You are on page 1of 19

JOURNAL OF SPORT & EXERCISE PSYCHOLOGY, 1996, 18, 17-35

O 1996 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

Development and Validation of a Scale to Measure


Optimal Experience: The Flow State Scale

Susan A. Jackson Herbert W. Marsh


University of Queensland University of Western Sydney, Macarthur

The Flow State Scale (FSS) is a new measure of flow in sport and physical
activity settings. The nine FSS scales of the 36-item instrument represent
the dimensions of flow discussed by Csikszentmihalyi (1990, 1993), and
each scale is measured by four items. Development of items was based on
(a) past research with flow state both within and outside of sport settings,
(b) qualitative analysis of interviews with elite athletes, and (c) quantitative
analyses conducted in the present investigation. Internal consistency estimates
for the nine FSS scales were reasonable (alpha M = 3 3 ) for administration
of the scale to 394 athletes. Confirmatory factor analyses supported the nine
scales. Consistent with the theoretical basis of the FSS, there was also support
for a hierarchical model in which one global (higher order) flow factor
explained correlations among the nine first-order FSS factors. Suggestions
for use of the scale and for further research are discussed.

Key words: flow research, scale development, confirmatory factor analysis,


construct validity

The flow state, a positive experiential state, occurs when the performer is
totally connected to the performance, in a situation where personal skills equal
required challenges. It is a state aspired to by elite athletes (Jackson, 1992,
in press), but also one that can be enjoyed by any level of sport participant
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1992; Stein, Kimiecik, Daniels, & Jackson, 1995). Research
of flow has lagged behind experiential awareness of the state due to the inherent
difficulties of applying empirical methods to phenomenological experiences. Due
to the importance of flow state to concepts such as motivation, peak performance,
peak experience, and enjoyment, attempts to develop ways of assessing flow in
sport and activity settings are warranted. This investigation was designed to

Susan A. Jackson is with the Departments of Human Movement Studies and Psychol-
ogy at the University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, 4072, Australia. Herbert W. Marsh
is with the University of Western Sydney, Macarthur, P.O. Box 555, Campbelltown, NSW
2560, Australia.
18 / Jackson and Marsh

develop a psychometrically valid scale to assess flow state in sport and physical
activity settings. The primary aims of the project were to develop a scale that
assesses flow state in sport and physical activity and to conduct psychometric
assessments to establish the validity, reliability, and factor structure of the scale.

The Construct of Flow


Flow is an optimal psychological state that has been described at length
by Csikszentmihalyi (1975, 1990, 1993) and substantiated by others in a variety
of settings, including work, school, leisure, and sports (see Csikszentmihalyi &
Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Jackson, 1992, in press). When in flow, aperson becomes
totally involved in an activity and experiences a number of positive experiential
characteristics, including freedom from self-consciousness and great enjoyment
of the process. Flow is an intrinsically enjoyable state and is accompanied by
an order in consciousness whereby the person experiences clarity of goals and
knowledge of performance, complete concentration, feelings of control, and
feelings of being totally in tune with the performance. Jackson and Roberts
(1992) hypothesized that flow is the psychological process underlying peak
performance, and through examining athletes' descriptions of optimal perfor-
mances and scores on measures of flow, found correlational support for this
idea. In addition, qualitative analyses showed athletes' best performances were
associated with process-focused descriptions and flow state characteristics.
Understanding how flow is experienced by athletes is an interesting question
whose answer can help develop the theoretical understanding of the flow con-
struct. Studies by Jackson (1992, in press) focused on understanding elite athletes'
descriptions of being in flow, as well as those factors these athletes perceived
as influencing their ability to experience flow. Support was found for the relevance
of Csikszentmihalyi's characterization of flow into nine dimensions (Jackson, in
press). These dimensions are defined below and are illustrated with data from
Jackson's (1994) qualitative content analysis of elite athletes' flow descriptions.

Challenge-Skill Balance
In flow, the person perceives a balance between the challenges of a situation
and one's skills, with both operating at a personally high level. Csikszentmihalyi
and Csikszentmihalyi (1988) explain this dimension as occumng when a person's
skill is at just the right level to cope with the situational demands, which are
above average for the person. "Was challenging, but also seemed automatic,"
is how a track and field athlete described this flow dimension.

Action-Awareness Merging
Involvement in the flow activity is so deep that it becomes spontaneous
or automatic. There is no awareness of self as separate from the actions one is
performing. Statements such as "in the groove" and "things happen automati-
cally" were used by several athletes to describe action-awareness merging.
Flow State Scale / 19

Clear Goals
Goals in the activity are clearly defined (either set in advance or developed
out of involvement in the activity), giving the person in flow a strong sense of
what he or she is going to do. "Really knowing what you were going to do,"
is an example of this dimension from a rower's perspective.

Unambiguous Feedback
Immediate and clear feedback is received, usually from the activity itself,
allowing the person to know he or she is succeeding in the set goal. For example,
for one rower, "receiving feedback from my movements that I was at the right
pace" illustrates the ongoing feedback that sport activities give the performer.
Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszentmihalyi (1988) state that the kind of feedback
can be very diverse, but the result is the same: information that one is succeeding
in one's goal.

Concentration on Task at Hand


Total concentration on the task at hand occurs when in flow. "Feel really
focused," describes this dimension for a marathon runner. Total concentration
is one of the most frequently mentioned flow dimensions (Csikszentmihalyi,
1990).

