You are on page 1of 12

4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

C U R R E N T ( H T T P : / /AW
RWCH
WI.VJ E
O U(R
HNTA
TLPO
S :F/E
A/ L
B
QEO
UCU
OTT
DR. O
L
(HINBTI .C
TUPM
U
: /IB/CL
WHI W
S
.EEH
D
WDI .U
N
TJ/O
G
OJR
.U
/OSJRRENG
(PH
A//L
)T
3OT
33F
P 6E
: /4L/5ES
W1C
U
.W*TB)R
W
MO.IJN
TOI U
C( H
P
RUT
NTBAP
LLSIOS: F
/H/EIJLNOEGUC.R
O
TNR
RAO
GLN
/O
AI C
B
FPE
O

YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and


Grati cations Approach
Gary Hanson and Paul Haridakis
Journal of Electronic Publishing

Volume 11, Issue 3, Fall 2008


DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3998/3336451.0011.305 [http://dx.doi.org/10.3998/3336451.0011.305]

Abstract
Internet sites such as YouTube represent important changes in the way in which video content can be delivered.
YouTube lets viewers access videos on demand and makes it easy for them to share videos with others. The unique
nature of on-demand user-supplied video content is of particular interest to the electronic publishing community
because of the relative ease with which videos can be produced, uploaded, and shared. Users are now active
participants in the media distribution chain. Because users play an active role in the production, distribution, and
receipt of YouTube’s media content (e.g., creating, sharing, and viewing), it is appropriate to examine YouTube use
from an audience-centered perspective. One such approach is a theoretical framework called “uses and gratifications.”
It is used in this study to look at how college students view and share news content on the YouTube Web site.

We found that different motives predicted watching and sharing different types of news-related content. Viewers of
news in a more traditional format were doing so primarily for information reasons; viewers of news in comedy and
satire formats were doing so primarily for entertainment. Interpersonal communication motives predicted sharing of
news videos on YouTube. The results suggest that viewers may be driven by one set of motives for watching news clips
on YouTube, and a different set of motives for sharing them.

Introduction
The proliferation of inexpensive video camera and computer-editing software has made it possible for individuals to
produce video content, a portion of which is available for distribution on video-sharing Web sites such as YouTube. Much
of the content on YouTube is of the homegrown, amateur video variety, but an important subset is produced by
professionals (i.e., by the mainstream media and other organizations uploading content to reach the large online audience
or by individuals who record and upload professional media content). Included in that subset are news videos. Some
industry studies suggest that news clips are the most widely viewed videos in the category of professionally produced
content (Vorhaus, 2007).

The estimated audience for YouTube in December 2007 was 64 million visitors (Nielsen Online Reports TopLine U.S.
Data for November 2007, 2007). Nielsen Net Ratings suggest that the audience for YouTube mirrors the U.S. online
population as a whole: 19% of them are between the ages of 18 and 34 (YouTube Demographics, 2007). That age group is
attractive to advertisers, and so their media use is important to traditional news companies, which have long attempted to
bring young news consumers into the fold—often without success.

But YouTube is more than a substitute for the traditional TV news delivery system. The diffusion of news depends on
interpersonal communication as viewers tell their friends about interesting things they’ve watched or read. Because it is
on the Web, YouTube adds an important component to traditional post-viewing activity that earlier forms of television
did not have—the ability to share the videos and to replay them within one’s circle of family and friends.

In a more traditional media world, post-viewing activity was typically a discussion about a particularly interesting
program around the office water cooler the following morning. In the social networking world, those discussions can take
place around an infinite number of virtual water coolers.

Individual viewers are now an important part of the media distribution chain. This study examines the viewing of news
video on YouTube to see if this online delivery system can be a vehicle for electronic publishing professionals to reach a
key demographic with news and information. It further examines the impact of certain background characteristics and
motives for viewing news-related videos on YouTube and sharing them with others.

Theoretical Grounding
https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 1/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

Uses and Grati cations Motivation and Audience Orientation


In the new electronic-publishing environment, where individual users are playing a more active role in disseminating
video (as well as written) content, researchers and practitioners should be aware of research conducted from an audience-
centered perspective. Uses and gratifications is one such perspective.

Uses and gratifications is a social and psychological theory that suggests human communication behavior is driven by
people’s needs and desires (e.g., Katz, Blumler, & Gurevitch, 1973, 1974; Rosengren, 1974). We communicate and use
media to satisfy those needs and desires. Thus, uses and gratifications is more concerned with the functions that media
serve for people than with what media do to people (e.g., Klapper, 1963; Rubin, 2002).

Specifically, uses and gratifications is an “audience-centered perspective that assumes (a) media behavior is purposive,
goal-directed and motivated, (b) people select media content to satisfy their needs or desires, (c) social and psychological
dispositions mediate that behavior, and (d) the ‘media compete with other forms of communication—or functional
alternatives—such as interpersonal interaction for selection, attention and use’” (Rubin et al., 2003, p. 129).

