms responsible for the (age-related)
even after controlling for age differences in health, interplay of nonexecutive processes, frontal/executive
education, and retrospective memory. Moreover, age did functioning, and prospective memory performance has
not contribute significantly to the prediction of to remain speculative at this point. Further research
prospective memory in two experimental paradigms needs to disentangle the nonexecutive and frontal/
testing time-based and event-based prospective memory executive processes that might be differentially related
performance after controlling for executive functioning. to different types of prospective tasks.
Thus, for these tasks, there is no support for the Overall, it seems clear that frontal/executive functions
assumption that prospective memory signals age-related are related to prospective memory performance across
decline that is differentiated from typical markers of age- a range of prospective paradigms. It also seems clear
PROSPECTIVE MEMORY PERFORMANCE 205

that age differences in prospective memory perform- M. J., & Cunfer, A. R. (1995). Aging and prospective
ance partially depend on age-related individual differ- memory: Examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval
processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
ences in frontal/executive functions. Further research Memory, and Cognition, 21, 996–1007.
involving complex prospective memory tasks is now Einstein, G. O., Smith, R. E., McDaniel, M. A., & Shaw, P.
needed to examine the exact mechanisms responsible (1997). Aging and prospective memory: The influence of
for these effects (Ellis & Kvavilashvili, 2000). increased task demands at encoding and retrieval.
Psychology and Aging, 12, 479–488.
Ellis, J. (1996). Prospective memory or the realization of
delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. In
REFERENCES M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel
(Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp.
Bisiacchi, P. S. (1996). The neuropsychological approach in 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
the study of prospective memory. In M. A. Brandimonte, Ellis, J. & Kvavilashvili, L. (2000). Prospective memory in
G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective 2000: Past, present, and future directions. Applied
memory: Theory and applications (pp. 297–317). Cognitive Psychology, 14, S1–S9.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Evans, J., & Wilson, B. A. (1992). A memory group for individuals
Bisiacchi, P. S. (July 2000). Prospective memory in with brain injury. Clinical Rehabilitation, 6, 75–81.
Parkinson’s disease. Paper presented at the 1st Glisky, E. L. (1996). Prospective memory and the frontal lobes.
International Conference on Prospective Memory, Hatfield, In M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel
UK.Burgess, P.W. (July 1996). Prospective memory (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp.
following frontal lobe damage. Paper presented at the 2nd 249–266). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
International Conference on Memory, Abano-Padua, Italy. Greve, K. W., Brooks, J., Crouch, J. A., & Williams, M. C.
Burgess, P. W. (July 2000a). The cognitive neuroscience of (1997). Factorial structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting
prospective memory. Paper presented at the 1st International Test. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 283–285.
Conference on Prospective Memory, Hatfield, UK. Guynn, M. J., McDaniel. M.A., & Einstein, G.O. (2001).
Burgess, P. W. (2000b). Strategy application disorder: The Remembering to perform actions: A different type of
role of the frontal lobes in human multitasking. memory? In H. D. Zimmer, R. L. Cohen, M. J. Guynn,
Psychological Research, 63, 279–288. J. Engelkamp, R. Kormi-Nouri, & M.A. Foley (Eds.),
Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., de Lacy Costello, A., & Shallice, Memory for action: A distinct form of episodic memory?
T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates (pp. 25–48). New York: Oxford University Press.
of multi-tasking. Neuropsychologia, 38, 848–863. Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., &
Cherry, K. E., & LeCompte, D. C. (1999). Age and Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual.
individual differences influence prospective memory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Psychology and Aging, 14, 60–76. Hon, J., Huppert, F. A., Holland, A. J., & Watson, P. (1998).
Cockburn, J., Keene, J., & Hope, T. (July 2000). Are The value of the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test
prospective memory measures of the RBMT more vulnerable (Children’s Version) in an epidemiological study of older
to cognitive impairment than retrospective memory adults with Down syndrome. British Journal of Clinical
measures? Paper presented at the 1st International Psychology, 37, 15–29.
Conference on Prospective Memory, Hatfield, UK. Houx, P. J., Jolles, J., & Vreeling, F. W. (1993). Stroop
Cockburn, J., & Smith, P. T. (1988). Effects of age and interference: Aging effects assessed with the Stroop color-
intelligence on everyday memory tasks. In M. M. word test. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 209–224.
Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Huppert, F. A. & Beardsall, L. (1993). Prospective memory
Practical aspects of memory: Current research and impairment as an early indicator of dementia. Journal of
issues. Vol. 2: Clinical and educational implications Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 805–821.
(pp.132–136). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Huppert, F. A., Johnson, T., & Nickson, J. (2000). High
Cockburn, J., & Smith, P. T. (1991). The relative influence prevalence of prospective memory impairment in the elderly
of intelligence and age on everyday memory. Journal of and in early-stage dementia: Findings from a population-
Gerontology, 46, P31–P36. based study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S63–S81.
Cockburn, J., & Smith, P. T. (1994). Anxiety and errors of Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G.
prospective memory among elderly people. British O. (2001). Varying the importance of a prospective
Journal of Psychology, 85, 273–282. memory task: Differential effects across time- and event-
Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory based prospective memory. Memory, 9, 1–11.
mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G.
and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45–75. O. (2002a). Complex prospective memory and executive
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and control of working memory: A process model.
prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Psychologische Beiträge, 44, 303–318.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717–726. Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G.
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996). Retrieval O. (April 2002b). Prospective memory and aging: How
processes in prospective memory: Theoretical approaches planning affects performance. Paper presented at the
and some new empirical findings. In M. A. Brandimonte, Cognitive Aging Conference in Atlanta, GA, USA.
G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G.
memory: Theory and applications (pp. 115–141). O. (in press). Why do we remember to perform intended
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. actions? Importance effects on performance in event-
Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Richardson, S. L., Guynn, based prospective memory. Memory.
206 MARTIN, KLIEGEL, McDANIEL

Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Plan Patton, G. W. R. & Meit, M. (1993). Effect of aging on
formation, retention, and execution in prospective memory: prospective and incidental memory. Experimental Aging
A new approach and age-related effects. Memory and Research, 19, 165–176.
Cognition, 28, 1041–1049. Roediger, H. L. III (1996). Prospective memory and episodic
Kopp, U., & Thöne, A. I. T. (July 2000). Role of executive memory. In M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A.
functions and memory processes in delayed intentions McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and
after head injury. Paper presented at the 1st International applications (pp. 149–155). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Conference on Prospective Memory, Hatfield, UK. Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Kvavilashvili, L. (1987). Remembering intention as a distinct Rypma, B., & D’Esposito, M. (2001). Age-related changes
form of memory. British Journal of Psychology, 78, 507–518. in brain-behaviour relationships: Evidence from event-
Kvavilashvili, L., & Ellis, J. (1996). Varieties of intention: related functional fMRI studies. European Journal of
Some distinctions and classifications. In M. Brandimonte, Cognitive Psychology, 13, 235–256.
G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective Schretlen, D., Pearlson, G. D., Anthony, J. C., Aylward, E.
memory: Theory and applications (pp. 23–51). Mahwah, H., Augustine, A. M., Davis, A., & Barta, P. (2000).
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Elucidating the contributions of processing speed,
Martin, Ma. (1986). Ageing and patterns of change in everyday executive ability, and frontal lobe volume to normal age-
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 04:46 23 March 2015

memory and cognition. Human Learning, 5, 63–74. related differences in fluid intelligence. Journal of the
Martin, Mi., Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 52–61.
O. (July 2000). The neuropsychology of prospective Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy
memory: The role of central executive functions. Paper application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain,
presented at the 1st International Conference on 114, 727–741.
Prospective Memory, Hatfield, UK. Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., & Squire, L. R. (1991).
Martin, Mi., & Schumann-Hengsteler, R. (2001). How task What is the role of frontal lobe damage in memory
demands influence time-based prospective memory disorders? In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L.
performance in young and older adults. International Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp.
Journal of Behavioural Development, 25, 386–391. 173–195). New York: Oxford University Press.
Maylor, E. A. (1993). Aging and forgetting in prospective Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop
and retrospective memory tasks. Psychology and Aging, performance in healthy younger and older adults and in
8, 420–428. individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.
Maylor, E. A. (1996). Does prospective memory decline with Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
age? In M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel and Performance, 22, 461–479.
(Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal
173–198). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
McDaniel, M.A., & Einstein, G. O. (1993). The importance Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1987). The frontal lobes and
of cue familiarity and cue distinctiveness in prospective control of cognition and memory. In E. Perecman (Ed.),
memory. Memory, 1, 23–41. The frontal lobes revisited (pp. 141–158). Hillsdale, NJ:
McDaniel, M.A. & Einstein, G.O. (2000). Strategic and automatic Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of
framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S127–S144. age-cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear
McDaniel, M. A., Glisky, E. L., Rubin, S. R., Guynn, M. J., and nonlinear age effects and structural models.
& Routhieaux, B. C. (1999). Prospective memory: A Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231–249.
neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 13, 103–110. Ward, G., & Allport, A. (1997). Planning and problem-
McDaniel, M.A., Robinson-Riegler, B., & Einstein, G. O. (1998). solving using five-disc Tower of London task. Quarterly
Prospective remembering: Perceptually-driven or conceptually- Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
driven processes? Memory and Cognition, 26, 121–134. Experimental Psychology, 50A, 49–78.
Mitchell, K. J., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., & D’Esposito, Wecker, N. S., Kramer, J. H., Wisniewski, A., Delis, D. C.,
M. (2000). FMRI evidence of age-related hippocampal & Kaplan, E. (2000). Age effects on executive ability.
dysfunction in feature binding in working memory. Neuropsychology, 14, 409–414.
Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 197–206. West, R. L. (1988). Prospective memory and ageing. In M. M.
Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical
W. (1987). Computer-aided assessment of dementia: aspects of memory: Current research and issues, Vol. 2:
Comparative studies of neuropsychological deficits in Clinical and Educational Implications (pp. 119–125).
Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson’s disease. In S. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
M. Stahl & S. D. Iversen (Eds.), Cognitive neurochemistry West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex
(pp. 21–36). New York: Oxford University Press. function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin,
Park, D. C., Hertzog, C., Kidder, D. P., Morrell, R. W., & 120, 272–292.
Mayhorn, C. (1997). The effect of age on event-based Wilson, B.A., Cockburn, J., & Baddeley, A.D. (1985). The
and time-based prospective memory. Psychology and Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test. Titchfield, UK:
Aging, 12, 314–327. Thames Valley Test Co.
Park, D. C., & Kidder, D. P. (1996). Prospective memory Wiseman, K. A., Ratcliff, G., Chase, S., Laporte, D. J.,
and medication adherence. In M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Robertson, D. U., & Colantonio, A. (2000). Does a test of
Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: functional memory during the post-acute period predict long-
Theory and applications (pp. 369–390). Mahwah, NJ: term outcome of traumatic brain injury? Brain and
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Cognition, 44, 14–18.

You might also like