Sense of Control
A sense of exercising control is experienced, without the person actively
trying to exert control. "Feel like can do anything in that state," and "You can't
imagine anything going wrong," illustrate how a runner and a rugby player,
respectively, experienced the sense of control when in flow. The labeling of this
dimension by Csikszentmihalyi has changed from being "in control" (1975, p.
44), to the "paradox of control" (1990, p. 59), to "sense of control" (1993, p.
181). What seems critical to this dimension is that it is the potential for control,
especially the sense of exercising control in difficult situations, that is central to
the flow experience.

Loss of Self-Consciousness
Concern for the self disappears during flow as the person becomes one
with the activity. When freed from self-consciousness, the athlete often becomes
a more natural performer, where "doing things instinctively and confidently"
becomes evident in the athlete's actions. The absence of preoccupation with self
does not mean the person is unaware of what is happening in mind or body, but
rather is not focusing on the information normally used to represent to oneself
who one is.

Transformation of Time
Time alters perceptibly, either slowing down, as illustrated by a track runner
saying she had "time to think," or speeding up, giving the perception that the
20 / Jackson and Marsh

event was "over so fast" for a cyclist. Alternatively, time may simply become
irrelevant and out of one's awareness. Although listed as one of the dimensions
of flow, Csikszentmihalyi (1990) acknowledges that there are situations where
awareness of time is nedessary to successful execution of the activity, implying
that this time dimension may not be as universal as the other dimensions. Cer-
tainly, in some sports, knowledge of time is part of performing well (e.g., knowing
one's splits in swimming), and in qualitative research by Jackson (1992, l994),
this dimension has received more equivocal support from athlete populations.

Autotelic Experience
An autotelic experience is an intrinsically rewarding experience. This di-
mension is described by Csikszentmihalyi as the end result of being in flow. It
is illustrated by statements from athletes such as "really enjoy the experience"
and "leaves you on a high." Csikszentmihalyi (1990) describes the evolution
of the term autotelic as being derived from the Greek words auto (self) and telos
(goal) (p. 67). An activity is autotelic if it is done for its own sake, with no
expectation of some future reward or benefit.
This study examined the flow construct from the nine dimensions described
above. Items were developed to reflect each of the dimensions, and psychometric
tests were conducted to determine the most appropriate items and empirically
test the proposed multidimensional structure of flow.

Measurement of Flow
Flow, as a concept, is regarded as a critical psychological state that epito-
mizes optimal experience during sport participation (Jackson, 1992; Jackson &
Roberts, 1992; Kimiecik & Stein, 1992). However, little sport and exercise
psychology research has been conducted with flow as a variable, due to the
difficulty in measuring the concept. One approach, the experience-sampling
method (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1992; Kimiecik & Stein, 1992), requires
individuals to respond to a short questionnaire (or experience-sampling form)
whenever they receive random signals from an electronic beeper that is worn
for a predetermined period of time (typically a week). Further research is required,
however, to establish the reliability and validity of responses in sport and exercise
settings, and to address practical problems inherent in using this technique in
these settings (see Jackson, 1992; Kimiecik & Stein, 1992).
More generally, a multimethod approach is needed to understand flow,
incorporating both qualitative and quantitative research. Interview-based research
(Jackson, 1992, 1995, in press) has provided richness of description and insights
into athletes' experiences of flow. Greater understanding of flow and how it
relates to other psychological constructs will be possible when assessment of the
flow state can take place close to when it occurs, and when the measurement
procedures allow for comparison among psychological constructs. The develop-
ment of a psychometrically valid scale will open up possibilities for quantitatively
based investigations of flow, which can involve state assessments and compari-
sons with other psychological states.
The richness and complexity of a construct such as flow necessitates mea-
surements that are inclusive rather than exclusive. Several dimensions of the
Flow State Scale / 21

flow experience have been theoretically discussed and supported by research


(Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Jackson, 1992, in press). It is im-
portant to establish (through construct validation approaches) the dimensional
nature of flow and to develop instruments designed to measure the dimensions.
Sport and exercise psychology research has recognized the need for multidimen-
sional and sport-specific measurement instruments (Gill, Dzewaltowski, &
Deeter, 1998; Vealey, 1986), and there are examples of systematic research
programs that have advanced the measurement of several psychological variables
important to the field through the development of such instruments (Gill et al.,
1988; Marsh, in press; Martens, Burton, Vealey, Bump, & Smith, 1990; Smith,
Smoll, & Schutz, 1990). This investigation aims to provide a measurement
instrument that assesses flow as a multidimensional construct, and that can be
used easily in sport and physical activity settings.

A Construct Validity Approach


Flow is a hypothetical construct, and therefore, its usefulness must be
established by investigations of construct validity. Marsh (1990) discusses con-
struct validity as incorporating two approaches: within- and between-network
studies. Within-network studies explore the internal structure of flow. For ex-
ample, a within-network study might examine the dimensionality of flow in order
to establish whether the flow construct has consistent, distinct multidimensional
components. Within-network studies often use a factor analytic approach. Be-
tween-network studies attempt to establish a logical, theoretically consistent
pattern of relations between measures of flow and other constructs. It is important
to address at least some of the within-construct issues before moving to between-
construct research.
Consistent with this construct validity approach, Gill et al. (1988) argued
for the construction of multidimensional sport and exercise instruments based
on theory, followed by item and reliability analysis, exploratory and confirmatory
factor analysis, tests of convergent and divergent validity, and application in
research and practice. The usefulness of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for
these purposes is widely endorsed but is just beginning to be widely used in the
sports sciences. Schutz and Gessaroli (1993), for example, claim that this is the
statistical tool for the 1990s in many disciplines but lament its nonuse in sport
psychology. Hence, the purpose of the present investigation is to use CFA to
evaluate within-network issues about the internal structure of FSS responses and
to compare the ability of alternative models to explain FSS responses.