Audience motivation is central to uses and gratifications research. Uses and gratifications researchers tap into more
abstract concepts such as needs and desires through measurable constructs such as motives. Motives, in turn, guide
communication behavior such as the selection of media and specific content. Specific motives have been linked to
choosing media such as radio (Lazarsfeld & Field, 1946), newspaper (Waples, Berelson, & Bradshaw, 1940), television
(Rubin, 1983), talk radio (Armstrong & Rubin, 1989), VCRs (Cohen, Levy, & Golden, 1988), the Internet and World Wide
Web (Hanson & Hanson, 2006; Kaye & Johnson, 2002; Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000), and specific content such as
entertainment fare (Haridakis, 2002; Perse, 1986) and news (Haridakis & Rubin, 2005; Palmgreen, Wenner, & Rayburn,
1980; Perse, 1990a). Recently, researchers have turned their attention to Web-based settings media (Lin, Salwen, &
Abdulla, 2003; Tewksbury, 2003).

Although uses and gratifications theory suggests that people are goal-directed and purposive in their media use, such
purposiveness is relative. For example, at times audience members actively seek information to satisfy their needs and
desires (e.g., Levy & Windahl, 1984). At other times, they use media more passively for diversion, out of habit, or simply
to pass the time. Such observations have led researchers to identify general viewer orientations toward media use to
reflect links between motives, degree of activity, and attitudes toward a medium, toward its content, or toward both.
Researchers have not agreed on the names for groups of motives, degree of activity, and attitudes toward a medium and
its content. Rubin (2002) called the combination of more purposive motivation, greater activity (e.g., intentional,
attentive, and involved use), and affinity for content “instrumental orientation.” He calls the combination of habitual or
passive use and greater affinity for the medium “ritualized orientation.” Perse (1990b) called similar combinations
“utilitarian” and “diversionary.”

Motives and broader media-use orientations are further influenced by the psychological and social contexts in which
media use occurs (Rubin, 2002). For example, those with limited opportunities for social interaction may turn to the
media for companionship and social activity (see, for example, Armstrong & Rubin, 1989; Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000).
People may turn to the media to satisfy their need for useful information (Palmgreen et al., 1980). Accordingly, it is
important to account for relevant social and psychological circumstances that may influence viewer motivation, activity,
and attitudes when studying particular media-use contexts.

Background Characteristics
One personality trait that has been linked with motives for watching television (e.g., Haridakis & Rubin, 2003),
perceptions derived from watching televised content (e.g., Wober & Gunter, 1982), and behavioral and attitudinal
outcomes (Haridakis, 2002) is locus of control. It reflects people’s perceptions of their ability to control events in their
lives. People who believe events in their lives are dictated largely by forces beyond their control (e.g., fate, chance) are said
to be externally controlled. Those who believe that they control events in their lives are considered internally controlled.
YouTube users can choose what to view, when they view it, and with whom to share it, suggesting that externally
controlled viewers and internally controlled viewers may select and use YouTube differently.

Sensation seeking is another personality trait found in media effects research that reflects a person’s need for stimulation
and relative tendency to approach rather than avoid novel stimuli. Sensation seeking and its individual dimensions (thrill
seeking, experience seeking, disinhibition and boredom) have been linked to motives for watching violent media content
(Haridakis, 2002; Krcmar & Greene, 1999) and lighter media content such as soap operas (e.g., Conway & Rubin, 1991).
Users of YouTube have access to a wide variety of content—including videos of sensational or off-beat stories—and can
access it and share it with others in a novel way. A user’s need for stimulation may affect what YouTube content is selected
or shared.

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 2/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

In addition to the two personality characteristics of locus of control and sensation seeking, we examined two social
characteristics of YouTube users: social activity and interpersonal interaction. By definition, social-networking
environments like YouTube contain a social component. Recent research has found links between Internet users’
motivation for using the Internet, and interpersonal interaction (e.g., Papacharissi, 2002; Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000)
and interpersonal relationship closeness (Pornsakulvanich, Haridakis, & Rubin, 2008). If general Internet use is linked to
interpersonal interaction, then it is logical to assume that the use of a particular Internet function also will be linked to
interpersonal interaction.

This study applies a uses and gratifications framework and considers how personality characteristics (i.e., locus of control
and sensation seeking), social background factors (i.e., social activity and interpersonal interaction) and motives for using
YouTube influence how and why the much-sought-after college-age cohort views and shares news videos.

Research Questions
Utilitarian motives such as surveillance and information seeking have long been associated with watching news
(Palmgreen et al., 1980; Perse, 1990b). Researchers also have noted that computer-mediated communication is often
used for relational communication (Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000; Walther, 1996; Wolfradt & Doll, 2001). This has been
the case with social-networking sites in particular (Rapacki, 2007). Uses-and-gratifications research also has suggested
that media selection and use, as well as the motives for selecting and using media, are influenced by a person’s social and
psychological background characteristics (e.g., Haridakis, 2002; Rubin, 2002).

Accordingly, this study considers two broad research questions: (1) How background characteristics and motivation
predict whether people will watch traditional and comedy-based news items on YouTube and (2) how these same factors
predict whether people will share the content with others.

Methodology
Sample
Students in one of our classes comprised the sample ; the survey was handed out and completed during a single class
period. Research and news reporting has suggested that college students tend to use the Internet in general (Morahan-
Martin & Schumacher, 2000), and to access YouTube in particular (Hansell, 2006). The sample reflects a desired
demographic for media companies and seemed appropriate for the exploratory nature of this study. A total of 291
questionnaires provided usable data. The sample was 33.9% men (coded 0) and 66.1 % women (coded 1). The mean age
was 19.83 (SD = 0.47) years.

Measurement
The questionnaire measured the following variables:

1. social and psychological antecedents (i.e., locus of control, sensation seeking, interpersonal interaction, social
activities)

2. motives for using YouTube

3. how often subjects watched and shared news videos on YouTube

Locus of Control.
We measured locus of control with Levenson’s (1974) measure. Respondents rated their agreement with 12 statements (1
= strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) reflecting three dimensions of locus of control: powerful others control (e.g., “my
life is chiefly controlled by powerful others”), chance control (e.g., “when I get what I want it’s usually because I’m lucky”),
and internal control (e.g., “my life is determined by my own actions”). This has been a valid and reliable method of
assessing level of locus of control in past research (Haridakis, 2002; Rubin, 1993). Responses to items reflecting external
control were recoded. Therefore, higher scores evidenced greater internal control. Responses were summed and averaged
to create the internal control index (M = 3.64, SD = 0.44, α = .72).

Sensation Seeking.
We measured sensation seeking with Form V of Zuckerman’s (1979) scale. This has been a widely used measure of risk-
taking behavior in social science research (Krcmar & Greene, 1999). It is comprised of four subscales: thrill seeking,
experience seeking, disinhibition, and boredom susceptibility. As in prior communication research (Conway & Rubin,
1991; Haridakis, 2002), respondents indicated their agreement (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) with

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 3/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach
sensation-seeking items. Items were summed and averaged to create indices for each dimension: thrill seeking (M = 3.19,
SD = 0.87, α = .86), experience seeking (M = 3.01, SD = 0.69, α = .74), disinhibition (M = 2.96, SD = 0.81, α = .86), and
boredom susceptibility (M = 2.73, SD = 0.53, α = .73).

Social Antecedents.
Two dimensions of a life-position measure adapted from prior research (Rubin & Rubin, 1982, 1986, 1989) were used to
measure respondents’ levels of social activity and interpersonal interaction. Respondents rated their agreement (1 =
strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) with five statements assessing their social activity (M = 3.53, SD = 0.68, α = .62)
and five statements measuring their interpersonal interaction (M = 4.00, SD = 0.55, α = .58).

Motivation for Using YouTube.


We measured YouTube-use motivation with a 51-item scale adapted from previous research. Specifically, we adapted an
Internet motives scale developed by Papacharissi and Rubin (2000), which was itself based on past measures of
interpersonal communication motives, various media-use motives, and additional items specifically directed at Internet
use (Rubin, 1981; Rubin, Perse, & Barbato, 1988). In response to focus-group discussions, we included three items
reflecting arousal motivation and three items reflecting social interaction motivation. These six items were adapted from
Rubin’s Television Viewing Motives Scale (Rubin, 1983).

Respondents were asked to indicate how much each of the 51 motive statements was like their own reasons for using
YouTube (1 = not at all, 5 = exactly). Principal components factor analysis with varimax rotation was used to analyze the
motive statements. Four factors, accounting for 67.62% of the total variance, were identified. Responses to items that
loaded on each factor were summed and averaged to create indices of the respective viewing motives. The four factors
were: leisure entertainment, interpersonal expression, information seeking, and companionship. Table 1
[https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305/--youtube-users-watching-and-sharing-the-news-a-uses?
trgt=tb_1;view=fulltext#tb_1] summarizes the results of the factor analysis.

YouTube Use.
We asked respondents to indicate how often (1 = never, 5 = very often) they used YouTube to watch traditional news
videos such as segments of CNN, ABC, local newscasts or videos of actual events (M = 2.03, SD = 1.05) and comedy-
related or satirical news videos such as The Daily Show, The Colbert Report, or monologues by Jay Leno or David
Letterman (M = 2.77, SD = 1.35). We also asked respondents to indicate how often they shared traditional news videos (M
= 1.76, SD = 1.04) and comedy-related news videos (M = 2.28, SD = 1.35).

Results
Using YouTube for News
Ninety-one percent of the students in the sample indicated they had watched at least one video on the YouTube Web site.
The median amount of time spent watching YouTube is just under 15 minutes per typical week. Of the sample, 69% said
they had watched at least some traditional-format news clips on YouTube; 74% indicated they had watched at least some
comedy-format news. Many students shared what they watched: 42% said they shared traditional news stories; 57% said
they shared comedy news. Fewer than 16% indicated they had uploaded original video clips to the YouTube site; fewer
than 7% said they had uploaded clips that someone else had produced. The median number of clips viewed during a single
visit to the site is between 1 and 2 irrespective of the content. The highest was 100. One subject reported sharing as many
as 30 video clips in a week.

Motivation for Watching and Sharing YouTube


We used multiple regression analysis to assess the relevant contribution of motives and the other antecedent variables to
predict the viewing and sharing of YouTube’s news content. The uses and gratifications model suggests that an
individual’s background characteristics can influence motives for media use. Accordingly, variables were entered into the
equations in the following conceptual order: first, user background characteristics, and second, user motives. We entered
age and gender on a first step in the regression equations for purposes of control. Final results of the hierarchical
regression analyses are summarized in table 2 [https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305/--youtube-users-watching-
and-sharing-the-news-a-uses?trgt=tb_2;view=fulltext#tb_2] .