Methods
Initial Item Development I

Items were developed from the definitions of each of the nine proposed
dimensions of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 1993; Jackson, in press). In forming
an initial pool of items, earlier self-report scales designed to measure flow
independently or in combination with other variables (Begly, 1979; Csikszentmi-
halyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Privette, 1984; Privette & Bundrick, 1991)
22 / Jackson and Marsh

were examined and used as a reference base from which items were developed.
Jackson's (1992, 1995, in press) qualitative studies of flow state in elite athletes
were particularly important in developing the wording of items in that it provided
actual descriptions of flow states in athlete's own words.
An initial pool of 5 4 items, 6 items per scale, was then evaluated indepen-
dently by seven researchers familiar with the flow concept and who had used it
in sport research. These evaluators rated each item in terms of perceived relevancy
to its proposed dimension and provided feedback in terms of item wording. Items
rated as less relevant were replaced, and the wording of the items was improved
based on feedback from these evaluators. This process resulted in the 54 items
used on the initial version of the instrument. A pilot study was conducted with
this instrument using a sample of 252 respondents, all of whom were actively
participating in a sport or physical activity. Levels of involvement varied from
primarily recreational (29%), to club level (39%), to regional, state, or national
representative (42%).
The current version of the FSS was based, in part, on changes made in
responses to this earlier pilot study. In particular, several negatively or ambigu-
ously worded items were found to be less effective in item analyses of this pilot
data. These weak items were replaced with more clearly stated, positively worded
items. Of particular relevance to the present investigation, reliability estimates
for the current version of the FSS are substantially higher than those based on
the earlier version used in this pilot study.

Participants and Procedures

Participants in the present investigation were 394 athletes (67% male, 33%
female) from the United States (n = 244) and Australia (n = 150). The sample
represented a total of 38 different nationalities, although the majority were from
the United States (49%) or Australia (35%). A total of 41 different sports and
physical activities were represented, with the most frequently mentioned being
basketball (20%), track and field (1 I%), and field hockey (7%). Physical activities
such as aerobics, hiking, weight training, and jogging comprised approximately
5% of the total sample. Participants varied in age from 14 to 50, with a mean
age of 22 (SD = 5.4). Levels of participation varied from primarily recreational
(39%), to club or league (17%) or university varsity teams (19%), to state
(15%) or national (10%) representatives. As a group, the study participants had
participated in their chosen sport or activity from 1 to 37 years (M = 9.7 years),
and 7 1% had participated for over 5 years. A wide variety of settings was included
in the sample, with participants coming from physical activity classes, recreational
leagues, and state or national sport teams. Participants were recruited by con-
tacting coaches and physical educational instructors who provided initial permis-
sion to administer the questionnaire. The purpose of the study and standardized
instructions were given to all subjects in written or verbal form, along with a
guarantee of anonymity.
When answering the FSS, participants were asked to recall an optimal
experience during their sport participation, defined as "one where you were
totally absorbed in what your were doing, and which was very enjoyable." This
definition focused on two major components of the flow experience-absorption
Flow State Scale / 23

and enjoyment-without actually using the term flow or possibly related con-
structs such as peak performance or peak experience. Initially, participants were
asked to think of one specific experience that occurred while they were participat-
ing in their sport or physical activity that constituted an optimal experience.
Athletes were asked to name the experience, when it occurred, and the degree
to which challenges and skills were in balance during that experience. They then
responded to the flow items using a 5-point Likert-type response format (1 =
strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree) and to provide background demographic
information including their age, gender, education level, and sporting involve-
ment.
In order to ascertain that participants in this study did experience flow, or
optimal experiences, a question was asked about the frequency with which an
optimal experience was encountered over a year, and any participants who re-
sponded zero were excluded from analysis. Further, any respondents who failed
to identify a specific optimal experience, as set out in the instructions for answer-
ing the scale, were also excluded. As a further check of the validity of the optimal
experience identified, participants were asked to rate the challenges and skills
of the situation, in line with Csikszentmihalyi's (e.g., Csikszentmihalyi & Csiks-
zentmihalyi, 1988) operational definition of flow as occurring when the perceived
situation is challenging but skills are perceived to be high enough to meet the
challenge. A mean score of 8.3 for challenges and 7.3 for skills, both on 10-
point scales, supported at a group level the assumption that the experiences
participants identified and responded to when answering the scale were flow
experiences.
Participants were asked to think back to a time when they were in flow
and to answer the scale in relation to that particular experience to enable a
sufficiently large sample of flow experiences to be included in the analyses. The
scale instructions were subsequently modified to enable it to be used immediately
after an event or activity (see Appendix), as this was considered to be a more
useful format for future research with the scale.