Watching Traditional News Videos

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 4/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach
Demographic variables entered on the first step (age and gender) accounted for less than 1% of the variance in watching
traditional news videos. Neither age nor gender was a predictor. Background characteristics (social activities,
interpersonal interaction, locus of control, and sensation seeking) on step 2 increased the explained variance by 4.7%. The
F change was not significant (p = .058). The only predictor was locus of control (β = −.16, p < .05). Entering the viewing
motives on the third step added an additional 9% to the variance. The F change was significant (p < .001). Information-
seeking motivation was a significant predictor of watching traditional news videos. Locus of control ceased to be a
predictor at this step.

Accordingly, after all variables were entered, information-seeking motivation was the only significant contributor to the
final equation (β = .25, p < .01). These results suggest, not surprisingly, that those who were seeking information were
more likely than their counterparts to watch traditional news content on YouTube.

Watching Comedy News Videos


Age and gender accounted for 6.5% of the variance when entered on the first step. Male gender (β = −.25, p < .001) was a
significant predictor.

Social activities, interpersonal interaction, locus of control, and sensation seeking, entered on the second step, increased
the explained variance by 8%. The F change was significant (p < .01). Locus of control (β = −.14, p < .05) and social
activities (β = .14, p < .05) were significant predictors.

Entering viewing motives on the third step added an additional 15.1% to the explained variance. The F change was
significant (p < .001). Leisure entertainment motivation (β = .32, p < .001) was the only motive that predicted watching
comedy news. Both locus of control and social activity ceased to be predictors.

Accordingly, significant contributors to the final equation were leisure entertainment motivation (β = .32, p < .001) and
male gender (β = −.18, p < .01). These results suggest that males motivated by a desire for leisure entertainment were
more likely than their counterparts in the study to watch comedy news videos on YouTube.

Sharing Traditional News Videos


Age and gender entered on the first step accounted for less than 1% of the variance in sharing traditional news video
content. Neither was a predictor. Background variables entered on step 2 accounted for an additional 5.6% of the
variance. The change in F was significant (p < .05). External locus of control (β = −.15, p < .05) was the only significant
predictor.

Entering motives increased explained variance by 5.5%. The F change was


significant (p < .01), and sharing for interpersonal expression (β = .22, p <
“YouTube users who shared
.01) was a significant predictor. Locus of control ceased to be a predictor.
traditional news videos These results suggest that YouTube users who shared traditional news videos
tended to do so for purposes
tended to do so for purposes of interpersonal expression.
of interpersonal expression.”

Sharing Comedy News Videos


Age and gender accounted for 1.9% of the variance in comedy news video
sharing. Male gender (β = −.13, p < .05) was a predictor. Social activities, interpersonal interaction, locus of control, and
sensation seeking increased explained variance by 7%. Two sensation-seeking variables—disinhibition (β = −.21, p < .01)
and experience seeking (β = .20, p < .01)—were predictors.

Entry of motivation factors (third step) increased explained variance by 23%, with a significant F change (p < .001).
Leisure entertainment motivation (β = .30, p < .001) and interpersonal expression motivation (β = .28, p < .01) were
significant predictors of sharing comedy news content. Thrill seeking (β = −.12, p < .05) also emerged as a predictor.

Accordingly, two motives (leisure entertainment and interpersonal expression) and one background characteristic
(experience seeking) were significant positive contributors to the final equation. Two background characteristics (thrill
seeking and disinhibition) were significant negative predictors.

Thus, participants most likely to share comedy news content were less disinhibited and less willing to engage in thrilling
behavior (but more willing to engage in experience seeking) and tended to share this content for purposes of leisure
entertainment and interpersonal expression.

YouTube as a Harbinger
https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 5/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach
Internet sites such as YouTube represent important changes in the way video
content, including news, will be delivered in the future. Viewers can access
“For electronic publishers, news on demand and play a more active role in the distribution channel by
there are similarities between forwarding video clips to other people. In this study we focused on factors
this phenomenon and the influencing viewing and sharing news videos. However, the growing role of
proliferation of desktop the user in the distribution process has important implications for anyone
publishing of printed material engaged in electronic publishing. For electronic publishers, there are
a generation earlier.” similarities between this phenomenon and the proliferation of desktop
publishing of printed material a generation earlier. Inexpensive video
recorders and cameras, coupled with easy-to-use editing software and the
ability to deliver videos online, have democratized the creation and distribution of video. Mainstream media companies
can no longer exert total control over the distribution of their own media content. It’s too soon to understand the long-
term impact of this change on news and information viewership. It does suggest that viewing and sharing news videos are
not mutually exclusive and that the number of gatekeepers is as infinite as the number of electronic water coolers around
which viewers gather to chat.

We found important links between viewing and sharing news on YouTube and users’ motives for these two activities.
Studies of television news viewing have suggested that information seeking and entertainment are important motives for
watching television news (Perse, 1992). We found this to be the case with YouTube as well. These results suggest, at least
for the young adults in this study, that motives for watching traditional and comedy-related news videos were similar to
ones identified in previous news-viewing research. However, the types of videos available seem to have made a difference,
too. Those who watched more traditional news content wanted information; those who watched news in comedy and
satire formats wanted entertainment. This may answer the critics who are concerned that some viewers may not
understand the difference between news and “news.” Participants in this study indicated they can tell one from the other.
An optimist might see the results as support for the position that people are less likely to be swayed by the satire of Jon
Stewart or Stephen Colbert. A pessimist might point out that people still absorb information and form opinions about the
news from the satirical programs—even when viewers tune in to be entertained.

These results also may support previous uses-and-gratifications arguments that people can use different communication
channels as functional (Rosengren & Windahl, 1972) or co-equal (Rubin & Rubin, 1985) alternatives for satisfying their
needs and desires. What researchers should examine more closely in future research is whether the opportunity to view
mediated fare on demand, as permitted by YouTube, makes YouTube the preferred channel for such fare for some
viewers. The on-demand nature of YouTube may make it more attractive than the traditional media for those viewers
seeking the immediate satisfaction that instant access provides.

It is important to understand the nuances behind the viewing choices in this study. Factor analysis revealed that the
entertainment motive stressed its leisure components, while items that emphasized the economy and convenience of
information acquisition characterized the information motive.

The results also suggest that viewers may be driven by one set of motives for watching news clips on YouTube, but have a
different set of motives for sharing them. The need for interpersonal expression was a predictor for sharing both
categories of content. A nuanced reading of the motive is important here, too. The factors comprising the interpersonal
motive stressed the need to express one’s self and to have a voice in the marketplace of information.

Uses and gratifications theory posits that viewer personality traits should be accounted for in studies of media use and
effects. Our analysis of YouTube viewer motivation supports this. Specifically, disinhibition and thrill seeking were
negative predictors of sharing comedy news content, while experience seeking, leisure entertainment, and interpersonal
expression motivations were significant positive predictors. This suggests a rather informal way of sharing videos for
entertainment and interpersonal expression among those who may be seeking experience, but otherwise are non–
sensation seekers.

These findings are significant for media professionals and researchers alike.
Video sites like YouTube give news organizations the opportunity to reach
“Viewers of news are now audiences through a secondary market of mouse clicks and forwarded e-mails,
part of the distribution
and viewers of news are now part of the distribution chain. The ability to tease
chain.” out the motivations for that activity can play a part in identifying opinion
leaders in the social networking spaces who will be attractive for media
companies to know. The changes in video distribution are important for
researchers also, as they struggle to understand the changing relationship between mass communication and
interpersonal communication functions—particularly for media content that’s delivered on demand and through new
https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 6/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

social networks. The predictive strength of the motives found in this study suggest that the uses-and-gratifications
approach, even though it emerged in a simpler media time, is still useful for understanding audiences’ media use and the
confluence of background characteristics and audience activity that impact their use, and ultimately, the effects of that
use.

Limitations of the Study and Future Research


Clearly these results have to be interpreted with caution. As we suggested earlier, YouTube, despite claims of a large
number of users and the proliferation of content on the site, is a relatively new phenomenon with which users are still
becoming acquainted. Thus, claims of its uses and effects for the general population cannot be made with confidence. Our
sample of college students was not a group of heavy YouTube users and they had not yet developed strong motivations for
using it. But it does provide a benchmark for future studies when YouTube becomes a more familiar medium.

Our results from this convenience sample should not be generalized to other populations of YouTube users. Future
research should test these preliminary observations by including additional measures of audience activity to get a clearer
picture of how active or passive YouTube use is. Research that assesses the level of viewing intention, attention, and
involvement while viewing, and the amount of post-viewing interaction stimulated via sharing, is needed to evaluate use
on an interactivity-passivity continuum.

In addition, further research should examine the role of a wider array of social
and psychological background characteristics that will help researchers
“Getting information into the
identify additional factors that could more fully explain how YouTube is used.
hands of the opinion leaders The measures for this study were taken from prior research of television
is important for the viewing and Internet use. Valid survey questions and measurement scales
dissemination of mediated
need to be developed for studying interactive and social-networked media.
fare.” The existence of the social networks needs to be explored further, too. We
know that getting information into the hands of the opinion leaders is
important for the dissemination of mediated fare. Do YouTube users who tend
to seek traditional news information share it for different interpersonal reasons than those who seek out more comedic
news fare? Do those who are opinion leaders with respect to traditional news fare, in turn, tend to be recipients of
comedic news or other entertainment fare from those who seek out that type of content? This study did not answer these
questions, but suggests a framework for doing so.

Finally, and perhaps most importantly, the results of this study and the theoretical framework used should be considered
to examine media use in other electronic-publishing contexts. We focused on YouTube because of its immense popularity
and phenomenal rate of adoption by users. But the on-demand nature of YouTube is a salient feature in a host of other
electronic-publishing contexts, all of which are new. Blogs, wikis, and user-generated comments posted to news stories
from the traditional media are just some examples of this phenomenon in the printed (written) world. Photo sharing sites
such as Flickr and audio and podcast aggregating sites such as iTunes expand the list to include other media. YouTube
itself is not the only video-sharing Web site. Other sites include videos on the social networking MySpace and Facebook
and on lesser-known sites such as Bolt and SingingFool. Each of these sites represents a form of electronic publishing in
which users can connect with each other and create, edit, and share their own content. We believe that these phenomena
expand the reach of electronic publishers by adding new creators, new distribution channels, and new messages to the
mix. These facts make it all the more necessary to examine user-generated electronic publishing from as part of the
electronic publishing landscape and to understand the background characteristics, motives, and use patterns of those who
are part of the process.