Statistical Analyses
Confirmatory Factor Analysis. CFAs, performed with the mainframe ver-
sion of LISREL 7 (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1989), were used to test the a priori
factor structure underlying the FSS responses. In CFA, the researcher posits an
a priori structure and tests the ability of a solution based on this structure to fit
the data by demonstrating that (a) the solution is well defined, (b) parameter
estimates are consistent with theory and a priori predictions, and (c) the chi-
square likelihood ratio and subjective indices of fit are reasonable (Marsh, Balla, &
McDonald, 1988; McDonald & Marsh, 1990). For present purposes the Relative
Noncentrality Index (RNI) and the Nonnormed Fit Index (NNFI, also called
the Tucker-Lewis Index) recommended by McDonald and Marsh (1990) are
considered, as well as the chi-square test statistic and the root mean square error
of approximation (RMSEA).
The NNFI and RNI vary along a 0-to-l continuum in which values greater
than .9 are typically taken to reflect an acceptable fit, whereas the optimal RMSEA
is 0 and values less than .05 are typically taken to reflect an acceptable fit. The
RNI contains no penalty for a lack of parsimony so that the addition of new
24 / Jackson and Marsh

parameters automatically leads to an improved fit that may reflect capitalization


on chance, whereas the NNFI and RMSEA contain a penalty for a lack of
parsimony.
In the present study, a series of CFA models were tested, and results of
these analyses were used to develop a shorter version of the FSS. Three models
were fit to responses to the 54 items hypothesizing: (a) one first-order factor
(i.e., all 54 items loaded on a single factor); (b) nine first-order factors in which
each item was allowed to load on only one factor, six items were used to
define each scale, and correlations among the nine first-order factors were freely
estimated; and (c) one higher order factor in which correlations among the nine
first-order factors were hypothesized to reflect a single higher order factor.
Support for the one first-order factor model may support a global measure of
flow but would be inconsistent with the multidimensional perspective underlying
the design of the FSS. Support for the nine first-order factor model would suppoh
the multidimensionality of the flow construct, whereas the juxtaposition of this
first-order model and the corresponding higher order model provides a test of a
global flow construct.
An important aim of the study was to develop a shorter version of the FSS
that retained most of the psychometric strength of the longer instrument. Our
goal was to select an optimal set of four items per scale such that responses to
each scale maintained high levels of reliability (coefficient alphas of at least .a),
discriminated well among the nine hypothesized factors, and were well described
by the a priori nine-factor solution. Criteria used to select items included (a) the
size of factor loadings in the nine first-order factor solution, (b) corrected item-
total correlations from a traditional item analysis (which are highly related to
the CFA factor loadings), (c) goodness-of-fit measures for the nine first-order
factor model, and (d) LISREL's modification indices (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1989),
which provide an index of how much or how highly each item would load on
factors other than it was intended to define if allowed to do so (thus allowing
us to eliminate complex items that were substantially related to more than one
factor.
Absolute cut-off values for these various criteria were not appropriate,
because the goal was to select the best four items from each scale so long as
adequate reliability and overall goodness-of-fit could be maintained, and because
we used a subjective combination of several different criteria in selecting the
best items. More items would have been retained if we could not achieve adequate
reliability with only four items, but this was not necessary. Although this process
of item selection involved a degree of subjectivity, the process is supported in
that we were able to achieve the desired criteria of psychometric support for FSS
responses. The same three models (above) that were tested with responses to the
original 54 items were subsequently tested with responses to the subset of 36
items selected on the basis of these criteria.

Results and Discussion


Reliability
Coefficient alpha estimates of reliability are presented (Table 1) for re-
sponses to the current 54-item and 36-item versions of the FSS used in the present
Flow State Scale / 25

Table 1 Coefficient Alpha Estimates of Reliability From Different Versions


of the Flow State Scale

54-item Current Current


pilot 54-item 36-item
version version version
Scale (n = 252) ( n = 394) (n = 394)

Challenge-skill
Action-awareness
Clear goals
Unambiguous feedback
Concentration
Sense of control
Loss of self-consciousness
Transformation of time
Autoletic experience
Mean

Note. Reliability estimates under the column "54-item pilot version" are based on re-
sults from a previously unpulished study using an earlier version of the FSS. The re-
maining results are based on responses collected as part of the present investigation
using the entire 54 items and a subset of 36 of these 54 items.

investigation, and for responses to the earlier, 54-item pilot version of the FSS.
These preliminary results show that responses to the current 54-item and the 36-
item versions considered here are both substantially more reliable than responses
to the previous 54-item version used in the earlier pilot study. Whereas the
average reliability of scales from the 36-item version ( M = .83) are marginally
lower than the 54-item version ( M = .84), the differences are very small given
the one-third reduction in length. Furthermore, all nine FSS scales in the 36-
item version have reliabilities of at least .8, thus satisfying this criterion.

Factor Structure Underlying Responses


Goodness of fit was evaluated for alternative models based on the current
54-item (6 items per scale) and 36-item (4 items per scale) versions of the FSS
(Table 2). Both sets of analyses are consistent in demonstrating that a model
positing only one first-order factor provides a poor fit to the data, whereas a
model positing nine first-order (freely correlated) factors fits the data slightly
better than a model with nine first-order factors and one higher order factor.
Both the first-order and higher order models provide a reasonable fit of responses
to the 36-item version of the scale (e.g., RNIs > .9), whereas the fit of these
models to responses to the 54-item version is marginal. These results, even more
strongly than the corresponding reliability estimates (see Table l), provide support
26 / Jackson and Mar-sh

Table 2 Goodness of Fit: Alternative Models Based on 54 and 36 Items

Model

Six items per factor-


1 First-order 5,83 1.47 1,377 .582 .566 .092
9 First-order 3,273.39 1,341 .819 .807 ,061
1 Higher-order 3,449.62 1,368 ,805 ,796 .063
Four- items per- jLhctor
1 First-order 3,447.37 594 .573 ,547 .I 12
9 First-order 1,124.95 585 .915 ,904 .05 1
1 Higher-order 1,254.21 585 .900 ,892 .055

Note. RNI = Relative Noncentrality Index. NNFI = Nonnormed Fit Index. RMSEA =
root mean square error of approximation. Alternative models posited one first-order fac-
tor, nine first-order factors (the a priori model), and one-higher order factor (based on
the correlations among the nine first-order factors).