Table 1: Primary factor loadings of motives

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4


Because it amuses me .890 .172 .109 .051
Because it’s enjoyable .877 .122 .128 .024
Because it’s entertaining .877 .096 .097 .060
Because it’s fun to play around and check things .869 .089 .105 .037
When I have nothing better to do .837 .168 .051 .116
Because it passes the time away particularly when I’m bored .817 .159 .161 .151
Because it gives me something to occupy my time .804 .196 .204 .046
Just because it’s there .774 .230 .179 .140

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 7/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

Because I like to use it .765 .236 .277 .072


Because I can use it any time .705 .256 .326 .128
To see what’s out there .699 .193 .390 −.070
Because it’s just a habit, just something to do .679 .213 .260 .073
Because it’s something to do when friends come over .646 .339 .178 −.068
Because I can view material in videos online, and I don’t have to pay for them .642 .144 .345 −.024
Because it’s exciting .641 .314 .271 .186
Because it’s thrilling .614 .350 .348 .176
To belong to a group with the same interests as mine .121 .759 .171 .262
To participate in discussions .181 .757 .224 −.106
To give my input .258 .759 .189 −.103
To communicate with family and friends .158 .749 .137 .046
Because I enjoy answering other people’s questions .192 .724 .105 .280
To show others encouragement .218 .703 .125 .245
Because I can express myself freely .258 .665 .251 .154
To meet new people .046 .639 .029 .385
To help others .178 .624 .341 .161
So I can talk to other people about what’s going on .326 .608 .294 .047
To get more points of view .289 .601 .459 .088
Because it provides an interesting way to do research .239 .299 .724 .214
To keep up with current issues or events .196 .284 .720 .158
To get information for free .419 .329 .650 −.093
To search for information .318 .197 .647 .180
Because it’s easier to get information .452 .328 .625 −.076
Because it makes me feel less lonely .186 .398 .167 .756
So I won’t have to be alone .120 .447 .174 .716

Eigenvalue 10.49 6.82 3.87 1.81


% variance expained 30.84% 20.06% 11.38% 5.34%
Mean 2.68 1.35 1.83 1.26
SD 1.12 0.58 0.91 0.63
Cronbach’s alpha .96 .92 .87 r=.74

Note: Factor 1=leisure entertainment; factor 2=interpersonal expression; factor 3=information seeking and factor
4=companionship.

Table 2: Summary of final results when regressing news video viewing and sharing on audience
background and motive variables

Traditional News Comedy News Traditional News Comedy News


Viewing Viewing Sharing Sharing
Predictor variables Final β Final β Final β Final β
Step Age .02 −.04 .05 −.03
1
Gender −.01 −.18** −.01 −.05
Step Social activity .12 .09 .10 −.00
2
Interpersonal .04 .09 .06 .09
interaction
Locus of control −.08 −.10 −.10 −.06
Thrill seeking −.02 −.06 −.07 −.12*
Disinhibition .03 −.02 .06 −.19**
Experience seeking .01 .08 .07 .18**
Boredom −.03 .02 −.04 .06
susceptibility
Step Motives
3

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 8/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

Leisure −.12 .32*** −.10 .30***


entertainment
Information seeking .25** .04 .13 .06
Interpersonal .08 .15 .22* .28**
expression
Companionship .13 −.08 −.03 −.08

Note: All betas are final betas on the last step of the regression. N = 291.
*p < .05
**p < .01
***p < .001.
Traditional news viewing (R = .38, R2 = .14, F(13, 274) = 3.44, p < .001)
Comedy news viewing (R = .55, R2 = .30, F(13, 274) = 9.16, p < .001)
Traditional news sharing (R = .34, R2 = .11 F(13, 274) = 2.67, p < .01)
Comedy news sharing (R = .57, R2 = .32, F(13, 273) = 9.83, p < .001).

[/j/jep/images/3336451.0011.305-00000001.jpg]

Gary Hanson is an associate professor in the School of Journalism and Mass Communications at Kent State University
and a 25-year veteran of television news. From 1984 to 1997 he was News Director of WKBN, the CBS television affiliate
in Youngstown, Ohio. He was Freedom and Responsibility Chair of the RTV-J Division of AEJMC, the Association for
Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, chairman of the Radio-Television News Directors Association,
representing 3,800 broadcast and cable journalists around the world. His work with RTNDA included the chairmanship
of the Education Liaison Committee, serving as the liaison between the industry association and the nation’s radio and
television journalism education community. He won three National Association of Television Arts and Sciences Regional
EMMY awards in the Cleveland region for his work as principal reporter on the WKBN-TV public affairs broadcast
“Newswatch Sunday.” Prior to joining WKBN, he was news director of KXON-TV in Mitchell, South Dakota. He began his
television career at WDAZ-TV in Grand Forks, North Dakota, in 1971. He earned a B.A. in Speech from the University of
North Dakota in 1973. He can be reached at glhanson@kent.edu [mailto:glhanson@kent.edu] .

[/j/jep/images/3336451.0011.305-00000002.jpg]

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 9/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

Paul Haridakis is an associate professor of Communication Studies at Kent State University. He teaches and conducts
research in the areas of freedom of speech, media law and policy, and media uses and effects. His research is cross-
disciplinary, appearing in journals and books in the fields of law, communication, political science, psychology, and
justice studies. He can be reached at pharidak@kent.edu [mailto:pharidak@kent.edu] .

References
Armstrong, Cameron B., & Alan M. Rubin. 1989. “Talk Radio as Interpersonal Communication.” Journal of
Communication 39 (2): 84–94.

Cohen, Akiba A., Mark R. Levy, & Karen Golden. 1988. “Children’s Uses and Gratifications of Home VCRs: Evolution or
Revolution.” Communication Research 15 (6): 772–80.

Conway, Joseph C., & Alan M. Rubin. 1991. “Psychological Predictors of Television Viewing Motivation.” Communication
Research 18 (4): 443–63.

Hansell, Saul. 2006. “Joining the Party, Eager to Make Friends.” New York Times, October 16. Available at
http://www.nytimes.com/2006/10/16/technology/16social.html?_r=1&sq=&pagewanted=all
[http://www.nytimes.com/2006/10/16/technology/16social.html?_r=1&sq=&pagewanted=all]

Hanson, G., & L. Hanson. 2006. Measuring the Young Audience. Paper presented at the International Conference on
Communication, Mass Media & Culture: Freedom and Prejudice, Istanbul, Turkey, October 6.

Haridakis, Paul M. 2002. “Viewer Characteristics, Exposure to Television Violence, and Aggression.” Media Psychology 4
(4): 323–54.

Haridakis, Paul M., & Alan M. Rubin. 2003. “Motivation for Watching Television Violence and Viewer Aggression.” Mass
Communication & Society 6 (1): 29–56.

Haridakis, Paul M., & Alan M. Rubin. 2005. “Third-Person Effects in the Aftermath of Terrorism.” Mass Communication
& Society 8 (1): 39–59.

Katz, Elihu, Jay G. Blumler, & Michael Gurevitch. 1973. “Uses and Gratifications Research.” Public Opinion Quarterly 37
(4): 509–23.

Katz, Elihu, Jay G. Blumler, & Michael Gurevitch. 1974. “Utilization of Mass Communication by the Individual.” In Jay G.
Blumler & Elihu Katz, eds., The Uses of Mass Communications: Current Perspectives on Gratifications Research, pp. 19–
32. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Kaye, Barbara K., & Thomas J. Johnson. 2002. “Online and in the Know: Uses and Gratifications of the Web for Political
Information.” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 46 (1): 54–71.

Klapper, Joseph T. 1963. “Mass Communication Research: An Old Road Resurveyed.” The Public Opinion Quarterly 27
(4): 515–27.

Krcmar, Marina, & Kathryn Greene. 1999. “Predicting Exposure to and Uses of Television Violence.” Journal of
Communication 49 (3): 24–45.

Krcmar, Marina, & Linda Godbold Kean. 2005. “Uses and Gratifications of Media Violence: Personality Correlates of
Viewing and Liking Violent Genres.” Media Psychology 7 (4): 399–420.

Lazarsfeld, Paul F., & Harry H. Field. 1946. The People Look at Radio. Chapel Hill: The University of North Carolina
Press.

Levenson, H. 1974. “Activism and Powerful Others: Distinctions within the Concepts of Internal-External Control.”
Journal of Personality Assessment 38: 377–83.

Levy, Mark R., & Sven Windahl. 1984. “Audience Activity and Gratifications: A Conceptual Clarification and Exploration.”
Communication Research 11 (1): 51–78.

Lin, C., M. Salwen, & R. Abdulla. 2003. Uses and Gratifications of Offline Newspaper and Online News. Paper presented
at the Conference Papers—International Communication Association, San Diego, CA, May 23.

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 10/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach

Morahan-Martin, J., & P. Schumacher. 2000. “Incidence and Correlates of Pathological Internet Use among College
Students.” Computers in Human Behavior 16 (1): 13–29.

Nielsen Online Reports TopLine U.S. Data for November 2007. (2007). New York, NY: Nielsen Online.

Palmgreen, Philip, Lawrence A. Wenner, & J. D. Rayburn II. 1980. “Relations Between Gratifications Sought and
Obtained: A Study of Television News.” Communication Research 7 (2): 161–92.

Papacharissi, Zizi. 2002. “The Self Online: The Utility of Personal Home Pages.” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic
Media 46 (3): 346–68.

Papacharissi, Zizi, & Alan M. Rubin. 2000. “Predictors of Internet Use.” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 44
(2): 175–96.

Perse, Elizabeth M. 1986. “Soap Opera Viewing Patterns of College Students and Cultivation.” Journal of Broadcasting &
Electronic Media 30 (2): 175–93.