for the shorter version of the FSS. For this reason, we only present parameter
estimates from the 36-item version.
Parameter estimates based on responses to the 36-item version of the FSS
(Table 3) provide good support for the a priori nine-factor structure with freely
estimated factor correlations and, thus, the construct validity of FSS responses.
The factor loadings are substantial in that all are greater than .5, and most are
greater than .7 (median factor loading = .74). It is also important to note that
although all 36 correlations among the nine a priori factors are all positive, none
is greater than .73 or approaches 1.0. The size of the correlations, varying from
.I77 to .724 (median r = SO), provides good support for the separation of the
FSS factors.
The corresponding higher order factor structure needs to be evaluated
carefully (for further discussion of the evaluation of higher order factor models
see Marsh, 1987; Marsh & Hocevar, 1985). The higher order model is nested
under the first-order model in that it attempts to explain the correlations among
the nine first-order factors in terms of a single higher order factor. Because the
two models are nested, the chi-square for the higher order model must be as
large or larger than the chi-square for the corresponding first-order model and
the difference in chi-squares for the two models (129.26) relative to the difference
in degree of freedom (27) can be used to test whether the difference between
the two models is statistically significant. Although the difference is statistically
significant, the goodness-of-fit indices demonstrate that the differences are not
large (e.g., NNFIs of .904 vs. .892).
Because the factor loadings (and, thus the uniquenesses) for the two models
are very similar, only these estimates for the first-order model (Table 3) are
presented. There are, however, some substantial differences in the sizes of the
factor correlations inferred from the two models. Although the freely estimated
factor correlations already summarized (Table 3) are relatively modest, the corre-
lations among factors based on the higher order model (Table 4) are somewhat
Flow State Scale / 27

Table 3 Nine-Factor Confirmatory Factor Analysis Solution

Factor loadings
Factor
item Chal. Act Goal Fdbk. Conc. Cont. Loss Tran. Enjoy Uniqueness

Challenge-skill balance
Q1 .608 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
Q10 ,770 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
Q19 ,725 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
Q28 .732 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
Action-a~jarenessmerging
Q2 .O ,662 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
Qll .O .746 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
420 .O .783 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
Q29 .O .790 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O
Clear goals
43 .O .O .713 .O .O .O .O .O .O
Q12 .O .O .772 .O .O .O .O .O .O
421 .O .O .787 .O .O .O .O .O .O
Q30 .O .O .731 .O .O .O .O .O .O
Unambiguous feedback
Q4 .O .O .O .636 .O .O .O .O .O
Q13 .O .O .O 232 .O .O .O .O .O
422 .O .O .O 304 .O .O .O .O .O
431 .O .O .O ,798 .O .O .O .O .O
Concentration on task at hand
Q5 .O .O .O .O .683 .O .O .O .O
Q14 .O .O .O .O .607 .O .O .O .O
423 .O .O .O .O 359 .O .O .O .O
432 .O .O .O .O 209 .O .O .O .O
Paradox of control
46 .O .O .O .O .o .749 .o .o .o
Q15 .O .O .O .O .O .746 .O .O .O
Q24 .O .O .O .O .O .765 .O .O .O
433 .O .O .O .O .O 369 .o .o .o
Loss of self-consciousness
47 .O .O .O .O .O .O .729 .O .O
416 .O .O .O .O .O .O .561 .O .O
425 .O .O .O .O .O .O .708 .O .O
Q34 .O .O .O .O .O .O .882 .O .O
Transformation of time
48 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .739 .O
417 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .748 .O
426 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .717 .O
Q35 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .715 .O

(continued)
28 / Jackson and Marsh

Table 3 (continued)

Factor Factor loadings


item Chal. Act Goal Fdbk. Conc. Cont. Loss Tran. Enjoy Uniqueness

Autotelic experience
Q9 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .659 .435
Q18 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .736 .541
427 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .689 .475
436 .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .O .771 .594

Factor correlations
Factor Chal. Act Goal Fdbk. Conc. Cont. Loss Tran. Enjoy

Chal.
Act.
Goal
Fdbk.
Conc.
Cont.
Loss
Tran.
Enjoy

Note. Chal. = challenge-skill balance: Act = action-awareness merging; Goal = clear


goals; Fdbk. = unambiguous feedback; Conc. = total concentration; Cont. = sense of
control; Loss = loss of self-consciousness; Tran. = transformation of time; Enjoy =
autotelic (enjoyable) experience. All parameter estimates are presented in completely
standardized form and are statistically significant at the .O1 level. See Appendix for the
wording of the items.

larger (rs vary from .224 to .795, median r = .53). This is, of course, consistent
with the requirement that all correlations among the factors fit with a single
higher order factor.
It is also relevant to compare the sizes of factor loadings of each first-
order on the higher order (global flow) factor or, equivalently, the correlation
between each first-order factor and the higher order factor. The largest factor
loading is for the Sense of Control factor, followed closely by the Challenge-Skill
Balance, Clear Goals, and Concentration factors. Although all first-order factors
load significantly on the higher order factor, it is interesting to note that the
factor loadings for the Transformation of Time and, to a lesser extent, Loss of
Self-Consciousness, are substantially lower. This may call into question the
importance of at least the Transformation of Time component of flow, although
the evaluation of this suggestion requires further research.
In evaluating the solution based on the higher order model, it is also
Flow State Scale / 29

Table 4 Correlations Among First-Order and Higher Order Factors

Factor Chal. Act Goal Fdbk. Conc. Cont. Loss Tran. Enjoy

First-order
Chal. 1.0
Act .624 1.0
Goal .726 .588 1.0
Fdbk. .658 .533 .620 1.0
Conc. .725 .588 .683 .620 1.0
Cont. .795 .644 .749 .679 .748 1.0
Loss .509 .412 .479 .435 .479 .525 1.0
Tran. .339 .275 .320 .290 .319 .350 .224 1.0
Enjoy .607 .492 .572 .519 .572 .626 .401 .267 1.0
Higher order
Global .878 .711 .827 .750 326 .905 .580 .387 ,692
Residual (unexplained) variance in first-order factors
.289 .591 .444 .543 .342 .242 .731 378 .549

Note. Chal. = challenge-skill balance; Act = action-awareness merging; Goal = clear


goals; Fdbk. = unambiguous feedback; Conc. = total concentration; Cont. = sense of
control; Loss = loss of self-consciousness; Tran. = transformation of time; Enjoy =
Autotelic (enjoyable) experience. Correlations among first-order factors are based on
the CFA model positing one higher order factor and tend to be higher than those based
on corresponding nine-factor solution (Table 3) with no higher order factor. The corre-
lation between each first-order factor and the higher order factor is equal to the factor
loading of each first-order factor on the higher order factor. Residual variances are the
proportion of "true score variance" (i.e., nonerror variance) in each first-order factor
that is unxplained by the higher order factor. All parameters were statistically signifi-
cant ( p < .01).

informative to evaluate the residual variance estimates for each first-order factor-
the proportion of variance in that factor that cannot be accounted for by the
higher order factor (see Marsh, 1987). These estimates vary from .242 to 3 7 8
(i.e., between 24% and 88% of the variance in the first-order factors is unexplained
by the higher order factor), indicating that much of the variance cannot be
explained by the higher order factor. This observation is also consistent with the
very poor fit of the one first-order factor model in which covariation among
responses to all 36 items is explained by a single factor. Hence, even though the
results support the higher order representation of the flow construct as assessed
by the FSS, the FSS responses cannot be explained very well by a single score
or factor.

Summary and Implications


This investigation is part of an ongoing attempt to'develop a psychometri-
cally valid and usable scale for assessing flow in sport and physical activity
30 I Jackson and Marsh

settings. The results showing good reliability and support for the hypothesized
factor structure are promising. The a priori model hypothesizing nine FSS factors
was supported, and there was also support for a hierarchical model. Of particular
importance these two models fit the data substantially better than the model
hypothesizing only one factor.
The comparison of the first-order model with nine correlated factors with
the higher order factor model has important practical implications about how to
best represent FSS responses. Support for the higher order model with a single
global flow factor does not mean that FSS responses can be explained in terms
of a single score. Support for this contention would require that the first-order
model positing one factor could fit the data, and this is clearly not the case. In
fact, the large residual variances for at least some of the nine first-order factors
demonstrates that there is considerable variance in at least some of the first-order
factors that cannot be explained in terms of a higher order factor. Hence, to
explain adequately the variance in FSS responses requires nine scores representing
I the nine FSS factors.
The measurement of distinct components of flow provides a better basis
for evaluating the theoretical underpinning of the FSS than reliance on a global
score. For example, the relatively lower factor loadings for the Transformation
of Time and Loss of Self-Consciousness factors found in the CFA may mean
these dimensions of the flow experience are less universally important than other
of the flow dimensions. Previous research has found both of these dimensions
to be less supported than the other dimensions in athletes' experiences (Jackson,
1992, 1994; Jackson & Roberts, 1992). It may be that the nature of sport perfor-
mances demands awareness of time and of how the self is being presented, making
these two factors less significant or relevant to the athlete's flow experience.
On the other hand, the moderate second-order factor loading relating Autotelic
Experience to global flow was unexpected. As described by Csikszentmihalyi
(1990), this dimension is crucial to the flow experience. Indeed, Csikszentmihalyi
often substitutes the terms autotelic experience or enjoyment for flow, implying
that this factor may be seen as having a more global nature than the other flow
dimensions.
The fact that other dimensions, such as concentration, control, and chal-
lenge-skill balance had higher factor loadings than autotelic experience may
indicate that the enjoyment dimension is less central than other aspects of flow
to athletes. Perhaps enjoyment is taken for granted to some extent in a free choice
activity like sport. Another possibility may be that because of the goal-directed
nature of competitive sport, enjoyment is seen as somewhat antithetical to the
serious nature of the endeavor. The Clear Goals factor was substantially related
to the higher order flow construct, providing some support to this suggestion.
Studies focusing on the relationship among the various first-order factors and
the higher order factor, as well as their relationship with specified person and
situation factors, are needed.
The relative usefulness of a single global FSS score compared to the set
of nine FSS scale scores is a question open to further consideration. In evaluating
this issue, it is informative to reflect on the analogous, more widely considered
issue of the relative usefulness of a global measure of intelligence (e.g., IQ)
compared to the specific components of intelligence that make up intelligence.
Although a single global measure of intelligence may suffice in some situations,
Flow State Scale / 3 1