Perse, Elizabeth M. 1990a. “Cultivation and Involvement with Local Television News.” In Nancy Signorielli & Michael
Morgan, eds., Cultivation Analysis: New Directions in Media Effects Research, pp. 51–69. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Perse, Elizabeth M. 1990b. “Involvement with Local Television News: Cognitive and Emotional Dimensions.” Human
Communication Research 16 (4): 556–81.

Perse, Elizabeth M. 1992. “Predicting Attention to Local Television News: Need for Cognition and Motives for Viewing.”
Communication Reports 5 (1): 40–49.

Pornsakulvanich, Vikanda, Paul Haridakis, & Alan M. Rubin. 2008. “The Influence of Dispositions and Internet
Motivation on Online Communication Satisfaction and Relationship Closeness.” Computers in Human Behavior 24 (5):
2292–2310.

Rapacki, Sean. 2007. “Social Networking Sites: Why Teens Need Places Like MySpace.” Young Adult Library Services 5
(2): 28–30.

Rosengren, K. E. 1974. “Uses and Gratifications: A Paradigm Outlined.” In Jay G. Blumler & Elihu Katz, eds., The Uses of
Mass Comunications: Current Perspectives on Gratifications Research, pp. 269–86. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Rosengren, K. E., & S. Windahl. 1972. “Mass Media Consumption as a Functional Alternative.” In Denis McQuail, ed.,
Sociology of Mass Communications, pp. 166–94. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.

Rubin, Alan M. 1981. “An Examination of Television Viewing Motivations.” Communication Research 8 (2): 141–65.

Rubin, Alan M. 1983. “Television Uses and Gratifications: The Interactions of Viewing Patterns and Motivations.” Journal
of Broadcasting 27 (1): 37–51.

Rubin, Alan M. 1993. “The Effect of Locus of Control on Communication Motivation, Anxiety, and Satisfaction.”
Communication Quarterly 41 (2): 161–71.

Rubin, Alan M. 2002. “The Uses-and-Gratifications Perspective of Media Effects.” In Jennings Bryant & Dolf Zillman,
eds., Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research, 2nd ed., pp. 525–48. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rubin, Alan M., Paul M. Haridakis, Gwen A. Hullman, Shaojing Sun, Pamela M. Chikombero, & Vikanda
Pornsakulvanich. 2003. “Television Exposure Not Predictive of Terrorism Fear.” Newspaper Research Journal 24 (1):
128–45.

Rubin, Alan M., & Rebecca B. Rubin. 1982. “Contextual Age and Television Use.” Human Communication Research 8 (3):
228–44.

Rubin, Alan M., & Rebecca B. Rubin. 1985. “Interface of Personal and Mediated Communication: A Research Agenda.”
Critical Studies in Mass Communication 2 (1): 36–53.

Rubin, Alan M., & Rebecca B. Rubin. 1986. “Contextual Age as a Life-Position Index.” International Journal of Aging and
Human Development 23 (1): 27–45.

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 11/12
4/7/2019 YouTube Users Watching and Sharing the News: A Uses and Gratifications Approach
Rubin, A. M., & R. B. Rubin. 1989. “Social and Psychological Antecedents of VCR Use.” In Mark R. Levy, ed., The VCR
Age: Home Video and Mass Communication, pp. 92–111. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Rubin, Rebecca B., Elizabeth M. Perse, & Carole A. Barbato. 1988. “Conceptualization and Measurement of Interpersonal
Communication Motives.” Human Communication Research 14 (4): 602–28.

Tewksbury, David. 2003. “What Do Americans Really Want to Know? Tracking the Behavior of News Readers on the
Internet.” Journal of Communication 53 (4): 694–710.

Vorhaus, Mike. 2007. “Who’s Really Viewing YouTube.” Advertising Age 78 (7, February 12): 18.

Walther, Joseph B. 1996. “Computer-Mediated Communication: Impersonal, Interpersonal, and Hyperpersonal


Interaction.” Communication Research 23 (1): 3–43.

Waples, Douglas, Bernard Berelson, & Franklyn R. Bradshaw. 1940. What Reading Does to People: A Summary of
Evidence on the Social Effects of Reading and a Statement of Problems for Research. Chicago, IL: The University of
Chicago Press.

Wober, Mallory, & Barrie Gunter. 1982. “Television and Personal Threat: Fact or Artifact? A British Survey.” British
Journal of Social Psychology 21 (3): 239–47.

Wolfradt, Uwe, & Jörg Doll. 2001. “Motives of Adolescents to Use the Internet as a Function of Personality Traits,
Personal and Social Factors.” Journal of Educational Computing Research 24 (1): 13–27.

YouTube Demographics. 2007. New York, NY: Nielsen NetRatings.

Zuckerman, M. 1979. Sensation Seeking: Beyond the Optimal Level of Arousal. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates.

Product of Michigan Publishing (http://www.publishing.umich.edu), University of Michigan Library (http://www.lib.umich.edu/) • jep-info@umich.edu (mailto:jep-
info@umich.edu?subject=Journal%20of%20Electronic%20Publishing) • ISSN 1080-2711

https://quod.lib.umich.edu/j/jep/3336451.0011.305?view=text;rgn=main 12/12

You might also like