there are other situations in which the more detailed information about specific
abilities is required. More than 100years of research into this debate in intelligence
testing has not resolved the issue. Similarly, there may be instances where a
single global flow will suffice, but other situations where the more detailed
information from the specific components of flow are more useful. Hence, there
is no absolute answer to the question about whether it is appropriate to limit
consideration to a single global flow score.
In pondering this dilemma, however, several considerations are relevant.
A global measure of flow implicitly implies that the weighting assigned to each
specific component is the same for all situations and all individuals, but a more
flexible definition of flow that allows the weighting of specific components to
vary depending on the application may be appropriate. Also, it is a mathematical
necessity that an optimally weighted average of the specific components will be
able to explain as much or more variance in any criterion measure as a single
global score derived from the same responses. Furthermore, it is possible that
very distinct profiles of specific flow components having substantively important
implications could result in the same global score component.
Because there is some variance in most of the first-order factors and substan-
tial amounts in a few that cannot be explained by the higher order factor, it is
possible (and perhaps likely) that some external criterion variables of interest
can be explained better by first-order factors than they can by the second-order
factor. Hence, the relative usefulness of the global and specific components of
FSS responses cannot be fully evaluated until the FSS instrument has been used
much more extensively and related to a much wider set of validity criteria.
Therefore, we prefer to consider both until there is a sufficient research basis
for providing a better evaluation of these alternative operationalizations of the
flow construct.
Two limitations of the present investigation are the retrospective ap-
proach to data collection and the problems inherent in attempting to quantify
experiential states. In relation to the retrospective approach taken to data
collection, it was decided that asking participants to respond to a previous
flow experience that stood out for them would be more useful than having
respondents complete the scale after a performance that may or may not
have been a flow experience. However, the scale as it is presented here (see
Appendix) is designed to be used immediately, or soon after, performance, as
an assessment of flow state characteristics experienced during the performance.
Hence, further research is needed to ascertain that results presented are general-
izable. Also, a trait version of the FSS instrument is currently being developed
to help establish whether there are individual differences that may be related
to ability to experience flow.
The second limitation of this study is one encountered in all research that
attempts to quantify experiential states. Qualitative research may be better able
to capture the richness of a complex phenomenological state such as flow.
However, there are limitations to qualitative approaches, including the problems
of retrospective recall and the time-consuming nature of the research process.
The richness and depth of data acquired through qualitative methods can be
combined with the more easily collected and comparable state assessments of a
flow scale to provide a more complete picture of the flow construct. The fact
that a diverse sample was used in the present study adds support to the potential
32 / Jackson and Marsh

generalizability of the scale. However, it is also possible that the factor structure
based on such a heterogeneous sample may not generalize to specific subgroups
within this larger group and to more specific samples in future research. Hence,
it is important to replicate these results with other samples, both diverse and
specific.
Any attempts to investigate flow are fraught with difficulties and limitations.
Part of the attraction of the flow state lies in its mystique. Flow cannot be fully
captured by a score on a questionnaire, experience sampling methods, or in-
depth interviews. Csikszentmihalyi (1992) cautions against putting too much
weight on any empirical measures of flow, lest the experience of flow be lost
in the process: "The moment we say that 'flow is the balance of challenges and
skills,' or that 'flow is a score of "x" on the flow questionnaire,' we have lost
it. We have mistaken the reflection for the reality" (p. 183).
In accord with this statement, the FSS instrument is presented as one
apparently useful indicator of the flow construct. The development of this
instrument was motivated by a desire to bring the flow construct to a level
of research potential that will hopefully lead to elucidation of its nature and
of the factors and situations conducive to its experience. Better understanding
of flow and the factors related to its occurrence (see Jackson, 1995) is the
path to making flow a more accessible experience to all athletes and all
physical activity participants.
We do not claim that the FSS instrument is the only, or even the best, way
to study the flow construct. Indeed, a useful direction for further research is to
more systematically compare the results of various methods of inferring flow
when used with the same group of respondents. This type of multimethod study
is a logical next step in the construct validity approach that is the basis of
the present investigation. Between-network studies that examine the pattern of
relationships between flow state, as measured by the FSS, and other psychological
constructs will also help to further understanding of the flow construct, its anteced-
ents, and its consequences.
We endorse a broad approach to construct validation (e.g., Marsh, in press).
From this perspective, useful directions of future research include relating FSS
responses to a wide variety of external validity criteria (including alternative
measures of flow), comparing FSS scores from a variety of different groups
predicted a priori to differ in terms of flow, relating changes in flow to changes
in other constructs in multiwave studies, and using the FSS as an outcome variable
in intervention studies. Consistent with this perspective, instrument design and
evaluation, theory, research, and practice are inexorably intertwined so that all
will suffer if any one is neglected.

References
Begly, G. (1979). A self-report measure to assess flow in physical activities. Unpublished
masters thesis, Pennsylvania State University.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York:
Harper & Row.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1992). A response to the Kimiecik & Stein and Jackson papers.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 4 , 181- 183.
Flow State Scale / 33

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self. New York: Harper & Row.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological
studies offlow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1992). Validity and reliability of the experience
sampling method. In M. de Vries (Ed.), The experience of psychopathology. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Gill, D.L., Dzewaltowski, D.A., & Deeter, T.E. (1988). The relationship of competitiveness
and achievement orientation to participation in sport and nonsport activities. Journal
of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 10, 139- 150.
Jackson, S.A. (1992). Athletes in flow: A qualitative investigation of flow states in elite
figure skaters. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 4, 161-180.
Jackson, S.A. (1995). Factors influencing the occurrence of flow state in elite athletes.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 7 , 138- 166.
Jackson, S.A. (in press). Athletes in flow: Toward a conceptual understanding of flow
state in elite athletes. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport.
Jackson, S.A., & Roberts, G.C. (1992). Positive performance states of athletes: Toward
a conceptual understanding of peak performance. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 156-
171.
Joreskog, K.G., & Sorbom, D. (1989). Lisrel 7: A guide to program applications (2nd
ed.). Chicago: SPSS.
Kimiecik, J.C., & Stein, G.L. (1992). Examining flow experiences in sport contexts:
Conceptual issues and methodological concerns. Journal of Applied Sport Psychol-
ogy, 4, 144-160.
Marsh, H.W. (1987). The hierarchical structure of self-concept: An application of
hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Educational Measurement,
24, 17-39.
Marsh, H.W. (1990). A multidimensional, hierarchical self-concept: Theoretical and empir-
ical justification. Educational Psychology Review, 2, 77-171.
Marsh, H.W. (in press). The measurement of physical self-concept: A construct validation
approach. In K.R. Fox, (Ed.) The physical self: From motivation to well-being.
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Marsh, H.W., Balla, J.R., & McDonald, R.P. (1988). Goodness-of-fit indices in confirma-
tory factor analysis: The effect of sample size. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 391-
410.
Marsh, H.W., & Hocevar, D. (1985). The application of confirmatory factor analysis to
the study of self-concept: First and higher order factor structures and their invariance
across age groups. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 562-582.
Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R., Bump, L.A., & Smith, D.E. (1990). Development
and validation of the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2. In R. Martens, R.S.
Vealey, & D. Burton, Competitive state anxiety in sport (pp. 117-190). Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics.
McDonald, R.P., & Marsh, H.W. (1990). Choosing a multivariate model: Noncentrality
and goodness-of-fit. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 247-255.
Privette, G. (1984). Experience Questionnaire. Pensacola: University of West Florida.
Privette, G., & Bundrick, C.M. (1991). Peak experience, peak performance, and flow:
Personal descriptions and theoretical constructs. Journal of Social Behavior and
Personality, 6, 169-188.
Schutz, R.W., & Gessaroli, M.E. (1993). Use, misuse, and disuse of psychometrics in
sport psychology research. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphy, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.),
Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 901-9 17). New York: Macmillan.
Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., & Schutz, R.W. (1990). Measurement and correlates of sport-
specific cognitive and somatic trait anxiety: The Sport Anxiety Scale. Anxiety
Research, 2, 263-280.
34 / Jackson and Marsh

Stein, G.1., Kimiecik, J.C., Daniels, J., & Jackson, S.A. (1995). Psychological antecedents
of flow in recreational sport. Personality and Social Psycholo,py Bulletin, 21, 125-
135.
Vealey, R.S. (1986). Conceptualization of sport-confidence and competitive orientation:
Preliminary investigation and instrument development. Journal oflport Psycholo,qy,
8, 221-246.

Appendix

Flow State Scale


Please answer the following questions in relation to your experience in the event you
have just completed. These questions relate to the thoughts and feelings you may have
experienced during the event. There are no right or wrong answers. Think about how
you felt during the event and answer the questions using the rating scale below. Circle
the number that best matches your experience from the options to the right of each
question.

Rating Scale:
Strongly Neither agree Strongly
disagree Disagree nor disagree Agree agree
1 2 3 4 5
Strongly Strongly
disagree agree

1. I was challenged, but I believed my skills


would allow me to meet the challenge.
2. I made the correct movements without think-
ing about trying to do so.
3. I knew clearly what I wanted to do.
4. It was really clear to me that I was doing well.
5. My attention was focused entirely on what I
was doing.
6. I felt in total control of what I was doing.
7. I was not concerned with what others may
have been thinking of me.
8. Time seemed to alter (either slowed down or
speeded up).
9. I really enjoyed the experience.
10. My abilities matched the high challenge of
the situation.
11. Things just seemed to be happening automati-
cally.
12. I had a strong sense of what I wanted to do.
13. I was aware of how well I was performing.
14. It was no effort to keep my mind on what was
happening.
15. I felt like I could control what I was doing.
16. I was not worried about my performance dur-
ing the event.
Flow State Scale / 35

17. The way time passed seemed to be different 1 2 3 4 5


from normal.
18. I loved the feeling of that performance and 1 2 3 4 5
want to capture it again.
19. I felt I was competent enough to meet the
high demands of the situation.
20. I performed automatically.
21. I knew what I wanted to achieve.
22. I had a good idea while I was performing
about how well I was doing.
23. I had total concentration.
24. I had a feeling of total control.
25. I was not concerned with how I was presenting
myself.
26. It felt like time stopped while I was per-
forming.
27. The experience left me feeling great.
28. The challenge and my skills were at an equally
high level.
29. I did things spontaneously and automatically
without having to think.
30. My goals were clearly defined. 1 2 3 4 5
31. I could tell by the way I was performing how 1 2 3 4 5
well I was doing.
32. 1was completely focused on the task at hand. 1 2 3 4 5
33. I felt in total control of my body. 1 2 3 4 5
34. I was not worried about what others may have 1 2 3 4 5
been thinking of me.
35. At times, it almost seemed like things were 1 2 3 4 5
happening in slow motion.
36. I found the experience extremely rewarding. 1 2 3 4 5
O S.A. Jackson, University of Queensland, I995

Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the following for their assistance: Wally Karnilowicz, for
assistance in early phases of data analyses; Daryl Marchant, Robert Eklund, Jeff Martin,
Nicole Djmarjin, Nicholas Francis, and Jeremy Dover, for their assistance with data
collection; and the seven researchers who evaluated the initial pool of items. We would
also like to thank the Victorian Institute of Sport and the many coaches who granted
access to their athletes as participants in this study. Part of this study was funded by a
grant from Victoria University of Technology when the first author was working there.
The Flow State Scale (Appendix) may be used for research purposes without any prior
written consent as long as appropriate recognition is given, but the first author would
appreciate being sent copies of resulting publications.

Manuscript submitted: April 4, 1995


Revision accepted: August 13, 1995

You might also like